Abstract
I use U.S. census data to perform the first large-sample, nationally representative tests of outcomes for children raised by same-sex couples. The results show that children of same-sex couples are as likely to make normal progress through school as the children of most other family structures. Heterosexual married couples are the family type whose children have the lowest rates of grade retention, but the advantage of heterosexual married couples is mostly due to their higher socioeconomic status. Children of all family types (including children of same-sex couples) are far more likely to make normal progress through school than are children living in group quarters (such as orphanages and shelters).
What types of outcomes can be expected for children raised by same-sex couples, relative to children in other types of families? The answer is vitally important both for public policy relating to same-sex marriage and adoption (Eskridge 2002; Koppelman 2002) and for theories of how family structure matters. Supporters and opponents of same-sex marriage rights agree that the legal issue of same-sex marriage rights should revolve around the question of childhood outcomes for children raised by same-sex couples (Alvaré 2005; Patterson 2002). In this article, using data from the 2000 U.S. Census, I examine progress through school—that is, normal progress versus grade retention—for children of same-sex couples compared with children of other family types.
The debate over same-sex unions and their children draws from and informs a more general literature concerning family structure’s effect on children. The literature on family structure has generally focused on structural variations within heterosexual-parented families, contrasting heterosexual married couples, heterosexual remarried couples, and (presumably heterosexual) single mothers (Cherlin 1992; McLanahan and Sandefur 1994). Even though same-sex couples are a small minority of all couples (1% of all couples in Census 2000 were same-sex couples), the inclusion of same-sex couples can provide researchers with more leverage over the key question of how family structure matters in general.
Studies of family structure and children’s outcomes nearly universally find at least a modest advantage for children raised by their married biological parents. The question that has bedeviled researchers, and that remains essentially unresolved, is why (Cherlin 1999). Some results have indicated that socioeconomic status explains most or all of the advantage of children raised by married couples (Biblarz and Raftery 1999; Gennetian 2005; Ginther and Pollak 2004), while other scholars have found that family structure has an enduring effect on children net of all other factors (McLanahan and Sandefur 1994; Zill 1996). Married couples tend to be the most prosperous type of family unit, and this economic prosperity undoubtedly has certain advantages for children (but also see Mayer 1997).
LITERATURE REVIEW
Same-Sex Parenting
The modern reality of same-sex couples raising children long postdates the classical psychological theories of child development (e.g., Freud [1905] 1975). Recent research on childhood socialization to gender roles has emphasized peer groups and genetics as much as direct parental influence (Harris 1998; Maccoby 1990). In-depth studies of the psychosocial development of children raised by lesbians and gay men has found that these children are normal and well adjusted (Chan, Raboy, and Patterson 1998; Flaks et al. 1995; Golombok et al. 2003). The existing studies have universally small sample sizes of children, and as I discuss shortly, the literature has critics for this reason.
Same-sex couples become parents in three main ways: through one partner’s (generally prior) heterosexual relationship; through adoption; or through donor insemination or surrogate parenting (Stacey 2006). Same-sex couples cannot become parents through misuse of or failure of birth control as heterosexual couples can. Parenthood is more difficult to achieve for same-sex couples than for heterosexual couples, which implies a stronger selection effect for same-sex parents. If gays and lesbians have to work harder to become parents, perhaps those gays and lesbians who do become parents are, on average, more dedicated to the hard work of parenting than their heterosexual peers, and this could be beneficial for their children.
In Judith Stacey’s (2006:39) discussion of gay adoption, she describes the gay men of Los Angeles as having to search through the state’s “…overstocked warehouse of ‘hard to place’ children, the majority of whom . . . have been removed from families judged negligent, abusive, or incompetent. Most of the state’s stockpiled children . . . are children of color, and disproportionately boys with ‘special needs.’” If it is the case that same-sex couples who adopt mainly have access to “special needs” children, the special needs of these children could exert a downward bias on the average outcomes for children of same-sex couples. Fortunately, the census distinguishes between the head of household’s “own children,” adopted children, stepchildren, and foster children.
Nearly all children of gay and lesbian parents attend schools and live in neighborhoods in which other children overwhelmingly come from families with heterosexual parents. In other words, children of same-sex couples share a common peer and school environment with children of heterosexual couples. To the extent that peer environment is a primary socializing environment for children (Harris 1998; Maccoby 1990; for a survey, see Rutter 2002), whatever differences sexual orientation of parents makes within the home may well be mediated and diffused by the common peer and school environments that children share regardless of the gender or sexual orientation of their parents.
How the Census Complements the Existing Literature
In 45 empirical studies of outcomes of children of same-sex couples—including all studies listed in Tasker’s (2005) comprehensive survey that examined childhood outcomes, several more recent studies listed by Wald (2006), all four studies listed by Meezan and Rauch (2005) as the highest-quality studies in this field,1 and all the more recent studies that cite the earlier ones—none found statistically significant disadvantages for children raised by gay and lesbian parents compared with other children. These studies are listed in table form in a supplement posted on Demography’s Web site (http://www.populationassociation.org/publications/demography).
The uniform finding of no significant disadvantage for children raised by gay or lesbian parents has been convincing to some scholars (Ball and Pea 1998; Meezan and Rauch 2005; Stacey and Biblarz 2001; Wald 2006), although others remain unconvinced (Lerner and Nagai 2001; Nock 2001; Wardle 1997). Several points are worthy of comments. First, as the critics have noted, convenience sampling dominated this literature in the past (Nock 2001). More recent scholarship has answered this criticism by using nationally representative probability samples derived from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health (Add Health; see Wainright and Patterson 2006, 2008; Wainright et al. 2004) as well as studies constructed from a hybrid of probability sampling and convenience sampling (Golombok et al. 2003; Perry et al. 2004).
A second critique of the literature—that the sample sizes of the studies are too small to allow for statistically powerful tests—continues to be relevant. The mean number of children of gay or lesbian parents in these studies is 39, and the median is 37; both numbers would be slightly lower if studies without comparison groups were excluded. The nationally representative studies in the series found only 44 children who were raised by lesbian couples in the Add Health survey. Golombok et al. (2003) found only 18 lesbian mothers of 14,000 mothers in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children, which is why they supplemented this sample with snowball sampling and their own convenience sample. The universally small sample sizes of the studies in the existing literature has left room for several critiques, including the argument that small sample studies would not have the statistical power to identify the effects of homosexual parents on childhood outcomes even if such effects did exist (Lerner and Nagai 2001; Nock 2001).
A third potential weakness of this literature is the narrowness of family structures under study (Tasker 2005). Of the 45 studies listed, only 7 examined the children of gay fathers, and only 2 of these 7 studies had a more traditional family control group built into the study.
Among the convenience sample studies, several of the most important have been based on samples of women who became parents through assisted reproductive technology (ART; Brewaeys et al. 1997; Chan, Raboy, and Patterson 1998; Flaks et al. 1995). Because individuals who become parents through assisted means can be identified through reproductive clinics—and are therefore easier to recruit than the general population of same-sex couple parents—the literature on same-sex couple parenting has tended to feature studies of the kind of women who can afford ART: white, upper-middle-class women. Nationally representative data tend to paint a different picture: in the U.S. census, same-sex couple parents tend to be more working class and are much more likely to be nonwhite compared with heterosexual married couples.
The debate over same-sex marriage and gay and lesbian adoption rights revolves around many competing sets of assumptions with political, religious, and ideological axes that cannot be resolved or even fully addressed in this article. To the extent that the debate is empirical—that is, to the extent that disagreement remains over the meaning of the empirical literature on the development of children of same-sex couples—this article offers a new perspective.
To supplement the existing small-scale studies, I offer a large-sample study of children from the 2000 U.S. census, including 3,502 children of same-sex couples who had been living with both parents for at least five years (2,030 children living with lesbian mothers and 1,472 children living with gay fathers; see Table 1) and more than 700,000 children in Grades 1–8 from other family types. This sample size more than satisfies Nock’s (2001) criteria of 800 as the minimum number of gay and lesbian couples required for statistically useful study.
Table 1.
Variable | Unweighted Number of Children in Grades 1–8 (A) | % Over-age at Grades 4 or 8 (B) | Implied % Retained in Grades 1–8 (C = 4B) |
---|---|---|---|
Family Type | |||
Heterosexual, married (ref.) | 612,790 | 1.71 | 6.8 |
Lesbian, cohabiting | 2,030 | 2.38* | 9.5 |
Gay male, cohabiting | 1,472 | 2.42* | 9.7 |
Separated, divorced, widowed women | 81,876 | 2.78*** | 11.1 |
Separated, divorced, widowed men | 21,019 | 2.86*** | 11.4 |
Heterosexual, cohabiting | 14,199 | 2.92*** | 11.7 |
Never-married women | 28,242 | 2.93*** | 11.7 |
Never-married men | 2,365 | 3.15*** | 12.6 |
Group quarters, noninmates | 436 | 8.59*** | 34.4 |
Group quarters, inmates | 352 | 19.50*** | 78.0 |
Child’s Relationship to Householder | |||
Own child (ref.) | 1.86 | 7.4 | |
Adopted child | 2.64*** | 10.6 | |
Stepchild | 3.47*** | 13.9 | |
Foster child | 5.15*** | 20.6 | |
Child’s Race/Ethnicity | |||
Asian American | 1.44** | 5.8 | |
Non-Hispanic white (ref.) | 1.70 | 6.8 | |
Hispanic | 2.24*** | 9.0 | |
Non-Hispanic black | 3.14*** | 12.6 | |
Household Income (in 1999 $) | |||
>100,000 (ref.) | 1.33 | 5.3 | |
50,000–99,999 | 1.53*** | 6.1 | |
25,000–49,999 | 2.17*** | 8.7 | |
<25,000 | 3.15*** | 12.6 | |
Metro Status | |||
Suburban (ref.) | 1.49 | 6.0 | |
Urban | 2.15*** | 8.6 | |
Rural | 2.58*** | 10.3 | |
Child’s Gender | |||
Female (ref.) | 1.63 | 6.5 | |
Male | 2.25*** | 9.0 | |
Head of Household’s Education | |||
BA+ (ref.) | 1.10 | 4.4 | |
Some college | 1.58*** | 6.3 | |
High school diploma | 2.18*** | 8.7 | |
Less than high school | 3.58*** | 14.3 |
Notes: Coefficients reflct census weights. Children include own children, adopted children, stepchildren, and foster children plus children in group quarters, where appropriate. All parents and children have at least five years of residential stability. With the omission of children of couples whose dual marital status was recoded, children of same-sex couples are more likely to be over-age (3.7%) and have a significantly higher implied grade retention rate (14.8%).
Source: Census 2000 microdata, via IPUMS.
p < .05;
p < .01;
p < .001 (two-tailed tests; significance compared with the reference category, determined by logistic regressions separately for each variable)
Using data from the U.S. census has several major disadvantages: normal progress through school is the only available children’s outcome, and even this outcome is measured with less precision than one would hope for. Although the census data have several important limitations for the research questions considered here, the strengths of the census data (large sample, national representativeness, and a full array of family structures) address important lacunae in the literature; as such, this study offers a potentially useful new perspective on how family structure matters to children. The census data are far from ideal for the subject under study here, but better data are nowhere on the horizon.2
Grade Retention
Grade retention (the opposite of normal progress through school) has been increasing in U.S. schools since former President Bill Clinton proposed ending social promotion in schools in his State of the Union Address in 1998 (Alexander, Entwisle, and Dauber 2003:viii; Hauser 2001). Grade retention is an important childhood outcome because retention in the primary grades is a strong indicator of a lack of childhood readiness for school, and effective parenting is a crucial ingredient in school readiness (Brooks-Gunn and Markman 2005). Brooks-Gunn and Markman argued that the lower school readiness of racial minority children is due, in part, to parenting practices that differ from the authoritative parenting style favored in middle-class white homes (Baumrind 1966; Lareau 2003).
Guo, Brooks-Gunn, and Harris (1996) studied grade retention among urban black children and found that some indicators of parental stress, such as unemployment and welfare participation, were associated with increased grade retention for children; in other words, they found childhood grade retention to be a useful measure of difficulties that the students were experiencing at home. Guo et al. (1996:218) identified three potential sources of grade retention: “weak cognitive ability, behavioral problems, and lack of engagement in school.” Of these three causes of childhood grade retention, the latter two might be partly associated with the quality of the home environment. Students with learning disabilities or physical disabilities that impact learning are also at risk of grade retention, and this type of grade retention would not be indicative of parenting deficits.
Grade retention is closely associated with more serious problems later in the life course. Students who are held back at least once are at much higher risk for eventually dropping out of high school (Alexander, Entwisle, and Horsey 1997; Guo et al. 1996; Moller et al. 2006; Roderick 1994; Rumberger 1987; Tillman, Guo, and Harris 2006). Failure to graduate from high school is associated with low earnings, high unemployment, low self-esteem, and high mortality rates (Guo et al. 1996; McLanahan 1985; Tillman et al. 2006). Even when grade retention takes place in the early grades, the “crystallization” of behaviors and academic abilities implies that the difficulties a child experiences when he or she is 7 or 8 carry forward (more so for girls than for boys) into adolescence and young adulthood (Kowalesi-Jones and Duncan 1999).
There are several theoretical reasons for supposing that children of same-sex couples might have lower school readiness (and therefore higher rates of grade retention) than own children of heterosexual married couples, net of race, parental income, and parental education. First, the legal privileges of marriage are numerous and have direct consequences for the well-being of children (Eskridge 1996; Pawelski et al. 2006). Second, evolutionary theory suggests that parents invest more in their own biological children (Wilson 2002; but see also Hamilton, Cheng, and Powell 2007), and same-sex couples (absent a prior sex change) cannot both be the biological parents of any one child. Third, the large majority of children of same-sex couples from the 2000 census were children from prior heterosexual relationships (only 11% were stepchildren, adopted children, or foster children of the head of household); thus, most of the children being raised by same-sex couples at the time of the 2000 census had previously lived through divorce or parental break-up, which research has shown to be traumatic for some children (Amato and Cheadle 2005; Chase-Lansdale, Cherlin, and Kiernan 1995; McLanahan and Sandefur 1994; Wallerstein and Kelly 1980; Wallerstein, Lewis, and Blakeslee 2000).
The Benefits of Legal Marriage
Legal marriage confers a host of protections and advantages to the couples who marry and to their children. Married couples generally share joint legal custody of their coresident children. In a system of employer-based health care insurance, either spouse in a married couple can usually provide health insurance for both spouses and all their children. Marriage is a long-term contract that allows and encourages parents to make long-term investments in their children (Waite and Gallagher 2000). Divorce rights, which are a corollary to marriage rights, provide guarantees for child support and visitation that are intended to minimize the damage of a breakup to a couple’s children. Given the many practical, legal, economic, and social advantages of marriage as a child-rearing family structure, it should come as no surprise that children of long-term married couples have the best outcomes (McLanahan and Sandefur 1994). The various benefits of marriage extend far beyond income, so one would generally expect children in married couples to have advantages, even after socioeconomic status (SES) is accounted for in regressions.
The moral claim for same-sex marriage rests in part on the many practical and psychological benefits of marriage, benefits for which conservative family scholars have made the most careful and enthusiastic case (Waite and Gallagher 2000; Wilson 2002). The benefits of marriage, combined with the exclusion of gays and lesbians (and their children) from those benefits, together form one cornerstone of the case for same-sex marriage (Eskridge 1996).
Relevant Comparison Sets for Same-Sex Couples
Along with the standard comparison group of heterosexual married couples, heterosexual cohabiting couples are a second logical comparison group for same-sex cohabiting couples. Both heterosexual cohabiters and same-sex cohabiters are two-parent families living without the rights and benefits of marriage. Certainly, there are differences: for example, heterosexual cohabiting couples can marry if they want to, whereas in the United States at the time of the 2000 census, same-sex couples could not marry. The comparison between children of same-sex cohabiting couples and children of heterosexual cohabiting couples allows for a more specific test of the effect of same-sex parenthood on children, while holding constant legal rights and the number of parents.
A third relevant comparison for children of same-sex couples are the children living in group quarters because these are the children presumably available for adoption, and because same-sex couples are more likely than heterosexual couples to participate in the adoption market. Some of the difference between children in group quarters and children living with parents and guardians must be due to selection effects: the most troubled children available for adoption may not be adopted and may do poorly in school as a result of emotional or physical disabilities. On the other hand, if gay and lesbian adoptive parents are choosing from the middle or the bottom of the adoptive pool (Stacey 2006), rather than from the population of the most desirable potential adoptees, then the selection effect will be less important. In either case, the census, as a cross-sectional survey, is poorly suited to the analysis of selection effects. Nonetheless, 2000 census data provide strong controls for individual student disabilities, and any comparison between children living with families and children living in group quarters will be made after individual disabilities have been controlled for.
DATA AND MEASURES
I use age and current grade (variable GRADEATT) from the 2000 U.S. census obtained from the Integrated Public Use Microdata Series (IPUMS; see Ruggles et al. 2004) to create a measure of prior grade retention.3 Delayed progress through school (also known as age-grade retardation) is a widely used proxy for prior grade retention (Hauser 2001; Hauser, Pager, and Simmons 2001). A strong correlation between being older than one’s classmates and having been retained a grade in the past can be documented using the October supplements to the Current Population Survey, which has more precise questions about childhood grade retention (although, unfortunately, a much smaller sample size). For example, for 8th grade students in October 2004, 2% of the 13-year-olds had ever been held back a year in school, but 31% of the 14-year-olds had ever been held back (author’s tabulation). The census survey, which occurs 6 months later in the school year (April rather than October), requires a later age cutoff, so I use age 15 as the cutoff age at which 8th graders are considered too old to be making normal progress through school.4 The 2000 U.S. census question about current grade for students collapsed Grades 1–4 into a single category and Grades 5–8 into a single category. Students attending Grades 1–4 can be identified as over-age only if they are too old to be in the 4th grade (i.e., at least age 11), and students attending Grades 5–8 can be identified as over-age only if they are too old to be in the 8th grade (i.e., at least age 15).
The 2000 census did not include a question about the number of times respondents had been married, so married coresident couples cannot, in general, be distinguished from remarried couples. This problem is mitigated somewhat by the ability of the census to distinguish the head of household’s “own children” from the head of household’s “stepchildren.” The census provides only a cross-sectional snapshot of family structure, which fails to capture the ways in which family changes over time can affect children (Wolfe et al. 1996; Wu and Martinson 1993).
Children’s tenure within their current family structure can be reasonably assured by limiting the analysis to children and parents who all have at least five years of coresidential stability. If the child and both parents all lived at the same address in 2000 as they did five years earlier, it is likely that the family structure at the time of the 2000 census was also in place five years earlier. For children living in group quarters, five years at the same address indicates long-term residence rather than a brief stay at a shelter. Five years with the same family structure at the same address is long enough to imply that the child’s primary school career through Grade 4, and most of the child’s primary school career through Grade 8, are likely to have been undertaken within the family structure reported to the census in 2000. For children living with single parents, five-year residential stability of child and parent is a bit more ambiguous because we do not know whether or when a partner or ex-spouse moved out of the home.
Unmarried partners were first distinguished from roommates in the 1990 census. For the 2000 census, the Census Bureau changed its long-standing policy by counting self-reported same-sex “married” couples as unmarried partners (Rosenfeld and Kim 2005; U.S. Census Bureau 2001). The recoded “married” couples accounted for roughly one-half of the same-sex partners and 80% of the children of same-sex couples in the 2000 census. The inclusion of the self-reported “married” couples among the same-sex partnered couples is thought to yield a more accurate population count of same-sex couples (U.S. Census Bureau 2001; but see also O’Connell and Gooding 2006).
In the 2000 U.S. census same-sex couple cohabiting data, self-reported married and self-reported partnered same-sex couples differ in some systematic ways. Not only do the self-reported same-sex married couples have more children than the self-reported same-sex partnered couples, but the self-reported same-sex married couples are more similar to heterosexual married couples along several other key dimensions. For example, the self-reported same-sex married couples are more likely to be white, less likely to be geographically mobile, and more likely to have high incomes (Rosenfeld and Kim 2005). Because the population of same-sex partners in the 2000 census is composed of these two rather distinctive subgroups, every table that includes statistics on same-sex couples and their children includes alternative versions of the same statistics calculated omitting the couples (and their children) whose dual marital status was recoded to indicate whether the results are robust with respect to this underlying diversity.
In the census data, all married couples are heterosexual married couples by Census Bureau definition. Since the 2000 census, however, several U.S. states and other countries have acknowledged married same-sex couples, so I add the modifier “heterosexual” to “married couples” for clarity.
First-Order Predictors of Childhood Grade Retention
Because denominator school populations cover four years (Grades 1–4, Grades 5–8), but the students who can be identified as over-age for their grade come only from the last grade of each four-year span (Grades 4 and 8),5 the implied grade retention rate is four times higher than the observed grade retention rate. Table 1 shows both the observed grade retention rate and the implied grade retention rate for primary school students using weighted data from the 2000 census.
Table 1 suggests that childhood grade retention is correlated with family type. Children of heterosexual married couples had the lowest implied rate of grade retention: 6.8%. Children of lesbian mothers and gay fathers had grade retention rates of 9.5% and 9.7%, respectively. Children of heterosexual cohabiting parents had a grade retention rate of 11.7%, while children of single parents had grade retention rates between 11.1% and 12.6%
The differences in childhood grade retention between all types of non–group quarters households were dwarfed by the high rates of grade retention of children living in group quarters. According to Table 1, children living in group homes, many of them awaiting adoption or foster parents, had an implied grade retention rate of 34.4%. Children who were incarcerated had a grade-retention rate of 78.0%. Later in this article, I show that the enormous difference in grade retention between children raised in families and children living in group quarters remains even after individual-level student disabilities are accounted for.
One way to gauge the advantage of living with families is to note that adopted children (10.6% grade retention) who spent the five years prior to the census living with their adoptive parents and foster children (20.6% grade retention) with five years of residential stability performed considerably better than children who spent the same five years living at a single group-quarters address (34.4% grade retention for noninmates). The performance hierarchy that favors own children, and then (in declining order of school performance) adopted children, foster children, and children in group quarters, confirms the long-standing research finding that children do best when living with parents who make a long-term commitment to the children’s development (Bartholet 1999). Selection bias (wherein the children with the most severe disabilities or children who have suffered the worst abuse are the least likely to be adopted) must also play a role, which unfortunately cannot be quantified with these data.
The rest of Table 1 shows implied grade retention along several other dimensions. Asian American children had the lowest rates of grade retention, and non-Hispanic black children had the highest. Girls were less likely to be held back in the primary grades than boys. Suburban schools had lower rates of grade retention than city schools, which in turn were lower than rural schools. Household SES was a crucial predictor of childhood school performance. In households with income less than $25,000, 12.6% of the primary school students were held back, compared with only 5.3% for children in households with incomes more than $100,000. Householder’s education had an even stronger effect on children’s progress through school: parents who had less than a high school diploma had primary school children who were retained 14.3% of the time, whereas householders with college degrees had children who were retained only 4.4% of the time.
Table 1 shows that the strongest factor in making normal progress through elementary school is living with a family rather than living in group quarters. For children living in a family, whether the family is headed by a heterosexual married couple or by some less-traditional parenting arrangement, the second–most-important factor in childhood progress through school appears to be parental educational attainment.
Consistency With Prior Findings
Although the U.S. census data have some limits for the purpose of studying grade retention, the first-order predictors of grade retention from the census are reassuringly consistent with the published research on normal progress through school using other sources. The gender and racial gradients for normal progress through school in Table 1 are similar to the gender and racial gradients found by the Census Bureau in its analysis of progress through school using data from the educational supplement of the October Current Population Surveys (CPS; Heubert and Hauser 1999:147–54; Jamieson, Curry, and Martinez 2001:3, Table A; Shin 2005:7, Table C). Table 1’s gradients of normal progress through school (versus grade retention) by family type (specifically, single parent versus married parents), household income, student gender, and parental education are entirely consistent with the broad existing literature on grade retention from other data sources, including the following: Dawson’s (1991) study using the 1988 National Health Interview Survey, Tillman et al.’s (2006) study using the Panel Study of Income Dynamics and the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health, Moller et al.’s (2006:171) results using the National Educational Longitudinal Study, Bianchi’s (1984) analysis of the CPS, and Zill’s (1996) analysis of the National Household Education Survey. I show (in an extended analysis available on request) that the multivariate analysis of grade retention from this same literature is consistent with my multivariate analysis of grade retention using U.S. census data. In all the studies, family SES plays a crucial role in shaping children’s educational experience.
Socioeconomic Status by Family Type
Table 2 shows that educational attainment for gays and lesbians was higher than average at 13.6 years (i.e., 1.6 years of college) compared with 13.4 years for heterosexual married heads of household. Across family types, gay couples had the highest median household income at $61,000 per household. It should also be noted that men have higher earnings than women, and gay male couples are the only household type that relied on the earnings of two men. The second four family types are all single-parent (i.e., single-income) families, so their household incomes were roughly half as high as the household incomes of the first four family types.
Table 2.
Variable | Number of Households | Head of Household’s Mean Education | Median Household Income (in 1999 $) | Head of Household’s Mean Age | % of Households That Have Children | % of Children Who Were Adopted, Step-, or Foster Children | % of Children Who Were Black or Hispanic |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
All Households | |||||||
Heterosexual, married | 55,477,124 | 13.4 | 57,640 | 48.7 | 44.8 | ||
Heterosexual, cohabiting | 4,566,338 | 13.0 | 44,200 | 36.0 | 37.2 | ||
Gay male, cohabiting | 331,747 | 13.6 | 61,000 | 44.6 | 22.7 | ||
Lesbian, cohabiting | 328,406 | 13.6 | 55,000 | 42.8 | 35.4 | ||
Separated, divorced, widowed men | 9,071,563 | 12.7 | 30,500 | 54.3 | 10.7 | ||
Separated, divorced, widowed women | 20,626,824 | 12.4 | 22,200 | 59.6 | 20.3 | ||
Never-married men | 7,456,114 | 13.7 | 30,500 | 36.7 | 2.6 | ||
Never-married women | 7,700,852 | 13.7 | 24,500 | 37.3 | 26.0 | ||
Households With Children | |||||||
Heterosexual, married | 24,862,111 | 13.4 | 58,000 | 39.0 | 8.9 | 22.9 | |
Heterosexual, cohabiting | 1,699,954 | 12.0 | 36,600 | 33.2 | 11.4 | 44.5 | |
Gay male, cohabiting | 75,414 | 12.2 | 50,000 | 37.6 | 10.2 | 41.6 | |
Lesbian, cohabiting | 116,329 | 12.8 | 47,000 | 37.0 | 12.4 | 37.1 | |
Separated, divorced, widowed men | 973,714 | 13.0 | 37,000 | 41.5 | 5.2 | 24.6 | |
Separated, divorced, widowed women | 4,180,122 | 12.8 | 23,000 | 38.0 | 3.4 | 40.3 | |
Never-married men | 191,988 | 11.9 | 28,600 | 34.3 | 9.8 | 54.9 | |
Never-married women | 2,002,598 | 12.1 | 14,000 | 31.4 | 2.6 | 75.1 |
Notes: In families with children, head of household’s mean education, median household income, and head of household’s mean age are weighted by number of children. Children include own children, adopted children, stepchildren, and foster children. With the omission of couples whose dual marital status was recoded, 11% of gay couples and 26.6% of lesbian couples have children; median household income is $42,000 for gay fathers and $43,350 for lesbian mothers. For children of gay fathers, 13.7% are adopted, step-, or foster children, and 53.7% are black or Hispanic; the corresponding figures for children of lesbian mothers are 18% and 42.0%.
Source: Census 2000 microdata, via IPUMS.
Despite the fact that the cost of becoming parents may be higher for gays and lesbians than for heterosexual couples, Table 2 shows that gay and lesbian couples who did have children had substantially lower income and educational attainment than gay and lesbian couples in general. Although gay and lesbian cohabiters had relatively high household incomes, gay and lesbian parents had lower SES than heterosexual married parents ($50,000 per household for gay parents compared with $58,000 for heterosexual married parents). Excluding recodes for dual marital status, the income and educational level of gay and lesbian parents was even lower. Among gay and lesbian couples, those with lower incomes are more likely to be raising children.
Not only were heterosexual married parents economically advantaged, but the heterosexual married couples were also racially/ethnically advantaged. Only 22.9% of children of heterosexual married couples were black or Hispanic, whereas 41.6% of children of gay men were black or Hispanic, and this percentage rose to 53.7% when recodes for dual marital status were excluded. The children of lesbians were similarly likely (37.1%) to be black or Hispanic. Never-married mothers were the most likely parenting family type to have black or Hispanic children. The racial/ethnic breakdown of parents was similar to the racial breakdown of children described in Table 2. Among heterosexual married heads of household, 22.2% were black or Hispanic, while 40.4% of gay fathers and 36.1% of lesbian mothers were black or Hispanic (not shown in Table 2).6
Among all family types, children of lesbian mothers were the most likely (more than 12%) to be adopted children, stepchildren, or foster children. Because economic disadvantage, minority racial/ethnic status, and experience with the adoption or foster care system are all challenges for children, a careful analysis of the school performance of children of gay and lesbian parents must take these disadvantages into account.
MULTIVARIATE ANALYSES
Comparisons With Children of Heterosexual Married Couples
Table 3 presents a series of multivariate logistic regression coefficients (for normal progress through primary school versus grade retention), of the following type:
Table 3.
Variable | Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | Model 4 | Model 5 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Family Type (heterosexual, married) | |||||
Same-sex couple cohabiting | −0.267* | −0.112 | −0.116 | −0.128 | −0.142 |
Separated, divorced, widowed women | −0.529*** | −0.422*** | −0.321*** | −0.312*** | −0.311*** |
Heterosexual, cohabiting | −0.547*** | −0.271*** | −0.202*** | −0.207*** | −0.236*** |
Separated, divorced, widowed men | −0.558*** | −0.467*** | −0.427*** | −0.402*** | −0.397*** |
Never-married women | −0.623*** | −0.319*** | −0.088* | −0.116** | −0.145*** |
Never-married men | −0.675*** | −0.400*** | −0.274* | −0.308* | −0.354** |
Ln of Household Income | 0.035*** | 0.026*** | 0.021*** | 0.013* | |
Head of Household’s Education (less than high school) | |||||
High school diploma | 0.507*** | 0.518*** | 0.515*** | 0.511*** | |
Some college | 0.828*** | 0.839*** | 0.804*** | 0.767*** | |
College degree+ | 1.118*** | 1.105*** | 1.026*** | 0.988*** | |
Child Is U.S.-Born | 0.622*** | 0.599*** | 0.629*** | ||
Child’s Gender: Female | 0.283*** | 0.282*** | 0.280*** | ||
Child Has Disability | |||||
Difficulty with memory | −0.911*** | −0.899*** | −0.893*** | ||
Physical disability | −0.376*** | −0.363*** | −0.362*** | ||
Hearing or vision disability | −0.401*** | −0.394*** | −0.377*** | ||
Personal care limitation | −0.207** | −0.235*** | −0.244*** | ||
Child’s Race/Ethnicity (non-Hispanic white) | |||||
Non-Hispanic black | −0.342*** | −0.371*** | −0.270*** | ||
Hispanic | 0.181*** | 0.111*** | 0.009 | ||
Asian American | 0.275*** | 0.199*** | 0.006 | ||
Metropolitan Status (rural) | |||||
City | 0.301*** | 0.152*** | |||
Suburbs | 0.377*** | 0.235*** | |||
Grade Attending: Grades 5–8 | −0.248*** | −0.248*** | |||
School Type: Private School | 0.154*** | 0.172*** | |||
State Dummy Variables | Yes | ||||
Constant | 4.094*** | 3.092*** | 2.524*** | 2.532*** | 2.723*** |
Unweighted N | 716,764 | 716,764 | 716,764 | 716,740 | 716,740 |
df | 7 | 11 | 21 | 27 | 77 |
Log-Likelihood | −65,352 | −64,284 | −63,315 | −63,080 | −62,561 |
Notes: Comparison categories are in parentheses. Some categories of metro status and race/ethnicity are excluded for clarity. With the omission of children of couples whose dual marital status was recoded, and combining gay and lesbian couples, coefficients for children of same-sex couples would be −0.691*, −0.452, −0.361, −0.392;, and −0.427 in Models 1–5, respectively (statistically significant only in Model 1).
Source: Census 2000 microdata, via IPUMS.
p < .05;
p < .01;
p < .001
where Pi is the predicted probability that the ith primary school student was making normal progress through school.7 The constant term is α, and βk represents a column of k coefficients in the model.8 Positive coefficients imply better outcomes (i.e., higher probability of making normal progress through school) for the students. Negative coefficients imply higher rates of grade retention. The sample in Table 3 includes only “own children” (excluding stepchildren, foster children, and adopted children) to reduce the number of children who are the result of previous relationships and to minimize the potential selection bias that could result from the nonrandom way in which children become available for adoption or fostering (Stacey 2006). This narrowing of the sample of children, along with the inclusion of household income and parental education (in Models 2–5), excludes children living in group quarters (by far, the worst performers in school) from the analysis. In a later section of this article, I present an analysis that includes the group-quarters children.
Model 1 of Table 3 shows the raw log odds ratios of normal progress through school for children of all less-traditional family types compared with heterosexual married couples. Similar to the result from Table 1, Model 1 shows that children from all nontraditional family types were less likely to be making good progress through school, with coefficients varying from −0.267 for children of same-sex couples (the most modest disadvantage among the nontraditional family types) to −0.675 for children living with never-married men.
Model 2 introduces controls for household SES, including the natural logarithm of household income, and a categorical variable for the head of household’s educational attainment. The presence of these controls for household SES reduces the magnitude of the negative coefficients for children of all types of nontraditional families compared with Model 1. For children of same-sex parents, the introduction of household SES in Model 2 reduces the grade retention gap (compared with children of heterosexual married couples) by 58% (from −0.267 in Model 1 to −0.112 in Model 2), which renders the contrast with children of heterosexual married couples statistically insignificant in Model 2 and all subsequent models.
Model 3 introduces student gender (girls were more likely to be making good progress), U.S. nativity, student race/ethnicity, and four dichotomous measures for disabilities among the students. The most influential type of disability was memory deficits, which reduced the odds of making good progress through school by more than half (e−0.911 = 0.40).
Model 4 adds controls for urban, rural, or suburban residence; for grade attending; and for private school versus public school. Model 5 adds dummy variables for the 50 U.S. states (plus the District of Columbia) to account for differences in social promotion policy between states. The introduction of new controls in Models 3–5 reaffirms the core finding that the disadvantage in progress through school of children raised by same-sex couples (compared with children of heterosexual married couples) is statistically insignificant when general predictors of school progress are included in the models.
Table 3 does not nullify the importance of family structure. Family structure remains a significant predictor of childhood progress through school, even after every available sociodemographic control is applied, as other scholars have found with other outcomes and other data sets (McLanahan and Sandefur 1994). What Table 3 does show, however, is that the gap in one particular children’s outcome (normal progress through school) is small between family types, and that the apparent disadvantage of children of same-sex couples (compared with children of heterosexual married couples) is especially small. After parental SES is accounted for, the disadvantage of children of same-sex couples (when compared with children of the most advantaged family group) is too small to be statistically significant.
When examining small minority populations, such as children raised by same-sex couples, sample size and statistical power are fundamental limitations of empirical research. The relatively large sample size of children raised by same-sex couples is the fundamental advantage of the census data in this case, but the question remains, Is the sample size large enough to provide sufficient statistical power? There are several ways of addressing the question of statistical power.9 One approach is to randomly reduce the sample of children from all the other family types to 3,174 children (the sample size of children of same-sex couples used in Table 3) to determine whether other relationships in the data remain significant (Cheng and Powell 2005). These results (available upon request) show, reassuringly, that the sample size of 3,174 is large enough to retain most of the important statistically significant predictors of grade retention in the data. The difference between children raised by same-sex couples and children raised by heterosexual married couples in making normal progress through school remains statistically insignificant.
It is also useful to compare the smaller group (children of same-sex couples) with the part of the larger group (other children) to which they are most similar. The process of finding and comparing the most similar subpopulations is known as propensity score matching (Lechner 2002; Lundquist 2004; Rubin 1979). In the first stage, children of same-sex couples are matched to the most similar children of heterosexual married parents via probit regression on a set of covariates, with ties randomly resolved. The resulting two groups, with equal sample sizes, are compared on grade retention by means of a two-sample t test. The t tests are nonparametric because no assumptions are made about how the control variables affect grade retention, and whether the shape of those effects might be different across family types.10
Model 1 of Table 4 matches the samples without using any covariates—that is, the sample of children raised by heterosexual married parents is a random sample— reflecting the unadjusted means of both groups. As expected, the unadjusted means show that own children of heterosexual married couples are significantly less likely to be left back in school than own children of same-sex couples. After the samples are matched on household income and householder educational attainment (Model 2), children of same-sex couples actually are less likely than their most similar peers among children of heterosexual married couples to be held back in school (9.07% compared with 9.45%), although the difference is not statistically significant. Introduction of additional controls into the matching process in Models 3–5 results in no significant differences in grade retention between children of same-sex couples and children of heterosexual married couples. The comparisons after propensity score matching, reported in Table 4, are entirely consistent with the large sample regressions reported in Table 3. After parental income and education are taken into account, the differences between children of same-sex couples and children of heterosexual married couples are small enough to be indistinguishable from zero.
Table 4.
Variable | Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | Model 4 | Model 5 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Controls | None | Household income, head of household’s education | Model 2 plus child is U.S.-born, child’s gender, child has disability, child’s race/ethnicity | Model 3 plus metro status, grade, school type | Model 4 plus state |
Implied Grade Retention Rate (%) | |||||
Children of same-sex couples | 9.07 | 9.07 | 9.07 | 9.07 | 9.07 |
Children of heterosexual married couples | 6.81 | 9.45 | 8.95 | 8.57 | 7.94 |
Difference in Implied Grade | |||||
Retention Rate | −2.27** | 0.38 | −0.12 | −0.50 | −1.13 |
Notes: 3 e propensity-score–matched comparison set varies across models. Actual measured grade retention is one-fourth as large as implied grade retention; see the text. With the omission of children of couples whose dual marital status was recoded, N = 412, and implied grade retention rate difference would be −5.82**, −1.94, −2.92, 1.94, and −1.94 in Models 1–5, respectively (significantly different only in Model 1).
Source: Census 2000 microdata, via IPUMS.
p < .05;
p < .01;
p < .001
Comparisons With Children of Unmarried Heterosexual Couples
Table 5 revisits the regressions from Table 3 (with the same models, covariates, and summary statistics), comparing children raised by same-sex couples with children raised by heterosexual cohabiting couples. Table 5 shows that children raised by same-sex couples are more likely to make normal progress through school than children raised by heterosexual cohabiting couples, but the difference is statistically significant only in Model 1, before parental SES has been accounted for. If children living with dual marital status–recoded couples are excluded, the signs are reversed (meaning that children raised by heterosexual cohabiting couples fare better), but none of the coefficients are statistically significant. These results suggest that for the outcome of normal progress through school, children raised by same-sex cohabiting couples are no different, and perhaps slightly advantaged, compared with children raised by heterosexual cohabiting couples. The similarity in school performance between children of same-sex couples and children of heterosexual cohabiting couples fails to support the gender essentialist theories of parenting, which argue that child development depends on having parental role models from both gender groups (Alvaré 2005; Popenoe 1996; Wardle 1997).
Table 5.
Family Type | Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | Model 4 | Model 5 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Heterosexual Couple, Cohabiting (ref.) | |||||
Same-Sex Couple, Cohabiting | 0.281* | 0.159 | 0.085 | 0.079 | 0.094 |
Notes: Models, covariates, and summary statistics are identical to those in Table 3. Sample size is the same as in Table 3: 716,764 in Models 1–3, and 716,740 in Models 4 and 5. With the omission of children of couples whose dual marital status was recoded, the coefficients are reversed in sign but remain insignificant: −0.138, −0.175, −0.155, −0.181, and −0.186 for Models 1–5, respectively.
Source: Census 2000 microdata, via IPUMS.
p < .05
Comparisons With Children in Group Quarters
Table 6 represents a different variation on the type of analysis from Tables 3–5. In Table 6, the sample of children includes children in group quarters, and these children are the comparison category for the analysis. Because neither household income nor parental education can be associated with children in group quarters, these variables are dropped from the analysis. The sample of children in Table 6 includes own children, adopted children, stepchildren, children living in group quarters, and foster children. Because the children in group quarters have no head of household to have a relationship with, it seems appropriate to use the broadest definition of “children” for children who were living with families. Furthermore, the adopted and foster children probably include some children who formerly lived in group quarters.
Table 6.
Variable | Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | Model 4 |
---|---|---|---|---|
Family Type (group quarters, noninmates) | ||||
Same-sex couple, cohabiting | 1.321*** | 0.886*** | 0.888*** | 0.988*** |
Heterosexual, married | 1.676*** | 1.191*** | 1.194*** | 1.302*** |
Separated, divorced, widowed women | 1.181*** | 0.851*** | 0.880*** | 0.998*** |
Heterosexual, cohabiting | 1.141*** | 0.810*** | 0.844*** | 0.932*** |
Separated, divorced, widowed men | 1.142*** | 0.698*** | 0.746*** | 0.864*** |
Never-married women | 1.115*** | 0.983*** | 0.991*** | 1.088*** |
Never-married men | 1.058*** | 0.785*** | 0.785*** | 0.851*** |
Group quarters (inmates) | −0.948*** | −0.801*** | −0.732** | −0.657*** |
Child Is U.S.-born | 0.675*** | 0.654*** | 0.678*** | |
Child’s Gender: Female | 0.278*** | 0.276*** | 0.275*** | |
Child Has Disability | ||||
Difficulty with memory | −0.944*** | −0.929*** | −0.922*** | |
Physical disability | −0.407*** | −0.388*** | −0.385*** | |
Hearing or vision disability | −0.403*** | −0.384*** | −0.364*** | |
Personal care limitation | −0.187*** | −0.218*** | −0.231*** | |
Child’s Race/Ethnicity (non-Hispanic white) | ||||
Non-Hispanic black | −0.512*** | −0.524*** | −0.398*** | |
Hispanic | −0.175*** | −0.221*** | −0.318*** | |
Asian American | 0.240*** | 0.151** | −0.032 | |
Metropolitan Status (rural) | ||||
City | 0.376*** | 0.218*** | ||
Suburbs | 0.531*** | 0.372*** | ||
Grade Attending: Grades 5–8 | −0.245*** | −0.245*** | ||
School Type: Private School | 0.271*** | 0.285*** | ||
State Dummy Variables | Yes | |||
Constant | 2.365*** | 2.219*** | 2.305*** | 1.716*** |
Unweighted N | 764,781 | 764,781 | 764,757 | 764,757 |
df | 8 | 18 | 24 | 74 |
Log-Likelihood | −72,171 | −70,885 | −70,454 | −69,788 |
Notes: Comparison categories are in parentheses. The sample includes own children, stepchildren, adopted children, foster children, and children living in group quarters. All children and all present parents have five years of residential stability. Some categories of metro status and race/ethnicity are excluded for clarity. With the exclusion of children of couples whose dual marital status was recoded, coefficients for children of same-sex couples would be 0.895**, 0.595*, 0.595*, and 0.661* for Models 1–4, respectively.
Source: Census 2000 microdata, via IPUMS.
p < .01;
p < .001
Table 6 confirms the robustness of a previous finding, from Table 1, that children who live with parents, regardless of family type, are much more likely to make normal progress through school than children living in group quarters. Even after student disabilities (more common among group quarters children than among children living with families) are taken into account, the difference remains between children raised by families and children living in group quarters. Children living at least five years with same-sex couples and children living at least five years with heterosexual cohabiting couples have odds of making good progress through school that are twice as high as noninmate children who spent the previous five years in group quarters. Based on coefficients from Model 2 of Table 6, which controls for children’s race/ethnicity and disabilities, children raised by same-sex couples have odds of making good progress through school that are 2.43 times higher than children living in group quarters (e0.886 = 2.43). Children raised by heterosexual cohabiting couples are similarly advantaged compared with children in group quarters (e0.810 = 2.25). The advantage of children raised by same-sex couples over children living in group quarters remains positive and statistically significant across all four models even after children of those whose marital status was recoded are excluded.
DISCUSSION
Children raised by same-sex couples are one of the most difficult populations in the United States to study systematically because of their small numbers and their geographic dispersion. Census data are far from ideal, and better data would, of course, be welcome. However, currently, the U.S. census is the only nationally representative data set with a large enough sample of children raised by same-sex couples to allow for statistically powerful comparisons with children of other family types.
To the extent that normal progress through primary school is a useful and valid measure of child development, the results confirm that children of same-sex couples appear to have no inherent developmental disadvantage. Heterosexual married couples are the most economically prosperous, the most likely to be white, and the most legally advantaged type of parents; their children have the lowest rates of grade retention. Parental SES accounts for more than one-half of the relatively small gap in grade retention between children of heterosexual married couples and children of same-sex couples. When one controls for parental SES and characteristics of the students, children of same-sex couples cannot be distinguished with statistical certainty from children of heterosexual married couples.
Children not living in group quarters, including children in households headed by same-sex couples, are dramatically more likely to make normal progress through school than students living in group quarters. Any policy that would deny gay and lesbian parents the right to adopt or foster children would force some children to remain in group quarters. A longer stay in group quarters would seem to be contrary to the best interest of the children. In recent years, scholars have arrived at a consensus that moving children out of group homes and into adoptive families should be the goal of public policy. Families, even suboptimal families, are better equipped than the state to raise children (Bartholet 1999; Goldstein, Freud, and Solnit 1979).
Historical restrictions against interracial adoption in the United States represent one relevant historical precedent for the current debate over the adoption rights of same-sex couples. Randall Kennedy (2003) argued that even though restrictions against interracial adoption have been proposed as a way of protecting children, such restrictions have victimized children by taking them away from loving homes or by forcing children to remain in group quarters for too long. Policies limiting the kinds of families that can adopt or foster children ignore the enormous advantages of personal attention that families have (even single parents and other nontraditional family types) over the state in raising children well.
The prior literature has found no evidence that children raised by same-sex couples suffer any important disadvantages (Chan, Raboy, and Patterson 1998; Patterson 1995; Stacey and Biblarz 2001; Wald 2006), yet this same literature has been heavily criticized on the methodological grounds that universally small sample sizes prevent the studies from having the statistical power to identify differences that might actually exist (Alvaré 2005; Lerner and Nagai 2001; Nock 2001). The analysis in this article, the first to use large-sample nationally representative data, shows that children raised by same-sex couples have no fundamental deficits in making normal progress through school. The core finding here offers a measure of validation for the prior, and much-debated, small-sample studies.
Acknowledgments
Thanks are due to Diana Baumrind, Michael Wald, Paula England, Judith Seltzer, Tim Biblarz, and anonymous reviewers for helpful comments.
Footnotes
The four articles featured in Meezan and Rauch (2005) are Wainright, Russell, and Patterson (2004); Golombok et al. (2003); Chan, Raboy, and Patterson (1998); and Brewaeys et al. (1997).
The 2010 census will reportedly not include the long form that was used to produce census microdata in the past (U.S. Census Bureau 2002). The American Community Surveys (ACS), which are supposed to replace the census long form, have not included the five-year mobility question that I use in this article to determine family stability.
Later educational milestones, such as college attendance, cannot be used in this analysis because college students do not generally live with (and therefore cannot be associated with) their parents in a household survey such as the U.S. census. Even secondary school students (especially students who are over-age for their grade) are old enough to live apart from their parents, which is why my analysis focuses on the primary grades.
Children ready to start kindergarten as young 5-year-olds are sometimes held back from school for one year by affluent parents so that their child might be one of the oldest students when they start kindergarten at age 6 rather than one of the youngest students in the class, a process known as “redshirting.” The late age cutoff that I use to define grade retention—11 years old for 4th grade (and, correspondingly, 7 years old for kindergarten)—places the redshirted children among those making normal progress.
This assumes that no children are held back more than one year in the primary grades, which is a fairly safe assumption based on October, CPS data.
Rosenfeld and Kim (2005) showed that same-sex couples were also more likely than heterosexual couples to be interracial.
Because of the four-year categories for current grade, only one-fourth of the actual cases of grade retention are identified in the data set. In the data, the category “over-age” includes only students who were demonstrably over-age, but the category “making normal progress” is a mixed category whose components include 91%–95% (depending on the grade retention rate by family type) of students who truly were making normal progress through school, and the remainder are students who were over-age but are misclassified as making normal progress because of the four-year-wide categories in the census question GRADEATT.
Coefficients are based on weighted census microdata, with weights renormed to average 1, so that model likelihoods and coefficient standard errors reflect the real unweighted sample size. In general, children of same-sex cohabiting parents appear to do slightly better in these multivariate tests when the models are run without taking the census weights into account.
If the odds of making normal progress through school were twice as high for children of heterosexual married couples as for children of same-sex couples, the existing census sample from Table 3 would provide a power of 0.999 (near certainty) to reject the null hypothesis of no difference. None of the family types in any of the models of Table 3 had a disadvantage this large. If the odds of grade retention were 1.5 times higher for children of heterosexual married parents than for children of same-sex couples, the power to detect the difference would be lower, but still substantial at 0.83. Children raised by divorced single mothers or divorced single fathers had this great a disadvantage, across several models. The main reason that the coefficients for children raised by same sex couples were not significantly different from zero in Models 2–5 of Table 3 is that their estimated disadvantage was relatively small. Power calculations assume alpha of .05, two-tailed tests, and Bernoulli distributions (for normal progress versus grade retention) with variance of p(1 – p).
In fact, the effect of parental education on children’s grade retention does differ across family types. Among same-sex couples, parental education has little effect on children’s grade retention; among heterosexual married couples, there is a strong positive correlation between parental education and children’s normal progress through school. At low parental educational levels, children of same-sex couples make better progress in school than children of heterosexual married couples. Among children raised by highly educated parents, the children of heterosexual married couples make better progress in school than the children of same-sex couples.
REFERENCES
- Alexander KL, Entwisle DR, Dauber SL. On the Success of Failure: A Reassessment of the Effects of Retention in the Primary Grades. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Alexander KL, Entwisle DR, Horsey CS. From First Grade Forward: Early Foundations of High School Dropout. Sociology of Education. 1997;70:87–107. [Google Scholar]
- Alvaré HM. The Turn Toward the Self in the Law of Marriage and Family: Same-Sex Marriage and Its Predecessors. Stanford Law and Policy Review. 2005;16:135–96. [Google Scholar]
- Amato PR, Cheadle J. The Long Reach of Divorce: Divorce and Child Well-being Across Three Generations. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2005;67:191–206. [Google Scholar]
- Ball CA, Pea JF. Warring With Wardle: Morality, Social Science, and Gay and Lesbian Parents. University of Illinois Law Review. 1998;1998:253–339. [Google Scholar]
- Bartholet E. Nobody’s Children: Abuse and Neglect, Foster Drift, and the Adoption Alternative. Boston: Beacon Press; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Baumrind D. Effects of Authoritative Parental Control on Child Behavior. Child Development. 1966;37:887–907. [Google Scholar]
- Bianchi S. Children’s Progress Through School: A Research Note. Sociology of Education. 1984;57:184–92. [Google Scholar]
- Biblarz TJ, Raftery AE. Family Structure, Educational Attainment, and Socioeconomic Success: Rethinking the ‘Pathology of Matriarchy’. American Journal of Sociology. 1999;105:321–65. [Google Scholar]
- Brewaeys A, Ponjaert I, Van Hall EV, Golombok S. Donor Insemination: Child Development and Family Functioning in Lesbian Mother Families. Human Reproduction. 1997;12:1349–59. doi: 10.1093/humrep/12.6.1349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brooks-Gunn J, Markman LB. The Contribution of Parenting to Ethnic and Racial Gaps in School Readiness. The Future of Children. 2005;15:139–68. doi: 10.1353/foc.2005.0001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chan RW, Raboy B, Patterson CJ. Psychosocial Adjustment Among Children Conceived Via Donor Insemination by Lesbian and Heterosexual Mothers. Child Development. 1998;69:443–57. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chase-Lansdale PL, Cherlin AJ, Kiernan KE. The Long-Term Effects of Parental Divorce on the Mental Health of Young Adults: A Developmental Perspective. Child Development. 1995;66:1614–34. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheng S, Powell B. Small Samples, Big Challenges: Studying Atypical Family Forms. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2005;67:926–35. [Google Scholar]
- Cherlin AJ. Marriage, Divorce, Remarriage. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Cherlin AJ. Going to Extremes: Family Structure, Children’s Well-being, and Social Science. Demography. 1999;36:421–28. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dawson DA. Family Structure and Children’s Health and Well-being: Data From the 1988 National Health Interview Survey on Child Health. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1991;53:573–84. [Google Scholar]
- Eskridge WN., Jr . The Case for Same Sex Marriage: From Sexual Liberty to Civilized Commitment. New York: Free Press; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Eskridge WN., Jr . Equality Practice: Civil Unions and the Future of Gay Rights. New York: Routledge; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Flaks DK, Ficher I, Masterpasqua F, Joseph G. Lesbians Choosing Motherhood: A Comparative Study of Lesbian and Heterosexual Parents and Their Children. Developmental Psychology. 1995;31:105–14. [Google Scholar]
- Freud S. Three Essays on the Theory of Sexuality. New York: Basic Books; [1905] 1975. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gennetian LA. One or Two Parents? Half or Step Siblings? The Effect of Family Structure on Young Children’s Achievement. Journal of Population Economics. 2005;18:415–36. [Google Scholar]
- Ginther DK, Pollak RA. Family Structure and Children’s Educational Outcomes: Blended Families, Stylized Facts, and Descriptive Regressions. Demography. 2004;41:671–96. doi: 10.1353/dem.2004.0031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldstein J, Freud A, Solnit AJ. Before the Best Interests of the Child. New York: Free Press; 1979. [Google Scholar]
- Golombok S, Perry B, Burston A, Murray C, Mooney-Somers J, Stevens M, Golding J. Children With Lesbian Parents: A Community Study. Developmental Psychology. 2003;39:20–33. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.39.1.20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guo G, Brooks-Gunn J, Harris KM. Parents’ Labor Force Attachment and Grade Retention Among Urban Black Children. Sociology of Education. 1996;69:217–36. [Google Scholar]
- Hamilton L, Cheng S, Powell B. Adoptive Parents, Adaptive Parents: Evaluating the Importance of Biological Ties for Parental Investment. American Sociological Review. 2007;72:95–116. [Google Scholar]
- Harris JR. The Nurture Assumption: Why Children Turn Out the Way They Do. New York: Touchstone; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Hauser RM. Should We End Social Promotion? Truth and Consequences. In: Orfield G, Kornhaber ML, editors. Raising Standards or Raising Barriers? Inequality and High Stakes Testing in Public Education. New York: Century Foundation; 2001. pp. 151–78. [Google Scholar]
- Hauser RM, Pager DI, Simmons SJ. Race-Ethnicity, Social Background, and Grade Retention: An Analysis of the Last Thirty Years. CEIC Review. 2001;10(5):11–12. [Google Scholar]
- Heubert JP, Hauser RM. High Stakes: Testing for Tracking, Promotion, and Graduation. Washington, DC: National Academies Press; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Jamieson A, Curry A, Martinez G. Current Population Reports. U.S. Census Bureau; Washington, DC: 2001. School Enrollment in the United States—Social and Economic Characteristics of Students: October 1999; pp. P20–533. [Google Scholar]
- Kennedy R. Interracial Intimacies: Sex, Marriage, Identity and Adoption. New York: Pantheon; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Koppelman A. The Gay Rights Question in Contemporary American Law. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Kowalesi-Jones L, Duncan GJ. The Structure of Achievement and Behavior Across Middle Childhood. Child Development. 1999;70:930–43. [Google Scholar]
- Lareau A. Unequal Childhoods: Class, Race, and Family Life. Berkeley: University of California Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Lechner M. Program Heterogeneity and Propensity Score Matching: An Application to the Evaluation of Active Labor Market Policies. The Review of Economics and Statistics. 2002;84:205–20. [Google Scholar]
- Lerner R, Nagai AK. No Basis: What the Studies Don’t Tell Us About Same-Sex Parenting. Washington, DC: Marriage Law Project; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Lundquist JH. When Race Makes No Difference: Marriage and the Military. Social Forces. 2004;83:731–57. [Google Scholar]
- Maccoby EE. Gender and Relationships: A Developmental Account. American Psychologist. 1990;45:513–20. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.45.4.513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayer S. What Money Can’t Buy: Family Income and Children’s Life Chances. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- McLanahan S. Family Structure and the Reproduction of Poverty. American Journal of Sociology. 1985;90:873–901. [Google Scholar]
- McLanahan S, Sandefur G. Growing Up With a Single Parent: What Hurts, What Helps. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Meezan W, Rauch J. Gay Marriage, Same-Sex Parenting, and America’s Children. The Future of Children. 2005;15:97–115. doi: 10.1353/foc.2005.0018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moller S, Stearns E, Blau JR, Land KC. Smooth and Rough Roads to Academic Achievement: Retention and Race/Class Disparities in High School. Social Science Research. 2006;35:157–80. [Google Scholar]
- Nock SL. Affidavit in the Ontario Superior Court in the Case of Halpern et al. v. Attorney General of Canada. 2001. Available online at http://www.marriagelaw.cua.edu/Law/cases/Canada/ontario/halpern/aff_nock.pdf.
- O’Connell Ma, Gooding G. The Use of First Names to Evaluate Reports of Gender and Its Effect on the Distribution of Married and Unmarried Couple Households. Presented at Population Association of America; Los Angeles. March 30–April 1.2006. [Google Scholar]
- Patterson CJ. Families of the Lesbian Baby Boom: Parents’ Division of Labor and Children’s Adjustment. Developmental Psychology. 1995;31:115–23. [Google Scholar]
- Patterson CJ. Lesbian and Gay Parenthood. In: Bornstein MH, editor. Handbook of Parenting. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2002. pp. 317–38. [Google Scholar]
- Pawelski JG, Perrin EC, Foy JM, Allen CE, Crawford JE, Del Monte M, Miriam K, Klein JD, Smith K, Springer S, Tanner JL, Vickers DL. The Effects of Marriage, Civil Union, and Domestic Partnership Laws on the Health and Well-being of Children. Pediatrics. 2006;118:349–64. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-1279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perry B, Burston A, Stevens M, Steele H, Golding J, Golombok S. Children’s Play Narratives: What They Tell Us About Lesbian-Mother Families. American Journal of Ortho-psychiatry. 2004;74:467–79. doi: 10.1037/0002-9432.74.4.467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Popenoe D. Life Without Father: Compelling New Evidence That Fatherhood and Marriage Are Indispensable for the Good of Children and Society. New York: Free Press; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Roderick M. Grade Retention and School Dropout: Investigating the Association. American Educational Research Journal. 1994;31:729–59. [Google Scholar]
- Rosenfeld MJ, Kim B-S. The Independence of Young Adults and the Rise of Interracial and Same-Sex Unions. American Sociological Review. 2005;70:541–62. [Google Scholar]
- Rubin DB. Using Multivariate Matched Sampling and Regression Adjustment to Control Bias in Observational Studies. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 1979;74:318–28. [Google Scholar]
- Ruggles S, Sobek M, Alexander T, Fitch CA, Goeken R, Hall PK, King M, Ronnander C. Integrated Public Use Microdata Series: Version 3.0 [Machine-readable database] Minneapolis, MN: Minnesota Population Center; 2004. Available online at http://www.ipums.org. [Google Scholar]
- Rumberger RW. High School Dropouts: A Review of Issues and Evidence. Review of Educational Research. 1987;57:101–21. [Google Scholar]
- Rutter M. Nature, Nurture, and Development: From Evangelism Through Science Toward Policy and Practice. Child Development. 2002;73:1–21. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shin HB. Current Population Reports. U.S. Census Bureau; Washington, DC: 2005. School Enrollment—Social and Economic Characteristics of Students: October 2003; pp. P20–554. [Google Scholar]
- Stacey J. Gay Parenthood and the Decline of Paternity as We Knew It. Sexualities. 2006;9:27–55. [Google Scholar]
- Stacey J, Biblarz TJ. (How) Does the Sexual Orientation of Parents Matter? American Sociological Review. 2001;66:159–83. [Google Scholar]
- Tasker F. Lesbian Mothers, Gay Fathers, and Their Children: A Review. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 2005;26:224–40. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200506000-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tillman KH, Guo G, Harris KM. Grade Retention Among Immigrant Children. Social Science Research. 2006;35:129–56. [Google Scholar]
- U.S. Census Bureau. Technical Note on Same-Sex Unmarried Partner Data from the 1990 and 2000 Censuses. 2001. Available online at http://www.census.gov/sdc/chap7pums.txt.
- U.S. Census Bureau. 2010 Census Re-Engineering. 2002. Available online at http://www.census.gov/Press-Release/www/2002/2_4_02Waite.html.
- Wainright JL, Patterson CJ. Delinquency, Victimization, and Substance Use Among Adolescents With Female Same-Sex Parents. Journal of Family Psychology. 2006;20:526–30. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.20.3.526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wainright JL. Peer Relations Among Adolescents With Female Same-Sex Parents. Developmental Psychology. 2008;44:117–26. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.1.117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wainright JL, Russell ST, Patterson CJ. Psychosocial Adjustment, School Outcomes, and Romantic Relationships of Adolescents With Same-Sex Parents. Child Development. 2004;75:1886–98. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00823.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waite LJ, Gallagher M. The Case for Marriage: Why Married People Are Happier, Healthier, and Better Off Financially. New York: Doubleday; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Wald M. Adults’ Sexual Orientation and State Determinations Regarding Placement of Children. Family Law Quarterly. 2006;40:385–439. [Google Scholar]
- Wallerstein JS, Kelly JB. Surviving the Breakup: How Children and Parents Cope With Divorce. New York: Basic Books; 1980. [Google Scholar]
- Wallerstein JS, Lewis JM, Blakeslee S. The Unexpected Legacy of Divorce: A 25 Year Landmark Study. New York: Hyperion; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Wardle LD. The Potential Impact of Homosexual Parenting on Children. University of Illinois Law Review. 1997;1997:833–920. [Google Scholar]
- Wilson JQ. The Marriage Problem: How Our Culture Has Weakened Families. New York: Harper Collins; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Wolfe B, Haveman R, Ginther D, An CB. The ‘Window Problem’ in Studies of Children’s Attainments: A Methodological Exploration. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 1996;91:970–82. [Google Scholar]
- Wu LL, Martinson BC. Family Structure and the Risk of a Premarital Birth. American Sociological Review. 1993;58:210–32. [Google Scholar]
- Zill N. Family Change and Student Achievement: What We Have Learned, What It Means for Schools. In: Booth A, Dunn JF, editors. Family-School Links: How Do They Affect Educational Outcomes? Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1996. pp. 39–84. [Google Scholar]