Abstract
Based on the unique longitudinal data of the elderly aged 65+ with a sufficiently large sub-sample of the oldest-old aged 85+ from the Chinese Longitudinal Healthy Longevity Survey, we construct a resilience scale with 7 indicators for the Chinese elderly, based on the framework of the Connor-Davidson Resilience Scale. Cox proportional hazards regression model estimates show that, after controlling for socio-demographic characteristics and initial health status, the total resilience score and most factors of the resilience scale are significantly associated with reduced mortality risk among the young-old and oldest-old. Although the causal mechanisms remain to be investigated, effective measures to promote resilience are likely to have a positive effect on longevity of the elderly in China.
1. Introduction
Resilience, a psychological construct, has been defined differently in extant literatures. In this paper, we adopt the straightforward definition specified in Lamond et al. (2009), that resilience connotes the ability to adapt positively to adversity.
Resilience scores have been demonstrated to be positively correlated with mental health and physical functioning of the elderly (Wagnild 2003; Hardy, Concato, and Gill 2002), as well as with self-rated successful aging (Lamond et al. 2009) in developed countries. Other studies have explored how resilience ameliorated the negative effect of adverse events on health and well-being. For instance, Windle, Woods, and Markland (2008) discovered that psychological resilience moderated the negative effect of chronic illness on subjective well-being among the elderly aged 60+ in England. Furthermore, several researchers have shown that resilient elderly people were more likely to experience positive emotions (Block and Block 1980; Masten 2001; Ong et al. 2006), which were found to promote health and longevity (Levy, Slade, and Kasl 2002; Giltay et al. 2004).
However, three aspects of the previous studies limited the development of research on resilience in aging. Above all, most of the prior studies were based on small samples, with especially limited numbers of the oldest-old subjects (Wagnild 2003; Lamond et al. 2009), which restricted the estimation efficiency. Second, many studies were cross-sectional (Hardy, Concato, and Gill 2002; Windle, Woods, and Markland 2008; Lamond et al. 2009) and few were based on longitudinal survey data. The cross-sectional design could not provide time of death, which would be the key to exploring the association between resilience and survival. Third, almost all previous studies on this topic dealt with developed countries. However, the elderly populations in developing countries such as China, South Korea, India, and Mexico are increasing at a much faster rate and on a much larger scale than that in the developed countries (U.N. 2005). Our current study tried to make contributions in these three aspects to enhance research on resilience in aging.
2. Data and measurements
The data used in this paper were from the Chinese Longitudinal Healthy Longevity Survey (CLHLS). The CLHLS was carried out in 1998, 2000, 2002, 2005 and 2008-2009 in a randomly selected half of the counties, smaller cities at county level and districts under the large cities at prefecture or provincial level in 22 Chinese provinces, covering 85% of the total population (Zeng et al. 2008). The 1998 baseline and the 2000 follow-up waves interviewed the oldest-old aged 80+ only; since the 2002 wave, younger elderly individuals, aged 65-79, were also included in the sample.
This study was based on the 2002-2005 longitudinal sample to explore the impact of resilience on survival among the elderly aged 65+ over a 3-year interval. The main advantage of applying a longitudinal dataset in this study is to learn about the influence of the resilience on mortality among the elderly who participated in 2002 survey, while controlling for various confounding factors including initial health status. We excluded those who were lost to follow-up in the 2005 survey (11.9% of the initial sample) and the 61 deceased who had missing information on the time of death.3 The final sample size in this study was 13,800 elderly people, consisting of 5,896 men (42.72%) and 7,904 women (57.28%).
As CLHLS was a demographic survey rather than a specific psychological study, we did not have as many indicators as the resilience scale typically used in the psychological studies, such as the Connor-Davidson Resilience Scale (CD-RISC) proposed by Connor and Davidson (2003). Furthermore, considering that few Chinese elderly people were Christians, and all of our subjects were not formally employed, the indicators such as “sometimes fate or God can help”, “you work to attain your goals” and “you prefer to take the lead in problem solving”, etc. in CD-RISC were not relevant in our application. Thus, we selected 7 available items in CLHLS to measure resilience (See Table 1). The respondents were asked whether the item statements fit them (denoted as 1) or not (denoted as 0). As questions concerning items 1-4 and item 7 were regarding feelings about self, and were required to be answered by the elderly person him/herself in the survey, 13.5% of the respondents were unable to answer these questions due to poor cognitive capacity. Simply excluding these cases might lead to sampling bias. Thus we conducted multiple imputations for the missing values of these five resilience items based on the respondents’ age, gender, race, education, physical and mental health measured by activities of daily living (ADL) and the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) (Royston 2005). The total resilience scores ranged from 0 to 7, with higher scores reflecting greater resilience.
Table 1.
Items | Respondents were asked whether the statements fit with them | Yes | No |
---|---|---|---|
Item 1 | I don’t feel the older I get, the more useless I am | 4,558 (33.03) |
9,242 (66.97) |
Item 2 | I always look on the bright side of things | 13,189 (95.57) |
611 (4.43) |
Item 3 | I don’t often feel fearful or anxious | 10,425 (75.54) |
3,375 (24.46) |
Item 4 | I don’t often feel lonely or isolated | 9,425 (68.30) |
4,375 (31.70) |
Item 5 | I talk frequently to family members or friends in daily life | 12,463 (90.31) |
1,337 (9.69) |
Item 6 | When I have problems, I can turn to my family or friends for help |
12,873 (93.28) |
927 (6.72) |
Item 7 | I can make my own decisions concerning my personal affairs | 11,794 (85.46) |
2,006 (14.54) |
Note: (1) Percentages are presented in parentheses.
As shown in Table 2, age, race (Han or minority), current residence (urban or rural), primary occupation before age 60 (non-manual job or manual job), education (literate or illiterate), and marital status (currently married or not) were included as socio-demographic controls. Because the follow-up mortality risk and resilience were both correlated with the initial health status, we further controlled for physical and mental health status in 2002 (see Table 2). Physical health was measured by ADL, consisting of eating, dressing, moving around the residence , using the toilet, bathing and continence. If the individual could complete all six activities without another’s assistance, he/she was classified as “ADL independent”; if the individual needed help in at least one activity, he/she was classified as “ADL dependent.” Mental health was measured by the MMSE, which included 24 items regarding orientation, registration, attention, calculation, recall and language, with a total score ranging from 0 to 30. We used the same cutoff as the MMSE international standard to define a total score ≥24 as “normal mental health” and a score <24 as “impaired mental health.”
Table 2.
Confounding variables | Number | Percent |
---|---|---|
Socio-demographic characteristics | ||
Mean age | 86.40 | --- |
Gender | ||
Male | 5,896 | 42.72 |
Female | 7,904 | 57.28 |
Race | ||
Han | 12,956 | 93.88 |
Minority | 844 | 6.12 |
Current residence | ||
Urban | 5,941 | 43.05 |
Rural | 7,859 | 56.95 |
Education | ||
Literate | 8,637 | 62.59 |
Illiterate | 5,163 | 37.41 |
Primary occupation before age 60 | ||
Non-manual job | 1,110 | 8.04 |
Manual job | 12,690 | 91.96 |
Marital status | ||
Currently married | 4,081 | 29.57 |
Currently not-married | 9,719 | 70.43 |
Initial health status | ||
Activities of daily living | ||
Independent | 9,697 | 70.27 |
Dependent | 4,103 | 29.73 |
MMSE | ||
Good | 8,160 | 59.13 |
Impaired | 5,640 | 40.87 |
Note: Variables are measured at the 2002 interview.
3. Methods
We conducted item-total correlations to test the internal consistency of the resilience scale as well as principle component analysis to assess its factor composition. We estimated Cox proportional hazards regression models controlling for the potential confounding variable to explore the association between resilience and survival of the elderly. Survival time from the 2002 interview to death (for those died) or to the 2005 interview (for those who survived who were considered as surveyed) was measured in months. We included the common factors of the resilience scale and the total score of resilience separately, to examine the impact of each factor and the overall effect of resilience. To better understand how the impact of resilience on mortality was moderated by confounding variables, we adopted a step by step approach.
4. Results
The item-total correlations ranged from 0.31 to 0.68, implying that none of the 7 items should be dropped from the resilience scale (Field 2005). Principle component analysis generated 3 factors with eigenvalues ≥1. These 3 factors could be interpreted as follows: Factor 1 contains 3 items reflecting self-approval, calmness and the social connection; Factor 2 contains 3 items reflecting close relationship with family and friends; Factor 3 contains 2 items reflecting optimism and control of own life (Table 3).
Table 3.
Item | Factors and item statements | Eigenvalue | Variance explained (%) |
Factor loading |
---|---|---|---|---|
Factor 1: self-approval, calmness and no loneliness | 1.65 | 23.6% | ||
1 | I don’t feel the older I get, the more useless I am | — | — | 0.61 |
3 | I don’t often feel fearful or anxious | — | — | 0.77 |
4 | I don’t often feel lonely or isolated | 0.79 | ||
Factor 2: close relationship with family and friends | 1.33 | 19.0% | ||
5 | I talk frequently to family members or friends in daily life | — | — | 0.79 |
6 | when I have problems, I can turn to my family or friends for help | — | — | 0.79 |
Factor 3: optimism and control of own life | 1.05 | 15.0% | ||
2 | I always look on the bright side of things | — | — | 0.45 |
7 | I can make my own decisions concerning my personal affairs | — | — | 0.87 |
The estimates in Tables 4 showed that, without controlling for any confounding factors, the elderly who were more self-approving, calm and sociable had a 14% lower mortality risk (Model 1). The effect was ameliorated after adjusting for socio-demographic characteristics and initial health status (Model 2 and 3), but remained significant at the P<0.01 level. Close relationship with family and friends significantly reduced the mortality risk by 13.5% (Model 1). After socio-demographic characteristics and initial health status were added to the model (Model 2 and 3), intimate relationship with family and friends reduced the mortality risk by 4.0-7.0%, and both estimates were highly significant. However, optimism and control of own life had no significant impact on mortality risk with and without controlling for other covariates (Model 1-3).
Table 4.
Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | |
---|---|---|---|
Resilience Measurements | |||
Factor1: self-approval, calmness and no loneliness (Containing item 1, 3 and 4) |
0.860*** | 0.905*** | 0.942*** |
Factor2 (close relationship with family and friends) (Containing item 5 and 6) |
0.865*** | 0.930*** | 0.960*** |
Factor3 (optimism and control of own life) (Containing item 2 and 7) |
1.022 | 1.016 | 1.007 |
Socio-demographic characteristics | |||
Age | 1.072*** | 1.055*** | |
Male (Female) | 1.277*** | 1.377*** | |
Han (Minority) | 1.174*** | 1.058 | |
Urban (Rural) | 0.989 | 0.979 | |
Literate (Illiterate) | 1.011 | 1.049 | |
Non-manual job (Manual job) | 0.872** | 0.900* | |
Currently married (Divorced, widowed, never married) | 0.767*** | 0.761*** | |
Initial health status | |||
Independent ADL (Dependent) | 0.611*** | ||
Good MMSE (Impaired) | 0.667*** | ||
| |||
Number of observations | 13,800 | 13,800 | 13,800 |
Notes: (1) The categories indicated in the parentheses are reference groups
(2) * p<0.1; **p<0.05; ***p<0.01.
As shown in the Model 1 in Table 5, without controlling for any covariates, the elderly with a high total resilience score (≥6) were faced with 31.5% lower mortality risk, as compared to those with a lower resilience score (<6). After adjusting for various covariates including physical and mental health, higher resilience reduced mortality risk at old ages by 9.2% and the estimate was still highly significant (Model 3 in Table 5). It was clear that a large part of the effect of resilience was moderated by socio-demographic characteristics and initial health status.
Table 5.
Model 1 (All elders) |
Model 2 (All elders) |
Model 3 (All elders) |
Model 4 (Young-old) |
Model 5 (Oldest-old) |
|
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Total resilience score | |||||
>=6 (<6) | 0.685*** | 0.827*** | 0.908*** | 0.831*** | 0.930** |
Socio-demographic characteristics | 1.077*** | 1.040*** | |||
Age | 1.072*** | 1.054*** | 1.310*** | 1.332*** | |
Male (Female) | 1.281*** | 1.380*** | 0.887 | 1.113* | |
Han (Minority) | 1.199*** | 1.068 | 0.892* | 0.985 | |
Urban (Rural) | 0.993 | 0.98 | 1.122 | 1.03 | |
Literate (Illiterate) | 1.008 | 1.049 | 0.924 | 0.906 | |
Non-manual job (Manual job) |
0.877** | 0.902 | 0.817*** | 0.802*** | |
Currently married (Currently not-married) | 0.757*** | 0.755*** | 0.465*** | 0.630*** | |
Initial health status | 0.546*** | 0.695*** | |||
ADL Independent (Dependent) |
0.608*** | ||||
Good MMSE (Impaired) | 0.659*** | ||||
| |||||
Number of observations | 13,800 | 13,800 | 13,800 | 5,990 | 7,810 |
Notes: the same as in Table 4
In order to explore differences in relationship between resilience and survival among different age ranges, we further conducted survival analyses among the young-old aged 65-84 and oldest-old aged 85+ separately, while also controlling for single-year-age (Model 4 and 5 in Table 5). As compared to the young-old, the positive effect of high resilience on mortality was somewhat smaller among the oldest-old, and it was highly significant among both the young-old and oldest-old (P< 0.01).
5. Discussion and conclusions
The present study was unique as it explored the association between resilience and longevity at old ages based on the unique longitudinal data with a sufficiently large sub-sample of the oldest-old in a developing country. Survival analyses showed that the total resilience score and most factors of the resilience scale had significantly positive impacts on longevity of the elderly, after controlling for socio-demographic characteristics and initial health status. The effect of resilience on survival among the young-old was somewhat larger than that among the oldest-old.
Although to our knowledge, few published studies have explored the relationship between resilience and survival, previous studies obtained comparable findings that resilient elderly have better mental health and physical function in developed countries (Wagnild 2003; Hardy, Concato, and Gill 2002). Resilience is considered as an important attribute in recovery from illness and other stressful event (Hardy, Concato, and Gill 2002; Lamond et al. 2009). When individuals reach advanced ages, accumulated negative conditions throughout the long process of aging, such as health deterioration and the loss of loved family members, represent serious challenges. The elderly of better resilience may have stronger capacity and potential for dealing successfully with constraints and adversity (Jopp and Rott 2006) to gain better health and longevity. The findings from the present study are in general consistent with the findings from the western world, which indicates that resilience performs as a source of protection for the elderly in both developing and developed countries.
Why was resilience positively associated with survival at old ages in China? One possible explanation was that resilience was positively correlated with better health, and better health lowered the mortality risk. This hypothesis was supported through our survival analyses, which showed that the effects of each resilience factor as well as the total resilience score on mortality risk shrank substantially after variables of initial health status entered the model. Prior investigations also lent support to this explanation (Wagnild 2003; Lamond et al. 2009; Ong et al. 2006). However, our analysis demonstrated that, even after controlling for the initial health status, the protective effects of the total resilience score and most of the resilience factors on mortality risk still remained significant. These results indicated that resilience may have independent effects on mortality risk.
The findings presented in this paper should be interpreted with caution given the limitations of our study. First, as the CLHLS was a demographic survey focusing on demographic characteristics, socio-economic status, life style, health status and mortality risk of the elderly, we did not have as many resilience indicators as specific psychological surveys did. Second, we have only examined the association between resilience and mortality, but we were unable to explore the causal relationship and the detailed mechanisms on how resilience works, due to data limitations. More detailed data and advanced methods such as instrumental analysis are called for to explore the causal relationship and the mechanisms in further studies.
In conclusion, the present study provided evidence to support the conclusion that better resilience was associated with reduced mortality risk among the young-old and oldest-old in a developing country, China. Policies aiming to promote resilience such as organizing group activities and encouraging individual expression (Waaktaar et al. 2004) are likely to have long-reaching effects on longevity of the elderly.
Acknowledgements
This study is part of the CLHLS project funded by NIA/NIH (R01 AG023627-01), China Natural Science Foundation (70533010), UNFPA, China Social Sciences Foundation, and Hong Kong Research Grants Council.
Footnotes
Gu and Zeng (2004) conducted careful evaluation and suggested that sample attrition in CLHLS did not yield significant bias.
References
- Block JH, Block J. The role of ego-control and ego-resiliency in the origination of behavior. In: Collins WA, editor. The Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology. Vol. 13. Erlbaum; Hillsdale, NJ: 1980. pp. 39–101. [Google Scholar]
- Connor KM, Davidson JR. Development of a new resilience scale: the Connor–Davidson Resilience Scale (CD-RISC) Depression and Anxiety. 2003;18:76–82. doi: 10.1002/da.10113. doi:10.1002/da.10113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gu D, Zeng Y. Data Quality Assessment of the CLHLS 1998, 2000, and 2002 Waves. In: Zeng Y, Liu Y, Zhang C, Xiao Z, editors. Analyses of the Determinants of Healthy Longevity. Peking University Press; Beijing: 2004. pp. 4–22. [Google Scholar]
- Field A. Discovering statistics using SPSS. 2nd ed. Sage; London: 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Giltay EJ, Geleijnse JM, Zitman FG, Hoekstra T, Schouten EG. Dispositional optimism and all-cause and cardiovascular mortality in a prospective cohort of elderly Dutch men and women. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2004;61:1126–1135. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.11.1126. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.61.11.1126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hardy SE, Concato J, Gill TM. Stressful life events among community-living older persons. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2002;17:832–838. doi: 10.1046/j.1525-1497.2002.20105.x. doi:10.1046/j.1525-1497.2002.20105.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jopp D, Rott C. Adaptation in very old age: exploring the role of resources, beliefs, and attitudes for centenarians’ happiness. Psychology and Aging. 2006;21(2):266–280. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.2.266. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.21.2.266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lamond AJ, Depp CA, Allison M, Langer R, Reichstadt J, Moore DJ, Golshan S, Ganiats TG, Jeste DV. Measurement and predictors of resilience among community-dwelling older women. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2009;43:148–154. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.03.007. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.03.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levy BR, Slade MD, Kasl SV. Longitudinal benefit of positive self perceptions of aging on functional health. Journals of Gerontology Series B-Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2002;57:409–417. doi: 10.1093/geronb/57.5.p409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Masten AS. Ordinary magic: Resilience processes in development. American Psychologist. 2001;56:227–238. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.56.3.227. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.56.3.227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ong AD, Bergeman CS, Bisconti TL, Wallace KA. Psychological resilience, positive emotions and stressful adaptation to stress in later life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2006;91(4):730–749. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.4.730. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.91.4.730. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Royston P. Multiple imputation of missing values: update. The Stata Journal. 2005;5(2):1–14. [Google Scholar]
- United Nations, Population Division . World population prospects: The 2004 revision. New York: 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Wagnild G. Resilience and successful aging: Comparison among low and high income older adults. Journal of Gerontology Nursing. 2003;29:42–49. doi: 10.3928/0098-9134-20031201-09. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waaktaar T, Christie HJ, Borge AIH, Torgersen S. How can young people’s resilience be enhanced? Experiences from a clinical intervention project. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2004;9(2):167–183. doi:10.1177/1359104504041917. [Google Scholar]
- Windle G, Woods R, Markland D. The effect of psychological resilience on the relationship between chronic illness and subjective well-being. The Gerontologist. 2008;48:179. [Google Scholar]
- Zeng Y, Poston D, Ashbaugh Vlosky D, Gu D, editors. Healthy Longevity in China: Demographic, Socioeconomic, and Psychological Dimensions. Springer Publisher; Dordrecht: 2008. [Google Scholar]