Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2012 Mar 1.
Published in final edited form as: Neuromolecular Med. 2010 Jul 15;13(1):37–43. doi: 10.1007/s12017-010-8126-6

Increased Cerebrospinal Fluid F2-Isoprostanes are Associated with Aging and Latent Alzheimer’s Disease as Identified by Biomarkers

Thomas J Montine 1,, Elaine R Peskind 2,3, Joseph F Quinn 4, Angela M Wilson 1, Kathleen S Montine 1, Douglas Galasko 5
PMCID: PMC3000441  NIHMSID: NIHMS230909  PMID: 20632131

Abstract

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is a common age-related chronic illness with latent, prodrome, and fully symptomatic dementia stages. Increased free radical injury to regions of brain is one feature of prodrome and dementia stages of AD; however, it also is associated with advancing age. This raises the possibility that age-related free radical injury to brain might be caused in part or in full by latent AD. We quantified free radical injury in the central nervous system with cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) F2-isoprostanes (IsoPs) in 421 clinically normal individuals and observed a significant increase over the adult human lifespan (P < 0.001). Using CSF amyloid (A) β42 and tau, we defined normality using results from 28 clinically normal individuals < 50 years old, and then stratified 74 clinically normal subjects ≥ 60 years into those with CSF that had normal CSF Aβ42 and tau (n=37); abnormal CSF Aβ42 and tau, the biomarker signature of AD (n=24); decreased Aβ42 only (n=4); or increased tau only (n=9). Increased CSF F2-IsoPs were present in clinically normal subjects with the biomarker signature of AD (P < 0.05) and those subjects with increased CSF tau (P < 0.001). Finally, we analyzed the relationship between age and CSF F2-IsoPs for those clinically normal adults with normal CSF (n=37) and those with abnormal CSF Aβ42 and/or tau (n=37); only those with normal CSF demonstrated a significant increase with age (P < 0.01). These results show that CSF F2-IsoPs increased across the human lifespan and that this age-related increase in free radical injury to brain persisted after culling those with laboratory evidence of latent AD.

Keywords: Alzheimer’s disease, cerebrospinal fluid, biomarkers, Aβ42, tau, F2-isoprostanes

INTRODUCTION

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is the most common neurodegenerative disease and demonstrates exponential increase in prevalence with advancing age beyond 60 years. Autopsy, neuroimaging, and biomarker studies all consistently support the proposal that AD is a chronic illness that begins years if not decades prior to onset of clinically detectable cognitive impairment (Table 1) (Sonnen et al. 2008b). Thus, like all chronic diseases AD can be conceptualized in three stages: latency when disease has started but is asymptomatic, prodrome when disease has progressed and very mild clinical signs and symptoms are present, and clinical when disease has advanced still further and now the full clinical spectrum is expressed. The prodromal stage of AD most commonly is operationally defined by amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) (Petersen and Negash 2008). The stage of full clinical expression of AD is the most common contributor to the dementia syndrome while vascular brain injury (VBI), especially from small vessel disease or micro VBI (μVBI), is the second most common chronic disease to contribute to dementia, often in combination with AD (Sonnen et al. 2007).

Table 1.

Chronic disease model of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and Vascular Brain Injury (VBI)

Stage of Chronic Disease Terms used in AD or VBI Research Disease Laboratory Evidence* Clinical Signs and Symptoms
No disease None None Normal
Latent Pre-clinical or Pre-symptomatic + + Normal
Prodrome MCI or CIND + to ++ + to ++ + Abnormal
Full clinical expression Dementia ++ to +++ ++ to +++ ++ to +++ Abnormal
*

Biomarkers, neuroimaging, or autopsy. Mild (+), Moderate (++), or Severe (+++).

Abbreviation: Cognitive Impairment Not Dementia (CIND), Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI)

It is critical to realize that latent disease cannot be distinguished from absence of disease by clinical examination or neuropsychological testing, but rather requires some ensemble of laboratory-based methods to detect disease initiation in the absence of symptoms. In the past, autopsy-based comparisons of carefully documented asymptomatic individuals were the only means to investigate latent disease vs. no disease, and Dr. Markesbery was a world leader in this important area of research (Schmitt et al. 2000; Petrovitch et al. 2005; Tyas et al. 2007; Iacono et al. 2009; Markesbery et al. 2009; Price et al. 2009). However, there are clear limitations to this approach — most importantly the focus on older individuals, and the asynchrony between last clinical evaluation and autopsy. More recently, the advent of neuroimaging and biomarker technologies have provided tools that appear to be helpful in distinguishing latent disease from absence of disease in the clinical research setting (Sonnen et al. 2008b); these laboratory tools currently are more advanced for AD than μVBI. Indeed, using decreased CSF Aβ42 and increased CSF tau concentrations, we and others have confirmed that an abnormal CSF tau/Aβ42 ratio indicates increased risk of subsequently developing MCI or AD-type dementia, strongly indicating that these two proteins may have utility as biomarkers of latent AD (Fagan et al. 2007; Li et al. 2007). Decreased soluble CSF Aβ42 is correlated with accumulation of fibrillar Aβ peptides in regions of cerebrum as insoluble aggregates (Fagan et al. 2006), including senile plaques, which arise during the latent stage of AD (Schmitt et al. 2000; Petrovitch et al. 2005; Tyas et al. 2007; Iacono et al. 2009; Price et al. 2009). It is important to stress that elevated CSF tau is not specific to AD but rather appears to derive from neuronal damage from a variety of etiologies, including VBI (Sonnen et al. 2008b).

We and others have pursued increased free radical injury as a mechanism by which AD damages neurons and perhaps other tissue elements in diseased regions of cerebrum, and have validated repeatedly the seminal observations by Dr. Markesbery and colleagues. Free radical-mediated damage to brain in AD and its transgenic mouse models (Smith et al. 1991; Sonnen et al. 2008a). Quantification of F2-Isoprostanes (IsoPs) by a stable isotope dilution technique with gas chromatography/mass spectrometry and selective ion monitoring is a robust quantitative in vivo biomarker of free radical injury to tissue under a variety of biological stressors or diseases (Roberts and Morrow 2000). We adapted this methodology to investigate CSF and showed that dementia stage AD is associated with significantly increased CSF F2-IsoPs (Montine et al. 1998); however, it is important to note that all of our studies observed approximately one-third overlap in CSF F2-IsoPs between patients with dementia stage AD and controls with the remainder of AD patients skewing to higher CSF F2-IsoP concentrations (Montine et al. 2007). We also demonstrated that CSF F2-IsoPs may be used in conjuction with CSF Aβ42 and tau to increase the accuracy of laboratory diagnosis of AD (Montine et al. 2001). Several groups validated our findings, and we and others expanded these observations to include individuals with MCI or some individuals with Clinical Dementia Rating 0.5 (Quinn et al. 2004), again showing overlap with controls, at least in our studies. Several publications report an increase in plasma or urine F2-IsoPs in patients with dementia stage AD or MCI (Pratico et al. 2000; Tuppo et al. 2001; Pratico et al. 2002); however, these results are not reproducible (Montine et al. 2002; Irizarry et al. 2007; Mufson and Leurgans).

Here we undertook the first in vivo investigation of quantitative biomarkers of free radical injury in latent AD.

METHODS

Subjects and CSF samples

Human Subject Divisions at Oregon Health & Science University, the University of California at San Diego, and the University of Washington approved this study. Subjects were compensated community volunteers in good health with no signs or symptoms suggesting cognitive decline or neurologic disease. Exclusion criteria included heavy cigarette smoking (more than 10 packs/year), alcohol use other than social, and any psychotherapeutic use. Following informed consent, all individuals underwent evaluation that consisted of family and medical histories, physical and neurologic examinations, laboratory tests, and neuropsychological assessment. Laboratory evaluations included complete blood count, serum electrolytes, blood urea nitrogen, creatinine, glucose, vitamin B12, and thyroid stimulating hormone; all results were within normal limits for samples included in this study. The neuropsychological battery consisted of a global cognitive measure, the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) (Folstein et al. 1975), memory tests including immediate and delayed (Wechsler 1987; Craft et al. 1996), and parts A and B of the Trail Making Test as a measure of attention (Reitan and Wolfson 1986). All subjects scored in the normal range for all tests.

CSF was obtained by lumbar puncture at all institutions by the same method (Peskind et al. 2005). Briefly, following written informed consent, individuals were placed in the lateral decubitus position and the L4-5 interspace was infiltrated with 1% lidocaine. Lumbar puncture was performed with a 20g or 24g spinal needle following which individuals remained at bed rest for one hour. All lumbar punctures performed in the morning to limit potential circadian fluctuation.

CSF Assays

All CSF for analysis was taken from the 15th to 25th ml collected, was stored in 0.5 ml aliquots at −80°C, and was never thawed prior to this study. CSF was analyzed for Aβ42 and total tau using multiplexed Luminex reagents from InnoGenetics (Alpharetta, GA), according to manufacturer’s instructions and as previously described by us (Montine et al.). CSF F2-IsoPs were quantified using a stable isotope dilution assay with gas chromatography/mass spectrometry and selective ion monitoring as described previously (Milatovic et al. 2005).

Statistical analysis was performed using GraphPad Prism (San Diego, CA).

RESULTS

We determined CSF F2-IsoP concentrations from 412 volunteers who were clinically normal and who performed within normal ranges on a battery of neuropsychological tests (Figure 1). These 412 clinically normal volunteers ranged in age from 21 to 89 years old (mean ± SD = 59 ± 17 years) and had average CSF F2-IsoPs concentration of 29.2 ± 8.4 pg/ml. Despite substantial variance across the adult human lifespan, we observed a statistically significant (P < 0.001) age-related increase in CSF F2-IsoPs that could be reflected in a best-fit line with a positive slope of 0.1 pg F2-IsoPs/ml CSF/yr and intercept of 24.3 ± 0.1 pg F2-IsoPs/ml. It is interesting to note that the CSF F2-IsoPs concentration in this clinically normal cohort was on average 16% greater than our previous clinically normal cohorts that had average CSF F2-IsoPs = 25 pg/ml (Montine et al. 2007), a difference that may be related to variation in inclusion or exclusion criteria. In summary, in this largest group of clinically normal individuals published to date, our results show a positive relationship between advancing adult age and increasing CSF F2-IsoPs but with substantial variance in CSF F2-IsoPs across all ages.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Clinically normal adults who had normal scores on a battery of neuropsychological tests volunteered for lumbar puncture (n=412). Data are cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) F2-isoprostanes (IsoPs) for each subject plotted against age. Regression yielded a statistically significant line (P < 0.001) plotted with 95% confidence intervals.

We sought to determine the extent to which this age-related increase in CSF F2-IsoPs was driven by latent AD by using CSF Aβ42 and tau as biomarkers. Of the 412 clinically normal individuals described above, we selected 102 subjects that also had CSF Aβ42 and tau concentrations quantified. Of these, 28 individuals were < 50 years old. Similar to our previous studies, we assumed these 28 clinically normal younger individuals to be very unlikely to harbor latent AD, and used their laboratory results to define normal cutoff values of Aβ42, tau, and the tau/Aβ42 ratio (Table 2) (Li et al. 2007). Since Aβ42 concentrations decrease in AD, the normal cutoff for Aβ42 was the mean for these younger controls minus 2 SD. Similarly, since tau and tau/Aβ42 ratio increase in AD, normal cutoff for these were the mean of younger controls plus 2 SD. Our normal cutoff values compare well with those recently reported by the AD Neuroimaging Initiative.

Table 2.

Clinically Normal Individuals Younger than 50 Years-Old (n=28).

Age (yr) CSF Aβ42 (ng/ml) CSF Tau (ng/ml) CSF Tau/Aβ42 (ng/ml)
Minimum 25 200.0 32.0 0.13
Maximum 49 362.4 85.6 0.26
Mean 34 279.9 54.2 0.19
SD 8 47.0 14.3 0.04
Normal --- > 185.9 < 82.8 < 0.27

We next applied these normal cutoff values to all subjects who were 60 years or older (n=74). We stratified our clinically normal older subjects into four groups based upon CSF Aβ42, tau, and tau/Aβ42 results: (i) Normal CSF who had normal Aβ42 and tau concentrations and a normal tau/Aβ42 ratio (n=37), (ii) Abnormal CSF Aβ42 & Tau who had low Aβ42 and high Tau concentrations (n=12), or a high tau/Aβ42 ratio even though concentrations of both analytes were in normal ranges (n=12), (iii) Low CSF Aβ42 Only who had low Aβ42 and normal tau concentrations (n=4), or (iv) High CSF Tau Only who had normal Aβ42 and high tau concentrations (n=9) (Figure 2). This distribution showed that 50% (n=37) of clinically normal adults ≥ 60 years of age had no evidence of latent disease as determined by CSF Aβ42 and tau measurements (blue). Thirty-eight percent of clinically normal controls ≥ 60 years-old had laboratory evidence of latent AD as defined in the Abnormal Aβ42 & Tau (red) or low CSF Aβ42 Only (green) groups. Finally, 12% of clinically normal older individuals had abnormally high CSF tau with normal CSF Aβ42 concentration (purple). Given the lack of specificity of CSF tau, it is not clear if members of this last group have latent AD, latent VBI, or some other latent disease of brain.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Data from all clinically normal subjects with cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) CSF Aβ42 and tau determinations (n=28) were used to define normal cutoff values that were than applied to all clinically normal subjects 60 years or older (n=74). This pie chart shows the stratification of these 74 older controls based on results of CSF Aβ42 and tau levels: (i) Normal CSF (n=37), (ii) Abnormal CSF Aβ42 & Tau (n=24), (iii) Low CSF Aβ42 Only (n=4), or (iv) High CSF Tau Only (n=9).

We then compared CSF F2-IsoPs among these four groups of clinically normal individuals who were defined by CSF Aβ42 and tau concentrations (Figure 3). Of note, the Normal CSF group (n=37) had CSF F2-IsoPs that averaged (±SD) 25.6 ± 0.7 pg/ml, identical to our previously reported value for normal CSF F2-IsoPs in older adults. Analysis of variance for CSF F2-IsoPs among these four groups had P < 0.001 and Bonferroni’s corrected multiple comparison test had P < 0.05 for Normal CSF vs. Abnormal Aβ42 & Tau, and P < 0.001 for Normal CSF vs. High CSF Tau Only. Although the number of samples is small, especially in the Low CSF Aβ42 Only group, these data indicate that CSF F2-IsoPs are increased in clinically normal individuals who show either absolute or relatively low CSF Aβ42 and high tau concentrations, and are even further increased in those who display only high CSF tau. Like the dementia stage of AD, these data are consistent with the interpretation that CSF F2-IsoPs are increased in some individuals with latent AD, a finding that will require validation in longitudinal studies and with different methods.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Concentrations of cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) F2-isoprostanes (IsoPs) among 74 clinically normal subjects aged 60 years or more were stratified by CSF Aβ42 and tau results into four groups (P < 0.001). Bonferroni’s corrected multiple comparison test had P < 0.05 for Normal CSF vs. Abnormal Aβ42 & Tau, and P < 0.001 for Normal CSF vs. High CSF Tau Only.

We returned to our assessment of age-related increase in CSF F2-IsoPs but this time stratified by presence or absence of Aβ42 and/or tau abnormalities. We divided the individuals shown in Figure 2 into two groups: the Normal CSF group (n=37; blue) and all other individuals with abnormalities of CSF Aβ42 and/or tau concentrations (n = 37; red, green, purple) and performed linear regression for age vs. CSF F2-IsoPs in each group. Clinically normal individuals 60 years or older with normal CSF Aβ42 and tau had a significantly positive slope for CSF F2-IsoPs vs. age (Figure 4). In contrast, clinically normal individuals with abnormal CSF Aβ42 and/or tau concentrations or an {Montine, 1999 #35}abnormal CSF tau/Aβ42 ratio did not have a significant correlation between age and CSF F2-IsoPs. We interpret these data as evidence that advancing age is associated with progressive free radical injury to brain even in the absence of disease(s) that cause abnormalities in CSF Aβ42 and/or tau. In contrast, disease(s) that cause abnormalities in CSF Aβ42 or tau concentrations or ratio are associated with increased free radical injury independent of age.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Concentrations of cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) F2-isoprostanes (IsoPs) among 37 clinically normal subjects aged 60 years or more with normal CSF Aβ42 and tau concentrations were analyzed by linear regression (P < 0.01). Best-fit line and 95% confidence intervals are plotted.

DISCUSSION

Free radical injury to diseased regions of cerebrum has been identified in autopsy samples from patients who died with AD or MCI (Smith et al. 1991; Lovell and Markesbery 2007), and, at least for measurements like F2-IsoPs and related isoprostanoids, these assessments are not confounded by agonal events (Sonnen et al. 2009). Although important to our understanding of disease mechanism, autopsy-based studies are limited in assessing changes that occur early in the course of disease and cannot be used in longitudinal studies of the natural history of disease or response to therapeutics. It is for these reasons that in collaboration with Dr. Markesbery we developed CSF F2-IsoPs as a means to quantify free radical injury to brain intra vitam (Montine et al. 1998; Montine et al. 1999). Several groups around the world have adopted this approach using different assays (Sonnen et al. 2008a). All have verified that the dementia stage of AD is associated with statistically significantly increased CSF F2-IsoPs, although there is substantial overlap with controls in most studies. Some have reported similar findings in CSF from patients with very mild dementia (CDR 0.5) or individuals with MCI. For the first time, here we extend these investigations to latent stage AD (Montine et al. 2005).

There is as yet no widely accepted means of detecting latent AD, although two promising research tools are neuroimaging for fibrillar Aβ peptides and quantification of CSF Aβ42 and tau (Fagan et al. 2007; Li et al. 2007; Rentz et al.). We wish to stress that although we caricature chronic disease progression as an orderly transition from latent to prodome to full expression, there is no requirement that this be so; indeed, medicine is replete with examples of patients with chronic disease that do not follow an orderly progression and others that progress only partially. Undoubtedly, the same will be true for AD and as we develop the tools necessary to probe disease mechanisms in vivo, we almost certainly will uncover some latent disease that does and some that does not progress to clinical expression. Clarity for these important points will come from longitudinal studies. Thus, although currently limited, here we used CSF Aβ42 and tau concentrations to define latent disease that at least indicates increased risk for subsequent MCI or AD.

Our results showed that 50% of individuals 60 years or older had normal CSF Aβ42 and tau, 33% had abnormal CSF Aβ42 and tau, 5% had low CSF Aβ42 only, and 12% had high CSF tau only. CSF F2-IsoPs concentration among those with normal CSF Aβ42 and tau were identical to control values reported by us in several previous publications, and very closely approximated the y-intercept for CSF F2-IsoPs vs. age across the human lifespan. This prevalence for abnormal CSF Aβ42 and tau approximated the proportion of individuals is this age range that would be expected ultimately to develop MCI or dementia from AD, and is remarkably consistent with PET imaging for fibrillar Aβ peptide accumulation in brain among cognitively normal older adults (Buckner et al. 2005; Mintun et al. 2006; Aizenstein et al. 2008). Consonant with many other studies of animal models and human tissue from patients with MCI or dementia from AD (Sonnen et al. 2008a), we observed significantly increased CSF F2-IsoPs in those clinically normal individuals ≥ 60 years-old who had laboratory evidence of latent AD: abnormal CSF Aβ42 and tau. As in our earlier studies of symptomatic subjects, there was considerable overlap in CSF F2-IsoP concentrations between those with normal and abnormal CSF Aβ42 and tau concentrations. These results suggest that, like prodrome and dementia stages of AD, increased free radical injury is a feature of some individuals with latent AD.

Only 5% of clinically normal individuals ≥ 60 years-old had low CSF Aβ42 and normal CSF tau concentrations. This is an interesting group that may be interpreted by some as an even earlier stage of latent AD than those with abnormal Aβ42 and tau, although this is speculative. A larger group, 12% of our clinically normal volunteers ≥ 60 years-old, had increased CSF tau and normal CSF Aβ42 concentrations. Increased CSF tau is not specific to AD but is characteristic of other diseases of brain, including VBI (Hesse et al. 2001). Large or territorial ischemic strokes, a type of VBI, are known to increase free radical injury to brain, and to increase CSF tau and F2-IsoP concentrations (Montine et al. 2005). Members of our study were clinically normal and so are very unlikely to have unrecognized territorial infarcts of brain. However, μVBI commonly is clinically silent and, based on autopsy studies, would be expected to be present in about 10 to 15% of those over 65 years of age. Moreover, we have shown that μVBI is associated with increased free radical injury to human cerebrum (Sonnen et al. 2009). Unknown is whether free radical injury from μVBI is reflected in increased CSF F2-IsoPs. Our results raise this possibility. If true, then we need at least to reconsider the interpretation of increased CSF F2-IsoPs in patients with MCI or dementia stage of AD. Free radical injury can derive from AD but perhaps may also derive, at least in part, from co-morbid μVBI.

Our results with CSF F2-IsoPs in clinically normal adults demonstrated that, like prodrome and dementia stages of AD, latent AD is associated with statistically significantly increased CSF F2-IsoPs. In further analogy to symptomatic stages of AD, there was substantial overlap between clinically normal subjects with and without laboratory evidence of latent AD. This consistent pattern of overlap with controls across all stages of AD has potentially profound implications for our understanding of disease mechanism, the interpretation of previous clinical trials, and the organization of future trials. If only a subset of individuals with AD have increased free radical injury, then this raises the possibility that the mechanism(s) by which neurons are damaged may differ among patients that meet criteria for AD and implicates the need for tailored therapeutic approaches. In this specific instance, if only a subset of individuals with AD have increased free radical injury, then clinical trials of anti-oxidant interventions in subjects with AD “not otherwise specified” may have been destined to fail since about one-third of subjects would not need suppression of free radical mechanisms. Going forward it would seem prudent to use tools like CSF F2-IsoPs to demonstrate first pharmacologic efficacy of any proposed anti-oxidant intervention, and then select for a therapeutic trial those subjects that show evidence of ongoing increased free radical injury to brain. Our results suggest that this strategy should be employed at all stages of AD.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by grants from the NIH (AG05131 and AG05136), the Fidelity Foundation, and the Nancy and Buster Alvord Endowment.

References

  1. Aizenstein HJ, Nebes RD, Saxton JA, Price JC, Mathis CA, Tsopelas ND, Ziolko SK, James JA, Snitz BE, Houck PR, Bi W, Cohen AD, Lopresti BJ, DeKosky ST, Halligan EM, Klunk WE. Frequent amyloid deposition without significant cognitive impairment among the elderly. Arch Neurol. 2008;65:1509–17. doi: 10.1001/archneur.65.11.1509. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Buckner RL, Snyder AZ, Shannon BJ, LaRossa G, Sachs R, Fotenos AF, Sheline YI, Klunk WE, Mathis CA, Morris JC, Mintun MA. Molecular, structural, and functional characterization of Alzheimer’s disease: evidence for a relationship between default activity, amyloid, and memory. J Neurosci. 2005;25:7709–17. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2177-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Craft S, Newcomer J, Kanne S, Dagogo-Jack S, Cryer P, Sheline Y, Luby J, Dagogo-Jack A, Alderson A. Memory improvement following induced hyperinsulinemia in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging. 1996;17:123–30. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(95)02002-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Fagan AM, Mintun MA, Mach RH, Lee SY, Dence CS, Shah AR, LaRossa GN, Spinner ML, Klunk WE, Mathis CA, DeKosky ST, Morris JC, Holtzman DM. Inverse relation between in vivo amyloid imaging load and cerebrospinal fluid Abeta42 in humans. Ann Neurol. 2006;59:512–9. doi: 10.1002/ana.20730. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Fagan AM, Roe CM, Xiong C, Mintun MA, Morris JC, Holtzman DM. Cerebrospinal fluid tau/beta-amyloid(42) ratio as a prediction of cognitive decline in nondemented older adults. Arch Neurol. 2007;64:343–9. doi: 10.1001/archneur.64.3.noc60123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189–98. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Hesse C, Rosengren L, Andreasen N, Davidsson P, Vanderstichele H, Vanmechelen E, Blennow K. Transient increase in total tau but not phospho-tau in human cerebrospinal fluid after acute stroke. Neurosci Lett. 2001;297:187–90. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(00)01697-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Iacono D, Markesbery WR, Gross M, Pletnikova O, Rudow G, Zandi P, Troncoso JC. The Nun study: clinically silent AD, neuronal hypertrophy, and linguistic skills in early life. Neurology. 2009;73:665–73. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181b01077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Irizarry MC, Yao Y, Hyman BT, Growdon JH, Pratico D. Plasma F2A isoprostane levels in Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s disease. Neurodegener Dis. 2007;4:403–5. doi: 10.1159/000107699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Li G, Sokal I, Quinn JF, Leverenz JB, Brodey M, Schellenberg GD, Kaye JA, Raskind MA, Zhang J, Peskind ER, Montine TJ. CSF tau/Abeta42 ratio for increased risk of mild cognitive impairment: a follow-up study. Neurology. 2007;69:631–9. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000267428.62582.aa. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Lovell MA, Markesbery WR. Oxidative damage in mild cognitive impairment and early Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurosci Res. 2007;85:3036–40. doi: 10.1002/jnr.21346. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Markesbery WR, Jicha GA, Liu H, Schmitt FA. Lewy body pathology in normal elderly subjects. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 2009;68:816–22. doi: 10.1097/NEN.0b013e3181ac10a7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Milatovic D, VanRollins M, Li K, Montine KS, Montine TJ. Suppression of murine cerebral F2-isoprostanes and F4-neuroprostanes from excitotoxicity and innate immune response in vivo by alpha- or gamma-tocopherol. J Chromatogr B Analyt Technol Biomed Life Sci. 2005;827:88–93. doi: 10.1016/j.jchromb.2005.03.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Mintun MA, Larossa GN, Sheline YI, Dence CS, Lee SY, Mach RH, Klunk WE, Mathis CA, DeKosky ST, Morris JC. [11C]PIB in a nondemented population: potential antecedent marker of Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2006;67:446–52. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000228230.26044.a4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Montine TJ, Kaye JA, Montine KS, McFarland L, Morrow JD, Quinn JF. Cerebrospinal fluid abeta42, tau, and f2-isoprostane concentrations in patients with Alzheimer disease, other dementias, and in age-matched controls. Arch Pathol Lab Med. 2001;125:510–2. doi: 10.5858/2001-125-0510-CFATAF. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Montine TJ, Markesbery WR, Morrow JD, Roberts LJ., 2nd Cerebrospinal fluid F2-isoprostane levels are increased in Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol. 1998;44:410–3. doi: 10.1002/ana.410440322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Montine TJ, Montine KS, McMahan W, Markesbery WR, Quinn JF, Morrow JD. F2-isoprostanes in Alzheimer and other neurodegenerative diseases. Antioxid Redox Signal. 2005;7:269–75. doi: 10.1089/ars.2005.7.269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Montine TJ, Quinn J, Kaye J, Morrow JD. F(2)-isoprostanes as biomarkers of late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. J Mol Neurosci. 2007;33:114–9. doi: 10.1007/s12031-007-0044-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Montine TJ, Quinn JF, Milatovic D, Silbert LC, Dang T, Sanchez S, Terry E, Roberts LJ, 2nd, Kaye JA, Morrow JD. Peripheral F2-isoprostanes and F4-neuroprostanes are not increased in Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol. 2002;52:175–9. doi: 10.1002/ana.10272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Montine TJ, Shi M, Quinn JF, Peskind ER, Craft S, Ginghina C, Chung KA, Kim H, Galasko DR, Jankovic J, Zabetian CP, Leverenz JB, Zhang J. CSF Aβ42 and tau in Parkinson’s disease with cognitive impairment. Movement disorders. 2010 doi: 10.1002/mds.23287. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Montine TJ, Sidell KR, Crews BC, Markesbery WR, Marnett LJ, Roberts LJ, 2nd, Morrow JD. Elevated CSF prostaglandin E2 levels in patients with probable AD. Neurology. 1999;53:1495–8. doi: 10.1212/wnl.53.7.1495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Mufson EJ, Leurgans S. Inability of plasma and urine F2A-isoprostane levels to differentiate mild cognitive impairment from Alzheimer’s disease. Neurodegener Dis. 2010;7:139–42. doi: 10.1159/000289224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Peskind ER, Riekse R, Quinn JF, Kaye J, Clark CM, Farlow MR, Decarli C, Chabal C, Vavrek D, Raskind MA, Galasko D. Safety and acceptability of the research lumbar puncture. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2005;19:220–5. doi: 10.1097/01.wad.0000194014.43575.fd. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Petersen RC, Negash S. Mild cognitive impairment: an overview. CNS Spectr. 2008;13:45–53. doi: 10.1017/s1092852900016151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Petrovitch H, Ross GW, Steinhorn SC, Abbott RD, Markesbery W, Davis D, Nelson J, Hardman J, Masaki K, Vogt MR, Launer L, White LR. AD lesions and infarcts in demented and non-demented Japanese-American men. Ann Neurol. 2005;57:98–103. doi: 10.1002/ana.20318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Pratico D, Clark CM, Lee VM, Trojanowski JQ, Rokach J, FitzGerald GA. Increased 8,12-iso-iPF2alpha-VI in Alzheimer’s disease: correlation of a noninvasive index of lipid peroxidation with disease severity. Ann Neurol. 2000;48:809–12. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Pratico D, Clark CM, Liun F, Rokach J, Lee VY, Trojanowski JQ. Increase of brain oxidative stress in mild cognitive impairment: a possible predictor of Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2002;59:972–6. doi: 10.1001/archneur.59.6.972. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Price JL, McKeel DW, Jr, Buckles VD, Roe CM, Xiong C, Grundman M, Hansen LA, Petersen RC, Parisi JE, Dickson DW, Smith CD, Davis DG, Schmitt FA, Markesbery WR, Kaye J, Kurlan R, Hulette C, Kurland BF, Higdon R, Kukull W, Morris JC. Neuropathology of nondemented aging: presumptive evidence for preclinical Alzheimer disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2009;30:1026–36. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.04.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Quinn JF, Montine KS, Moore M, Morrow JD, Kaye JA, Montine TJ. Suppression of longitudinal increase in CSF F2-isoprostanes in Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis. 2004;6:93–7. doi: 10.3233/jad-2004-6110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Reitan RM, Wolfson D. The neuropsychology handbook: behavioral and clinical perspectives. W. D. New York: Springer Publishing Co; 1986. The Halstead-Reitan neuropsychological test battery; pp. 134–160. [Google Scholar]
  31. Rentz DM, Locascio JJ, Becker JA, Moran EK, Eng E, Buckner RL, Sperling RA, Johnson KA. Cognition, reserve, and amyloid deposition in normal aging. Ann Neurol. 2010;67:353–64. doi: 10.1002/ana.21904. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Roberts LJ, Morrow JD. Measurement of F(2)-isoprostanes as an index of oxidative stress in vivo. Free Radic Biol Med. 2000;28:505–13. doi: 10.1016/s0891-5849(99)00264-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Schmitt FA, Davis DG, Wekstein DR, Smith CD, Ashford JW, Markesbery WR. “Preclinical” AD revisited: neuropathology of cognitively normal older adults. Neurology. 2000;55:370–6. doi: 10.1212/wnl.55.3.370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Smith CD, Carney JM, Starke-Reed PE, Oliver CN, Stadtman ER, Floyd RA, Markesbery WR. Excess brain protein oxidation and enzyme dysfunction in normal aging and in Alzheimer disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1991;88:10540–3. doi: 10.1073/pnas.88.23.10540. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Sonnen JA, Breitner JC, Lovell MA, Markesbery WR, Quinn JF, Montine TJ. Free radical-mediated damage to brain in Alzheimer’s disease and its transgenic mouse models. Free Radic Biol Med. 2008a;45:219–30. doi: 10.1016/j.freeradbiomed.2008.04.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Sonnen JA, Larson EB, Crane PK, Haneuse S, Li G, Schellenberg GD, Craft S, Leverenz JB, Montine TJ. Pathological correlates of dementia in a longitudinal, population-based sample of aging. Ann Neurol. 2007;62:406–13. doi: 10.1002/ana.21208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Sonnen JA, Larson EB, Gray SL, Wilson A, Kohama SG, Crane PK, Breitner JC, Montine TJ. Free radical damage to cerebral cortex in Alzheimer’s disease, microvascular brain injury, and smoking. Ann Neurol. 2009;65:226–9. doi: 10.1002/ana.21508. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Sonnen JA, Montine KS, Quinn JF, Kaye JA, Breitner JC, Montine TJ. Biomarkers for cognitive impairment and dementia in elderly people. Lancet Neurol. 2008b;7:704–14. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70162-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Tuppo EE, Forman LJ, Spur BW, Chan-Ting RE, Chopra A, Cavalieri TA. Sign of lipid peroxidation as measured in the urine of patients with probable Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Res Bull. 2001;54:565–8. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(01)00450-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Tyas SL, Snowdon DA, Desrosiers MF, Riley KP, Markesbery WR. Healthy ageing in the Nun Study: definition and neuropathologic correlates. Age Ageing. 2007;36:650–5. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afm120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Wechsler W. Weschler Memory scale. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1987. Paragraph recall as well as category fluency and letter fluency. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES