Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Nov 1.
Published in final edited form as: Am J Hum Biol. 2010 Nov-Dec;22(6):725–730. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.21073

Breastfeeding and Later Psychosocial Development in the Philippines

Paulita Duazo 1,*, Josephine Avila 1, Christopher W Kuzawa 2
PMCID: PMC2957551  NIHMSID: NIHMS224699  PMID: 20721986

Abstract

Objectives

Evaluate whether breastfeeding duration predicts later psychosocial development in a large low socioeconomic status (SES) sample in the Philippines.

Methods

The sample consists of 2,752 children aged 5-6 years who were measured in 2004 as part of the Philippine government's Early Childhood Development Project (ECD). Duration of any breastfeeding was the primary independent variable in regression models predicting a cumulative index of psychosocial development that has been shown previously to predict school readiness.

Results

In this sample, mothers who breastfed their children for longer tended to have lower educational attainment and to come from lower income households. Despite this, breastfeeding duration was a positive predictor of future psychosocial development (PD) measured in late childhood, but only after adjustment for SES and related confounders.

Conclusions

These findings add to growing evidence that breastfeeding could provide lasting economic and social benefits and underscore the importance of continuing current public health efforts to promote breastfeeding in the Philippines and across the globe.

Keywords: child growth, human capital, infant nutrition, school readiness

Introduction

The health benefits of breastfeeding for child nutritional status and growth are a primary rationale for promoting breastfeeding in developing nations (Popkin et al., 1990). Breastfeeding has also received attention for its demographic consequences as a suppressor of ovulation and protection against short interbirth intervals (Thapa et al., 1988; Labbok, 2008). More recent attention has focused on the potential for improved early life nutrition, including breastfeeding, to lead to improvements in offspring health and economic performance that persist into adulthood (Barker, 1994; Gluckman and Hanson, 2006; Palloni, 2006; Stuebe, 2009). Breastfeeding has been linked to lower rates of adult chronic diseases, including obesity and diabetes (Schack-Nielsen and Michaelsen, 2006; Ip et al., 2007). Studies also find that children exclusively breastfed or who were breastfed for longer score better in mid-childhood (Daniels and Adair, 2005; Quinn et al., 2001; Michaelsen et al., 2003; Sacker et al., 2006; Kramer et al., 2008) and in adulthood (Richards et al., 2002) on tests of motor or cognitive development.

In less developed countries, children often fail to attain optimum cognitive development because of poverty, malnutrition, and poor health (Grantham-McGregor et al., 2007; Behrman, 1996). In this setting, women who are less educated or who have lower socioeconomic status may not be able to afford infant formula, and may also have more time available to breastfeed compared to women with higher income or education (Negi and Kandpal, 2004; Skafida, 2009). Thus, breastfeeding may be most readily available as a strategy to improve offspring health, and may also be most common, among the poorest mothers whose offspring are at highest risk for poor developmental outcomes.

The broader social and economic impacts of breastfeeding could be highly relevant in the Philippines, where the government currently has a policy of promoting breastfeeding (CWC, 2000; Philippines, 1992). Despite this, few studies have investigated the potential impacts of breastfeeding on offspring health and development in the country. One study in the Philippines reported a beneficial effect of longer duration of breastfeeding on IQ score measured in mid- and late-childhood (Daniels and Adair, 2005). The apparent benefits of having been breastfed were detected only after adjusting for the lower educational attainment and lower socioeconomic standing that characterized women in the sample who breastfed longer. These findings, which used data collected from metropolitan Cebu City in the 1980s, suggest that the benefits of breastfeeding in the Philippines extend to child IQ, which is an important measure of human capital.

In this paper, we build upon this finding and evaluate possible benefits of breastfeeding on child development among a predominantly rural poor population in the Philippines measured in 2005. The outcome that we investigate is a composite index of psychosocial development obtained when children were 5 or 6 years old. This scale reflects a child's performance across 7 developmental domains that collectively index readiness to enter and succeed in primary school. Data come from a large, population-based longitudinal survey of child nutrition and growth that was recently conducted in central regions of the Philippines (Armecin et al., 2006). Follow-up data on psychosocial development, used together with data on duration of breastfeeding and on a range of potential socioeconomic, educational and other confounding factors, provide a unique opportunity to evaluate the broader effects of breastfeeding on psychosocial development in a lower income, rural subset of Philippine society.

Materials and Methods

Study population

This study uses data from a longitudinal evaluation of the Early Childhood Development (ECD) intervention, which was part of the larger Baseline Indicators Study of the Early Childhood Development Project initiated by the Philippine government. The program was initiated in 3 regions with a focus on relatively disadvantaged municipalities, measured by child and maternal characteristics (Ghuman et al., 2006). Communities included in the study were identified as either “at risk” or “in need” of additional services, as indicated by high rates of infant mortality, childhood wasting, and a high elementary school dropout rate. The goal of the ECD Project was to improve the survival and developmental potential of at-risk children in predominantly rural regions of the country (Office of Population Studies, 2002). Rather than changing existing programs, it provided technical and financial support to local government units (LGUs) with the intent of enabling delivery of improved services for pregnant mothers and children.

The evaluation study collected information on program implementation, and on the quantity and quality of service delivery in health- and non-health services. It also collected household information, child-rearing behaviors, and maternal health and health care-seeking practices. Outcome measures included indices of child growth, health and development. At baseline, the study enrolled 7,925 children representative of 0-4 year old infants and children in project areas (Fig. 1). Follow-ups were conducted at yearly intervals with a total of 4 surveys completed by the end of the study in 2005. During the Year 4 survey, members of the baseline cohort ranged in age from 4 to 9 years, with a sample size of 6,871. Attrition was due mainly to migration (9.3%), deaths (0.4%), refusals and others (2.2%) (Armecin et al., 2006; Office of Population Studies, 2005).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Map of the Philippines. Project areas are presented in dark grey and include a total of 288 barangays (administrative units) located in 96 municipalities.

For the present analyses, we limit our sample to children who were followed since baseline and who were 5-6 years of age during the psychosocial assessment conducted in Study Year 4. This corresponds to the age of primary school entry in this sample. In addition, children with physical and neurological abnormalities that could influence test performance, such as esotropia, autism or cerebral palsy (57 or 2.1%), and those with incomplete data (32 or 1.2%), were also excluded from the analysis. The final analysis sample was 2,752. We evaluated whether the sub-sample differed significantly from the total sample with respect to key socioeconomic and other characteristics. Children in the analysis sample had mothers who were 10 months younger (p<0.05) and were more likely to have their fathers present (p<0.05). However, there were no significant differences in mother's or father's educational attainment, breastfeeding duration, birth weight and child's health at two years.

Variables

Psychosocial development

Psychosocial development was assessed using the Philippines Revised Early Childhood Development Checklist (REC) in which the skills listed were developmentally sequenced by age. The REC was designed primarily to emphasize specific strengths and weaknesses in the child's developmental stage and to aid caregivers and health providers in their assessment of the different stages of development among children aged 0 to 84 months (Bautista-de los Angeles and Reyes, 2001; Office of Population Studies, 2005). For older children the tool was designed to measure the personal and social adjustment, literacy, and school readiness skills of children. It includes items, which are important pre-requisites for school readiness including emergent literacy skills and psychosocial attributes essential to adjust to school life. It covers seven developmental domains including gross motor, fine motor, receptive language, expressive language, social-emotional, self help and cognitive function. Overall psychosocial development was defined as a composite of the seven underlying domains (Cronbach's alpha =0.86). Prior work has shown that it is a strong predictor of school readiness in this cohort of children (r=0.48). Before analysis, each domain score and the composite psychosocial development scores were converted into age-specific norms based upon a reference population composed of 10,915 children from six regions in the Philippines (Office of Population Studies, 2002). The scaled scores for each domain and the composite scores were derived and used to classify developmental indices for each child in the study. The scaled score ranged from 1-19 with a standard deviation of 3; the composite score was reported as a standard score ranging from 35-150 with a standard deviation of 15 (Office of Population Studies, 2005).

The psychosocial development checklist was administered during the 4 surveys. During the administration of the checklist, children were observed by the tester as they performed the developmental tasks, while for other questions mothers or caretakers were asked whether children were able to perform each task. At the end of the Year 4 survey, children were 4 to 9 years of age. The composite score at age 5 years had a relatively large correlation (Pallant, 2000) with fine motor, self-help, expressive language, cognitive, and social-emotional domains (r's=0.55,0.58,0.53,0.64 & 0.64, p<0.05, respectively), with a comparable pattern observed at age 6. As expected, mean scores increased with age (108.6 at 6 years vs. 99.4 at 5 years, p<0.05).

Duration of any breastfeeding

Our main exposure was duration of any breastfeeding (BF), which was evaluated through maternal recall at each of the yearly follow up surveys. Maternal recall of BF duration has been shown to be accurate and reliable in validation studies, many of which have included a longer period between breastfeeding and recall than our yearly surveys (Gillespie et al., 2006; Promislow et al., 2005; Kark et al., 1984). Individuals were assigned to one of five categories representing duration of breastfeeding, corresponding to the recent study of Daniels and Adair (2005): 0 - 5 months (comparison group), 6-11, 12-17, 18-23 and 24+ months of breastfeeding. All analyses incorporated the “never breastfed” individuals into the 0-5 month group, since similar results were obtained when “never breastfed” individuals (∼5% of the sample) were grouped together with the 0-5 month group and when “never breastfed” was modeled using a separate dichotomous variable.

Potential confounding variables

A range of potential confounders of any association between breastfeeding and later psychosocial development were identified a priori based upon similar published studies (Daniels and Adair, 2005; Clark et al., 2006; Sacker et al., 2006) and from associations within the data set. These included gender, age, daycare attendance, existence of offspring health problems at birth, prenatal care, mother's education, father's presence within the household, access to electricity, an assessment of household hygiene, household assets and ECD program exposure. Exposure to program services has been shown to significantly improve child psychosocial status (Ghuman et al., 2006). Household wealth or non-income producing assets were evaluated by tallying ownership of common household appliances. Hygiene status, with scores ranging from 0-12 (0 being poorly kept), reflected cleanliness inside the house, food storage, presence of excreta outside the house, and garbage and waste disposal.

Statistical analysis

All statistical analyses were conducted using STATA Version 10 (College Station, TX). Analysis began with consistency testing of the composite index derived from the 7 domains of psychosocial development using Cronbach's Alpha. To examine selectivity due to sample attrition of the children between baseline and the Year 4 survey, a likelihood test of children participating in the Year 4 survey based on selected baseline characteristics was applied. Bivariate analyses were performed using simple descriptive statistics, with differences across levels of breastfeeding duration evaluated using chi-square and ANOVA. Unadjusted relationships between breastfeeding duration and of the composite development score in the final survey were evaluated using least squares regression. Finally, the relationship between duration of breastfeeding and psychosocial development was considered after adjusting for factors that might confound or mask the association between breastfeeding duration and later psychosocial maturity using multiple regression models. Backward elimination using the change-in-estimate approach was used to determine which a priori potential confounding factors significantly influenced the models, and thus, which were retained in the final model. If exclusion of the variable in the model substantially (>10%) changed the breastfeeding duration effect estimate, the variable was retained in the model. Because prior research in the Philippines has shown a diminishing effect of breastfeeding on IQ with increasing age/time elapsed since breastfeeding (Daniels and Adair, 2005), we stratified all models by age during the final survey when psychosocial development was evaluated.

Results

Children were breastfed for an average of 14 months. The majority had mothers who sought prenatal care during pregnancy and for whom birth complications were not a problem. At baseline, less than one-fourth of the children were given multivitamin supplementations. At the time of psychosocial evaluation, more than half of the children in the sample had attended daycare. More than half of the children lived in ECD program barangays which provided greater access to the ECD interventions. Maternal age was 32 years on average and about 94% were legally married (Table 1).

TABLE 1. Sample characteristics.

Value1
Child
 Male (%) 53.6
 Breastfeeding duration (months) 13.7 ± 8
 Had prenatal care (%) 92.4
 Birth complications (%) 3.2
 Received multivitamin (%) 23.8
 Attendance in daycare (%) 58.7
Family
 Maternal age (years) 31.9 ± 8.2
 Father present (%) 90.2
 Paternal education (years) 7.4 ± 3.4
 Maternal education (years) 8.2 ± 3.7
 Legally married (%) 94.4
 Electricity in home (%) 71.2
 Owns television (%) 29.5
 Lives in ECD program barangay (%) 60.6
1

means ± SD unless marked otherwise

In bivariate analysis, children of mothers with lower educational attainment tended to be breastfed longer (p<0.01). They had fathers with fewer years of education, and lived in households with lower levels of material assets and wealth (Table 2). In unadjusted models, there were no trends apparent between duration of breastfeeding and the psychosocial development scores of children at any age. However, after adjusting for measures of maternal education, presence of father in home, day care attendance, and a range of wealth and status measures, a relatively consistent relationship between breastfeeding duration and psychosocial development emerged (Table 3). When compared to children who were breastfed 5 months or less (reference group), those who were breastfed longer tended to have higher psychosocial scores. Positive associations between breastfeeding and psychosocial development peaked sometime during the second year of life and then declined. This pattern was strongest in those individuals who were youngest at the age of psychosocial assessment (5 years of age). The regression coefficients declined by 6 years of age but retained the same general pattern (Fig. 2).

TABLE 2. Means and standard deviations of parental and household characteristics.

Duration of breastfeeding (months) p1

0 to 5 6 to 11 12 to 17 18 to 23 24+
Mother's education (years) 9.6 ± 3.9 8.4 ± 3.8 7.7 ± 3.5 8.0 ± 3.4 7.8 ± 3.5 0.001
Father's education (years) 8.8 ± 4.3 8.1 ± 4.0 6.9 ± 3.6 6.9 ± 3.7 6.9 ± 3.6 0.001
Mother's age (years) 34.2 ± 10.4 31.0 ± 8.7 30.7 ± 7.4 31.2 ± 7.4 32.5 ± 6.9 0.001
Father's age (years) 36.6 ± 10.2 33.6 ± 8.7 33.9 ± 8.2 34.4 ± 8.7 36.2 ± 8.3 0.001
Asset scale (0-20) 2.7 ± 3.0 1.8 ± 2.5 1.2 ± 1.9 1.1 ± 2.0 1.0 ± 1.9 0.001
Hygiene scale (0-12) 6.8 ± 2.1 6.2 ± 2.2 6.1 ± 2.2 6.1 ± 2.2 5.9 ± 2.3 0.001
Electricity in home (%) 82.2 74.6 68.6 68.4 65.0 0.001

n 551 334 832 395 640
1

p-values based on one-way ANOVA or Pearson's χ2.

TABLE 3. Regression models relating duration of breastfeeding to psychosocial test score measured at 5 and 6 years of age1.

Breastfeeding Duration (months) 5 years (n=1, 405) 6 years (n=1,347)

crude adjusted2 crude adjusted2
0-5 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0
6-11 0.31 (-2.11, 2.73) 1.62 (-0.75, 3.99) 0.44 (-2.07, 2.95) 1.72 (-0.72, 4.16)
12-17 -0.62 (-2.55, 1.30) 2.05 (0.08, 4.02)** 0.70 (-1.26, 2.67) 1.92 (-0.01, 3.86)*
18-23 0.83 (-1.61, 3.28) 3.21 (0.78, 5.65)** 0.53 (-1.73, 2.80) 1.37 (-0.85, 3.60)
24+ -1.0 (-3.0, 1.0) 1.54 (-0.49, 3.57) -1.17 (-3.32, 0.98) -0.15 (-2.27, 1.96)
R2 0.00 0.08 0.00 0.09
1

β(95% CI), *p<0.1, **p<0.05

2

Adjusted for gender of child, daycare attendance, maternal education, father's presence at home, hygiene, non-income producing assets

Fig 2.

Fig 2

Association between duration of breastfeeding and psychosocial development score of 5 and 6 year-old children (from multiple regression, value is β adjusted for gender of child, daycare attendance, maternal education, father's presence at home, hygiene, non-income producing assets). *p<0.1, **p<0.05

Discussion

In this large, representative sample of rural Filipinos, we find evidence suggestive of a beneficial effect of breastfeeding on the psychosocial development of children. Our findings are consistent with the results of other studies documenting benefits of breastfeeding in both developed and developing nations, which have found that, compared to formula fed infants, breastfed infants have better growth status (Sacker et al., 2006), cognitive skills and school performance (Quinn et al., 2001; Horwood and Fergusson, 1998; Horwood et al., 2001; Angelsen et al., 2001; Oddy et al., 2004; Michaelsen et al., 2003) and emotional development (Woodward and Liberty, 2005). The psychosocial scale used in our study is a predictor of abilities related to language acquisition, cognition and psychosocial maturity deemed essential for primary school entrance, and is also correlated with school readiness itself. Thus, our findings extend prior work to suggest that there may be benefits of breastfeeding to the domains of cognitive and psychosocial maturity that allow successful adjustment to school life in the primary grade levels. Importantly, these benefits were present in a large predominantly rural sample of Filipinos, among whom poverty is more common. Our findings thus underscore the utility of breastfeeding as a means to improve child development among populations with limited resources.

Women who breastfed longest in this sample tended to be rural, to have attained a lower level of education, and to come from households with lower socioeconomic status. Despite the negative effect of such factors on offspring developmental outcomes (Clark et al., 2006), we found evidence for a protective effect of breastfeeding on later psychosocial development but only after adjusting for socioeconomic indicators. In the 5-year old age group, the psychosocial score among children breastfed 12 months or more was 2 to 3 points higher compared to peers who were breastfed for less than 6 months. However, for those breastfed for more than 2 years, the apparent protective effect of breastfeeding was diminished. Although in theory this could indicate that breastfeeding for longer durations has declining benefits, we feel that residual confounding is a more likely explanation for this finding. In this sample, infants who were breastfed longest come from the lowest income households and were born to mothers of low educational attainment. Thus, it seems likely that our ability to adjust adequately for the negative impacts on development that poverty and its related exposures have in the long breastfeeding duration subgroup would be constrained by our imperfect measures of these confounding influences. An intervention study would be necessary to establish whether the apparent declining benefit associated with the longest breastfeeding durations is real or an artifact of such residual confounding. While these details remain to be clarified, our findings suggest that, compared to children who were not breastfed or breastfed for a shorter duration, breastfeeding has important benefits for developmental outcomes of relevance to education and human capital.

The relationships documented here are consistent with and complement prior findings in the Philippines reported by Daniels and Adair (2005) who used 1983-1986 infant feeding data to predict IQ in a sample living in Metropolitan Cebu City. In their sample, longer periods of breastfeeding predicted higher childhood IQ, with benefits of breastfeeding peaking during the second year of breastfeeding. Similar to our findings, the relationships that they documented were only detectable after adjusting for strong negative confounders that tend to be correlated with breastfeeding duration and that have negative impacts on offspring psychosocial development. Our analysis reveals a similar pattern of benefits to psychosocial characteristics that predict school readiness in a contemporary sample representing rural regions of the country where breastfeeding is common.

The tendency for highly-educated women to breastfed their offspring for less time in the sample studied here is consistent with observations in similar populations elsewhere (Negi and Kandpal, 2004; Skafida, 2009), and likely reflects the limitations imposed upon breastfeeding among working women. Although women who have higher education are more likely to recognize the benefits of breastfeeding, they are also more likely to be in the labor force, which may limit their opportunities for breastfeeding. In the Philippines, working women, especially nonprofessional, low income mothers, are rarely able to negotiate a “breastfeeding-friendly” work environment while maintaining employment. Our findings suggest that policies to promote such opportunities for mothers of infants and young children would have developmental and perhaps long-term economic benefits for the offspring generation.

There is currently much interest in the lasting influence of early environments on later health and well-being (Gluckman and Hanson, 2006). While much research has focused on the influence of nutrition on later chronic disease (e.g.Barker, 1994), including in the Philippines (Adair et al., 2001; Kuzawa and Adair, 2003), the impact of similar developmental processes on economically-relevant outcomes, such as cognitive or school performance, is gaining increasing attention (e.g. Palloni, 2006). The present findings add to this body of knowledge by suggesting that breastfeeding could have beneficial effects on psychosocial development, which is a significant predictor of school readiness in this sample. These longer-term benefits are in addition to the better-established reductions in infant morbidity and mortality among breastfed infants (Popkin et al., 1990), and are detectable despite the fact that women who breastfeed in the sample tend to have lower educational attainment and come from poorer households. We conclude that breastfeeding is a potentially effective strategy available to Filipino mothers to improve child developmental outcomes even in contexts of rural poverty and limited resources. These results underscore the importance of continuing current public health efforts to promote breastfeeding in the Philippines and across the globe.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a FIRCA grant R03-HD051555-01-A1 and through a parent grant RO1-TW05604-01 (“Filipino Early Childhood Development: Longitudinal Analysis”) funded by the National Institutes of Health/Fogarty International Center, National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD). The authors are grateful to Jere R. Behrman for his useful comments on the manuscript. The authors would also like to thank Alan Feranil, Judith Borja, Socorro Gultiano, Nanette Lee and the Study Team from the Office of Population Studies for their assistance and feedback on the analysis and manuscript.

Literature Cited

  1. Adair LS, Kuzawa CW, Borja J. Maternal Energy Stores and Diet Composition During Pregnancy Program Adolescent Blood Pressure. Journal of the American Heart Association. 2001;104:1034–1039. doi: 10.1161/hc3401.095037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Angelsen NK, Vik T, Jacobsen G, Bakketeig LS. Breast feeding and cognitive development at age 1 and 5 years. Arch Dis Child. 2001;85(3):183–8. doi: 10.1136/adc.85.3.183. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Armecin G, Behrman J, Duazo P, Ghuman S, Gultiano S, King E, Lee N. Early Childhood Development Programs and Children's Development: Evidence from the Philippines. World Bank Policy Research Working Paper 3922.2006. [Google Scholar]
  4. Barker D. Mothers, Babies, and Disease in Later Life. BMJ Publishing; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  5. Bautista-de los Angeles F, Reyes A. The Early Childhood Development Project (Philippines) Development Assessment Package. Manila, Philippines: 2001. [Google Scholar]
  6. Behrman JR. The impact of health and nutrition on education. The World Bank Research Observer. 1996;11:23–37. [Google Scholar]
  7. Clark KM, Castillo M, Calatroni A, Walter T, Cayazzo M, Pino P, Lozoff B. Breast-feeding and mental and motor development at 51/2 years. Ambul Pediatr. 2006;6(2):65–71. doi: 10.1016/j.ambp.2005.11.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. CWC. Philippine National Strategic Framework for Plan Development for Children, 2000-2005. Child 21: A Legacy to the Filipino Children of the 21st Century 2000 [Google Scholar]
  9. Daniels MC, Adair LS. Breast-feeding influences cognitive development in Filipino children. J Nutr. 2005;135(11):2589–95. doi: 10.1093/jn/135.11.2589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Ghuman S, Behrman JR, Gultiano S. Children's Nutrition, school Quality and Primary School Enrollment in the Philippines. 2006-24. The International Centre for the Study of East Asian Development; 2006. p. 33. [Google Scholar]
  11. Gillespie B, d'Arcy H, Schwartz K, Bobo JK, Foxman B. Recall of age of weaning and other breastfeeding variables. Int Breastfeed J. 2006;1:4. doi: 10.1186/1746-4358-1-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Gluckman PD, Hanson MA. Developmental Origins of Health and Disease. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2006. p. 519. [Google Scholar]
  13. Grantham-McGregor S, Cheung YB, Cueto S, Glewwe P, Richter L, Strupp B, Group ICDS Developmental potential in the first 5 years for children in developing countries. Lancet. 2007;369(9555):60–70. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)60032-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Horwood LJ, Darlow BA, Mogridge N. Breast milk feeding and cognitive ability at 7–8 years. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2001;84 doi: 10.1136/fn.84.1.F23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Horwood LJ, Fergusson DM. Breastfeeding and later cognitive and academic outcomes. Pediatrics. 1998;101(1):E9. doi: 10.1542/peds.101.1.e9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Ip S, Chung M, Raman G, Chew P, Magula N, DeVine D, Trikalinos T, Lau J. Breastfeeding and maternal and infant health outcomes in developed countries. Evid Rep Technol Assess (Full Rep) 2007;(153):1–186. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Kark JD, Troya G, Friedlander Y, Slater PE, Stein Y. Validity of maternal reporting of breast feeding history and the association with blood lipids in 17 year olds in Jerusalem. J Epidemiol Community Health. 1984;38(3):218–25. doi: 10.1136/jech.38.3.218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Kramer MS, Aboud F, Mironova E, Vanilovich I, Platt RW, Matush L, Igumnov S, Fombonne E, Bogdanovich N, Ducruet T, et al. Breastfeeding and child cognitive development: new evidence from a large randomized trial. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65(5):578–84. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.5.578. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Kuzawa CW, Adair LS. Lipid profiles in adolescent Filipinos: relation to birth weight and maternal energy status during pregnancy. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2003;77(4):960–6. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/77.4.960. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Labbok MH. Transdisciplinary breastfeeding support: Creating program and policy synergy across the reproductive continuum. International Breastfeeding Journal. 2008;3(16) doi: 10.1186/1746-4358-3-16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Michaelsen KF, Lauritzen L, Jorgensen MH, Mortensen E. Breast-feeding and brain development. Scandanivian Journal of Nutrition. 2003;47(3):147–151. [Google Scholar]
  22. Negi KS, Kandpal SD. Breast Feeding Practices in a Rural Area of District Dehradun, Uttaranchal. Indian J Prev Soc Med. 2004;35(3 & 4) [Google Scholar]
  23. Oddy WH, Kendall GE, Blair E, de Klerk NH, Silburn S, Zubrick S. Breastfeeding and cognitive development in children. Adv Exp Med Biol. 2004;554:365–9. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4757-4242-8_42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Office of Population Studies. A Study of the Effects of Early Childhood Interventions on Children's Physiological, Cognitive and Social Development. Cebu City, Philippines: Office of Population Studies- University of San Carlos; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  25. Office of Population Studies. A Study of the Effects of Early Childhood Interventions on Children's Physiological, Cognitive and Social Development. Cebu City, Philippines: Office of Population Studies- University of San Carlos; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  26. Pallant J. SPSS Survival Manual: A step-by-step guide to data analysis using SPSS for Windows (version15) Allen & Unwin; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  27. Palloni A. Reproducing inequalities: luck, wallets, and the enduring effects of childhood health. Demography. 2006;43(4):587–615. doi: 10.1353/dem.2006.0036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Republic Act No 7600: The Rooming-In and Breastfeeding Act of 1992. Philippines: 1992. Jun 2, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  29. Popkin BM, Adair L, Akin JS, Black R, Briscoe J, Flieger W. Breast-feeding and diarrheal morbidity. Pediatrics. 1990;86(6):874–82. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Promislow JH, Gladen BC, Sandler DP. Maternal recall of breastfeeding duration by elderly women. Am J Epidemiol. 2005;161(3):289–96. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwi044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Quinn PJ, O'Callaghan M, Williams GM, Najman JM, Andersen MJ, Bor W. The effect of breastfeeding on child development at 5 years: a cohort study. J Paediatr Child Health. 2001;37(5):465–9. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1754.2001.00702.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Richards M, Hardy R, Woodsworth ME. Long-term effects of breastfeeding in a national birth cohort educational attainment and middle cognitive functions. Public health Nutrition. 2002;5(5):631–635. doi: 10.1079/PHN2002338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Sacker A, Quigley MA, Kelly YJ. Breastfeeding and developmental delay: findings from the millennium cohort study. Pediatrics. 2006:e682–9. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-3141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Schack-Nielsen L, Michaelsen KF. Breast feeding and future health. Curr Opin Clin Nutr Metab Care. 2006;9(3):289–96. doi: 10.1097/01.mco.0000222114.84159.79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Skafida V. Breastfeeding duration: The impact of maternal education and employment. SPA conference 2009; 2009. working paper. [Google Scholar]
  36. Stuebe A. The risks of not breastfeeding for mothers and infants. Rev Obstet Gynecol. 2009;2(4):222–31. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Thapa S, Short RV, Potts M. Breast feeding, birth spacing and their effects on child survival. Nature. 1988;335(6192):679–82. doi: 10.1038/335679a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Woodward LJ, Liberty KA. Breastfeeding and Child Psychosocial Development. Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development 2005 [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES