Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 Nov 8.
Published in final edited form as: Read Writ. 2010;23(8):889–912. doi: 10.1007/s11145-009-9179-5

Reading interventions for struggling readers in the upper elementary grades: a synthesis of 20 years of research

Jeanne Wanzek 1, Jade Wexler 2, Sharon Vaughn 3, Stephen Ciullo 4
PMCID: PMC2975107  NIHMSID: NIHMS246016  PMID: 21072128

Abstract

A synthesis of the extant research on reading interventions for students with reading difficulties and disabilities in fourth and fifth grade (ages 9–11) is presented. Thirteen studies with treatment/comparison study designs and eleven single group or single subject studies were located and synthesized. Findings from the 24 studies revealed high effects for comprehension interventions on researcher-developed comprehension measures. Word recognition interventions yielded small to moderate effects on a range of reading outcomes. Few studies were located implementing vocabulary and multi-component interventions.

Keywords: Reading intervention, Reading difficulties, Learning disabilities

Introduction

Considerable research conducted over the past 30 years provides extensive knowledge regarding early intervention for young readers with reading difficulties (Blachman et al., 2004; Denton, Fletcher, Anthony, & Francis, 2006; Felton, 1993; Fletcher, Lyon, Fuchs, & Barnes, 2007; Jenkins & O’Connor, 2002; Lovett et al., 2000; Mathes et al., 2005; McMaster, Fuchs, Fuchs, & Compton, 2005; Torgesen et al., 1999; Vellutino et al., 1996). These reports indicate that the highest student effects result when explicit, systematic instruction is provided in both foundation skills such as phonological awareness and phonics as well as higher level reading tasks, such as fluency, with increased attention to word meaning and understanding text (National Reading Panel, 2000). Incorporating these elements of instruction has been associated with reducing the incidence of reading difficulties (Torgesen, 2000).

In addition, recent syntheses have examined the efficacy of methods to improve reading outcomes for older students with reading difficulties that persist into grades 4–12 (Edmonds et al., 2009; Kamil et al., 2008; Scammacca et al., 2007; Torgesen et al., 2007). These reports indicate positive reading outcomes for older students when providing explicit instruction in (a) word study strategies to decode words, (b) word meanings and strategies for deriving the meanings of unknown words, and (c) comprehension strategy instruction. These findings hold specifically for students with reading difficulties (Edmonds et al., 2009) and learning disabilities (Scammacca et al., 2007) as well. Furthermore, recent reviews indicate that providing ample opportunities to practice and receive corrective feedback during instruction are associated with improved academic outcomes (Hattie & Timperley, 2007; Shute, 2008).

Thus, the necessary components of effective reading instruction have been identified and synthesized for students in the younger grades (K-3) who struggle with reading acquisition, and the groundwork has been laid for research regarding effective reading intervention for students who struggle to read and comprehend in the secondary grades. Although a few studies in the previous syntheses of reading instruction for older readers have included students in grades 4–5, the findings largely reflect studies conducted with students in grades 6–12. Typically, there is an underlying assumption that 4th and 5th grade students are more similar to secondary students than elementary students. Kamil et al. (2008) best explained this assumption in a recently published Institute of Education Sciences practice guide document, “The panel purposefully included students in 4th and 5th grades within the realm of adolescents because their instructional needs related to literacy have more in common with those of students in middle and high school than they do with students in early elementary grades” (p. 1).

While there is some evidence from the previous syntheses that upper elementary students in grades 4–5 can benefit from the same interventions designed to meet the needs of students in grades 6–12, the findings for students in the upper elementary grades (4th–5th) have not previously been disaggregated and the recommended practices have been based mainly on studies conducted with students in grades 6–12. Furthermore, a synthesis focusing on reading interventions for students in grades 4 and 5 has not previously been conducted.

Teaching reading in the upper elementary grades: the unique needs of teachers

Unfortunately, despite our knowledge regarding effective instruction for young readers in the early elementary grades, it is estimated that 69% of fourth grade students cannot read at proficient levels with 36% of the fourth grade population unable to read at or above basic levels of understanding (National Center for Educational Statistics, 2005). In the upper elementary grades, a shift from “learning to read” to “reading to learn” typically occurs. Thus, in addition to expectations that students have adequately mastered the basic reading skills such as decoding accurately and fluently, there are also expectations that students understand word meanings and are able to read text with comprehension (Chall, 1983). The focus on these comprehension skills may be difficult for struggling readers who may still be learning to accurately and fluently decode grade level text. In addition, as early as fourth grade, students are presented with the supplementary challenge of transitioning from reading and understanding narrative text to reading and understanding content area expository text (Grigg, Daane, Jin, & Campbell, 2003).

With the decreased emphasis on learning to read in the upper elementary grades, students who do not read proficiently by the end of the early elementary grades (K-3) may face serious consequences. Chall and Jacobs (1983) noted that many low income third graders reading at grade level experience a sudden drop in normative reading scores by the fourth grade, referring to this phenomenon as the “fourth grade slump”, indicating not that students go “backwards” in reading, but instead that they fail to thrive and cannot meet grade level expectations. The increased demands placed on students beginning in fourth grade may cause a slowing of reading growth relative to expected growth for some students who previously seemed on track in their reading growth. Teachers must be able to detect when a student is not thriving and intervene before the gap widens even more. Therefore, upper elementary teachers are often faced with the challenge of providing intervention not only for students with previously identified reading difficulties that have not been adequately remediated, but also students whose reading difficulties have manifested in the upper elementary grades.

Additionally, the trajectory of a young person’s academic success begins in the elementary grades, making it even more crucial to find ways to intervene and remediate deficits that persist into the upper elementary grades. When students experience a lack of success starting in elementary school, they may begin to disengage from school and be more inclined to drop out in the future (Dynarski et al., 2008). It is necessary to determine appropriate methods to intervene with students in the upper elementary years before they reach the secondary grades and are then faced with a multitude of additional academic and social challenges.

Rationale and research question

We conducted this synthesis to examine the effects of reading interventions for students with reading difficulties and disabilities in the upper elementary grades including students in grades 4–5. The findings are expected to contribute to the research and practice knowledge regarding interventions for students who struggle with reading beyond third grade. We addressed the following research question: How effective are reading interventions on reading outcomes for students with reading difficulties and disabilities in fourth and fifth grade?

Method

Selection of studies

Studies were identified through a two-step process. First, we conducted an electronic search of ERIC and PsychInfo for studies published in the last 20 years (1988–2007). Key disability search terms and roots (reading difficult*, disab*, dyslex*, special education) were used in combination with key reading terms and roots (reading, phonics, fluency, vocabulary, comprehen*) to capture relevant articles. Second, we conducted a hand search of nine major journals (Exceptional Children, Journal of Educational Psychology, Journal of Learning Disabilities, Journal of Special Education, Learning Disabilities Quarterly, Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, Reading and Writing, Remedial and Special Education, and Scientific Studies of Reading) from 2006 through 2007 to ensure that all recently published studies meeting criteria were identified.

A total of 24 studies met selection criteria for the synthesis. Studies were selected based on the following criteria:

  1. More than 50% of the participants in the study were enrolled in 4th or 5th grade, or were 9–11 years old. Studies with less than 50% of the participants in 4th/5th grade were included if data were disaggregated for the 4th/5th grade population.

  2. Participants were struggling readers. Struggling readers were defined as low achievers, students with unidentified reading difficulties, dyslexia, and/or with reading, learning or speech/language disabilities. Studies also were included if disaggregated data were provided for struggling readers regardless of the characteristics of other students in the study.

  3. The interventions targeted reading instruction and articles were published in English.

  4. Reading intervention was provided for 15 sessions or more to ensure students with reading difficulties and disabilities received a sustained intervention prior to measurement of outcomes.

  5. A reading intervention including word study, fluency, vocabulary, comprehension, or a combination of these was provided as part of school programming. Home teaching, clinic, or camp programs were excluded.

  6. The research design was treatment-comparison, single-group, or single-subject.

  7. Reading or reading related outcomes were measured.

Coding procedures

An extensive coding document was developed and used to organize essential information about each study. The code sheet was based on code sheets used in previous research (Edmonds et al. 2009; Vaughn et al. 2003) as well as the What Works Clearinghouse Design and Implementation Assessment Device (Institute of Education Sciences, 2003).

The pertinent information coded included the following: (a) participants, (b) methodology, (c) intervention and comparison information, (d) clarity of causal inference, (e) measures, and (f) findings. There were 3 coders for the articles. Interrater reliability was established by having each coder independently code a single article. Responses from each coder were used to calculate the percentage of agreement (i.e., agreements divided by agreements plus disagreements). Interrater reliability was calculated separately for each codesheet category (e.g., participants, design, etc.). An interrater agreement of 90% or above was achieved for each category (range 90–100%). Each study was then independently coded by 2 raters. If disagreements occurred, meetings were held to discuss the coding with final judgments reached by consensus.

Effect size calculation

In order to provide additional quantitative information for this systematic review of the literature, effect sizes were calculated where data were available. For studies with treatment and comparison groups, effect sizes were calculated adjusting for pre-test differences using a procedure by Bryant and Wortman (1984). The quantity of the pretest treatment mean minus the pretest comparison mean was divided by the quantity of the pretest comparison standard deviation. This quantity was subtracted from the quantity of the posttest treatment mean minus the posttest comparison mean divided by the posttest comparison standard deviation. Thirteen of the 24 studies in this synthesis used a treatment/comparison design (9 experimental and 4 quasi-experimental). Data for calculation of effect sizes were available in 10 of these 13 studies.

Results

Experimental and quasi-experimental studies

Nine experimental studies (Mason, 2004; Mathes & Fuchs, 1993; Miranda, Villaescusa, & Vidal-Abarca, 1997; O’Connor et al., 2002; O’Connor, White, & Swanson, 2007; Therrien, Wickstrom, & Jones, 2006; Takala, 2006; Torgesen et al., 2001; Xin & Rieth, 2001) and four quasi-experimental studies (Das, Mishra, & Pool, 1995 [Study 1 and 2]; Das-Smaal, Klapwijk, & van der Leij, 1996; Lederer, 2000) examined reading interventions for students with reading difficulties and disabilities in the fourth and fifth grade. Summaries of the study characteristics and findings are presented in Tables 1 and 2. We present the effects of these studies by the type of intervention that was implemented in the study (e.g., word recognition, fluency).

Table 1.

Summary of study characteristics

Study N Grade/age Duration Implementer Design
Vocabulary and comprehension
Bruce & Chan (1991) 2 SR 5th 18, 30 min sessions Researchers Single subject
Lederer (2000) 25 LD 4th–6th* 15–17, 45 min sessions Researcher Quasi-experimental
Mason (2004) 32 SR (2 LD) 5th 11–15, 20 min sessions Researcher Experimental
Mason et al. (2006) 9 SR (2 LD) 4th 18 + sessions Researcher Single subject
Miranda et al. (1997) 60 LD 5th–6th* 20, 50 min sessions Researcher Experimental
Rich & Blake (1994) 5 LI 4th–5th 16, 45 min sessions Teachers Single subject
Takala (2006) 16 SLI 4th 15, 45–90 min sessions Teacher & Researcher Experimental
Taylor et al. 2002) 2 LD 10–11 years 5, 40 min sessions Teacher Single subject
Xin & Rieth (2001) 76 LD 4th–6th* 18, 30 min sessions Teachers Experimental
Fluency
Daly & Martens (1994) 3 LD 10–11 years 21 sessions Teacher & Researcher Single subject
Mathes & Fuchs (1993) 67 LD 4th–6th* 30, 25–40 min sessions Teachers Experimental
O’Connor et al. (2007) 37 SR (16 LD) 2nd, 4th* ~42, 15 min sessions Researchers Experimental
Word recognition
Butler (1999) 10 LD 4th–5th 30–37, 15 min sessions Teacher Single subject
Das et al. (1995): Study 1 51 RD 8–11 years* 15, 50–60 min sessions Researchers Quasi-experimental
Das et al. (1995): Study 2 51 RD 8–11 years* 15, 50–60 min sessions Researchers Quasi-experimental
Das-Smaal et al. (1996) 33 RD 9–10 years 16, 30 min sessions Researchers Quasi-experimental
Ferkis et al. (1997): Study 1 3 LD 4th 23–27 sessions Researcher Single subject
Ferkis et al. (1997): Study 2 2 LD 4th 12–17 sessions Researcher Single subject
Gillon & Dodd (1997): Experimental 2 10 RD 10–11 years 12–20 h School support staff Single group
Thaler et al. (2004) 3 SR 4th 11–25, 15 min sessions Researcher Single group
Torgesen et al. (2001) 60 LD 8–10 years* 80, 50 min sessions Researchers Experimental
Wright & Mullan (2006) 10 RD 9–11 years mean of 23.4 h Researcher Single subject
Multi-component
O’Connor et al. (2002) 46 SR (25 LD) 3rd–5th* ~65, 30 min sessions Researcher Experimental
Therrien et al. (2006) 30 SR (16 LD) 4th–8th* 50, 10–15 min sessions Researchers Experimental

SR Struggling readers; LD learning disabilities; RD reading; disabilities; LI language impaired; SLI speech/language impaired

*

More than 50% of participants in 4th–5th grade or 9–11 years old

Table 2.

Summary of study findings

Study/Intervention Measures Findings
Vocabulary and comprehension
Bruce & Chan (1991) Comprehension questions (RD) Baseline
T (Reciprocal Teaching): Teacher modeling of question generating,
summarizing, clarifying, and predicting with text followed by student application
in partners in resource room. Generalization prompting provided for homeroom
reading and social studies settings
T
0–30%
40–100%
Lederer (2000) RD Measures ns
T (Reciprocal Teaching): Questioning, summarizing, predicting, and clarifying
taught then used in collaborative groups with social studies text
Answering Questions ns
Question generation T > C (p < .05)
C (Control): Typical social studies instruction Summary composition
Mason (2004) RD Measures Posttest F-U
T1 (Think Before Reading, While Reading, After Reading [TWA]): Nine
comprehension strategies taught with self-regulation strategies for use before,
during, and after reading text with practice in pairs
Main idea 1.88 1.08
Summary 1.00 .94
Oral retell quality 1.59 .82
T2 (Reciprocal Questioning): Strategies for question asking taught. Question
asking practiced in collaborative groups after silent reading
Oral retell information 1.25 .72
Oral retell main ideas 1.11 .94
Written retell quality .09 na
Written retell information .63 na
Written retell main ideas .33 na
Mason et al. (2006) RD Measures Baseline Means Posttest Means
T (TWA + Pick Goals, List Ways to Meet Goals, And, Make Notes and Sequence
Notes): TWA as described in Mason (2004), plus a strategy for writing essays
Oral Retell Quality .89–1.33 3.11–4.33
Oral Retell Information 3.47–4.33 9.33–10.17
Written Retell Quality 1.00–1.67 3.47–4.44
Written Retell Information 3.44–5.08 11.67–22.67
Miranda et al. (1997) RD Measures T1 vs. C F-U T2 vs. C F-U
T1 (Self-instruction Training): 5 questions taught for regulating work. Activating
knowledge, previewing, questioning, clarifying, and mapping also taught
Main Idea 1.93 2.35 1.37 1.95
Recall 5.89 2.01 4.67 3.24
T2 (Self-instruction plus Attribution Training): T1 plus training in giving
positive attributes
Cloze 2.56 1.57 1.85 1.07
C (Control): No training
Rich & Blake (1994) RD Measures Pretest (8 total) Posttest (8 total)
T (Strategy Training): Main idea, paraphrasing, self-questioning, and predicting/
activating knowledge taught along with self-evaluation. Teachers read the text
Listening Comp. Questions 1–6 4.5–8
Reading Comp. Questions 2–6 5–8
Takala (2006) RD Measures
T(Reciprocal Teaching): Prediction, clarification, questioning, and summarizing
strategies taught with whole group
Summarizing ns
Questioning ns
C (Control): Typical instruction
Taylor et al. (2002) C T1 T2
T1 (Story Mapping): Taught story elements and story mapping procedure Comp. Questions Correct (RD) 4–9 6–10 7–10
T2 (Self-Questioning): Taught self-questioning and answering procedure
C (No Intervention): Assessment only
Xin & Rieth (2001) RD Measures Posttest F-U
T (Video Instruction): Videos and class discussion used to teach content
vocabulary
Word Definitions .64 .53
Sentence Cloze .51 .16
C (Traditional Instruction): Dictionaries and class discussion used to teach
content vocabulary
Passage Comprehension .02 −.04
Fluency
Daly & Martens (1994) Baseline T1 T2 T3
T1 (Subject Passage Preview): Student read passage without help Words Correct Passage 55–70 58–80 55–73 69–92
T2 (Taped Words): Student read word list with audiotape Words Correct List 34–45 45–58 46–58 40–62
T3 (Listening Passage Preview): Student followed along in passage with
audiotape
WCPM Passage 12–27 11–46 9–30 15–59
WCPM List 4–11 5–24 7–26 6–18
Mathes & Fuchs (1993) T1 vs. C T2 vs. C
T1 (Sustained Reading): Peer tutoring with text reading orally continuously CRAB Words Correct .16 .12
T2 (Repeated Reading): Peer tutoring with text read 3x CRAB Questions Correct .07 .20
C (Control). Typical school instruction CRAB Mazes Correct −.14 −.09
O’Connor et al. (2007) T1 vs. C T2 vs. C
T1 (Repeated Reading): Students read each page 3 times GORT 4 Fluency .52 .57
T2 (Continuous Reading): Students read text continuously GORT 4 Comprehension .67 .92
C (Control): Typical school instruction WRMT Word ID .66 .41
WRMT WA .57 .61
WRMT PC .83 .75
WCPM .98 .88
Word recognition
Butler (1999) Sight Words Acquired (RD) 40–79 words
T (Classwide-Peer Tutoring): Students worked in partners to teach/practice
new sight words, taking turns as the tutor
Das et al. (1995): Study 1 WRMT Word ID .57
T (Planning, Attention, Simultaneous, and Successive Remedial
Program[PREP]): Sequencing, categorization, matching, and sound blending
were practiced through global (without words) and bridging (with words) tasks
WRMT WA .82
C (No Treatment)
Das et al. (1995): Study 2 T1 vs. C T2 vs. C
T1 (PREP Global tasks only) WRMT Word ID −.24 .10
T2 (PREP Bridging tasks only) WRMT WA .44 .58
C (Control). Study 1 T students. No further intervention
Das-Smaal et al. (1996) RD Measures
T (Unit Detection Training): Computer-based program for detecting multiletter
units within words with feedback on speed and accuracy
Trained Unit RT T > C (p <.05)
Trained Unit Accuracy ns
C (Math): Computer-based program similar to T but with mathematical
exercises provided
Untrained Unit RT T > C (p < .05)
Untrained Unit Accuracy ns
Word RT ns
Word Accuracy ns
Flash Pseudowords T > C (p < .05)
Ferkis et al. (1997): Study 1 RD Measures T1 T2
T1 (Single Response[SR]): Practice each word 1x Word Reading 13–14 words 13–14 words
T2 (Repeated Response[RR]): Practice incorrect words 5x Training Time per Word 33.8–40.3 s 57.2–71.3 s
Ferkis et al. (1997): Study 2 RD Measures T1 T2
T1 (SR): 3 practice trials with 1 response per word Word Reading 13–23 words 12–21 words
T2 (RR): 3 practice trials with 5 responses per word Training Time per Word 45.3–52.2 s 89.1–98.1 s
Gillon and Dodd, Experiment 2, 1997 Pretest Posttest
T (Phonological Processing Skills): Segmenting, manipulating, and
blending sounds in syllables and integration to print
LAC 44–88% 82–100%
NARA Accuracy (AE) 7.2–9.8 8.6–11.6
NARA Comp. (AE) 7.1–10.6 7.8–12.3
Thaler et al. (2004) Reading Time for Trained Words
 (RD)
All three students decreased reading time from
 pretest to posttest. 5 week follow-up showed an
 increase in time from the posttest, but still below
 pretest levels
T1 (Passive): Words presented with computer pronunciation. Words then
reappear with onset highlighted and pronounced followed by highlighting and
pronounciation of each sound of onset. Student then reads word
T2 (Active): Same as T1 but student pronounced onset and graphemes
Torgesen et al. (2001) T1 vs. T2 F-U 1 F-U 2
T1 (Auditory Discrimination Depth): PA, encoding, and decoding taught
with articulatory cues. Reading decodable text and questioning included
WRMT WA .91 .59 .36
WRMT Word ID −.09 .11 −.03
• T2 (Embedded Phonics): Phonemic decoding and encoding taught. Reading
in trade books, writing sentences with new words, and fluency of word reading
included
WRMT PC −.12 .05 −.26
TOWRE PDE .16 .16 .38
TOWRE SWE −.09 .19 .13
GORT-III Accuracy .50 .56 .42
GORT-III Rate .44 .24 .18
GORT-III Comp. .46 .67 .54
CELF Total .33 .30 .38
LAC .72 .72 −.003
CTOPP Elision −.17 −.17 −.48
KTEA Spelling −.58 .16 −.09
Developmental Spelling −.35 .45 .16
Wright & Mullan (2006) Pretest Posttest
T (Phono-Graphix): Phonics instruction including basic advanced code and
multisyllabic word reading
Phoneme Manipulation 1–6 5–10
Segmentation 15–38 58–63
Blending 7–14 13–15
Code Knowledge 22–50 60–96
NARA (SS) 74–88 70–101
Vernon Spelling Test (SS) 75–87 74–93
Multi-component
O’Connor et al. (2002) T1 vs. C T2 vs. C
T1 (Reading-Level Matched): PA, phonics, reading text, fluency, and
comprehension taught with text at students’ reading level
CBM Segmenting 1.56 1.25
WRMT Word ID 1.16 1.07
T2 (Classroom Matched): T1 with text from general class WRMT WA 2.00 1.49
C (Control): Typical school instruction WRMT Comp. 1.39 1.46
WCPM 1.35 .52
ARI Comprehension 1.87 1.76
Therrien et al. (2006) DIBELS ORF .44
T (Reread-Adapt and Answer-Comprehend [RAAC]): Student reread the passage
2-4x to meet fluency criterion, then answered questions with scaffolding
WJ-III BRC .37
C (Control): No treatment

T, Treatment; RD, Researcher-developed; WCPM, words correct per minute; C, comparison/control group; WRMT, Woodcock Reading Mastery Test–Revised; Word ID, word identification; WA, word attack; PC, passage comprehension; RT, response time; ns, non-significant; LAC, Lindamood Auditory Conceptualization Test; NARA, Neale Analysis of Reading Ability; AE, age equivalent; Comp., comprehension; F-U, Follow-up; na, not applicable; CRAB, Comprehensive Reading Assessment Battery; CBM, Curriculum-Based Measure; ARI, Analytical Reading Inventory; GORT-4, Gray Oral Reading Tests 4th Ed.; DIBELS, Dynamic Indicators of Basic Early Literacy Skills; ORF, oral reading fluency; WJ-III, Woodcock Johnson III: BRC, Broad Reading Cluster; TOWRE, Test of Word Reading Efficiency; GORT-III, Gray Oral Reading Test 3rd Ed.; CELF, Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals; LAC, Lindamood Auditory Conceptualization Test; PDE, phonetic decoding efficiency; SWE, sight word efficiency; CTOPP, Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing; KTEA, Kaufman Test of Educational Achievement; SS, standard score

Vocabulary and comprehension

Five studies implemented interventions with a focus on comprehension skills and strategies (Lederer, 2000; Mason, 2004; Miranda et al., 1997; Takala, 2006; Xin & Rieth, 2001). All of these studies measured outcomes with researcher-developed measures that measured the specific skills taught in the intervention; no norm-referenced measures were administered. In one experimental study, Mason compared the effects of a self-regulated strategy (Think before you reading, think While reading, think After reading [TWA]) to a second treatment of reciprocal questioning for students with both reading difficulties and disabilities. There was no business as usual or typical instruction control group. Students in the TWA intervention were taught to think about the author’s purpose, think about what they already know, and think about what they want to learn prior to reading. During reading, students were taught to think about their reading speed, linking knowledge, and rereading sections. Finally, students developed the main idea for each paragraph and summarized the information after reading. In the reciprocal questioning condition, students were taught to generate questions for the teacher about the passage read as well as answer questions about the text from the teacher. Effects were higher at posttest for the TWA intervention on researcher-developed measures assessing main ideas, summarizing, and retell (mean ES = .99). Effects in favor of TWA were similar when measures were administered 3 weeks following intervention (mean ES = .90).

Miranda et al. (1997) also compared the relative effects of two interventions with a comprehension focus, self-instruction and self-instruction plus attribution training, to a control group that did not receive either of the interventions. Students with learning disabilities were identified for participation in the interventions. Self-instruction included training and practice in strategies for activating previous knowledge, previewing text, self-questioning, clarifying unknown words, and mapping main ideas. Students were also taught a general self-instruction procedure to follow when completing a reading task (i.e., Stop, Think and Decide, Check, Confirm, Evaluate). The self-instruction plus attribution condition consisted of all the elements in the self-instruction condition plus teacher modeling and student practice using positive attributions in relation to their work. As a result of the time spent on attribution training this treatment group spent less time on the comprehension skills and strategies. Students in the self-instruction condition outperformed students in the control condition at posttest on researcher-developed measures assessing main ideas, recall, and cloze (mean ES = 3.46). Students in the self-instruction plus attribution training also outperformed the control group on the posttest measures (mean ES = 2.63). Two months following the completion of intervention the same measures were administered with mean effect sizes of 1.98 and 2.09 for the self-instruction group and the self-instruction plus attribution training group respectively.

A third experimental study investigated student understanding of text with a focus on teaching target vocabulary words in two conditions (Xin & Rieth, 2001). Students with learning disabilities in both conditions read the same passages, were taught the same target vocabulary words to aid understanding of the passage, and completed the same comprehension activities. However, one group received video-assisted instruction, watching chapters of a videodisc with content related to the topic and including the target words while the second group received instruction using only printed texts. The video instruction group outperformed the nonvideo group on researcher-developed measures of word definitions and cloze using the vocabulary words taught during instruction (mean ES = .58). There were no differences between the groups on a researcher-developed measure of comprehension on the content taught in the interventions (ES = .02). Follow-up measures were administered 2 weeks following intervention with effects: (a) maintained in favor of the video instruction group for word definitions (ES = .53), (b) decreased for sentence cloze with the target words (ES = .16), and (c) consistent to the posttest for passage comprehension (ES = −.04).

The final two studies examined reciprocal teaching as an intervention for students with disabilities (Lederer, 2000; Takala, 2006). Lederer implemented reciprocal teaching in social studies instruction for students with learning disabilities in inclusive classrooms while Takala investigated the intervention for students with language and reading disabilities in special education classrooms in Finland. Neither study provided disaggregated data for the student participants meeting criteria for this synthesis that would allow for calculation of effect sizes. However, Lederer ran analyses on disaggregated data for the students with learning disabilities and reported no significant differences between the treatment and control groups on researcher-developed comprehension measures of answering questions and generating questions. Students with learning disabilities in the treatment group significantly outperformed students in the control group on composing summaries (p < .05). Takala reported no significant differences between pretest and posttest scores for students with disabilities on researcher-developed measures of selecting the best title and main idea, and generating a question.

Fluency

Two experimental studies implemented interventions with a focus on fluency instruction (Mathes & Fuchs, 1993; O’Connor et al., 2007). Both studies examined treatment conditions using repeated reading of text or sustained/continuous reading of text along with a control condition. Mathes and Fuchs implemented the intervention with classwide peer-mediated instruction in special education resource rooms. Students with reading difficulties and disabilities in the O’Connor et al. study met one-on-one with an adult listener. In both studies, the number of minutes spent reading text was kept constant, with 9 min. of reading in the Mathes and Fuchs study (as well as 9 min. of listening to a peer) three times a week for 10 weeks, and 15 min of reading for the O’Connor et al. study implemented three times a week for 14 weeks. However, in the repeated reading condition of each study students reread the passages three times each. In the sustained or continuous reading conditions the students continuously read the text without repeating. In each condition either peers (Mathes & Fuchs) or the adult (O’Connor et al.) corrected errors during reading.

In the Mathes and Fuchs (1993) study, effects were low for the treatment conditions in comparison to the control condition across measures of fluency and comprehension (repeated reading mean ES = .08; sustained/continuous reading mean ES = .03). In contrast, higher effect sizes were found for both treatment conditions in the O’Connor et al. (2007) study across norm-referenced measures of fluency, word reading, and comprehension (repeated reading mean ES = .71; sustained/continuous reading mean ES = .69).

Word recognition

Four studies focused on word reading instruction as an intervention (Das et al., 1995 [Study 1 and Study 2]; Das-Smaal et al., 1996; Torgesen et al., 2001). In an experimental study, Torgesen et al. examined two treatment conditions for students with learning disabilities that differed in the extent of instruction in phonemic awareness and phonemic decoding skills. No control group was included in the design of this study. In the auditory discrimination in depth (ADD) condition students spent approximately 95% of the lesson working with sounds and individual words including introduction to individual phonemes, practice reading and spelling individual words regular words and instruction of irregular words. Students then practiced reading with decodable text. Alternatively, the students in the embedded phonics (EP) condition spent about 50% of the instructional time on sounds and individual words and 50% in connected text activities. Explicit instruction was provided in phonics and reading/spelling words along with ample opportunities for students to practice reading connected text using trade books and basals. The students were introduced to sounds and practiced reading and spelling regular and irregular words. The students practiced reading with trade books and the basal and wrote sentences containing words from their sight word lists. A number of standardized measures were administered at posttest, 1 year follow-up, and 2 year follow-up to assess phonological awareness, word reading, comprehension, fluency, spelling, and expressive and receptive language (see Table 2 for measures). A mean effect size of .16 on these norm-referenced measures was found at posttest in favor of the ADD group. These effect sizes increased for the ADD group at 1 year (mean ES = .29), and for the 2 year follow-up were consistent with posttest (mean ES = .13).

In two studies conducted by Das et al. (1995), the Planning, Attention, Simultaneous, and Successive Remedial Program (PREP) was implemented for students with reading disabilities. Study 1 examined the full program including global (strategies such as rehearsal, categorization, and prediction for successive or simultaneous processing) and bridging (extending these strategies to word identification) components. In Study 2, one group of students received intervention in the global components only and, thus, practiced the strategies without words (e.g., sequencing geometric shapes) while a second group received intervention in the bridging components only and, thus, practiced the strategies only with words and text (e.g., sequencing letters to form a word and then reading the word). A control group in Study 1 became the treatment groups in Study 2 while the treatment group in Study 1 became the control group in Study 2. Thus, all students in Study 2 had received some form of PREP (global, bridging, or previously instructed combined program). In Study 1, students receiving PREP outperformed students in the no treatment control group on the word attack and word identification subtests of the Woodcock Reading Mastery Test (mean ES = .70). In Study 2, effect sizes on the same measures favored the global (mean ES = .10) and bridging (mean ES = .34) groups over the control group of students who had previously received the combined PREP program in Study 1.

The study by Das-Smaal et al. (1996) implemented a computer-based program for students to practice detecting multi-letter units in words in Dutch. Students assigned to the control group received computer-based training in mathematical exercises similar to the cognitive and motor exercises of the training program provided to the treatment group. Posttest measures assessed student accuracy and speed on the computer tasks, detecting units that were trained and untrained, and reading real and pseudowords. The treatment group performed significantly better than the control group on reaction time for detecting units and reading pseudowords (p < .05). No significant differences were reported on the accuracy of detecting units or reading real words. No norm-referenced measures were administered.

Multi-component

Two experimental studies examined the effects of a multi-component intervention for students with reading difficulties and disabilities (O’Connor et al., 2002; Therrien et al., 2006). O’Connor et al. included phoneme awareness, word recognition and spelling, fluency, and comprehension in a 30 min, one-on-one intervention. Students were randomly assigned to receive this treatment with text matched to their reading level (reading level matched), receive the treatment using text from the classroom (classroom matched), or a control condition. Both treatment conditions outperformed the control condition on norm-referenced measures of phonemic awareness, word reading, comprehension, and fluency (reading level matched mean ES = 1.56; classroom matched mean ES = 1.26).

Therrien et al. (2006) incorporated fluency and comprehension components in 10–15 min one-on-one intervention. Students in the treatment condition read a new passage 2–4 times with feedback to reach a pre-established number of correct words per minute. This fluency instruction was followed by scaffolded assistance answering factual, inferential, and story structure questions. The treatment group demonstrated higher effects in comparison to the no-treatment control group in oral reading fluency (ES = .44) and general reading achievement as measured by the Broad Reading scale of the Woodcock-Johnson Achievement Test III (ES = .37).

Single group and single subject studies

Eleven studies examined the effects of reading interventions for single groups or individual students with reading difficulties and disabilities by examining student improvement (Bruce & Chan, 1991; Butler, 1999; Daly & Martens, 1994; Ferkis, Belfiore, & Skinner, 1997 [Study 1 and 2]; Gillon & Dodd, 1997; Mason, Snyder, Sukhram, & Kedem, 2006; Rich & Blake, 1994; Taylor, Alber, & Walker, 2002; Thaler, Ebner, Wimmer, & Landerl, 2004; Wright & Mullan, 2006). We describe these studies and their outcomes by intervention type.

Comprehension

Four single subject studies implemented interventions with a comprehension focus (Bruce & Chan, 1991; Mason et al., 2006; Rich & Blake, 1994; Taylor et al., 2002). Following up on the experimental study of the self-regulated strategy TWA described earlier, Mason et al. implemented a single subject study of the TWA reading strategy instruction combined with PLANS (Pick goals, List ways to meet goals, And, make Notes and Sequence notes) writing strategy instruction. Three instructional groups of 3 students each were included in the study. Participants with both reading difficulties and disabilities were included. Reading outcome measures consisted of oral and written retells of expository science or social studies passages. Students were scored according to the number of information units included in the retell as well as the quality of the retell. Quality was rated on a 7-point scale (0 points to 6 points) researcher-developed scale based on the student capturing the main ideas of the passage in the retell. Mean increases in information units from baseline to postinstruction ranged from 5.34–5.86 for oral retell and 8.23–18.87 for written retell across the three instructional groups. Mean increases in quality scores ranged from 2.17 to 3.00 for oral retell and 2.47–3.00 for written retell.

Rich and Blake (1994) also implemented a comprehension intervention that included instruction in self-regulated learning. Students with language/learning disabilities received instruction in identifying main ideas, self-questioning, and paraphrasing with the teacher reading the expository text. During the intervention, students kept daily journals evaluating their cognitive and affective behaviors. Reading outcomes were measured with expository passages excerpted by the researchers from informal reading inventories and students responded to 8 questions about each passage. The authors report that all 5 students made improvements from the pretest to the posttest in listening comprehension with scores on the outcome measure ranging from 56–100% (2 students below 75% on posttest). Four of the students also improved from pretest to posttest in reading comprehension with scores ranging from 63–100% on the posttest measure (1 student below 75% on posttest).

However, Bruce and Chan examined reciprocal teaching in the resource room as well as techniques for assisting students with reading difficulties in generalizing strategies learned to the general education classroom. Student’s total comprehension scores on measures that included main ideas and passage details increased to 75–90% accuracy (with average baseline levels ranging from 16–20%). However, no unprompted transfer of skills was reported and student levels were lower in the transfer phase than in the resource room instructional phase.

Taylor et al. implemented an alternating treatments design to examine the effects of story mapping, self-questioning, and no intervention for individual students with learning disabilities. The accuracy of students’ responses during each phase of instruction was collected. Two of the students in this study met criteria for inclusion for this synthesis. One student, Joseph, demonstrated slightly higher comprehension scores in the self questioning and story mapping conditions over the no intervention phase. The second student, Michelle, had some overlap in scores between the no intervention and intervention phases initially with scores improving further during the intervention phases. Accuracy was high for both students in each of the intervention conditions (80.9 and 86.4% for Joseph and Michele in story mapping; 88.2 and 94.6% for Joseph and Michele in self-questioning).

Fluency

One fluency intervention with students with learning disabilities utilized a single subject study (Daly & Martens, 1994). A multi-element design was used to compare student accuracy and fluency under 3 pre-reading conditions: (1) subject passage preview with the student doing a first read of the passage without help from the teacher, (2) taped words with the student reading a word list of words from the passage along with an audio tape speeded at 80 words per minute, or (3) listening passage preview with the subject following along in the text while listening to the passage read on audiotape. Following each of these prereading conditions, the student read the passage for assessment. The largest increases for oral reading accuracy and fluency were seen under the listening passage preview. However, no discernible differences between baseline and the three conditions could be seen on word list reading.

Word reading

Six single group or single subject studies examined student outcomes from interventions focusing on word reading instruction (Butler, 1999; Ferkis et al., 1997 [Study 1 and 2]; Gillon & Dodd, 1997; Thaler et al., 2004; Wright & Mullan, 2006). Four of the studies incorporated training in sight word reading: students practicing reading unknown words to mastery with a peer (Butler), an adult (Ferkis Study 1 and 2), or a computer (Thaler et al.). Butler reported an increase in word reading on words taught from 50–79% for students with reading disabilities. Similarly, Ferkis et al. reported students with learning disabilities mastered 12–14 words taught in each condition of Study 1 and 2, with one student obtaining mastery of 21–23 words taught during the intervention phases. Study 1 consisted of 2 conditions, one with 1 correct response per word required in each training session and a second condition requiring 5 correct responses per word during training. Study 2 continued with similar conditions to Study 1 except that students practiced the set of words three times. No discernible differences in the number of words learned based on the number of repeated responses required during training in either Study 1 or Study 2 were noted. Thaler et al. measured the reading time on trained words following intervention for students with reading difficulties and found that students showed decreases in reading time for the words following intervention. The students who pretested with higher reading times made the most gains in decreasing their reading times.

Two of the word reading interventions taught phonological skills to students with reading disabilities and measured students’ phonological awareness, reading accuracy, and comprehension using standardized measures of general skills in these areas (Gillon & Dodd, 1997; Wright & Mullan, 2006). All students made gains in each area from pretest to posttest. The largest gains appeared on the phonological measures for both studies.

Discussion

The primary purpose of this research synthesis was to determine the effectiveness of reading intervention for students in the upper elementary grades (fourth and fifth grade) on reading outcomes. We prioritized this grade group because previous syntheses have examined extensively the effectiveness of reading practices for students in grades K-3 (e.g., McCardle & Chhabra, 2004) and more recently reading interventions for older students (e.g., Edmonds et al., 2009; Scammacca et al., 2007) leaving many upper elementary teachers unclear about how these findings apply to their instruction. In addition, considerable evidence suggests that student’s reading comprehension takes a negative turn in the upper elementary grades, often referred to as the “fourth grade slump” (Chall & Jacobs, 2003), and determining research-based practices for intervening is important.

Overall, the number of experimental studies available for analysis was relatively few (n = 9) and represented a range of treatment foci that included comprehension, word reading, fluency, vocabulary, and two that were multi-component addressing multiple elements of reading. The largest number of experimental studies (n = 5) addressed reading comprehension or vocabulary development and all of these studies used researcher-developed measures to address outcomes. We think it is encouraging that the majority of outcomes for the comprehension and vocabulary treatments yielded effects that were moderate to large in size. However, it is typical for researcher-developed measures to yield higher effect sizes (Swanson, Hoskyn, & Lee, 1999). This provides support for the influence of vocabulary and comprehension interventions on improving students’ understanding of text. However, the confidence in these findings would be more robust if the studies had not relied solely on researcher-developed measures. For vocabulary treatments, it is common that researcher-developed measures are used to tap the extent to which students learn the vocabulary words taught (Scammacca et al., 2007). The rationale is that most vocabulary interventions are not perceived as being powerful enough to influence more broadly acquisition of untaught vocabulary which is what would be measured on more normative vocabulary measures (Scammacca et al.). The use of researcher-developed measures for comprehension is less necessary and it would be expected that researchers would use norm-referenced measures either solely or in combination with researcher-developed measures to assess the effects of treatment. Considering these caveats, we have learned from both the experimental studies and single-subject studies that for upper elementary students, comprehension practices that provided opportunities for students to preview text and connect with their knowledge, use self-questioning and self-regulating practices while reading, and summarize what they are learning were associated with moderate to high outcomes. It may be that these practices enhance the language functioning of target students with reading comprehension problems, many of whom are likely to also demonstrate low language (Nation, Clarke, Marshall, & Durand, 2004). These findings are in line with a previous research syntheses on reading comprehension outcomes with older students (Edmonds et al., 2009; Scammacca et al., 2007).

Two of the studies addressed fluency in which repeated reading of text was compared with continuous reading. The amount of time students read the text was held constant but in one treatment condition students read the text only one time and continuously (sustained/continuous treatment) and in the other condition the text was read three times (repeated reading). Findings in one of the studies (Mathes & Fuchs, 1993) yielded very low effects for both treatment conditions using peers, whereas in the second study (O’Connor et al., 2007) moderate to large effects for fluency and comprehension resulted when students were paired with adults. Because these two studies do not provide converging outcomes for students with reading difficulties and disabilities, we would suggest that teachers integrate both repeated reading and continuous reading into their interventions and monitor students’ progress to determine effectiveness. Also, it appears as though an adult or very able reader as a model is associated with improved fluency outcomes (Daly & Martens, 1994).

Word study interventions that assisted students in learning to map the sounds of language to letters and words were associated with small to moderate effects for fourth and fifth graders. In contrast to the comprehension interventions that may have inflated effects due to the administration of researcher-developed outcome measures, the three word recognition studies providing data for effect size calculation administered norm-referenced measures at posttest. The Edmonds et al. (2009) meta-analysis examining interventions for secondary students with reading difficulties also revealed that reading comprehension outcomes were positively affected by word study treatments; however as with the current studies, the results were small to moderate. These findings are similar to previous research that suggests for many students oral language proficiency as well as phonological knowledge relates to their course of reading development (Nation & Snowling, 2004).

It also likely that many students with reading difficulties or disabilities in fourth and fifth grade may continue to have word recognition difficulties; whereas other students suffering from the “fourth grade slump” may struggle more specifically with the increased vocabulary and understanding the variety of complex texts in the content areas. Thus, a word recognition treatment may have a greater effect for students who continue to struggle with word recognition. None of the studies synthesized has examined differential effects for students participating in the interventions based on level of reading, but investigation in this area may help further explain effects.

Only two studies in this synthesis examined multi-component reading interventions. The findings of these two studies revealed that treatments that included two or more components of reading (e.g., word study and comprehension) were associated with moderate to large effects. The value of multi-component interventions for older students was confirmed in three syntheses examining the effects of treatments with secondary students (Kamil et al., 2008; Scammacca et al., 2007; Torgesen et al., 2007). Our interpretation of these findings is that older students may benefit when interventions focus on more than one element of reading. However, given the very small number of multi-component studies for students in grades 4–5 as well as the range of effects reported, additional research is needed to confirm the positive effects for multi-component interventions.

Summary of implications and further research

This synthesis of research for students with reading difficulties and disabilities in the upper elementary grades suggests: (a) instruction in comprehension strategies for application before, during, and after reading produces increased comprehension outcomes on researcher-developed measures, (b) mixed results for fluency interventions, (c) limited evidence (one study) for the effects of vocabulary instruction, and (d) multi-component interventions demonstrate promise for increasing student outcomes on a variety of measures. Fourth graders who struggle with reading can demonstrate a range of distinctive patterns of performance that contribute to their low reading comprehension difficulties and represent variation in performance on word identification, phonemic awareness, comprehension, vocabulary, rate of reading and expression (Buly & Valencia, 2002). Future research may implement interventions that consider the type of reading comprehension problem and mapping interventions to specific comprehension problems.

Based on the current research, we also conclude that further research is needed to examine the effects of comprehension interventions on broad comprehension outcomes with standardized measures. Furthermore, we located only one vocabulary study and two multi-component studies for students with reading difficulties in the upper elementary grades. While previous syntheses have reported large effects for vocabulary interventions for secondary readers (Kamil et al., 2008; Scammacca et al., 2007) and moderate effects for multi-component interventions (Edmonds et al., 2009; Scammacca et al., 2007), additional work is needed to determine the effects of these interventions for upper elementary students.

E.D. Hirsch (2003) states, “We’re finding that even though the vast majority of our youngest readers can manage simple texts, many students-particularly those from low-income families-struggle when it comes time in grade four to tackle more academic texts.” (p. 10). This synthesis was designed to reveal those instructional practices that research documents are associated with improved outcomes for upper elementary students with reading difficulties. While this synthesis, like many in education, is only as good as the extant research, we believe that the findings from this report provide initial guidance to teachers and educators about practices that they can integrate into their interventions.

Contributor Information

Jeanne Wanzek, Florida State University, School of Teacher Education and Florida Center for Reading Research, C234B Psychology, 1107 Call St., P.O. Box 306-4304, Tallahassee, FL 32306, USA.

Jade Wexler, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA.

Sharon Vaughn, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA.

Stephen Ciullo, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA.

References

  1. Blachman BA, Schatschneider C, Fletcher JM, Francis DJ, Clonan S, Shaywitz B, et al. Effects of intensive reading remediation for second and third graders. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2004;96:444–461. [Google Scholar]
  2. Bruce ME, Chan LKS. Reciprocal teaching and transenvironmental programming: A program to facilitate the reading comprehension of students with reading difficulties. Remedial and Special Education. 1991;12:44–53. [Google Scholar]
  3. Bryant FB, Wortman PM. Methodological issues in the meta-analysis of quasi-experiments. New Directions for Program Evaluation. 1984;24:5–24. [Google Scholar]
  4. Buly MR, Valencia SW. Below the bar: Profiles of students who fail state reading assessments. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis. 2002;24(3):219–239. [Google Scholar]
  5. Butler FM. Reading partners: Students can help each other learn to read! Education and Treatment of Children. 1999;22:415–426. [Google Scholar]
  6. Chall JS. Stages of reading development. McGraw-Hill; New York: 1983. [Google Scholar]
  7. Chall JS, Jacobs VA. Writing and reading in the elementary grades: Developmental trends among low-SES children. Language Arts. 1983;60:617–626. [Google Scholar]
  8. Chall JS, Jacobs VA. Poor children’s fourth-grade slump. American Educator. 2003;2(1):14–15. see also 44. [Google Scholar]
  9. Daly EJ, III, Martens BK. A comparison of three interventions for increasing oral reading performance: Application of the instructional hierarchy. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 1994;27:459–469. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1994.27-459. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Das JP, Mishra RK, Pool JE. An experiment on cognitive remediation of word-reading difficulty. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 1995;28:66–79. doi: 10.1177/002221949502800201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Das-Smaal EA, Klapwijk MJG, Leij A. Training of perceptual unit processing in children with a reading disability. Cognition and Instruction. 1996;14:221–250. [Google Scholar]
  12. Denton CA, Fletcher JM, Anthony JL, Francis DJ. An evaluation of intensive intervention for students with persistent reading difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2006;35:447–466. doi: 10.1177/00222194060390050601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Dynarski M, Clarke L, Cobb B, Finn J, Rumberger R, Smink J. Dropout Prevention: A Practice Guide (NCEE 2008–4025) National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education; Washington, DC: 2008. Retrieved December 4, 2008, from http://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc. [Google Scholar]
  14. Edmonds MS, Vaughn S, Wexler J, Reutebuch CK, Cable A, Tackett KK, et al. A synthesis of reading interventions and effects on reading outcomes for older struggling readers. Review of Educational Research. 2009;79:262–300. doi: 10.3102/0034654308325998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Felton R. Effects of instruction on the decoding skills of children with phonological-processing problems. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 1993;26:583–589. doi: 10.1177/002221949302600904. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Ferkis MA, Belfiore PJ, Skinner CH. The effects of response repetitions on sight work acquisition for students with mild disabilities. Journal of Behavioral Education. 1997;7:307–324. [Google Scholar]
  17. Fletcher JM, Lyon GR, Fuchs LS, Barnes MA. Learning disabilities: From identification to intervention. Guilford; New York: 2007. [Google Scholar]
  18. Gillon G, Dodd B. Enhancing the phonological processing skills of children with specific reading disability. European Journal of Disorders of Communication. 1997;32:67–90. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-6984.1997.tb01625.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Grigg WS, Daane MC, Jin Y, Campbell JR. National assessment of educational progress. The nation’s report card: Reading 2002. U.S. Department of Education; Washington, DC: 2003. [Google Scholar]
  20. Hattie J, Timperley H. The power of feedback. Review of Educational Research. 2007;77:81–112. [Google Scholar]
  21. Hirsch ED., Jr. Reading comprehension requires knowledge- of words and the world: Scientific insights into the fourth-grade slump and stagnant reading comprehension. American Educator. 2003;27(1):10–13. see also 16–22, 28–29, 48. [Google Scholar]
  22. Institute of Education Sciences What works clearinghouse study review standards. 2003 Retrieved January 10, 2005 from What Works Clearinghouse Web site: http://www.whatworks.ed.gov/reviewprocess/study_standards_final.pdf.
  23. Jenkins JR, O’Connor RE. Early identification and intervention for young children with reading/learning disabilities. In: Bradley R, Danielson L, Hallahan DP, editors. Identification of learning disabilities: Research to practice. Erlbaum; Mahwah, NJ: 2002. pp. 99–149. [Google Scholar]
  24. Kamil ML, Borman GD, Dole J, Kral CC, Salinger T, Torgesen J. Improving adolescent literacy: Effective classroom and intervention practices: A practice Guide (NCEE#2008-4027) National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education; Washington, DC: 2008. [Google Scholar]
  25. Lederer JM. Reciprocal teaching of social studies in inclusive elementary classrooms. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2000;33:91–106. doi: 10.1177/002221940003300112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Lovett MW, Lacerenza L, Borden SL, Frijters JC, Steinbach KA, DePalma M. Components of effective remediation for developmental reading disabilities: Combining phonological and strategy-based instruction to improve outcomes. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2000;92:263–283. [Google Scholar]
  27. Mason LH. Explicit self-regulated strategy development versus reciprocal questioning: Effects on expository reading comprehension among struggling readers. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2004;96:283–296. [Google Scholar]
  28. Mason LH, Snyder KH, Sukhram DP, Kedem Y. TWA + PLANS strategies for expository reading and writing: Effects for nine-fourth-grade students. Exceptional Children. 2006;73:69–89. [Google Scholar]
  29. Mathes PG, Denton CA, Fletcher JM, Anthony JL, Francis DJ, Schatschneider C. An evaluation of two reading interventions derived from diverse models. Reading Research Quarterly. 2005;40:148–183. [Google Scholar]
  30. Mathes PG, Fuchs LS. Peer-mediated reading instruction in special education resource rooms. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice. 1993;8:233–243. [Google Scholar]
  31. McCardle P, Chhabra V. The voice of evidence in reading research. Brookes Publishing Company; Baltimore, MD: 2004. [Google Scholar]
  32. McMaster KL, Fuchs D, Fuchs LS, Compton DL. Responding to nonresponders: An experimental field trial of identification and intervention methods. Exceptional Children. 2005;71:445–463. [Google Scholar]
  33. Miranda A, Villaescusa MI, Vidal-Abarca E. Is attribution retraining necessary? Use of self-regulation procedures for enhancing the reading comprehension strategies of children with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 1997;30:503–512. doi: 10.1177/002221949703000506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Nation K, Clarke P, Marshall CM, Durand M. Hidden language impairments in children: Parallels between poor reading comprehension and specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing research. 2004;47:199–211. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Nation K, Snowling MJ. Beyond phonological skills: broader language skills contribute to the development of reading. Journal of Research in Reading. 2004;27(4):342–356. [Google Scholar]
  36. National Center for Education Statistics . National assessment of educational progress. U.S. Department of Education; Washington, DC: 2005. [Google Scholar]
  37. National Reading Panel . Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction. U.S. Government Printing Office; Washington, DC: 2000. [Google Scholar]
  38. O’Connor RE, Bell KM, Harty KR, Larkin LK, Sackor SM, Zigmond N. Teaching reading to poor readers in the intermediate grades: A comparison of text difficulty. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2002;94:474–485. [Google Scholar]
  39. O’Connor RE, White A, Swanson HL. Repeated reading versus continuous reading: Influences on reading fluency and comprehension. Exceptional Children. 2007;74:31–46. [Google Scholar]
  40. Rich RZ, Blake S. Collaborating for autonomy: Inducing strategic behaviors in students with learning disabilities. Journal of Educational and Psychological Consultation. 1994;5:359–372. [Google Scholar]
  41. Scammacca N, Roberts G, Vaughn S, Edmonds M, Wexler J, Reutebuch CK, et al. Intervention for adolescent struggling readers: A meta-analysis with implication for practice. RMC Research Corporation, Center on Instruction; Portsmouth, NH: 2007. [Google Scholar]
  42. Shute VJ. Focus on formative feedback. Review of Educational Research. 2008;78:153–189. [Google Scholar]
  43. Swanson HL, Hoskyn M, Lee C. Interventions for students with learning disabilities. Guilford; New York: 1999. [Google Scholar]
  44. Takala M. The effects of reciprocal teaching on reading comprehension in mainstream and special (SLI) education. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research. 2006;50:559–576. [Google Scholar]
  45. Taylor LK, Alber SR, Walker DW. The comparative effects of a modified self-questioning strategy and story mapping on the reading comprehension of elementary students with learning disabilities. Journal of Behavioral Education. 2002;11:69–87. [Google Scholar]
  46. Thaler V, Ebner EM, Wimmer H, Landerl K. Training reading fluency in dysfluent readers with high reading accuracy: Word specific effects but low transfer to untrained words. Annals of Dyslexia. 2004;54:89–113. doi: 10.1007/s11881-004-0005-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Therrien WJ, Wickstrom K, Jones K. Effect of a combined repeated reading and question generation intervention on reading achievement. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice. 2006;21:89–97. [Google Scholar]
  48. Torgesen JK. Individual differences in response to early interventions in reading: The lingering problem of treatment resisters. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice. 2000;15:55–64. [Google Scholar]
  49. Torgesen JK, Alexander AW, Wagner RK, Rashotte CA, Voeller KKS, Conway T. Intensive remedial instruction for children with severe reading disabilities: Immediate and long-term outcomes from two instructional approaches. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2001;34:33–58. doi: 10.1177/002221940103400104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Torgesen JK, Houston DD, Rissman LM, Decker SM, Roberts G, Vaughn S, et al. Academic literacy instruction for adolescents: A guidance document from the Center on Instruction. RMC Research Corporation, Center on Instruction; Portsmouth, NH: 2007. [Google Scholar]
  51. Torgesen JK, Wagner RK, Rashotte CA, Rose E, Lindamood P, Conway T, et al. Preventing reading failure in young children with phonological processing disabilities: Group and individual responses to instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1999;91:579–593. [Google Scholar]
  52. Vaughn S, Kim A, Sloan CVM, Hughes MT, Elbaum B, Sridhar D. Social skills interventions for young children with disabilities: A synthesis of group design studies. Remedial and Special Education. 2003;24:2–15. [Google Scholar]
  53. Vellutino FR, Scanlon DM, Sipay ER, Small SG, Pratt A, Chen R, et al. Cognitive profiles of difficult to remediate and readily remediated poor readers: Early interventions as a vehicle for distinguishing between cognitive and experiential deficits as basic causes of specific reading disability. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1996;88:601–638. [Google Scholar]
  54. Wright M, Mullan F. Dyslexia and the phono-graphix reading programme. Support for Learning. 2006;21:77–84. [Google Scholar]
  55. Xin JF, Rieth H. Video-assisted vocabulary instruction for elementary school students with learning disabilities. Information Technology in Childhood Education Annual. 2001;12:87–103. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES