Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 Dec 6.
Published in final edited form as: Rev Colomb Psiquiatr. 2010 Mar 1;39(1):93–109. doi: 10.1016/s0034-7450(14)60239-0

Validity and Reliability of the Abbreviated Barratt Impulsiveness Scale in Spanish (BIS-15S)*

Luis Orozco-Cabal 1, Maritza Rodríguez 2, David V Herin 3, Juanita Gempeler 4, Miguel Uribe 5
PMCID: PMC2996610  NIHMSID: NIHMS217963  PMID: 21152412

Abstract

Objective

This study determined the validity and reliability of a new, abbreviated version of the Spanish Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-15S) in Colombian subjects.

Method

The BIS-15S was tested in non-clinical (n=283) and clinical (n=164) native Spanish-speakers. Intra-scale reliability was calculated using Cronbach’s α, and test-retest reliability was measured with Pearson correlations. Psychometric properties were determined using standard statistics. A factor analysis was performed to determine BIS-15S factor structure.

Results

447 subjects participated in the study. Clinical subjects were older and more educated compared to non-clinical subjects. Impulsivity scores were normally distributed in each group. BIS-15S total, motor, non-planning and attention scores were significantly lower in non-clinical vs. clinical subjects. Subjects with substance-related disorders had the highest BIS-15S total scores, followed by subjects with bipolar disorders and bulimia nervosa/binge eating. Internal consistency was 0.793 and test-retest reliability was 0.80. Factor analysis confirmed a three-factor structure (attention, motor, non-planning) accounting for 47.87% of the total variance in BIS-15S total scores.

Conclusions

The BIS-15S is a valid and reliable self-report measure of impulsivity in this population. Further research is needed to determine additional components of impulsivity not investigated by this measure.

Keywords: Personality assessment, validation studies, Colombia

Introduction

Impulsivity is a multidimensional personality trait related to the control of emotions and behavior (1). Numerous studies suggest that high levels of impulsivity are associated with deficits in regulating behavioral and physiological responses associated with reward, behavioral inhibition, and decision-making processes (2). Accordingly, impulse control disorders, such as violent outbursts, substance-related problems, accidental and self-inflicted injuries, are prevalent in impulsive subjects and significantly impact society (3). Similarly, impulsivity has been implicated in numerous psychiatric disorders including cluster B personality disorders (e.g. borderline and antisocial personality disorders), substance abuse, bipolar disorders, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, eating disorders (i.e. bulimia nervosa and binge eating disorder) and suicide (2). More specifically, high levels of impulsivity seem to increase subject’s vulnerability to develop these disorders, or if already symptomatic, high impulsivity decreases treatment-adherence, predates or coexists with symptomatic exacerbations, and increases the likelihood of complications or co-morbid conditions (2,3). Thus, an assessment of impulsivity levels should be incorporated to the clinical evaluation of individuals with these conditions.

The Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (11th version, BIS-11) is one of the most commonly used self-report measures of impulsivity; its psychometric properties have been determined in both clinical and non-clinical subjects (4,5). During the past decade, multiple translations of the BIS-11 have been published (611), and neuroimaging studies indicate that BIS-11 scores correlate to prefrontal integrity and function, as well as central serotoninergic function (12). This instrument comprises 30 items that assess three independent sub-dimensions of impulsivity: (a) the ability to focus and be vigilant (attention); (b) living for the moment (non-planning); (c) acting without thinking (motor). Collectively, the three sub-dimensions represent a total impulsivity score (5).

Recently, Oquendo et al. (6) adapted the BIS-11 to Spanish using a sample of 29 dominant Spanish-speaking psychiatric outpatients from the Caribbean. Their analyses showed adequate linguistic, conceptual, and scale equivalence between the English and Spanish version of the BIS-11. More recently, Spinella (13) reported normative and demographic influence on BIS-11 scores in a large community sample (n=700) in the US. The results of this study are in agreement with previous studies in restricted populations: the factor analyses confirmed a three-factor structure for the personality construct impulsivity; the reliability of the instrument was high (Cronbach’s α=0.81). Importantly, the five items with the highest loading on the factor analysis for each sub-dimension were extracted to conform to a shorter (15 items) version of the BIS-11. The short form (BIS-15) correlated well with the original instrument (r=0.94, p<0.01) and retained a three-factor structure. Also, factor analysis demonstrated that items 14 and 30 of the original instrument had to be included in the motor, and planning, groups, respectively, to improve the characteristics of the instrument.

Unfortunately, the psychometric properties of the Spanish adaptation of the BIS-11 have not been determined in South American populations, thus limiting its application in clinical practice. In order to mend this shortfall and based on the findings of Spinella (13) and Oquendo et al. (6), we have constructed an abbreviated version of the BIS-15 in Spanish (BIS-15S) and determined its validity and reliability in non-clinical and clinical samples of Colombian male and female subjects. The results of this study are critical to improve the assessment of impulsivity in Spanish-speaking communities.

Methods

BIS-15S

The BIS-15S (Table 1) consists of 15 items (BIS11 items No. 15, 18, 2, 12, 21, 11, 30, 8, 1, 10, 29, 9, 7, 19, 16) extracted from the Spanish adaptation of the BIS-11 by Oquendo et al. (6). These 15 items were selected based on the items included in Spinella English version of the BIS-15 (13)

Table 1.

Escala de Impulsividad de Barratt-15S

Nombre:_____ Fecha:_____
Instrucciones: las personas son diferentes en cuanto a la forma en que se comportan y piensan en distintas situaciones. Esta es una prueba para medir algunas de las formas en que usted actúa y piensa en distintas situaciones. No se detenga demasiado tiempo en ninguna de las oraciones. Por favor responda rápida y honestamente.
Lea cada una de la preguntas y marque con una X su respuesta Raramente Ocasionalmente A menudo Siempre ocasi siempre
1. Actúo impulsivamente
2. Hago las cosas en el momento que se me ocurren
3. Hago las cosas sin pensarlas
4. Digo las cosas sin pensarlas
5. Compro cosas impulsivamente
6. Planifico para tener un trabajo fijo. (Me esfuerzo por asegurar que tendré dinero para pagar mis gastos)
7. Planifico para el futuro. (Me interesa más el futuro que el presente)
8. Ahorro con regularidad
9. Planifico mis tareas con cuidado
10. Pienso las cosas cuidadosamente
11. Me siento inquieto en clases o charlas. (Me siento inquieto si tengo que oír a alguien hablar por un largo periodo)
12. Se me hace difícil estar quieto por largos periodos
13. Me concentro con facilidad. (Se me hace fácil concentrarme)
14. Soy una persona que piensa sin distraerse. (Puedo enfocar mi mente en una sola cosa por mucho tiempo)
15. Me aburro con facilidad tratando de resolver problemas en mi mente. (Me aburre pensar en algo por demasiado tiempo)

Pre-testing BIS-15S

Initially, a preliminary version of the BIS-15S was administered to 30 subjects who were randomly selected from a sample of 800 undergraduate students at Los Andes University (Bogota, Colombia). Subjects were instructed to answer the instrument and indicate any items that were formulated ambiguously or difficult to understand. All data was collected and analyzed by a research assistant external to the study. Based on these results of this preliminary assessment, the investigators refined the instrument to improve its comprehension and application procedure. None of the items were modified in the instrument, however it was necessary to emphasize the time frame of the items: “Esta es una prueba para medir algunas de las formas en que usted actúa o piensa en distintas situaciones a lo largo de su vida.”

Validity and Reliability Assessment of the BIS-15S

Following preliminary assessment and refinement of the BIS-15S, validity and reliability of the instrument were determined.

Subjects

Male and female participants in this subsequent study included non-clinical and clinical subjects. The non-clinical sample group was comprised of 283 undergraduate and graduate students attending Los Andes University (Bogota, Colombia) that were selected according to a randomized sampling method. Non-clinical subjects were included if the were ages 18 through 65 and Spanish literate. However, they were excluded if the reported a history of moderate to severe head trauma or epilepsy. The clinical sample group was comprised of 164 psychiatric outpatients attending Equilibrio Program and Clínica La Inmaculada outpatient psychiatry clinics in Bogota (Colombia) between June 2007 and July 2009. Clinical subjects were selected by convenient sampling. Clinical subjects were included if they were ages 18 through 65, were Spanish literate, and were currently receiving medical treatment for major depression, dysthimia, any bipolar disorder, anxiety disorders, substance abuse or dependence, eating disorders or impulse control disorders according to DSM-IV criteria. Clinical subjects were excluded if they had a history of moderate to severe head trauma, mental retardation, epilepsy, psychotic disorders or were in a dissociative state during testing.

The sample size was determined using data (means, SD) from previous BIS-11 normatization studies in clinical and non-clinical populations and BIS-15S pre-test results (5,9,14). In addition, sample size recommendations for principal component analysis were followed (15,16); a significance level of 0.05 and power of 80% were used.

Testing and Data Collection

The study purpose and procedures were presented in written format and explained verbally to all interested subjects. Prior to participation, all subjects gave informed consent on forms approved by the Research and Ethics Committee at all participating institutions in compliance with resolution No. 8430/1993 of the Ministerio de Salud de Colombia regarding research with human subjects. Once enrolled in the study, each participant received a numeric identification code. Subsequently, demographic information was collected with standardized forms and the BIS-15S was administered to all participants. Research assistants external to the study verified subjects’ responses and calculated BIS-15S scores.

Statistical Analyses

Descriptive statistics for demographic variables and BIS-15S scores (total and sub-dimension scores) were calculated for non-clinical subjects. Extreme values in this sample group were identified and excluded from final analyses. Kolmorogov-Smirnov tests were employed to confirm normal distribution. Linear regression analyses were performed to identify possible associations between selected demographic variables (sex, age and education) and BIS-15S scores. Intra-scale reliability (Cronbach’s α) and test-retest reliability were also calculated. For test-retest reliability, 20 participants were randomly selected to re-take the BIS-15S two weeks after the first evaluation. Pearson correlations were used as a measure of test-retest reliability since two evaluators were responsible for data collection (17).

Concurrent validity for the BIS-15S was determined against clinical criterion, as there is no “gold standard” to assess the personality trait impulsivity. For the clinical sample, experienced psychiatrists made diagnoses with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID) (18). Stratified data analyses were performed according to primary diagnostic categories including: anorexia nervosa (AN), bulimia nervosa and binge eating disorders (BUL), anxiety disorders (ANX), bipolar disorders (BD), and substance-related disorders (SRD). Descriptive statistics were used to calculate demographic variables and BIS-15S scores in the total clinical sample group and each diagnostic category. Kolmorogov-Smirnov tests were employed to confirm normal distribution. Linear regression analyses were conducted to identify possible associations between selected demographic variables (sex, age and education) and BIS-15S scores. Student t-tests and non-parametric tests were employed to test for significant differences among genders and in demographic variables between clinical and non-clinical sample groups, respectively. ANOVAS and post hoc tests were performed to test for significant differences for all BIS-15S scores (total and sub-dimensions scores) among clinical and non-clinical sample groups. BIS-15S total scores were subject to factor analysis by principal components using an orthogonal (Varimax) rotation to confirm the factor structure of the instrument (7,10). Statistical analyses were performed using SAS® (Version 9) and SPSS® (Version 14.5).

Results

Subjects

447 subjects participated in the study (non-clinical sample group n=283, clinical sample group n=164). Demographic characteristics for each sample group are indicated in Table 2. Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests indicated that age and education values were not normally distributed within non-clinical [Age: D(283)=0.199, p<0.01; Education: D(164)=0.186, p<0.01] and clinical [Age: D(283)=0.257, p<0.01; Education: D(164)=0.117, p<0.01] sample groups. Generally, clinical subjects were older and more educated compared to non-clinical subjects.

Table 2.

Demographic Characteristics by Sample Group

Variable Non-Clinical Subjects* (n=283) Clinical Subjects** (n=164) U p r
MEAN SD MDN MEAN SD MDN
Age (years) 19.59 1.73 19.00 25.94 9.37 23.00 13367 <0.01 −0.35
Education(years) 12.3 1.64 12.00 13.84 2.70 14.00 14841 <0.01 −0.31
*

No significant differences were found between male and female subjects for age (t=0.56, p=0.57) or education (t=−0.57, p=0.56).

**

No significant differences were found between male and female subjects for age (t=0.84, p=0.39) or education (t=1.28, p=0.20).

MDN: Median.

These findings were expected as subjects drawn from students attending colleges or universities tend to be younger and exhibit less variability across demographic characteristics compared to community samples. Also, clinical subjects included in our study were expected to show a greater, but restricted, variability across demographic characteristics compared to non-clinical subjects, as these mental diseases typically affect young adults (2). In fact, 78.66% of our clinical sample group is composed by subjects 32 years of age or younger.

Of the clinical group, age and education were compared across each diagnostic category (See Table 3). Post hoc tests with Bonferroni correction (Significance p≤0.003) revealed that subjects in the BUL (p<0.001), ANX (p<0.001) and BD (p<0.001) groups were older and more educated than non-clinical subjects. No significant differences were found between the latter group and subjects in AN and SRD groups. Similarly, subjects in the ANX (p<0.001) and BD (p<0.001) groups were older than subjects in AN and BUL groups, but only more educated than AN (p<0.001). Finally, subjects in the SRD (p<0.001) group were significantly more educated compared to subjects in the ANX group.

Table 3.

Demographic Characteristics by Diagnostic Category

Variable Non-Clinical (n=283) AN (n=33) BUL (n=57)
MEAN SD MDN MEAN SD MDN MEAN SD MDN
Age (years) 19.59 1.73 19.00 21.48 7.26 19.00 24.17 7.68 22.00
Educationb(years) 12.31 1.64 12.00 12.18 12.76 12.00 13.86 2.67 15.00
ANX (n=27) BD (n=32) SRD (n=15) H(5) p
MEAN SD MDN MEAN SD MDN MEAN SD MDN
31.14 9.56 31.00 29.50 10.24 26.50 25.53 11.04 21.00 97.74 <0.01
15.44 2.02 16.00 14.59 2.51 14.00 12.93 1.98 13.00 74.59 <0.01

AN: Anorexia Nervosa; BUL: Bulimia Nervosa and Binge eating disorder; ANX: Anxiety Disorders; BD: Bipolar Disorders; SRD: Substance-Related Disorders. MDN: Median.

BIS-15S Scores

Non-Clinical Subjects

Descriptive statistics for BIS-15S total and sub-dimension scores are indicated in Table 4. A one-sample Kolmogorov-Smirnov test indicated that BIS-15S total scores were normally distributed (Z=1.31, p=0.06, two-tailed). Linear regression analysis indicated that age, sex and education did not predict BIS-15S total scores (F(3,282)=0.427, p=0.73).

Table 4.

Non-clinical Subjects Descriptive Statistics for BIS-15S Total and Sub-dimension Scores (n=283)

Scores Min Max Mean SD
Total 18.00 50.00 30.97 5.87
Motor 5.00 19.00 10.01 2.41
Non-Planning 5.00 17.00 10.18 2.70
Attention 5.00 19.00 10.78 2.93

Clinical Subjects

Descriptive statistics for BIS-15S total and sub-dimension scores for all the clinical sample and according to diagnostic categories are indicated in tables 5 and 6. A one-sample Kolmogorov-Smirnov test demonstrated that total scores from clinical subjects were normally distributed (Z=0.85, p=0.45, two tails). Linear regression analyses indicated that age, sex and education did not predict BIS-15S total scores was (F(2,161)=0.533, p=0.58).

Table 5.

Clinical Subjects Descriptive Statistics for BIS-15S Total and Sub-dimension Scores (n=164)

Scores Min Max Mean SD
Total 17.00 57.00 34.39 8.50
Motor 5.00 20.00 11.28 3.73
Non-Planning 5.00 20.00 11.60 3.69
Attention 5.00 20.00 11.51 4.16
Table 6.

Descriptive Statistics for BIS-15S Total and Sub-dimension Scores in Clinical Subjects According to Diagnostic Category

Scores AN (n=33) BUL (n=57) ANX (n=27) BD (n=32)
Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
Motor 9.55 3.02 11.54 3.69 9.22 3.03 12.97 3.65
Non-Planning 10.33 3.01 11.51 3.55 11.04 3.55 12.38 3.64
Attention 10.64 3.5 11.23 4.45 11.37 4.82 11.88 3.63
Total 30.52 6.73 34.28 8.32 31.63 7.60 37.22 7.75
Scores SRD (n=15) Non-Clinical Ss (n=283) F (4,159) p
Mean SD Mean SD
Motor 14.20 3.00 10.01 2.41 9.2 <0.01
Non-Planning 14.07 4.71 10.18 2.69 2.92 <0.01
Attention 14.00 3.58 10.78 2.93 1.67 >0.01
Total 42.27 9.23 30.97 5.86 6.40 <0.01

AN: Anorexia Nervosa; BUL: Bulimia Nervosa and Binge eating disorder; ANX: Anxiety Disorders; BD: Bipolar Disorders; SRD: Substance-Related Disorders.

Descriptive statistics for BIS-15S total and sub-dimension scores according to diagnostic categories in the clinical sample are indicated in Table 6. One-way ANOVAS demonstrated significant differences for BIS-15S total, motor and non-planning impulsivity scores among diagnostic categories (see Table 6). Subjects in the SRD group had the highest BIS-15S total scores, followed by BD and BUL groups. Specifically, post hoc tests demonstrated that the SRD group had the highest BIS-15S total scores compared to all other groups (p<0.01 for all comparisons); BD group total scores were only significantly higher than those in the AN group (p<0.01); BUL scores were significantly smaller that SRD group scores (p<0.01), but not significantly different from BD (p>0.01), ANX (p>0.01) or AN (p>0.01) scores. Total impulsivity scores for subjects with AN and ANX were very similar (p>0.01). This is in agreement with the observation that lower impulsivity levels and increased risk-aversion usually characterize subjects with these disorders (14). Similar tendencies were found for BIS-15S motor and non-planning sub-dimensions. SDR and BD groups motor impulsivity scores were significantly higher compared to ANX and AN group scores (p<0.01 for all comparisons), while BUL scores were only significantly higher than ANX group score (p<0.01). Non-planning scores for the SRD group were significantly higher compared to AN (p<0.01) group scores only. No significant differences were found among groups for attention impulsivity scores. These data support a significant role for impulsivity in substance-related disorders.

One way ANOVAS also demonstrated significant differences for BIS-15S total, motor impulsivity, non-planning and attention impulsivity scores between clinical and non-clinical subjects (BIS-15S TOTAL: F(1,445)=25.16 p<0.01; BIS-15S Motor: F(1,445)=19.05 p<0.01; BIS-15S Non-Planning: F(1,445)=21.71 p<0.01; Attention F(1,445)=4.75 p<0.05). Overall, non-clinical subjects exhibited lower BIS-15S total, motor, non-planning and attention impulsivity scores compared to clinical subjects.

Internal consistency as measured by Cronbach’s α was 0.793 overall. Cronbach’s α values for each item are shown in Table 7. Test-retest reliability as measured by Pearson correlation coefficient was 0.80, p<0.01.

Table 7.

Cronbach’s Alpha for BIS-15S Items

Item Cronbach’s α
1 0.772
2 0.791
3 0.776
4 0.781
5 0.786
6 0.787
7 0.793
8 0.784
9 0.785
10 0.777
11 0.774
12 0.786
13 0.773
14 0.779
15 0.786

Factor Analysis

All items on the BIS-15S were subject to factor analysis (see Table 8). A three-factor structure was defined a priori based on reported solutions (4,5). The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure of sampling adequacy and Bartlett’s Test were adequate (0.801 and 0.000, respectively). All items had sampling sufficiency values greater than 0.70 (16).

Table 8.

Factor loadings after VARIMAX rotation

Factor (Loadings) Item BIS-15S Sub-dimensions
1 2 3
0.270 0.712 0.089 1 Motor
−0.052 0.589 −0.036 2
0.172 0.770 0.071 3
0.091 0.658 0.122 4
0.148 0.486 0.084 5
0.022 0.078 0.728 6 Non-Planning
0.015 −0.060 0.665 7
0.003 0.236 0.644 8
0.211 −0.014 0.651 9
0.160 0.402 0.508 10
0.737 0.192 0.045 11 Attention
0.657 0.140 −0.126 12
0.655 0.146 0.300 13
0.728 −0.026 0.153 14
0.598 0.147 0.066 15

Factor analysis confirmed a three-factor structure for the BIS-15S. The first factor (attention) accounted for 25.54% of the total variance. A second factor named “motor” accounted for 11.79% of the total variance. A third factor, named “non-planning” accounted for 10.54% of the total variance. Collectively these three factors accounted for 47.87% of the total variance in BIS-15S total scores. Factor loadings after VARIMAX rotation are given in Table 8.

Discussion

This study describes an abbreviated form of BIS-15S. We report normative data obtained from Colombian subjects, with the instrument’s properties tested in non-clinical and clinical subjects. Compared to non-clinical subjects, clinical subjects were older, more educated and more widely distributed across age ranges. Linear regression failed to show significant associations between demographic characteristics and impulsivity scores. Impulsivity scores were normally distributed in each sample group. BIS-15S total, motor, non-planning and attention impulsivity scores were significantly lower in non-clinical subjects compared to clinical subjects. Similarly, BIS-15S total, motor and non-planning impulsivity scores were significantly different among diagnostic categories within the clinical population. Specifically, subjects in the SRD group exhibited greater BIS-15S total scores compared to all other groups, followed by subjects in the BD and BUL groups. Motor impulsivity scores were significantly greater for the SDR and BD groups compared to ANX and AN groups. Motor impulsivity scores in the BUL group were greater compared to those from the AN group. Non-planning impulsivity scores were also greater for the SDR group compared to AN. No additional differences were demonstrated among diagnostic groups. The internal consistency, as measured by Cronbach’s α, was 0.776 and test-retest reliability, as measured by Pearson correlation coefficient, was 0.80, p<0.01. The factor analysis confirmed a three-factor structure (attention, motor, non-planning) accounting for 47.87% of the total variance in BIS-15S total scores.

Our study significantly contributes to the assessment of impulsivity by clinicians in this country. The BIS-15S is a novel and short, easy to use, self-report measure of the trait impulsivity. The items included in the instrument are easy to understand by native Spanish-speaking Colombians and can be answered in less than 10 minutes. These characteristics are important because language variations among Spanish-speaking communities can modify item comprehension; consultation times have been restricted to 20 minutes or less in most health services in this country; and, subjects with high levels of impulsivity exhibit deficits in sustaining attention on continuous tasks (1). Thus, shorter instruments are more likely to be thoroughly and accurately completed by impulsive subjects.

Significant differences in demographic variables among non-clinical and clinical subjects were anticipated in this study. First, clinical subjects were included in the study to test the concurrent validity of the instrument against clinical criteria. Thus, it was essential to assure heterogeneity within clinical subjects and between clinical and non-clinical subjects to correctly assess the behavior of the BIS-15S across subjects with various types of impulsivity-related psychiatric conditions. Second, it is expected that samples drawn from students attending colleges or universities exhibit less variability across demographic variables compared to community samples as they have already been preselected from the general population according to institutional enrollment policies. Finally, sample size differences among groups may also account for the observed results as sample size significantly affects variability distribution within sample groups (19).

Spinella (13) has previously shown that BIS-15 scores decreased with age and education, although the model in his study only accounted for 10.7% of the variance. Conversely, in our study linear regression analyses for demographic variables as predictors of BIS-15S total scores were not significant. Sample size differences between studies and variability restrictions imposed by the type of subjects participating in our study may account for the observed findings (17). Thus, based on our data we cannot exclude a significant association between age and education and BIS-15S scores.

Consistent with Patton et al. (5) and Stanford (4) our analyses failed to show significant gender differences for either the BIS-15S total or sub-dimension scores. However, Stanford (4) did find a significant gender difference for the first-order subscale perseverance, being lower in women compared to men. However, first-order factors were not determined in this study as BIS-15 (13) has only three first-order factors. Thus, we cannot exclude a gender difference in these factors if present in the BIS-15S.

Non-clinical subjects exhibited BIS-15S total, motor, non-planning and attention impulsivity scores significantly lower than clinical subjects. These findings are in agreement with previous reports of overall low impulsiveness in non-clinical subjects vs. subgroups of subjects diagnosed with antisocial personality disorder (20), borderline personality disorder (2123), substance abuse/dependence (24), bipolar disorders (25,26), attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and conduct disorders (2, 27,28), eating disorders (14), suicide (29) and impulse control disorders (30).

In general, our findings indicate that subjects with SRD, BD and BUL compared to subjects with AN or ANX have elevated total impulsivity scores as measured by the BIS-15S. More specifically, high impulsivity appears to be mediated by motor and non-planning components in SRD and BD groups, but only by motor components in the BUL group. This finding is in agreement with previous studies demonstrating that impulsivity is an important component in bipolar disorders (3133), substance abuse/dependence (34) and eating disorders (14). In fact, high levels of impulsivity are associated with an increased vulnerability to suffer from these disorders, significantly modify their clinical presentation and worsen clinical outcomes (2). Specifically, elevated BIS-11 scores are associated with early onset, more frequent episodes of illness, and a history of suicide attempts in bipolar subjects (35). Preclinical and clinical studies have demonstrated that impulsivity is a significant vulnerability factor for substance abuse/dependence. Importantly, BIS-11 scores have shown to be exquisitely sensitive to distinction within substance abuse disorders (4). For example, BIS-11 score are predictive of the level of an individual’s cocaine use (36). Finally, impulse control disorders frequently co-exist in patients with bipolar disorders and patients with substance abuse, indicating a common substrate in these subjects that may be related to affective instability, automaticity, response inhibition, and decision making deficits (33,37). Thus, it is desirable that impulsivity may be correctly assessed in these individuals using this or similar self-report instruments and behavioral measures of impulsivity as suggested by Barratt et al. (1,38).

The factor analysis confirmed a three-factor structure (attention, motor, non-planning). This factor structure only accounted for 47.87% of the total variance in BIS-15S total scores. Although a three subscale substructure for the BIS-11 has been consistently demonstrated in the literature, the amount of variability explained by the solution is below 50%. The latter suggests that there might be other dimensions relevant to impulsivity, which have not been included in the BIS or characterized to date. Nonetheless, BIS has become one of the most widely used self-report measures of impulsivity in the world and reflects our current understanding about this phenomenon.

The internal consistency and test-retest reliability measures of the instrument are above acceptable levels (15), which suggests that the items included in the BIS-15S consistently measure the construct impulsivity and are highly reliable.

One particular strength of our study is the sample used. Participants include non-clinical and clinical subjects in sufficient number to detect significant differences among groups. In addition, including a clinically diverse or heterogeneous sample is necessary to determine the concurrent validity of the instrument as there is no “gold-standard” to measure impulsivity. The latter proves to be an important limitation of every study that examines the psychometric properties, validity and reliability of a novel self-report measure of impulsivity. Nonetheless, our study suggests that the BIS-15S can be applied to measure the trait impulsivity in non-clinical and clinical subjects in Colombia and perhaps, other Spanish speaking communities. The ease of its application and scoring procedures, plus the fact that the instrument can be applied by any physician at any primary care facility without using most of the 15 to 20 minute consultation time imposed by most health services in this country, are important characteristics that increase the attractiveness of the instrument. However, caution should be advised in the interpretation of the results as our data suggest that other sub-dimensions of impulsivity or factors related to its expression might not be accounted for in our current understanding of the construct impulsivity.

Although the BIS effectively measures the trait of impulsivity, determination of self-report measures of state-like impulsivity are problematic. For example, some participating subjects find it difficult to differentiate BIS item scores across various portions of their lifetime. Additionally, the time course of co-occurring psychiatric illnesses may affect the impact of impulsivity on cognition and behavior, potentially biasing impulsivity scores. Given these concerns, development of state-like impulsivity measures will be important in the future.

Footnotes

*

Supported by SEMILLA grant from Los Andes University (LO-C), U. S. National Institute on Drug Abuse DA023548 (DVH), and Program Equilibrio (MR and JG).

Conflicto de interés: los autores manifiestan que no tienen ningún conflicto de interés en este artículo.

References

  • 1.Barratt ES, Orozco-Cabal LF, Moeller FG. Impulsivity and sensation seeking: a historical perspective on current challenges. In: Stelmack R, editor. On the psychobiology of personality: essays in honor of Marvin Zuckerman. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science; 2004. pp. 3–17. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Moeller FG, Barratt ES, Dougherty DM, Schmitz JM, Swann AC. Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(11):1783–93. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.11.1783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Hollander E, Evers M. New developments in impulsivity. Lancet. 2001;358(9286):949–50. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(01)06114-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Stanford M, Mathias C, Dougherty D, Lake S, Anderson N, Patton J. Fifty years of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale: An update and review. Pers Indiv Dif. 2009;47(5):385–95. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES. Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. J Clin Psychol. 1995;51(6):768–74. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::aid-jclp2270510607>3.0.co;2-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Oquendo MA, Baca-Garcia E, Graver R, Morales M, Montalvan V, Mann JJ. Spanish adaptation of the Barratt impulsiveness scale (BIS-11) EurPsychiatry. 2001;15(3):147–55. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Bayle FJ, Bourdel MC, Caci H, Gorwood P, Chignon JM, Ades J, et al. Structure factorielle de la traduction Francaise de l’echelle d’impulsivite de Barratt (BIS-10) Can J Psychiatry. 2000;45(2):156–66. doi: 10.1177/070674370004500206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Preuss UW, Rujescu D, Giegling I, Koller G, Bottlender M, Engel RR, et al. Factor structure and validity of a German version of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. Fortschr Neurol Psychiatr. 2003;71(10):527–34. doi: 10.1055/s-2003-42872. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Fossati A, Di Ceglie A, Acquarini E, Barratt ES. Psychometric properties of an Italian version of the Barratt impulsiveness scale-11 (BIS-11) in nonclinical subjects. J Clin Psychol. 2001;57(6):815–28. doi: 10.1002/jclp.1051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Someya T, Sakado K, Seki T, Kojima M, Reist C, Tang SW, et al. The Japanese version of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale, 11th version (BIS-11): its reliability and validity. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2001;55(2):111–4. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1819.2001.00796.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Chung YO, Lee CW. A study of factor structures of the Barratt impulsiveness scale in Korean university students. Korean J Clin Psychol. 1997;16:117–29. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Baca-García E, Salgado BR, Segal HD, Lorenzo CV, Acosta MN, Romero MA, et al. A pilot generic study of the continuum between compulsivity and impulsivity in females: the serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2005;29(5):713–7. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2005.04.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Spinella M. Normative data and a short form of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. Int J Neurosci. 2007;117(3):359–68. doi: 10.1080/00207450600588881. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Rosval L, Steiger H, Bruce K, Israël M, Richardson J, Aubut M. Impulsivity in women with eating disorders: problem of response inhibition, planning, or attention? Int J Eat Disord. 2006;39(7):590–3. doi: 10.1002/eat.20296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Nunnally J, Bernstein I. Psychometric theory. 3. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Pett MA, Lackey NR, Sullivan JJ. Making sense of factor analysis: the use of factor analysis for instrumental development in health care research. Thousand Oaks: Sage; [2003]. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Miles J, Shelvin M. Applying regression and correlation: a guide for students and researchers. London: Sage; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders IV– Text Revised. Washington: American Psychiatric Association; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Field A. Discovering statistics using SPSS. London: Sage; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Barratt ES, Stanford MS, Kent TA, Felthous A. Neuropsychological and cognitive psychophysiological substrates of impulsive agression. Biol Psychiatry. 1997;41(10):1045–61. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(96)00175-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Dougherty DM, Bjork JM, Huckabee HC, Moeller FG, Swann AC. Laboratory measures of agression and impulsivity in women with borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Res. 1999;85(3):315–26. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(99)00011-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Koenisberg HW, Harvey PD, Mitropoulou V, New AS, Goodman M, Silverman J, et al. Are interpersonal and identity disturbances in borderline personality disorder criteria linked to traits of affective instability and impulsivity? J Pers Disord. 2001;15(4):358–70. doi: 10.1521/pedi.15.4.358.19181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Links PS, Heslegrave R, van Reekum R. Impulsivity: core aspect of borderline personality disorder. J Pers Disord. 1999;13(1):1–9. doi: 10.1521/pedi.1999.13.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.De Wit H. Impulsivity as a determinant and consequence of drug use: a review of the underlying process. Addict Biol. 2009;14(1):22–31. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2008.00129.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Swann AC, Anderson JC, Dougherty DM, Moeller FG. Measurement of inter-episode impulsivity in bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2001;101(2):195–7. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(00)00249-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Swann AC. Impulsivity in mania. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2009;11(6):481–7. doi: 10.1007/s11920-009-0073-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Willcutt EG, Pennington BF, Chhabildas NA, Friedman MC, Alexander J. Psychiatric comorbidity associated with DSM-IV ADHD in nonreferred sample twin. J Am Acad Adoslec Psychiatry. 1999;38(11):1355–62. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199911000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Malloy-Diniz L, Fuentes D, Leite WB, Correa H, Bechara A. Impulsive behavior in adults with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: characterization of attention, motor and cognitive impulsiveness. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2007;13(4):693–8. doi: 10.1017/S1355617707070889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Dougherty DM, Mathias CW, Marsh DM, Papageorgiou TD, Swann AC, Moeller FG. Laboratory measured behavioral impulsivity relates to suicide attempt history. Suicide Life Threat Behav. 2004;34(4):374–85. doi: 10.1521/suli.34.4.374.53738. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Lojoyeaux M, Arbaretaz M, McLoughlin M, Ades J. Impulse control disoders and depression. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2002;190(5):310–4. doi: 10.1097/00005053-200205000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Henry C, Mitropoulou V, New AS, Koenigsberg HW, Silverman J, Siever LJ. Affective instability and impulsivity in borderline personality and bipolar II disorders: similarities and differences. J Psychiatr Res. 2001;35(6):307–12. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3956(01)00038-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.McElroy SL, Pope HG, Jr, Keck PE, Jr, Hudson JI, Phillips KA, Strakowski SM. Are impulse-control disorders related to bipolar disorder? Compr Psychiatry. 1996;37(4):229–40. doi: 10.1016/s0010-440x(96)90001-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Najt P, Pérez J, Sanches M, Peluso MA, Glahn D, Soares JC. Impulsivity and bipolar disorder. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2007;17(5):313–20. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2006.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Verdejo-García A, Lawrence AJ, Clark L. Impulsivity as a vulnerability marker for substance-use disorders: Review of findings from high-risk research, problem gamblers and genetic association studies. Neuroscience and Biobehav Rev. 2008;32(4):777–810. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Swann AC, Lijffijt M, Lane SD, Steinberg JL, Moeller FG. Increased trait-like impulsivity and course of illness in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2009;11(3):280–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2009.00678.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Lejuez CW, Bornovalova MA, Reynolds EK, Daughters SB, Curtin JJ. Risk factors in the relationship between gender and crack/cocaine. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2007;15(2):165–75. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.15.2.165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Adinoff B, Rilling LM, Williams MJ, Schreffler E, Schepis TS, Rosvall T, et al. Impulsivity, neural deficits, and the addictions: the “oops” factor in relapse. J Addict Dis. 2007;26 (Suppl 1):25–39. doi: 10.1300/J069v26S01_04. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Barratt ES, Felthous AR. Impulsive versus premeditated aggression: implications for mens rea decisions. Behav Scis Law. 2003;21(5):619–30. doi: 10.1002/bsl.555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES