Skip to main content
Learning & Memory logoLink to Learning & Memory
. 2010 Dec;17(12):639–644. doi: 10.1101/lm.1932310

Long-term maintenance of immediate or delayed extinction is determined by the extinction-test interval

Justin S Johnson 1, Martha Escobar 1,1, Whitney L Kimble 1
PMCID: PMC2998334  PMID: 21097607

Abstract

Short acquisition-extinction intervals (immediate extinction) can lead to either more or less spontaneous recovery than long acquisition-extinction intervals (delayed extinction). Using rat subjects, we observed less spontaneous recovery following immediate than delayed extinction (Experiment 1). However, this was the case only if a relatively long extinction-test interval was used; a relatively short extinction-test interval yielded the opposite result (Experiment 2). Previous data appear consistent with this observation suggesting that, although delayed extinction appears more beneficial in the short term, immediate extinction may have more favorable long-term effects. These observations may have important implications for attenuation of relapse in clinical situations.


Extinction refers to attenuated conditioned responding following presentations of a conditioned stimulus (CS) in the absence of its unconditioned stimulus (US; i.e., CS–US then CS–). Although extinction has sometimes been described as a form of unlearning (e.g., Rescorla and Wagner 1972), it likely reflects learning of a new association during the CS-alone trials (e.g., CS–no US) (Bouton 1993). Extinguished behavior is known to spontaneously recover some time after extinction (Pavlov 1927; Rescorla 2004a), although under certain conditions it may become resistant to spontaneous recovery (SR). For example, Lin et al. (2003a,b) reported that synaptic depotentiation (which opposes long-term potentiation [LTP] at most levels) induced less than 1 h after LTP attenuated subsequent expression of conditioned fear. Myers et al. (2006) reasoned that extinction conducted shortly after acquisition should benefit from such depotentiation process and exhibit attenuated relapse. Indeed, they observed that immediate extinction was more resistant to SR than delayed extinction. Nonetheless, other researchers have failed to observe such a benefit of immediate extinction (Rescorla 2004b; Maren and Chang 2006; Norrholm et al. 2008; Schiller et al. 2008; Woods and Bouton 2008; Chang and Maren 2009).

The present studies investigated the relationship between the acquisition-extinction interval and SR from extinction. The subjects were 144 experimentally naïve male Sprague-Dawley rats (Experiment 1: 64, Experiment 2: 80), approximately 60 d old at initiation of the studies. Subjects were pair-housed in standard plastic cages on a 12:12-h light:dark cycle. Water availability was restricted to 30 min/d (food was available ad libitum).

The apparatus was eight identical chambers (30.5 × 24.1 × 21.0 cm). Each chamber was housed in a melamine sound-attenuation cubicle illuminated with a 100-mA houselight delivered at 28 VDC, and equipped with an exhaust fan that provided a constant, 70 dB(A), background noise. The sidewalls of the chamber were made of aluminum sheet metal, and the remaining walls and ceiling were made of clear polycarbonate. The floor consisted of 4.8 mm stainless-steel rods, spaced 1.6 cm center-to-center, which could be electrified with a scrambled footshock. All chambers had two speakers that delivered a 60-sec white noise pulsating two times/sec or 60-sec 4000-Hz tone, both at 80 dB(A). These stimuli served as X and A (counterbalanced). Depressing a response lever (protracted on a side wall) was associated with delivery of 0.05-mL droplets of water reinforcement into a cup located inside a 5.1 × 5.1 × 5.1 cm niche, 1.5 cm above the grid floor.

Experiment 1 was conducted in two replications, in one of which half of the boxes had a polycarbonate plate placed on a 41° angle inside the chamber, reducing its bottom to 17.1 cm in length without occluding the lever, houselight, or speakers. All polycarbonate walls were covered with a checkerboard pattern. Chamber type was irrelevant to the present studies. Neither chamber type nor replication had any statistical effects; thus, all data were pooled across these factors.

Lever-pressing was trained in four 1-h autoshaping sessions, during which reinforcement was delivered on concurrent FT-5-min/FR-1 (Day 1), FR-1 (Days 2 and 3), and VI-20-s (Day 4 and remainder of the study) schedules. On Day 5 (Delayed condition) or Day 6 (Immediate condition), all subjects received three pairings of CS X and the 0.85-mA footshock US, which occurred at 2, 12, and 22 min after placement in the test chambers. Two minutes after the third conditioning trial, all animals were removed from the testing chambers and returned to their home cages. Subjects were then returned to the testing chambers for the extinction phase. The total time elapsed between the last conditioning trial and the first extinction trial was 12 min (Immediate condition) or 24 h (Delayed condition). Extinction subjects received 20 presentations of CS X, whereas Control subjects received 20 presentations of CS A (mean intertrial interval: 3 min). Seventy-two hours after completion of extinction, subjects were returned to the testing chambers and received six presentations of CS X, spaced 10 min apart. Further extinction was expected to result from the test exposures after SR (relapse). Consequently, testing was repeated 48 h later to assess whether the timing (immediate vs. delayed) of initial extinction affected the effectiveness of subsequent extinction after relapse (re-extinction would be the natural option in a clinical relapse situation). Note that the training described above equated with the extinction-test interval, but varying the acquisition-extinction interval necessarily varied the acquisition-testing interval. However, behavioral changes resulting from extinction are more sensitive to the passage of time than those resulting from acquisition (e.g., Bouton 1993; Woods and Bouton 2008), and previous research suggests that the acquisition-test interval does not interact with the acquisition-extinction interval (also see Maren and Chang 2006).

Shock treatment usually disrupts baseline behavior, which precludes the calculation of suppression ratios (see below for elaboration). To improve the baseline during the testing phase, we provided a 1-d restabilization-of-baseline treatment on the day preceding testing. Furthermore, to determine whether baseline disruptions were due to incidental lever-pressing–shock pairings, the levers were not available during acquisition and extinction for half of the subjects in Experiment 1 (they were available during shaping, restabilization, and testing). The number and timing of exposures to the CS and US were equated in the lever and no-lever conditions. Lever availability had no statistical impact on the SR test; thus, all test data were collapsed across this factor.

Data were converted to suppression ratios, using the formula A/[A + (B / 3)], where A represents responding during the 30-sec CS period and B represents responding during a 90-sec baseline period. All data were analyzed using standard repeated-measures analyses of variance (ANOVA) with a significance level of P < 0.05. Test data were analyzed in three-trial blocks. The data from five subjects (Groups Extinction-Immediate and Control-Delayed: one subject each; Group Control-Immediate: three subjects) were excluded from the repeated-measures analyses because, for all trials in a block, both the A and B measures were zero and suppression ratios could not be calculated.

A Condition (extinction vs. control) × Acquisition-Extinction Interval (immediate vs. delayed) × Test Day × Block ANOVA revealed the main effects of Condition, Test Day, and Block, Fs(1,55) = 9.01, 24.34, and 67.75, respectively, and a Condition × Acquisition-Extinction Interval interaction, F(1,55) = 6.71. CS X elicited less suppression in the Immediate-Extinction than the Immediate-Control group in all blocks across both testing days, with this difference growing larger as the extinction test progressed, Fs(1,55) = 6.70 (test) and 11.28 (retest). In contrast, CS X elicited similar levels of suppression in the Delayed-Extinction and Delayed-Control groups in all blocks across both testing days, with re-extinction after SR resulting in little separation among the two groups, Fs < 0.06 (see Fig. 1).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Spontaneous recovery test, Experiment 1 (three-trial blocks). (A) Immediate condition (extinction 10 min after acquisition); (B) delayed condition (extinction 24 h after acquisition). Extinction subjects received X presentations during the extinction phase, whereas Control subjects received equivalent A presentations; “Immediate” refers to extinction 10 min after acquisition, whereas “Delayed” refers to extinction 24 h after acquisition. Trial blocks 1 and 2 present the data collected during the first test, which occurred 72 h after extinction; trial blocks 3 and 4 present the data collected during the re-test, which occurred 48 h later. Brackets represent SEMs.

Our data are consistent with the report by Myers et al. (2006) that immediate extinction undergoes less SR than delayed extinction. However, our results are inconsistent with other reports that immediate extinction results in equivalent (Norrholm et al. 2008; Schiller et al. 2008) or greater (Rescorla 2004b; Maren and Chang 2006; Woods and Bouton 2008; Chang and Maren 2009) SR than delayed extinction. In order to elucidate the variables that determine these inconsistent reports, we analyzed previous studies that specifically assessed SR after manipulating the acquisition-extinction interval. Table 1 summarizes our analysis. Studies that obtained more SR after immediate than delayed extinction are presented in white cells, and studies that obtained similar or less SR after immediate than delayed extinction are presented in shaded cells. The only variable that appeared to divide these two groups was the extinction-test interval: Studies in the white cells assessed behavior 24–48 h after extinction, whereas studies in the shaded cells assessed behavior 72 or more hours after extinction.

Table 1.

Comparison of previous studies investigating the acquisition-extinction interval

Trial #a
ITIb
Interval
Study Species Measure CS US Acq. Ext. Acq. Ext. Acq.-Ext.c Ext.-Test
Rescorla (2004b)d Rat Head entries; Chain pull Auditory Visual Food 32; 128 12 2.5 min 2.5 min 24 h vs. 9 d 48 h
Pigeon Keypecks Visual Food 120 16 2.5 min 2.5 min 9 d vs. 29 d 7 d
Myers et al. (2006) Rat Fear potentiated startle Visual Shock 15 90 2 min 5 min 10 min vs. 1 h vs. 72 h 21 d
Maren and Chang (2006) Rat Conditioned freezing Auditory Shock 5; 1 45; 225 62 sec 60 or 12 sec 15 min vs. 24 h 48 h from acquisition or 48 h from extinction
Norrholm et al. (2008) Humans Startle response (eyeblink) Auditory Visual Air puff to larynx 8 16 15.5 sec 15.5 sec 10 min vs. 72 h 96 h for immediate condition, 24 h for delayed condition
Schiller et al. (2008)e Rat Conditioned freezing Auditory Shock 12 16 5 min 2 min 12 min vs. 72 h 21 d
Woods and Bouton (2008) Rat Conditioned emotional response Visual Shock 4 16; 32 20 min 2.7 or 6.5 min 10 min vs. 24 h 24 h
Head entries Auditory Food 40 20 2.08 min 2.08 min
Chang and Maren (2009) Rat Conditioned freezing Auditory Shock 5 45 60 sec N/A 15 min vs. 1 h vs. 6 h vs. 24 h 48 h for animals with acq.-ext. intervals of less than 24 h, 24 h for the rest
Experiment 1 Rat Conditioned emotional response Auditory Shock 3 20 10 min 3 min 12 min vs. 24 h 72 h from extinction

Note: CS, conditioned stimulus; US, unconditioned stimulus; Acq., acquisition (CS–US trials); and Ext., extinction (CS–no US presentations).

aNumbers separated by a semicolon represent various trial numbers used in the different studies (not a parametric manipulation).

bRandom or variable ITIs are presented with their mean value.

cFor training that occurred over multiple days, the interval was measured from the last day of conditioning.

dThere was one more study in this paper, which extinguished the operant response of lever-pressing, but those parameters were not included in the table (only CS-driven responses were considered).

eThere was a human study in this paper, which yielded spontaneous recovery after immediate (12 sec) extinction, but this was the only acq.-ext. interval assessed in the paper. Shaded cells highlight experiments in which spontaneous recovery was observed to be equivalent independent of acquisition-extinction interval or greater with a longer interval (delayed condition) than a shorter interval (immediate condition).

The two studies obtaining similar levels of SR after immediate and delayed extinction may have been less sensitive to group differences for various reasons. Schiller et al. (2008) (Experiment 3) observed similar levels of SR 21 d after immediate or delayed extinction. However, this study confounded the acquisition-extinction interval with where subjects spent this interval (extinction context for the immediate condition; home cages for the delayed condition). Norrholm et al. (2008) observed more SR after delayed than immediate extinction, but SR became equivalent in the two conditions after controlling for contextual fear. However, this study equated the acquisition-test interval in the two delay conditions (4 d), thus confounding the acquisition-extinction (10 min vs. 24 h) and extinction-test intervals (96 h vs. 24 h; see Table 1).

One study may be problematic for the account described above. Rescorla (2004b, Experiment 4) trained three different stimuli, with acquisition-extinction intervals of 29, 15, or 1 d. After a 7-d retention interval, he observed less SR to the distally trained stimulus than the proximally trained stimuli. However, the immediate interval in Rescorla's studies was as long as the other studies' delayed intervals. Furthermore, in Rescorla's study extinction occurred over multiple days (as opposed to 1 d in the other studies), and the ensuing longer intertrial intervals may have led to better retention of extinction to the more distal stimulus (Tsao and Craske 2000; Urcelay et al. 2009).

The apparatus, design, and procedure of Experiment 2 were the same as those of Experiment 1, with three exceptions. First, the level of footshock was decreased (from 0.85 to 0.70 mA) to better maintain baseline responding during acquisition and testing and observe more moderate levels of SR than those observed in Experiment 1. Second, levers were available for all subjects during acquisition and extinction. Third, the extinction-test interval was manipulated, and SR was assessed either 48 h (Short interval) or 7 d (Long interval) after completion of extinction. A second test was given 24 h later as described in Experiment 1 (see Fig. 2A).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

(A) Training protocol, Experiment 2. (B) Acquisition and extinction data (four-trial blocks). (C–F) Spontaneous recovery test, Experiment 2 (three-trial blocks). Each Extinction group is presented in juxtaposition to its Control group. (C) Immediate-Short condition; (D) Delayed-Short condition; (E) Immediate-Long condition; and (F) Delayed-Long condition. In all panels, Extinction subjects received X presentations during the extinction phase, whereas Control subjects received equivalent A presentations; “Immediate” refers to extinction 10 min after acquisition, whereas “Delayed” refers to extinction 24 h after acquisition; and “Short” refers to testing 48 h after extinction, whereas “Long” refers to testing 7 d after extinction. In panels C and D, Ext presents responding during the last block of extinction as displayed in panel B, trial blocks 1 and 2 present the data collected during the first test (at the Short or Long Interval), and trial blocks 3 and 4 present the data collected during the retest (48 h later). Brackets represent SEMs.

Experiment 2 was conducted in two identical replications. Replication had no statistical effect; thus, all data were collapsed across this factor. Figure 2B presents the data collected during the extinction phase in four-trial blocks. A Condition × Acquisition-Extinction Interval × Block ANOVA revealed main effects of Condition, F(1,50) = 19.72, and Block, F(2,200) = 32.78, but no effect or interaction with acquisition-extinction interval, Fs < 1. A similar analysis conducted on the last block of extinction revealed no effects or interaction, Fs < 1.20. Thus, we failed to observe faster extinction in the immediate than delayed condition as previously reported by others (Maren and Chang 2006; Schiller et al. 2008; Woods and Bouton 2008). Unfortunately, baseline disruptions significantly reduced the number of subjects entering the repeated-measures analyses. Missing scores were replaced with group means, but if more than two scores were missing, the subject was excluded from the analysis. Of the 350 data points that entered this analysis, 7% were replaced with the mean, and eight subjects were excluded from the analyses (Control-Immediate condition: two; Extinction-Delay condition: three; Extinction-Immediate condition: three). Although mean replacement artificially reduces within-group data variability, thus maximizing between-group differences, we still observed neither an effect of acquisition-extinction interval nor differences among groups in the last block of extinction, Fs < 1.15.

During SR testing, the data from six subjects (Control-Immediate-Short and Control-Delayed-Long: one subject each; Extinction-Delayed-Short and Extinction-Immediate-Long: two subjects each) could not be included in the repeated-measures analyses. A Condition × Acquisition-Extinction Interval × Extinction-Test Interval (short vs. long) × Block ANOVA conducted on the data collected on the first day of testing revealed main effects of condition and block, Fs(1,66) = 12.62 and 46.40, respectively, and a Condition × Acquisition-Extinction Interval × Extinction-Test Interval interaction, F(1,66) = 4.14. (To simplify the analyses, the two test days were analyzed separately. Including Test Day as a factor revealed the same effects and interaction, as well as a Condition × Acquisition-Extinction Interval × Extinction-Test Interval × Block interaction.) After the short extinction-test interval, extinction was better maintained in the Delayed than the Immediate condition, Fs(1,66) = 4.75 and 0.20, respectively. Conversely, after the long extinction-test interval, extinction was better maintained in the Immediate than the Delayed condition, Fs(1,66) = 11.03 and 1.91, respectively. A similar ANOVA conducted on the retest data revealed main effects of Condition and Block, Fs(1,67) = 16.74 and 19.60, respectively. Re-extinction after SR maintained fear suppression in the conditions in which extinction had been effective in the first place (Immediate-Long and Delayed-Short, Fs(1,67) = 8.13 and 5.10, respectively), as well as the condition that received Immediate extinction and re-extinction after SR (Immediate-Short, F(1,67) = 4.36). Consistent with the observations of Experiment 1, neither extinction nor re-extinction were effective to attenuate fear in Condition Delayed-Long, F(1,67) = 1.05.

Extinction is the presumed mechanism underlying exposure therapy in clinical practice (e.g., Craske 1999; Foa 2000), and relapse due to phenomena such as SR can have serious implications for the long-term effectiveness of psychological interventions (e.g., Jacobs and Nadel 1985). The animal studies described above mirror the ongoing debate of whether post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is better addressed with immediate crisis intervention (psychological debriefing) (e.g., Campfield and Hills 2001), or by delaying treatment until the immediate effects of trauma have subsided, which is usual in cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) (e.g., McNally et al. 2003). Immediate intervention apparently fails to attenuate the long-term effects of trauma (e.g., Raphael and Wilson 2000), and may even increase the incidence of PTSD (van Emmerik et al. 2002). Conversely, CBT appears to successfully attenuate the effects of trauma in the long term (McNally et al. 2003). Notably, in a psychological debriefing situation, subjects are encouraged to talk about the trauma and its emotional implications, which may not necessarily result in extinction of fear. Because early memories are very susceptible to disruption (e.g., Lin et al. 2003a), reactivating them in conjunction with verbal descriptions of the event may result in reconsolidation of the memory with an added emotional component (e.g., Lee 2008). This memory could become different enough from that of acquisition to result in lowered effectiveness of subsequent extinction treatment.

Our data suggest that the interaction between the acquisition-extinction and extinction-test intervals is one of the determinants of long-term retention of extinction. Possibly, immediate and delayed extinction have different recovery functions. For example, if immediate extinction had a higher recovery rate parameter but a lower recovery asymptote than delayed extinction, SR should occur earlier after immediate than delayed extinction but, after a relatively long retention interval, there should be higher levels of SR in the delayed than the immediate extinction condition. Another possibility is that the acquisition-extinction interval determines the time course of memory recovery after extinction (which can be viewed as a form of retroactive interference) (e.g., Bouton 1993; Miller and Escobar 2002). Immediate testing of potentially interfering memories appears to favor recall of recently acquired information (recency), whereas delayed testing of potentially interfering memories appears to favor recall of remotely acquired information (primacy) (e.g., Wheeler et al. 2004). Short acquisition-extinction intervals may attenuate the “recency-to-primacy shift” usually observed with delayed testing. Whatever the mechanism, our data suggest that immediate extinction-based interventions could provide long-term beneficial effects for fear attenuation and subsequent treatment after relapse, even if delayed interventions appear more beneficial in the short term.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by Grant R15 81269 from the National Institute of Mental Health to M.E. We thank Scott Bragan, Laura E. Coursen, Aaron R. Plitt, and W. Travis Suits for their assistance with data collection, and Francisco Arcediano for his comments on an earlier version of this manuscript.

References

  1. Bouton ME 1993. Context, time, and memory retrieval in the interference paradigms of Pavlovian learning. Psychol Bull 114: 80–99 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Campfield KM, Hills AM 2001. Effect of timing of critical incident stress debriefing (CISD) on posttraumatic symptoms. J Trauma Stress 14: 327–340 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Chang C-H, Maren S 2009. Early extinction after fear conditioning yields a context-independent and short-term suppression of conditioned freezing in rats. Learn Mem 16: 62–68 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Craske MG 1999. Anxiety disorders: Psychological approaches to theory and treatment. Westview Press, Boulder, CO [Google Scholar]
  5. Foa EB 2000. Psychosocial treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder. J Clin Psychiatry 61: 43–48 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Jacobs WJ, Nadel L 1985. Stress-induced recovery of fears and phobias. Psychol Rev 92: 512–531 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Lee JL 2008. Memory reconsolidation mediates the strengthening of memories by additional learning. Nat Neurosci 11: 1264–1266 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Lin C-H, Yeh S-H, Leu T-H, Chang W-C, Wang S-T, Gean P-W 2003a. Identification of calcineurin as a key signal in the extinction of fear memory. J Neurosci 23: 1574–1579 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Lin C-H, Yeh S-H, Lu H-Y, Gean P-W 2003b. The similarities and diversities of signal pathways leading to consolidation of conditioning and consolidation of extinction of fear memory. J Neurosci 23: 8310–8317 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Maren S, Chang C-H 2006. Recent fear is resistant to extinction. Proc Natl Acad Sci 103: 18020–18025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. McNally RJ, Bryant RA, Ehlers A 2003. Does early psychological intervention promote recovery from posttraumatic stress? Psychol Sci Public Interest 4: 45–79 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Miller RR, Escobar M 2002. Associative interference between cues and between outcomes presented together and presented apart: An integration. Behav Process 57: 163–185 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Myers KM, Ressler KJ, Davis M 2006. Different mechanisms of fear extinction dependent on length of time since fear acquisition. Learn Mem 13: 216–223 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Norrholm SD, Vervliet B, Jovanovic T, Boshoven W, Myers KM, Davis M, Rothbaum B, Duncan EJ 2008. Timing of extinction relative to acquisition: A parametric analysis of fear extinction in humans. Behav Neurosci 122: 1016–1030 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Pavlov IP 1927. Conditioned reflexes. Oxford University Press, London, UK [Google Scholar]
  16. Raphael B, Wilson JP 2000. Psychological debriefing: Theory, practice and evidence. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK [Google Scholar]
  17. Rescorla RA 2004a. Spontaneous recovery. Learn Mem 11: 501–509 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Rescorla RA 2004b. Spontaneous recovery varies inversely with the training-extinction interval. Learn Behav 32: 401–408 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Rescorla RA, Wagner AR 1972. A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and non-reinforcement. In Classical conditioning II: Current theory and research (ed. Black AH, Prokasy WF), pp. 64–99 Appleton-Century Crofts, New York [Google Scholar]
  20. Schiller D, Cain CK, Curley NG, Schwartz JS, Stern SA, LeDoux JE, Phelps EA 2008. Evidence for recovery of fear following immediate extinction in rats and humans. Learn Mem 15: 394–402 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Tsao JCI, Craske MG 2000. Timing of treatment and return of fear: Effects of massed, uniform-, and expanding-spaced exposure schedules. Behav Therapy 31: 479–497 [Google Scholar]
  22. Urcelay GP, Wheeler DS, Miller RR 2009. Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery. Learn Behav 37: 60–73 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. van Emmerik AAP, Kamphuis JH, Hulsbosch AM, Emmelkamp PMG 2002. Single session debriefing after psychological trauma: A meta-analysis. Lancet 360: 766–771 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Wheeler DS, Stout SC, Miller RR 2004. Interaction of retention interval with CS-preexposure and extinction treatments: Symmetry with respect to primacy. Learn Behav 32: 335–347 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Woods AM, Bouton ME 2008. Immediate extinction causes a less durable loss of performance than delayed extinction following either fear or appetitive conditioning. Learn Mem 15: 909–920 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Learning & Memory are provided here courtesy of Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

RESOURCES