Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Oct 1.
Published in final edited form as: Pediatrics. 2010 Sep 27;126(4):e788–e795. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-1187

Gender, ADHD, and Reading Disability in a Population-Based Birth Cohort

Kouichi Yoshimasu a, William J Barbaresi b, Robert C Colligan c, Jill M Killian d, Robert G Voigt e, Amy L Weaver d, Slavica K Katusic a
PMCID: PMC2995945  NIHMSID: NIHMS243460  PMID: 20876182

Abstract

Objective

This study determined the incidence of reading disability (RD) among children with and without research-identified attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), separately by gender, in a population-based birth cohort.

Method

Subjects included all children born 1976–1982 remaining in Rochester, MN after age five (n = 5718). Information from medical, school, and private tutorial records was abstracted. Cumulative incidence of RD, by any of three RD formulas, in children with and without ADHD and corresponding hazard ratios (HR) were calculated separately by gender.

Results

Cumulative incidence of RD by age 19 was significantly higher in children with ADHD (51% in boys, 46.7% in girls) compared to those without ADHD (14.5% in boys, 7.7% in girls). Among children with ADHD, the risk for RD was similar in boys vs. girls (HR=1.0). However, among children without ADHD, boys were 2.0 times more likely than girls to meet RD criteria. Among girls, the HR for the risk for RD associated with ADHD (vs. non-ADHD) was 8.1 (95% CI 5.7–11.5); this was significantly higher than the corresponding HR among boys (3.9, 95% CI, 3.2–4.9).

Conclusions

The risk for RD is significantly greater among ADHD children compared to non-ADHD children. Among ADHD children, the risk for RD is the same for both boys and girls. However, among non-ADHD children, boys are more at risk for RD than girls. Among girls, the magnitude of increased risk for RD associated with ADHD is nearly twice that among boys because non-ADHD girls are less likely to have RD than non-ADHD boys.

Keywords: ADD, ADHD, Dyslexia, Reading disability, Comorbidity, Epidemiology

INTRODUCTION

Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) characterized by developmentally inappropriate and disabling levels of inattentiveness, and/or hyperactivity/impulsivity is often accompanied by associated learning problems.1 It is not surprising that children with ADHD have difficulties in learning as their inattentive, and/or hyperactive/impulsive behaviors in school may impede their academic productivity. While it has also been thought that specific learning disabilities (LD), such as reading disability (RD) or dyslexia, occur more commonly in children with ADHD than in those without,2 results of previous studies focusing on the comorbidity between ADHD and LDs (including RD) have been inconsistent.17 This is primarily due to inconsistency in the diagnostic threshold for RD among studies.1, 8 Furthermore, because most of these studies involved clinically referred samples, results may have been influenced by referral bias. For instance, if children having both ADHD and RD are more likely to be referred due to increased difficulties in school compared to those with only ADHD or RD, the observed comorbid rate between ADHD and RD might be overestimated. As disruptive behaviors of boys with ADHD might lead to increased referrals compared to girls with less disruptive behavior, an overestimation of the male/female ratio may occur in studies of clinic-referred samples. However, Willcutt et al. conducted a series of twin studies regarding comorbidity between ADHD and RD, and concluded that these disorders co-occur more frequently than expected by chance even in community samples.911 In these studies, teachers and parents rated ADHD symptoms. However, as the authors acknowledged,2, 9 the possibility remains that parents or teachers may be more likely to endorse ADHD symptoms when they know that the child is experiencing difficulty in learning to read. Furthermore, the characteristics of the twin study design do not always reflect those of the general population.

To address limitations of prior studies, we evaluated the incidence rate of research-identified RD among children with and without research-identified ADHD in children from a population-based birth cohort, with explicit assessment of gender differences in the comorbidity between these two disorders.

METHODS

Study Setting and Data Source

Rochester, Minnesota is 90 miles southeast of Minneapolis-St Paul, the closest major urban center. In 1990, when subjects in this birth cohort were school-aged children, there were 70,745 residents who were 96% white, fairly young (72% ≤45 years old), and primarily middle class. The demographic characteristics of Olmsted County (Rochester Standard Metropolitan Statistical Area) residents resembled those of the US white population during the timeframe relevant to this study.12

The capacity for population-based epidemiologic research on RD and ADHD in Rochester is the result of a unique set of circumstances. First, Rochester is relatively isolated in southeastern Minnesota, and as a result, virtually all medical care is provided locally by Mayo Clinic and Olmsted Medical Center and their three affiliated hospitals. Through the Rochester Epidemiology Project, all diagnoses and surgical procedures recorded at the Rochester medical facilities are indexed for automated retrieval.12, 13 The medical record includes a detailed history of all encounters in the community, including psychiatry, psychology reports and psychological test results from birth until patients no longer reside in the community. Second, through a contractual research agreement, all 41 public, parochial and private schools in Minnesota Independent School District (ISD) No. 535, the school system for the city of Rochester, MN, gave us permission to access their richly documented cumulative educational records for every child from our birth cohort. These cumulative school records are permanently maintained for all children who have attended public, private or home school in the district. Third, under a separate research agreement we also obtained permission to access the resources of the privately owned Reading Center/Dyslexia Institute of Minnesota (the only private tutoring agency in existence in the community during the school years of our birth cohort members), as well as of the only private community psychiatric practice in the area. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of Mayo Clinic and Olmsted Medical Center.

Birth Cohort

Our birth cohort consisted of all children born between January 1, 1976 and December 31, 1982 to mothers residing in the townships comprising Minnesota ISD No. 535 (n = 8458). The target population consisted of 5718 children (2956 boys and 2762 girls) who still lived in Rochester at or after the age of five14, 15 who were followed retrospectively from birth until the initial occurrence of either death, emigration, or high school graduation. The steps and resources used for identification and follow-up of this birth cohort, and analysis of potential influence of migration bias, have been previously reported.16

RD and ADHD Incidence Cases - Identification and Case Definition

Our strategy in identifying all RD and ADHD incidence cases consisted of several steps, used multiple sources of information and relied on recorded history of symptoms, individual test results, and treatment, available for all members of the birth cohort. The details of the case-identification procedures were previously described in detail.14,15 In short, several steps were used to narrow the pool of potential RD and ADHD incidence cases, starting with cumulative school records of each child in the birth cohort (n = 5718). School records were searched for any indication of concerns about learning and behavior and 1961 children had those concerns observed by teachers, parents, school psychologists, physicians, social workers, and school nurses. Further work on these 1961 children consisted of abstracting data from the school, medical records and the records from two other private facilities described above.

The following data were abstracted: all individually administered academic achievement and cognitive ability test results and detailed information related to behavioral problems (symptoms, clinical diagnoses, results from teacher/parent questionnaires, medication treatment). Identification of RD incidence cases consisted of applying three psychometric criteria. Specifically, for each child designated with learning/behavioral concern, all reading achievement and IQ test scores were used to form pairs of cognitive ability and reading performance measure within each calendar year. The details of the three formulas were previously published.14 In short, in each of the formulas, x represents the study subject’s IQ score, and y represents the standard score from the reading achievement test. Children classified as having RD by the Regression Formula-Minnesota17 (y < 17.40 + 0.62 x) had standard scores in reading achievement that were >1.75 SD below their predicted standard score from an individually administered measure of cognitive ability (IQ). In the Discrepancy Formula approach, differences between age-based standard scores of measures of individually administered intelligence and reading achievement varied by grade (i.e., xy ≥ 15, 19, or 23 points, for kindergarten-3rd, 4th–6th, and 7th –12th grade, respectively). Finally, the Low-Achievement Formula (x ≥ 80 and y ≤ 90) represents an alternative method to identify RDs.1820

Identification of ADHD incidence cases consisted of applying research criteria to the 1961 children (34% of the birth cohort) from our birth cohort who had any recorded behavioral or learning concerns. Subjects were defined as research-identified ADHD incidence cases if their school and/or medical records included various combinations of the following three different categories of information: 1) meets DSM-IV criteria for ADHD, 2) positive ADHD questionnaire results,21 3) clinical diagnosis of ADHD (with or without specific subtype) was documented. Details of information regarding those criteria as well as identification process of ADHD cases were described elsewhere.15 A total of 379 ADHD cases were identified (Table 1).

TABLE 1.

Research Criteria for ADHD Case Definition

ADHD Cases Meetsa DSM-IV Research Criteria for ADHD ADHD Questionnaire Results Clinical Diagnosis of ADHDb Number of Subjects
Research identified ADHD cases + + + 170 graphic file with name nihms243460t1.jpg graphic file with name nihms243460t2.jpg
+ + -- 41
+ -- + 17
-- + + 122 graphic file with name nihms243460t3.jpg
-- -- + 29

Pluses and minuses indicate the presence or absence, respectively, of a given criterion.

a

All DSM-IV criteria were met, only age criterion was not used.

b

Clinical diagnosis of ADHD (with or without subtype) was recorded in the medical record.

Reprinted from Annals of Epidemiology, 15, Katusic SK et al., Case definition in epidemiologic studies of AD/HD, pp430-437, Copyright (2005), with permission from Elsevier.

Non-ADHD Subjects

All the members of the population-based birth cohort who still lived in Rochester at or after the age of five, who were not identified as ADHD incidence cases, and who did not have severe intellectual disability, were designated as non-ADHD subjects.

Statistical Analysis

The Kaplan-Meier method was used to calculate the probability of not meeting the RD criteria from birth to 19 years of age, taking into account the varying duration of each subject’s follow-up. In the absence of competing risks, the cumulative incidence of RD was calculated as 1 minus the Kaplan-Meier probabilities and plotted versus age (Fig 1, Fig 2). Children were classified as having RD by any of the three formulas upon the date when they initially met the criteria for one of the three RD formulas. Children who did not meet research criteria for RD were censored on the initial occurrence of migration from the community, death, last follow-up date, or at 19 years of age.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Cumulative incidence rates of RD identified by any of three formulas for boys and girls, separately by ADHD status.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Cumulative incidence rates of RD identified by any of three formulas for children with and without ADHD, separately by gender.

Cox proportional hazards model was applied to obtain hazard ratios and corresponding 95% confidence intervals (CIs). In each Cox model, incidence of RD was regarded as the outcome variable whereas ADHD (cases versus non-cases) and gender (boys versus girls) were regarded as explanatory variables. The assumptions of proportional hazards was assessed by graphical methods (plotting the scaled Schoenfeld residents versus ranked time) and by introducing a time-dependent coefficient in the Cox models. Both unadjusted and adjusted hazard ratios were calculated. In the latter case, children’s race (white vs. non-white), mother’s educational level and age at birth of child were found to be significantly different between ADHD and non-ADHD subjects (Table 2), and considered to be possibly confounding for the incidence of RD. Therefore, these factors were included in the model. Father’s educations level was not included in the model due to the number of missing values, which may not be missing at random. P-values (two-sided) less than 0.05 were considered statistically significant.

TABLE 2.

Demographic and Perinatal Factorsa between Children with and without ADHD

N missing ADHD (N=379) Non-ADHD (N=5320) P-value
Factors of the child
Boys, n (%) 0 284 (74.9) 2666 (50.1) <0.001
White, n (%) 6 376 (99.2) 5182 (97.5) 0.036
Birth weight <2500 mg, n (%) 11 16 (4.2) 233 (4.4) 0.88
Paternal/Maternal Factors at Birth
Father’s years of education, n (%) 786 0.001
 <12 22 (6.9) 208 (4.5)
 12 121 (38.1) 1415 (30.8)
 13–15 75 (23.6) 1053 (22.9)
  16+ 100 (31.5) 1919 (41.8)
Mother’s years of education, n (%) 494 0.002
 <12 29 (8.4) 315 (6.5)
 12 138 (40.1) 1662 (34.2)
 13–15 116 (33.7) 1594 (32.8)
  16+ 61 (17.7) 1290 (26.5)
Father’s age, mean (SD) 291 28.6 (5.5) 28.0 (5.3) 0.39
Mother’s age, mean (SD) 0 25.9 (4.8) 26.6 (4.7) 0.004
Father White, n (%) 215 360 (99.5) 5022 (98.1) 0.057
Mother White, n (%) 6 377 (99.5) 5203 (97.9) 0.035
Marital status, n (%) 1 0.12
 Married 345 (91.0) 4953 (93.1)
 Not married 34 (9.0) 366 (6.9)
Pregnancy/Labor/Delivery Factors
Pregnancy complications, n (%) 0 37 (9.8) 432 (8.1) 0.26
Labor/delivery complications, n (%) 0 135 (35.6) 1945 (36.7) 0.71
Congenital anomalies, n (%) 0 5 (1.3) 35 (0.7) 0.14
a

Computerized birth certificate information (continuous and dichotomous variables) for all birth cohort children were obtained from the Minnesota Department of Health.

RESULTS

Among the 5718 subjects in the birth cohort, 19 subjects with severe intellectual disability were excluded, leaving 5699 subjects for the analysis. A total of 379 children fulfilled the research criteria for ADHD at a mean (SD) age of 10.4 (3.6) years (median 9.8 years).

Demographic and perinatal factors obtained from the birth certificate of children with and without ADHD in the birth cohort are shown in Table 2. Subjects with ADHD were significantly more likely to be male (P < 0.001), Caucasian (P = 0.036), have parents with less years of education (P = 0.001 for fathers, and 0.002 for mothers), and have younger mothers at birth (P = 0.004), compared to subjects without ADHD. There were no statistically significant differences regarding perinatal factors between those with and without ADHD.

The cumulative incidence of RD identified by any of the three formulas, in boys and girls, as well as the hazard ratio for boys versus girls, separately for those with and without ADHD, are shown in Table 3. In the ADHD group, the cumulative incidence of RD was similar between boys and girls, while among children without ADHD, boys showed approximately a two-fold cumulative incidence of RD compared to girls (Fig 1).

TABLE 3.

Risk of RDa Associated with Gender, Separately by ADHD Status

N Number of RD cases by age 19 Cumulative incidence by age 19, % (95%CI) Unadjusted HRb (95% CI), P-value Adjusted HRc (95% CI), P-value
ADHD
 Boys 284 137 51.0 (44.5–56.6) 1.10 (0.78–1.55), 0.58 1.04 (0.73–1.50), 0.83
 Girls 95 43 46.7 (35.5–56.0) 1.00 1.00
Non-ADHD
 Boys 2666 332 14.5 (13.0–15.9) 1.97 (1.64–2.36),<0.001 2.03 (1.67–2.45), <0.001
 Girls 2654 176 7.7 (6.6–8.8) 1.00 1.00

HR, hazard ratio for boys vs. girls.

a

RD, reading disability as determined by any of the three formulas.

b,c

There is a statistically significant difference between the HR (for boys vs. girls) among ADHD children and that among non-ADHD children (P = 0.003 in the unadjusted model and P = 0.001 in the adjusted model).

c

Adjusted for children’s race (white v. non-white), mother’s educational level (four categories) and mother’s age at birth of child.

Table 4 and Fig 2 show the risk of RD incidence associated with ADHD, separately by gender. ADHD was significantly associated with an increased risk of RD in both genders. However, the risk of RD associated with ADHD was significantly higher in girls than in boys (approximately twice as high as boys), as shown by the significant ADHD × gender interaction on the incidence of RD (P = 0.001 in the adjusted model). In children with ADHD, the cumulative incidences of RD by age 19, separately for the three RD formulas, ranged from 21.1% to 43.2% for boys and from 19.5% to 43.4% for girls.

TABLE 4.

Risk of RDa Associated with ADHD, Separately by Gender

N Number of RD cases by age 19 Cumulative incidence by age 19, % (95%CI) Unadjusted HRb (95% CI), P-value Adjusted HRc (95% CI), P-value
Boys
 ADHD 284 137 51.0 (44.5–56.6) 4.27 (3.50–5.21), <0.001 3.94 (3.19–4.85), <0.001
 Non-ADHD 2666 332 14.5 (13.0–15.9) 1.00 1.00
Girls
 ADHD 95 43 46.7 (35.5–56.0) 7.73 (5.54–10.80),<0.001 8.13 (5.73–11.54),<0.001
 Non-ADHD 2654 176 7.7 (6.6–8.8) 1.00 1.00

HR, hazard ratio for ADHD vs. Non-ADHD.

a

RD, reading disability as determined by any of the three formulas.

b,c

There is a statistically significant difference between the HR (for ADHD vs. non-ADHD) among boys and that among girls (P = 0.003 in the unadjusted model and P = 0.001 in the adjusted model).

c

Adjusted for children’s race (white v. non-white), mother’s educational level (four categories) and mother’s age at birth of child.

DISCUSSION

Population-based, non-referred samples of boys and girls with ADHD and RD are of critical importance in order to increase our understanding of the natural history of the comorbidity between ADHD and RD. The core of this epidemiologic study is the population-based sample of carefully defined, research-identified ADHD and RD incidence cases.

Our primary findings are:

  1. The cumulative incidence of RD is (a) higher in children with ADHD than in those without ADHD in both genders, and (b) there is a significant ADHD × gender interaction on the incidence of RD. Thus, the risk of RD associated with ADHD is higher in girls than in boys.

  2. Among children with ADHD, both genders show similar cumulative incidence rates of RD, while the cumulative incidence rates of RD are higher in boys than in girls among children without ADHD.

In previous studies using clinic-referred samples, the diversity of definitions used to diagnose RD has resulted in inconsistent reports of the comorbidity between ADHD and RD.1 We employed three formulas for determining RD incidence. The cumulative incidence rate of RD by 19 years of age in children with ADHD identified by each of the three formulas in our population-based birth cohort fell in the range of 20%–43% for both genders, which is fairly consistent with the previous studies of clinical samples selected for ADHD, 1, 2 and those by Willcutt et al.9, 11 In addition, our population-based study showed an overall approximate 50% cumulative incidence rate of RD (identified by any of the three formulas) in children with ADHD for both genders. Thus, the comorbidity between ADHD and RD in our community sample was as high as previously reported in clinical samples.

Although the cumulative incidence of RD in boys was twice as high as in girls among children without ADHD, the similar incidence rates of RD between boys and girls among children with ADHD suggests that ADHD is more strongly associated with RD in girls. This means that both boys and girls with ADHD are at high risk to be affected by RD. The relative preponderance of ADHD subtypes has been reported to vary greatly based on gender.2225 Furthermore, the comorbidity of ADHD and RD has been reported to vary according to ADHD subtype.10, 11, 26 Willcutt et al.10 found that the inattentive symptoms of ADHD were more strongly associated with RD than the hyperactive-impulsive symptoms in a community sample. However, they also implied that the relationship between RD and ADHD subtypes are likely to be complex, and therefore, warrant additional research.9 Moreover, recent studies of ADHD subtypes suggest a lack of stability of subtypes over time and thus question their validity as categorical diagnoses.2729 The retrospective nature of this study and changes in the terms of ADHD subtype over time prevented us from obtaining precise information about ADHD subtype for our subjects, and we are therefore unable to address the issue of the influence of ADHD subtype on the extent of comorbidity between ADHD and RD.

Recently, both ADHD and RD have been found to be strongly influenced by genetic factors.11, 26, 30 Candidate gene and linkage studies of RD and ADHD have suggested the presence of common genes contributing to both disorders.2 There is no additional evidence to support that two disorders are transmitted independently in families.31, 32 These findings suggest that both RD and ADHD are disorders based, at least in part, on common genetic etiological factors that increase susceptibility to the comorbidity of both disorders.

Several limitations should be considered when interpreting the present results. First, it should be noted that it was not possible for us to determine whether symptoms of ADHD preceded those of RD. As this investigation is a retrospective cohort study in which the relevant information regarding diagnoses depended on retrospective review of medical and school records, the precise age of onset of both ADHD and RD among children in our sample was not possible to determine; rather, we were able to determine the age at which sufficient information had been documented in the medical and school records to fulfill research criteria for RD and ADHD. It is also possible that some RD or ADHD cases remained unidentified because no screening measures were performed on every child in our birth cohort. However, it should be emphasized that for every child in the birth cohort a systematic, multistaged, multi-resource process was implemented to identify 1961 children (34% of the birth cohort) who had learning behavior concerns. In addition, we uniformly applied clearly defined research criteria for the identification of all incident cases of ADHD and RD, regardless of gender.14, 15 Finally, at the time of the study Rochester, Minnesota was primarily a white, middle class community so inferences to other populations or settings may be limited. However, Rochester has no access problems to medical care and a homogeneous population (95% white) thereby minimizing the confounding effect of ethnicity and race on the study questions.

CONCLUSIONS

This population-based birth cohort study demonstrates that the incidence of RD is significantly higher in children with ADHD than in those without ADHD in both genders. However, the risk of RD associated with ADHD is significantly higher in girls than in boys. This implies that ADHD is more strongly associated with RD in girls than in boys. In the future, epidemiologic and genetic studies with larger population-based prospective designs and more clearly delineated subtypes of ADHD will lead to a more thorough understanding of the gender differences in the comorbidity between ADHD and RD. Although the American Academy of Pediatrics clinical practice guideline on the diagnosis and evaluation of children with ADHD does not specifically recommend psychoeducational testing for every child with ADHD,33 our findings clearly demonstrate that it is essential for clinicians to assess all children with ADHD for the presence of comorbid RD.

Acknowledgments

Funding: The project was supported by research grants from the Public Health Service, National institutes of Health (HD29745 and AR30582).

The study was supported by Public Health Service research grants HD29745 and AR30582 from the National Institutes of Health and the Mayo Foundation (Rochester, Minnesota).

We acknowledge Leonard T. Kurland, MD for his vision in initiating the Rochester Epidemiology Project, and we thank Dr. Steven Jacobsen for sharing the excitement of the real science of epidemiology. We also thank Ms. Susanne Daood for primary data processing; Ms. Diane Siems, Study Coordinator; Ms. Candice Klein, Ms. Peg Farrell, and other members of the LD team for data collection; Ms. Sondra Buehler for assistance in manuscript preparation; and Independent School District #535; and the Reading Center/Dyslexia Institute of Minnesota for their cooperation and collaboration.

Abbreviations

ADHD

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

LD

Learning disability

RD

Reading disability

ISD

Independent School District

DSM

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders

IQ

Intelligence Quotient

CI

Confidence Interval

HR

Hazard Ratio

Footnotes

Conflict of Interest: None.

References

  • 1.Barkley RA. Associated cognitive, developmental, and health problems. In: Barkley RA, editor. Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. 3. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2006. pp. 122–183. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Pennington BF, Willcutt E, Rhee SH. Analyzing comorbidity. Adv Child Dev Behav. 2005;33:263–304. doi: 10.1016/s0065-2407(05)80010-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Mayes SD, Calhoun SL, Crowell EW. Learning disabilities and ADHD: overlapping spectrumn disorders. J Learn Disabil. 2000;33 (5):417–424. doi: 10.1177/002221940003300502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Brook U, Boaz M. Attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and learning disabilities (LD): adolescents perspective. Patient Educ Couns. 2005;58 (2):187–191. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2004.08.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Huang YF, Chang HL, Wu YY, Huang YS, Liang HY. Clinical characteristics of children with learning disorders in Taiwan. Chang Gung Med J. 2007;30 (5):423–429. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Pastor PN, Reuben CA. Diagnosed attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and learning disability: United States, 2004–2006. Vital Health Stat. 2008;10 (237):1–14. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Karande S, Satam N, Kulkarni M, Sholapurwala R, Chitre A, Shah N. Clinical and psychoeducational profile of children with specific learning disability and co-occurring attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Indian J Med Sci. 2007;61 (12):639–647. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Peterson RL, McGrath LM. Dyslexia. In: Pennington BF, editor. Diagnosing Learning Disorders. 2. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2009. pp. 45–82. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Willcutt EG, Pennington BF, Olson RK, Chhabildas N, Hulslander J. Neuropsychological analyses of comorbidity between reading disability and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: in search of the common deficit. Dev Neuropsychol. 2005;27(1):35–78. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2701_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Willcutt EG, Pennington BF. Comorbidity of reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: differences by gender and subtype. J Learn Disabil. 2000;33 (2):179–191. doi: 10.1177/002221940003300206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Willcutt EG, Pennington BF, Olson RK, DeFries JC. Understanding comorbidity: a twin study of reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Am J Med Genet Part B. 2007;144B (6):709–714. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Melton LJ., III History of the Rochester Epidemiology Project. Mayo Clin Proc. 1996;71(3):266–274. doi: 10.4065/71.3.266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Kurland LT, Molgaard CA. The patient record in epidemiology. Sci Am. 1981;245 (4):54–63. doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican1081-54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Katusic SK, Colligan RC, Barbaresi WJ, Schaid DJ, Jacobsen SJ. Incidence of reading disability in a population-based birth cohort, 1976–1982, Rochester, Minn. Mayo Clin Proc. 2001;76(11):1081–1092. doi: 10.4065/76.11.1081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Katusic SK, Barbaresi WJ, Colligan RC, Weaver AL, Leibson CL, Jacobsen SJ. Case definition in epidemiologic studies of AD/HD. Ann Epidemiol. 2005;15(6):430–437. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2004.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Katusic SK, Colligan RC, Barbaresi WJ, Schaid DJ, Jacobsen SJ. Potential influence of migration bias in birth cohort studies. Mayo Clin Proc. 1998;73 (11):1053–1061. doi: 10.4065/73.11.1053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Minnesota Department of Children, Families, and Learning, Division of Special Education. SLD Companion Manual. Roseville, MN: Minnesota Educational Services; 1998. Rev. ed. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Fletcher JM, Shaywitz SE, Shankweiler DP, et al. Cognitive profiles of reading disability: comparisons of discrepancy and low achievement definitions. J Educ Psychol. 1994;86 (1):6–23. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Shaywitz BA, Fletcher JM, Holahan J, Shaywiiz SE. Discrepancy compared to low achievement definitions of reading disability: results from the Connecticut Longitudinal Study. J Learn Disabil. 1992;25 (10):639–648. doi: 10.1177/002221949202501003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Stanovich KE, Siegel LS. Phenotypic performance profile of children with reading disabilities. A regression-based test of the phonological-core-variable-difference model. J Educ Psychol. 1994;86 (1):24–53. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Barbaresi WJ, Katusic SK, Colligan RC, et al. How common is attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Incidence in a population-based birth cohort in Rochester, Minn. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2002;156 (3):217–224. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.156.3.217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Milich R, Balentine AC, Lynam DR. ADHD combined type and ADHD predominantly inattentive type are distinct and unrelated disorders. Clin Psychol Sci Prac. 2001;8 (4):463–488. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Barkley RA. Primary symptoms, diagnostic criteria, prevalence, and gender differences. In: Barkley RA, editor. Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. 3. New York: Guilford Press; 2006. pp. 76–121. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Ramtekkar UP, Reiersen AM, Todorov AA, Todd RD. Sex and age differences in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms and diagnoses: implication for DSM-V and ICD-11. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49 (3):217–228. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Froehlich TE, Lanphear BP, Epstein JN, Barbaresi WJ, Katusic SK, Kahn RS. Prevalence, recognition, and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a national sample of US children. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2007;161 (9):857–864. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.161.9.857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Willcutt EG, Pennington BF, DeFries JC. Twin study of the etiology of comorbidity between reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Am J Med Genet (Neuropsychiatr Genet) 2000;96 (3):293–301. doi: 10.1002/1096-8628(20000612)96:3<293::aid-ajmg12>3.0.co;2-c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Lahey BB, Pelham WE, Loney J, Lee SS, Willcutt E. Instability of the DSM-IV subtype of ADHD from preschool through elementary school. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62 (8):896–902. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.8.896. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Solanto MV, Alvir J. Reliability of DSM-IV symptom ratings of ADHD: implications for DSM-V. J Atten Disord. 2009;13 (2):107–116. doi: 10.1177/1087054708322994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Swanson JM, Wigal T, Lakes K. DSM-V and the future diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2009;11 (5):399–406. doi: 10.1007/s11920-009-0060-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Rommelse NN, Altink ME, Fliers EA, et al. Comorbid problems in ADHD: degree of association, shared endophenotypes, and formation of distinct subtypes. Implication for a future DSM. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2009;37 (6):793–804. doi: 10.1007/s10802-009-9312-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Friedman MC, Chhabildas N, Budhiraja N, Willcutt EG, Pennington BF. Etiology of the comorbidity between RD and ADHD: exploration of the non-random mating hypothesis. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2003;120B (1):109–115. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.20029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Doyle AE, Faraone SV, DuPre EP, Biederman J. Separating attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and learning disabilities in girls: a familial risk analysis. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158 (10):1666–1672. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.10.1666. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.American Academy of Pediatrics Committee on Quality Improvement, Subcommittee on Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Clinical practice guideline: diagnosis and evaluation of the child with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics. 2000;105(5):1158–1170. doi: 10.1542/peds.105.5.1158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES