Abstract
Vulnerability to drug abuse is related to both reward seeking and impulsivity, two constructs thought to have a biological basis in the prefrontal cortex (PFC). This review addresses similarities and differences in neuroanatomy, neurochemistry and behavior associated with PFC function in rodents and primates. Emphasis is placed on monoamine and amino acid neurotransmitter systems located in anatomically distinct subregions: medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC); lateral prefrontal cortex (lPFC); anterior cingulate cortex (ACC); and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC). While there are complex interconnections and overlapping functions among these regions, each is thought to be involved in various functions related to health-related risk behaviors and drug abuse vulnerability. Among the various functions implicated, evidence suggests that mPFC is involved in reward processing, attention and drug reinstatement; lPFC is involved in decision-making, behavioral inhibition and attentional gating; ACC is involved in attention, emotional processing and self-monitoring; and OFC is involved in behavioral inhibition, signaling of expected outcomes and reward/punishment sensitivity. Individual differences factors (e.g., age and sex) influence functioning of these regions, which, in turn, impacts drug abuse vulnerability. Implications for the development of drug abuse prevention and treatment strategies aimed at engaging PFC inhibitory processes that may reduce risk-related behaviors are discussed, including the design of effective public service announcements, cognitive exercises, physical activity, direct current stimulation, feedback control training and pharmacotherapies. A major challenge in drug abuse prevention and treatment rests with improving intervention strategies aimed at strengthening PFC inhibitory systems among at-risk individuals.
Keywords: Anterior cingulate cortex, Dopamine, Drug abuse, GABA, Glutamate, Impulsivity, Lateral prefrontal cortex, Medial prefrontal cortex, Norepinephrine, Orbitofrontal cortex, Serotonin
1. Introduction
Drug abuse vulnerability is related to the personality trait of “sensation seeking," as defined by the need for novel experiences and a willingness to take risks for these experiences (Zuckerman, 1979). Using Cloninger’s Novelty Seeking Scale or Zuckerman's Sensation Seeking Scale, high sensation seekers consistently use and abuse drugs more often than low sensation seekers assessed across various populations (Andrucci et al., 1989; Ball, 2004; Crawford et al., 2003; Donohew et al., 1991; Kosten et al., 1994; Wills et al., 1994; Wills et al., 1998; Zuckerman, 1994). In controlled laboratory studies, high sensation seekers are more sensitive than low sensation seekers to the rewarding effects of abused drugs (Fillmore et al., 2009; Kelly et al., 2006; Stoops et al., 2007). The association between sensation seeking and drug use has led investigators to develop more effective anti-drug prevention messages that specifically target high sensation seekers (Donohew et al., 1998; Palmgreen et al., 2001). To the extent that sensation seeking is biologically-based, a comprehensive understanding of the neural mechanisms underlying this trait may enhance the design and implementation of targeted interventions.
Sensation seeking does not represent a single construct, but instead represents at least two component constructs that can be designated as reward seeking and inhibitory control, each contributing to drug abuse vulnerability (Baler and Volkow, 2006; Bechara, 2005; Dawe and Loxton, 2004; de Wit and Richards, 2004; Fillmore, 2003; Jentsch and Taylor, 1999). Reward seeking reflects positive incentive motivation and action readiness, whereas inhibitory control refers to the general tendency to show response constraint and sensitivity to punishment. These constructs are similar to “extraversion” and “constraint” defined by Depue and Collins (1999), and both may underlie or contribute to impulsivity.
Similar to sensation seeking, impulsivity is a multi-faceted construct. Two aspects of impulsivity – impulsive choice (choice of a small, immediate reinforcer over a large, delayed reinforcer) and impulsive action (inability to stop a prepotent behavior) predominate the drug abuse literature (Winstanley et al., 2010), and will be the focus of our discussion of impulsivity. Impulsive choice may arise because of reward seeking (i.e., the subject chooses the less advantageous small-immediate reinforcer due to action readiness or an enhanced sensitivity to immediate reinforcement), or because of a lack of inhibitory control (i.e., the subject chooses a small-immediate reinforcer because they are not able to inhibit the desire for the immediate reinforcer). Similarly, impulsive action may arise from reward seeking (i.e., the action readiness or enhanced sensitivity of a subject propels them into action, even when such action may be disadvantageous) or lack of inhibitory control (i.e., subject cannot inhibit prepotent responses). Because both impulsive choice and impulsive action have operational measures that allow for between-species translation and have been studied frequently with respect to drug abuse, these constructs provide insight into the relationship between sensation seeking and drug abuse, as well as the associated neural systems.
Reward seeking and inhibitory control are each subserved by a distinct neural system. With reward seeking, ascending mesocorticolimbic dopamine (DA) projections emanating from midbrain ventral tegmental area (VTA) to nucleus accumbens and prefrontal cortex (PFC) represent an important component of the neural circuitry (Bardo et al., 1996; Berridge and Robinson, 1998; Kelley and Berridge, 2002; Wise, 1998), and this reward circuitry is most dominant during the high-risk adolescent period (Galvan et al., 2006). With inhibitory control, various PFC subregions have been implicated, including medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), lateral prefrontal cortex (lPFC), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC). Since the biological connection between these traits and drug abuse vulnerability cannot be assessed fully in human subjects, this area of investigation has benefited from controlled experiments using laboratory animal models.
The main purpose of this article is to review key findings about the involvement of PFC subregions in drug abuse vulnerability based on research from rodents and humans. While various cortical and subcortical regions are involved in drug abuse vulnerability, PFC is an important nexus where reward seeking and inhibitory control processes are integrated (Kalivas and Volkow, 2005). Although several excellent basic science reviews in rodents and humans have been published previously (de Wit and Richards, 2004; Jentsch and Taylor, 1999; Kalivas et al., 2006), these reviews have not emphasized potential applications for drug abuse prevention and treatment. To the extent that the neural substrates of inhibitory control are understood, it may be possible to tailor interventions that engage inhibitory control processes among individuals who are at highest risk.
2. Prefrontal (PFC) areas across species
A comprehensive review of PFC neuroanatomy and function is beyond the scope of the current review. However, there are anatomically-specific PFC subregions implicated in various cognitive processes related to risky behaviors. Although a variety of nomenclatures and subdivisions of PFC structures have been described, the current review uses the general definitions described for both rodents and primates by Price (2007). Price (2007) differentiates between two major PFC networks: the medial and orbital networks. The medial network is viewed primarily as having visceromotor outputs, in large part to nucleus accumbens shell. In contrast, the orbital network is viewed primarily as sensory in nature, receiving input from sensory cortices and sending output to striatum and associated nucleus accumbens core.
The extent to which region-specific neurobehavioral processes are similar across species is controversial, primarily because PFC shows species-specific variation in size relative to other cortical areas (encephalization), anatomical cytoarchitecture, neurochemistry and connectivity (Ongur and Price, 2000; Preuss, 1995). Rather than emphasizing cross-species differences, this review instead will use a common nomenclature where possible to allow for generalizing behavioral results across rodents and primates. The nomenclature and corresponding anatomical features are illustrated in Figures 1 and 2 for rat and human PFC.
2.1. Rodent PFC anatomy
Rodent PFC receives projections from basal ganglia, substantia nigra, VTA, amygdala, lateral hypothalamus, hippocampus, and other cortical areas (Dalley et al., 2004; Groenewegen et al., 1997). These projections are largely reciprocal – PFC projects to substantia nigra, ventral tegmental area (VTA), amygdala, lateral hypothalamus, and hippocampus, as well as lateral septum, mesencephalon, and autonomic regions of the brainstem (Dalley et al., 2004; Groenewegen et al., 1997). Because rodent cortex is exclusively agranular (compared to primate cortex that is made up of agranular, granular, and dysgranular regions; Barbas and Pandya, 1989; Carmichael and Price, 1994), equivalent areas are based on interconnections between specific areas within PFC, as well as between PFC and other brain regions (Price, 2007; Rose and Woolsey, 1948). Based on these interconnections, PFC can be divided into medial and orbital networks as described previously.
Rat mPFC often refers to various structures located along the medial wall of PFC (Dalley et al., 2004; Price, 2007). The precentral cortex (PrC) and ACC together are often referred to as dorsal mPFC, whereas the prelimbic (PrL), infralimbic (IL), medial orbital (MO), and ventral orbital (VO) cortices together are referred to as ventral mPFC. Two additional structures are located more laterally – dorsal agranular insular (AID) and dorsolateral orbital (DLO) cortex. The inclusion of lateral structures in mPFC may seem surprising (in fact, the AID is sometimes included in OFC; Schoenbaum et al., 2006), but there appear to be extensive interconnections between AID and PrL/IL areas, which also have strong connections to other areas on the medial wall (Conde et al., 1995; Vertes, 2004). For the purpose of this review, we will demarcate mPFC (PrL + IL) and ACC as separate PFC regions because, as discussed later, discrete lesions have dissociated the function of these two regions.
mPFC is connected to other brain regions that have been implicated in drug abuse. For example, both PrL and IL project to amygdala, although IL projects to medial, basomedial, cortical and central nuclei and PrL projects mainly to the basolateral nucleus of amygdala (Vertes, 2004; 2006). PrL also sends projections to VTA and medial parts of dorsal striatum, including nucleus accumbens (Vertes, 2004; 2006). mPFC sends projections to hypothalamus and periaqueductal grey (Floyd et al., 2000; 2001; Price, 2007), as well as to ACC and OFC, allowing for communication between different PFC regions (Vertes, 2006). ACC and mPFC also receive dense projections from hippocampus (Swanson, 1981; Tierney et al., 2004; Vertes, 2006). Together, both mPFC and ACC receive substantial input from amygdala and other limbic areas (Price, 2007).
Rat OFC is comprised of ventral lateral orbital (VLO), lateral orbital (LO), and ventral agranular insular (AIV) cortices (Price, 2007). In contrast to mPFC, these areas send few projections to hypothalamus or periaqueductal grey (Price, 2007). VLO and LO project to central parts of caudate-putamen and to the lateral part of nucleus accumbens shell (Schilman et al., 2008). There are reciprocal connections between basolateral amygdala and areas within OFC (Kita and Kitai, 1990; Krettek and Price, 1977). These regions also receive afferents from medial temporal lobe, ventral pallidum, and VTA (Groenewegen, 1988; Krettek and Price, 1977; Ray and Price, 1992). Rat OFC is part of the olfactory system, and also receives input from other sensory modalities (Price, 1985; Price, 2007).
2.2. Human PFC anatomy
In humans, there have been various demarcations for different functional networks in PFC. The current review relies primarily on the nomenclature outlined by Ridderinkhof et al. (2004b), which divides PFC into three main cytoarchitecturally distinct divisions, namely medial, lateral and orbital. Within the medial region, we will further demarcate mPFC and ACC, as these regions likely represent similar structures in the rat and are relevant for drug abuse behaviors. One caveat to this nomenclature is that human lPFC, consisting of dorsolateral and ventrolateral subregions, does not generalize directly to rat based on connectivity (Price, 2007). In fact, it has been suggested that lPFC may be unique to human and nonhuman primates (Brown and Bowman, 2002), and thus we used the term for humans, but not for rats (cf. Figures 1 and 2).
The connections involving human mPFC, lPFC, ACC and OFC are numerous. Although ACC is often considered part of the limbic system, its close anatomical and functional connections with mPFC and other PFC regions makes it a transitory region between cortical and limbic systems. Historically, cingulate gyrus (including both anterior and posterior extents) has been considered the main interface between cortical regions and subcortical structures like hippocampus, parahippocampal cortex, hypothalamus, and thalamus (Papez, 1958). Indeed, cingulate gyrus communicates with these structures, but also has connections with amygdala and brainstem. Importantly, cingulate gyrus is a target region for DA projections originating in midbrain VTA as part of the mesocorticolimbic DA pathway, which is strongly implicated in reward seeking. Primate ACC is a large area that extends posterior to about midway along the cingulate gyrus. While classification of this region is controversial, primate mPFC and ACC cuts across various Brodmann areas that have been termed the rostral cingulate zone (RCZ; (Ridderinkhof et al., 2004b). RCZ includes portions of both Brodmann areas 24 and 32, areas that correspond roughly to ACC and mPFC respectively in rat.
Human lPFC is an extensive region often demarcated into several subregions, each of which is thought to play a role in inhibitory control. Ridderinkhof et al. (2004b) have demarcated three main lPFC subregions: (1) dorsolateral region, which includes Brodmann areas 8, 9 and 46; (2) ventrolateral region, which includes Brodmann areas 44 and 45; and (3) inferior frontal junction, which consists of posterior sulcus located between the medial and inferior frontal gyri, just anterior to Brodmann area 6. These subregions, especially the dorsolateral region, have reciprocal connections to OFC. Although there are no direct connections to primary motor cortex, lPFC is heavily interconnected with premotor areas, which in turn project to motor cortex and spinal cord, thus making it well situated to control a range of behaviors.
Human OFC receives inputs from numerous cortical and subcortical areas and is highly interconnected with other PFC regions. In both macaque and human brain, Brodmann area 47/12 has similar cytoarchitecturally distinct regions within OFC (Petrides and Pandya, 1999; Wallis, 2007). Functional subdivisions within OFC can be demarcated according to the sensory modality providing input to that region. In addition to receiving information from visual, somatosensory, olfactory and gustatory modalities, OFC also receives visceral information. Similar to ACC, there are reciprocal connections with hippocampus and amygdala. OFC also receives projections from midbrain nuclei associated with different neurotransmitter systems, such as VTA, lateral tegmental area and locus coeruleus.
3. PFC involvement in drug abuse
As mentioned above, both medial and orbital PFC networks are implicated in drug abuse vulnerability. Each of these networks mediates reward-related behaviors in a different way, but they appear to do so in both rodents and primates (see Table 1). For example, mPFC monitors task contingencies. That is, when a particular response no longer yields reinforcement, mPFC activation appears to be associated with altering behavior and orienting towards a new response in order to obtain reinforcement. In contrast, OFC activation is associated with reward value and is important for reversal learning and inhibitory control. However, direct comparison of rodent and primate literature is difficult because the tasks used are often species-specific. A challenge for future research will be develop more tasks that translate readily across rodents and primates (e.g., no/no-go), and to continue to identify between-species similarities in PFC structures.
Table 1.
Rat | Human | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Brain Region |
Putative Functions | Example References | Putative Functions | Example References |
mPFC | Reward processing Attention Impulsivity Drug reinstatement (prelimbic) Habit learning (infralimbic) |
Bardo et al. (1996) Cardinal et al. (2001) Coutureau and Killcross (2003) McLaughlin and See (2003) |
Reward processing Attention Conflict decision making |
Kerns et al. (2004) Ridderinkhof et al. (2004a) |
lPFC | N/A | N/A | Decision making Attentional gating Behavioral inhibition Working memory Emotion regulation |
Fuster (2008) Shackman et al. (2009) Wager et al. (2008) |
ACC | Attention Discrimination learning Timing of reward Drug reinstatement |
Dalley et al. (2004) Dietrich and Allan (1998) McFarland et al. (2004) |
Attention Emotional processing Self-Monitoring Processing of social stimuli |
Fuster (2008) Kringelbach and Rolls (2004) Posner et al. (2007) |
OFC | Behavioral Inhibition Signaling expected outcomes Reversal learning |
Chudasama and Robbins (2003b) Winstanley (2007) Schoenbaum and Roesch (2005) |
Behavioral Inhibition Reward sensitivity (medial) Punishment sensitivity (lateral) Emotional decision-making Reversal learning |
Kringelbach and Rolls (2004) Liu et al. (2007) Cools et al. (2002) |
Abbreviations: mPFC, medial prefrontal cortex; lPFC, lateral prefrontal cortex; ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; OFC, orbitofrontal cortex; N/A, not applicable.
3.1. Medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC)
In rats, mPFC activation is associated with “supervisory” functions such as attention to stimulus features and task contingencies, attentional set-shifting, and behavioral flexibility (Dalley et al., 2004). For example, lesions to this area impair performance when task contingencies change such that a previously-reinforced response is no longer reinforced, and a new response is reinforced instead. mPFC is also necessary for learning tasks with increased cognitive demands, such as those involving complex discriminations and working memory (Frysztak and Neafsey, 1991; 1994; Runyan and Dash, 2004), and it plays a role in extinction (Lebron et al., 2004; Morgan and LeDoux, 1995; Morgan et al., 1993; Quirk et al., 2000), most likely through interactions with hippocampus (Maren and Quirk, 2004).
PrL and IL subregions within mPFC appear to play distinct roles in learning and impulsive action. IL may influence habitual responding by inhibiting goal-directed actions, while PrL may maintain representations of goals and the relationship between actions and goals (Balleine and Dickinson, 1998; Coutureau and Killcross, 2003; Killcross and Coutureau, 2003). Results from lesioning studies have also implicated PrL and IL in performance on 5-choice serial reaction time task (5CSRT), which measures impulsive action, attention, and compulsivity (Carli et al., 1983); the 5CSRT task is based on the continuous performance test of sustained and divided attention used in clinical settings (Beck et al., 1956). IL damage results in impulsive action (i.e., increased premature responses), whereas PrL damage results in compulsive (i.e., perseverative) responding (Chudasama and Muir, 2001; Chudasama et al., 2003a). PrL may also play a role in active inhibition of habitual responses, suggesting a mutually inhibitory role for PrL and IL, and this active inhibition of habits may allow for more planful cognitive decision-making processes to guide behavior (Jentsch and Taylor, 1999). As would be expected, widespread damage to both IL and PrL results in a loss of goal-directed responding, as well as perseverative habitual responding (Ragozzino et al., 1999).
A number of studies indicate that mPFC function is involved in drug abuse behaviors related to impulsivity. Impulsive choice has been associated with both serotonin release and DA receptor tone in mPFC (Winstanley et al., 2006b). Further, mPFC lesions increase impulsive choice on the delay discounting task (Weissenborn et al., 1997). Individual differences in impulsive action on the 5CSRT task also predict faster rates of self-administration acquisition using cocaine (Dalley et al., 2007; Perry et al., 2005; Perry et al., 2008) or nicotine (Diergaarde et al., 2008), perhaps reflecting altered mPFC function. Together, these results implicate an involvement of mPFC in drug self-administration and in both types of impulsivity.
The involvement of mPFC in drug relapse has been evaluated using the reinstatement model (see Shaham et al., 2003). Similar to results cited previously, there is a differential involvement of IL and PrL subregions within mPFC. Specifically, while inactivation of PrL decreases cocaine- (Capriles et al., 2003), cue- (McLaughlin and See, 2003), context- (Fuchs et al., 2005) and stress- (Capriles et al., 2003) induced reinstatement of cocaine-seeking, inactivation of IL has no effect (Capriles et al., 2003; McFarland et al., 2004; McLaughlin and See, 2003). However, IL inactivation enhances spontaneous recovery of cocaine seeking (Peters et al., 2008), suggesting that while PrL circuitry excites drug-seeking in the presence of reinstating cues, IL circuitry has an inhibitory influence on spontaneous recovery of drug-seeking (Kalivas et al., 2006).
It is difficult to ascertain what preclinical information described above translates directly to human mPFC function. Neuroimaging results support the idea that human mPFC is involved in evaluation of decision-making processes in conflict situations (Kerns et al., 2004; Ridderinkhof et al., 2004a; Ridderinkhof et al., 2004b). As anatomically demarcated previously, activation of primate RCZ is associated with sensitivity to reward contingencies (Ito et al., 2003) and is modulated by DA (Holroyd and Coles, 2002). Anatomical studies in non-human primates and humans indicate subregions in RCZ (mPFC and ACC) have extensive reciprocal interconnections (Bates and Goldman-Rakic, 1993; Koski and Paus, 2000), which may link short-term memory to goal-directed action (Ridderinkhof et al., 2004b). mPFC neurons encode past reward-bearing choices (Paulus et al., 2002), thus providing an update of present reinforcement contingencies. As a unit, RCZ activation is associated with conflicts, identification of errors in choice tasks, and directing attention by recruiting PFC, including OFC, to execute cognitive control (Ridderinkhof et al., 2004a). Based on this information, one would anticipate that a dysfunction in mPFC, or RCZ more broadly, would impair monitoring of changes in reinforcement contingencies and thus, would impair decision-making aimed at maximizing positive outcomes. In the case of drug use, this could be expressed as a relative insensitivity to judging the value of drug and non-drug alternative reinforcers in a choice situation.
Some evidence indicates that drug abuse in humans is associated with functional alterations in mPFC, which may play a role in the deficits in executive cognitive functioning and gambling decisions typical among at-risk individuals (Fishbein et al., 2007). Acute cocaine administration increases activity in various PFC regions (Breiter et al., 1997) and stimulant abusers show a general loss of gray matter across these various PFC regions (Matochik et al., 2003). With reference to mPFC specifically, abstinent cocaine abusers tested in the Iowa gambling task display less activity in left mPFC, as well as right lPFC, compared to controls (Bolla et al., 2003), which may relate to the deficits in decision-making and response inhibition observed among individuals with substance use disorders (Monterosso et al., 2005; Paulus et al., 2003). This effect is not specific to mPFC, as individuals with a cocaine abuse history also display decreased activity in left ACC and right lPFC while performing a Stroop test (Bolla et al., 2004a). However, since these results in humans are correlational, they need to be supplemented by experiments in which discrete PFC regions are manipulated in laboratory animals under controlled conditions.
3.2. Lateral prefrontal cortex (lPFC)
As indicated previously, rodent PFC does not have a region similar to primate lPFC. In humans, however, damage to this region leads to the so-called “lateral syndrome” (Fuster, 2008). A hallmark feature of the lateral syndrome is disordered attention. Since the lPFC is a relatively large area, including Brodmann areas 8, 9, 10 and 46, the specific characteristics of the attentional deficits depend on the subareas most affected. Some lateral syndrome patients show deficits in selective attention, which is attention focused on a particular item or experience, whereas other patients show deficits in exclusionary attention, which is the inability to suppress unwanted items that interfere with attentional processing. In both cases, the attentional deficits are often overshadowed by aphasia, dysfunction in working memory and general apathy that may be profoundly debilitating.
Studies conducted with normal human subjects also point to a role of lPFC activity in behavioral inhibition and attention. Shackman et al. (2009) examined dorsal lPFC activity using high-resolution electroencephalography among individuals high or low on the Behavioral Inhibition System (BIS) self-report scale. Individuals high on BIS had greater activity in right lPFC. Right lPFC activation also is associated with response inhibition in a stop-signal task (Aron and Poldrack, 2006) and reduced emotional experience during appraisal of aversive images (Wager et al., 2008). Evidence from non-human primates implicate a dorsal-ventral dissociation within lPFC, with ventral lPFC being more directly involved than dorsal lPFC in attentional control (Kennerley and Wallis, 2009).
There is strong evidence that lPFC is involved in drug abuse behaviors. Similar to medications such as amphetamine and methylphenidate that are used to treat attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), acute cocaine increases performance on tasks that require inhibitory control (Garavan et al., 2008). In contrast, however, chronic cocaine abuse impairs inhibitory control (Fillmore and Rush, 2002), which may explain the escalating pattern of use that typifies addiction. The loss of inhibitory control following chronic use is accompanied by reduced activity in lPFC, as well as in ACC, during performance on a Stroop test (Bolla et al., 2004a). Similar results are obtained with abstinent heavy marijuana users (Bolla et al., 2005; Eldreth et al., 2004), indicating drug-induced lPFC impairment is not restricted to a single pharmacological class.
lPFC activation is also implicated in cue-induced craving following a period of drug abstinence. Among individuals with a cocaine abuse history, exposure to drug paraphernalia increases glucose utilization and brain activity in lPFC (Bonson et al., 2002; Grant et al., 1996). However, cue-elicited brain activation is not limited to lPFC, but rather involves extensive activation in ACC, OFC and associated limbic structures. Similar findings are obtained following presentation of cigarette-related stimuli to tobacco smokers (Brody et al., 2007; Yalachkov et al., 2009).
3.3. Anterior cingulate cortex (ACC)
ACC is involved in attentional selectivity and discrimination learning. In rats, lesions of this area impair acquisition of a task involving sequencing different responses (Delatour and Gisquet-Verrier, 2001) and attentional selectivity (Dalley et al., 2004). ACC may also be involved in impulsive action, as premature responding on the 5CSRT task is increased after lesioning this area (Muir et al., 1996; but see Dalley et al., 2004); however, the premature responding also may reflect impaired timing (Chudasama et al., 2003a; Passetti et al., 2002). While ACC lesions do not alter impulsive choice (Cardinal et al., 2001) or compulsivity defined by continued responding despite negative consequences (Joel et al., 2005b), they increase preference for small, low-effort rewards over larger, high-effort rewards (Walton et al., 2002; 2003). The precise involvement of ACC in impulsive action and impulsive choice warrants future study, as these are related to drug abuse.
A key neural substrate underlying reinstatement of drug-seeking behavior involves glutamatergic input to nucleus accumbens core from dorsal mPFC, including ACC (Kalivas and Volkow, 2005; McFarland et al., 2004). ACC inactivation decreases cue-induced reinstatement (McLaughlin and See, 2003), but not context-induced reinstatement (Fuchs et al., 2005), suggesting that this region processes discrete and diffuse drug-associated stimuli differently. In addition, ACC activity is increased when rats are presented with cues associated previously with cocaine self-administration (Thomas et al., 2003). While inactivation of both ACC and PrL has no effect on spontaneous recovery of cocaine-seeking (Peters et al., 2008), stress- and cocaine-induced reinstatement is decreased (McFarland et al., 2004; McFarland and Kalivas, 2001), thus implicating these regions in drug relapse.
ACC also plays a crucial role in fast adaptations of behavior based on immediate reward values. In monkeys, ACC pre-reward activity is correlated with reward value and ACC neuronal activity is observed prior to the monkey’s discovery of the rewarding stimulus value (Amiez et al., 2006). Amiez et al. (2006) also showed that ACC inactivation impaired the ability to choose stimuli associated with optimal reward value. Consistent with the idea that ACC plays a role in reward value assessment, discrete cocaine-associated cues elicit sustained neuronal firing in ACC (Baeg et al., 2009). These results indicate that ACC guides behavior by encoding general task values and received rewards.
ACC and lPFC are proposed to subserve two distinct inhibitory control systems in humans (Garavan et al., 2002). The first system is thought to be important for fast urgent inhibitions, involving primarily ACC. This ACC-driven system is especially important for emotional self-regulation (Posner et al., 2007). In contrast, the second system is thought to be important for slower deliberate inhibitory processes, involving primarily lPFC. Such a distinction may have important implications for the inhibitory control of risk-related behaviors. In the case of drug abuse, exposure to proximal drug stimuli, such as being handed a tobacco cigarette, requires a rapid decision that may involve ACC. In contrast, a more deliberate decision, such as whether to attend a party where drugs will be available, may involve lPFC. Thus, depending on the temporal proximity of the risky event, different PFC areas may be engaged when choosing or refusing drugs.
Alterations in ACC function have been associated with drug abuse. In non-abusing subjects, methamphetamine increases activity in ACC and OFC (Vollm et al., 2004), suggesting that both decision making and action are affected. In a study by Fishbein et al. (2005), abstinent drug abusers and healthy controls were monitored for brain activity during a risky decision-making task. Drug abusers displayed greater risky choices despite the penalties enforced. The greater risks assumed by the drug abusers was associated with a failure to activate ACC compared to controls. Thus, the deficit in ACC function among drug abusers may play a role in the escalation of intake that occurs during the addiction cycle, despite increasing punishment.
3.4. Orbitofrontal cortex (OFC)
OFC neurons fire in anticipation of expected reward, and activity of these neurons reflects the value of the expected outcome when assessed across different species (Murray et al., 2007; Schoenbaum et al., 2006). With training, these neurons become active during presentation of cues that predict their preferred outcomes. Therefore, OFC plays a role in evoking original learning and integrating it with the new value of an outcome to guide responding (Murray et al., 2007). Consistent with this, OFC lesions impair reversal learning (Chudasama and Robbins, 2003b; Dalley et al., 2004; Murray et al., 2007; Schoenbaum et al., 2003) and prevent integration of information about the consequences of responding for a reward with the subjective value of that rewarding outcome (Schoenbaum et al., 1999; Winstanley, 2007). Furthermore, OFC lesions increase both impulsive action and compulsive responding (Joel et al., 2005a; Joel et al., 2005b; Winstanley, 2007), and the ability to inhibit responding is thought to be mediated by connections between OFC and dorsomedial striatum (Eagle and Robbins, 2003; Eagle et al., 2007).
In human drug abusers, impulsive behavior is proposed to result from drug-induced OFC hyperactivity and this hyperactivity results in a compensatory inhibitory process to normalize OFC activity (Winstanley, 2007). If drug use ceases, drug-induced OFC hyperactivity is no longer present and the compensatory inhibitory process acts to suppress normal OFC activity. Consistent with this idea, Winstanley et al. (2009) showed that cocaine self-administration in rats initially increased impulsive action on the 5CSRT, but that tolerance developed with repeated self-administration. Upon withdrawal, rats again displayed increased impulsive action that was associated with an overexpression of the transcription factor ΔFosB in OFC, thus implicating changes in gene expression in this PFC region.
Accumulating evidence indicates that OFC is involved in drug abuse vulnerability (Schoenbaum and Shaham, 2008). OFC is activated by presentation of stimuli associated previously with self-administered cocaine in rats, nonhuman primates, and humans (Thomas et al., 2003; Thomas and Everitt, 2001). Interestingly, OFC lesions have no effect on acquisition of cocaine self-administration using a simple continuous reinforcement schedule in rats (Hutcheson and Everitt, 2003). However, under a second-order schedule of reinforcement in which completion of a minor chain requirement leads to presentation of a conditioned stimulus, OFC lesions impair acquisition (Hutcheson and Everitt, 2003). Inactivation of lateral OFC also attenuates context-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking (Lasseter et al., 2009). Thus, the OFC appears to be required for drug-seeking in a task that depends on the conditioned reinforcing properties of cocaine-associated cues (Everitt et al., 2007).
Clinical case studies and controlled laboratory studies in humans also support a critical role of OFC in risk-related behavior. Humans suffering from OFC damage are often characterized as pathologically impulsive (Kringelbach and Rolls, 2004; Lishman, 1998; Torregrossa et al., 2008). Among the various tasks that have been assessed following OFC damage, impairments in reversal learning are observed consistently in both human and nonhuman primates (Hornak et al., 2004; Izquierdo et al., 2004; Meunier et al., 1997). A role for OFC in reversal learning is supported further by imaging studies in normal human subjects (Cools et al., 2002; Hampton et al., 2006). In addition to its involvement in reversal learning, OFC plays a role in guiding behavior based on the value of an expected outcome (Murray et al., 2007; Schoenbaum and Roesch, 2005; Torregrossa et al., 2008). Neuronal activity in monkey OFC represents not only the value of expected reward (Roesch and Olson, 2004; Tremblay and Schultz, 2000), but also adapts to reflect the importance of different types of reward information in different contexts and time periods (Simmons and Richmond, 2008). Since many gambling tasks involve expectations about the value of an outcome, it is not surprising that OFC-damaged humans make risky gambling decisions (Bechara, 2005).
OFC dysfunction also is implicated in humans with substance use disorders. Individuals with cocaine abuse history show decreased gray matter in OFC (Franklin et al., 2002), suggesting reduced synaptic connectivity. In support of its role in inhibitory control, monkeys exposed to cocaine display a deficit in reversal learning and other cognitive deficits indicative of OFC dysfunction (Jentsch et al., 2002; Olausson et al., 2007). These results suggest that OFC dysfunction in individuals with a substance use disorder is, at least in part, a consequence of drug exposure, rather than an antecedent condition that precedes drug exposure.
A general view has emerged that OFC dysfunction promotes risky behavior because OFC plays a primary role in decision-making, especially decisions that involve adaptation of behavior due to punishing stimuli (Ridderinkhof et al., 2004b; Wrase et al., 2007). Within OFC, the medial portion is proposed to be more sensitive to rewarding stimuli, whereas the lateral portion is more sensitive to punishing stimuli (Kringelbach and Rolls, 2004). For example, Liu et al. (2007) found that human subjects performing a monetary decision-making task showed medial OFC activation to reward outcomes and lateral OFC activation to loss outcomes. However, not all findings fit the medial-lateral distinction of OFC function put forth by Kringelbach and Rolls (2004). In an fMRI study by Alia-Klein et al. (2007), subjects were exposed to the words “Yes” or “No” in a vocally emphatic manner. Rather than observing a medial-lateral distinction in OFC activity, “Yes” evoked a positive signal in right lateral OFC, while “No” evoked a negative signal in right lateral OFC. These latter results suggest that verbally presented rewarding and punishing stimuli are processed differently from nonverbal stimuli. Because “Yes” and “No” are words that have important meaning for either encouraging or prohibiting various behaviors, it will be important to determine if such valenced words are processed differently among individuals who are low or high for drug abuse risk. For example, at-risk individuals who lack strong parental bonding and tend to be noncompliant to authority may be less receptive to externally applied constraints (“no” command) than low-risk individuals who are more responsive to these constraints.
4. Neurochemistry of PFC
Drugs of abuse alter a variety of neurotransmitter systems, including DA, serotonin (5-HT), norepinephrine (NE), glutamate and γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA) systems. Much of our knowledge regarding the neurochemistry of PFC, including the localization and function of these neurotransmitters and the associated receptors and transporters, has been garnered using animal models because neurochemical mechanisms underlying drug abuse vulnerability cannot be assessed easily using human subjects. In this section, we highlight some key findings about neurotransmitter function within specific PFC regions as they relate to impulsivity.
4.1 Dopamine (DA)
Clinical literature shows that nonspecific DA agonists such as amphetamine or methylphenidate are highly effective in decreasing impulsivity among individuals with ADHD, likely due weakened prefrontal catecholamine function (Arnsten, 2009). However, controlled laboratory studies in humans and rats indicate that DA agonists can either decrease or increase impulsive choice, depending on various factors such as the dose used, baseline level of impulsive choice behavior, and methodological details of the task used (i.e., whether the delay is signaled or unsignaled, type of reinforcer offered), thus making it difficult to generalize across laboratories (Perry and Carroll, 2008). In one study, amphetamine decreased impulsive choice in rats, an effect that was attenuated by the D2 receptor antagonist flupenthixol (Winstanley et al., 2003a). Similarly, increasing extracellular DA concentrations via the DA uptake inhibitor GBR 12909 decreases impulsive choice (van Gaalen et al., 2006). However, administration of selective D1 or D2 DA antagonists has yielded variable results, suggesting that the therapeutic effect of agonist medications involves stimulation of more than one DA receptor subtype.
Dopamine staining is present in most areas of rodent PFC, except for the most lateral portions of OFC (Van De Werd et al., 2010). Regarding the role of DA in PFC subregions in impulsive choice, results indicate that both ACC and OFC are involved, particularly when less effort is required to obtain the small reinforcer than the larger reinforcer. In ACC, blockade of D1 DA receptors, but not D2 DA receptors, enhances preference for a low-cost/low-reward response (Schweimer and Hauber, 2006). Similarly, systemic administration of the D1/D2 antagonist flupenthixol increases impulsive choice for a low-cost/low-reward response (Floresco et al., 2008). In OFC, DA utilization is increased during performance on a delay discounting task (Winstanley et al., 2006b); however, inactivation of DA receptors in OFC has yielded mixed results (Winstanley et al., 2007; Winstanley et al., 2004; Zeeb et al., 2010), depending on baseline levels of impulsive behavior and whether cues are used to signal the delay to the larger reward. Nonetheless, the bulk of results suggest that impulsive choice that is both effort- and delay-based is associated with reduced DA activity in ACC and OFC.
The role of specific PFC DA systems in impulsive action remains to be elucidated. Similar to its effects on impulsive choice, systemic administration of nonspecific DA agonists yields inconsistent results on impulsive action (Pattij and Vanderschuren, 2008; Perry and Carroll, 2008). In humans, psychostimulants tend to decrease impulsive action, an effect that is more dramatic in individuals with high levels of impulsive action. These effects are likely moderated by D1 and D2 DA receptors, as antagonism of either of these subtypes increases impulsive action (Passetti et al., 2003; van Gaalen et al., 2006).
Related to its involvement in impulsivity, PFC DA also plays a role in other cognitive functioning that may be important in drug abuse vulnerability, especially when stress is involved. DA is the most reactive monoamine in response to psychological stress (Goldstein et al., 1996), and high levels of DA activity in mPFC lead to working memory impairment. In rats, stress-induced working memory deficits are correlated with increased DA turnover in mPFC (Murphy et al., 1996). These deficits can be reversed in both rats and monkeys by administering D1 or D4 DA receptor antagonists (Arnsten and Goldman-Rakic, 1998; Murphy et al., 1996; Murphy et al., 1997). Stress responses can be mimicked in the rat via direct infusion of a selective D1 antagonist into mPFC (Zahrt et al., 1997), whereas D4 receptor antagonists block memory deficits induced by a pharmacological stressor (Arnsten et al., 2000). Increased DA function may impair mPFC-associated memory capacity in non-human primates. External factors (i.e., noise) can impair memory capacity in a visual memory task, an effect that is improved by administration of DA receptor antagonists (Arnsten and Goldman-Rakic, 1998). In contrast, decreases in PFC DA turnover induced by chronic amphetamine administration also result in impairments in performance in a spatial delayed response task in nonhuman primates (Castner et al., 2005), indicating cognitive impairment. Combined, these results suggest that perturbing mPFC DA neurotransmission, whether it is an increase or decrease in utilization, results in cognitive impairments. Further, they may have important translational relevance, as cognitive impairments induced by stressful life events may explain, at least in part, the relationship between impulsive behavior and drug abuse in humans (Helen et al., 2010).
4.2 Serotonin (5-HT)
PFC receives 5-HT innervation from medial and dorsal raphe nuclei (Mamounas et al., 1991; Steinbusch, 1981; Wilson and Molliver, 1991a; 1991b). 5-HT raphe neurons synthesize and release 5-HT from growing axonal processes as soon as one day after their appearance in brain (Lambe et al., 2000; Lidov and Molliver, 1982), and the effects of 5-HT within the synapse are terminated by re-uptake of the neurotransmitter into the presynaptic nerve terminals through a high-affinity 5-HT transporter (SERT). After re-uptake, 5-HT is degraded subsequently by monoamine oxidase (MAO).
Both preclinical and clinical evidence points to a role of PFC 5-HT in impulsivity. Among humans with pathological impulsive aggressivity, deficits in 5-HT exist in ACC (Frankle et al., 2005). Rats that display high impulsive action on a go/no-go task also show deficits in 5-HT turnover in mPFC and ACC (Masaki et al., 2006). While these results suggest that PFC 5-HT deficits lead to impulsivity, contradictory evidence exists. For example, 5-HT release in mPFC increases premature responding on the 5CSRT task (Dalley et al., 2002; Puumala and Sirvio, 1998). Moreover, the role of specific 5-HT receptor subtypes are poorly understood and are complicated by the identification of 15 different 5-HT receptor subtypes belonging to one of seven families (5-HT1–7; Barnes and Sharp, 1999; Hoyer et al., 2002). The 5-HT1 (specifically the 5-HT1A/1B/1D) and 5HT2 (5-HT2A/2B/2C) receptor families have received the most attention for their ability to alter impulsivity, with somewhat mixed results (Evenden and Ryan, 1999; Pattij and Vanderschuren, 2008).
In general, impulsive choice is increased following administration of 5-HT1A receptor agonists (Evenden and Ryan, 1999; Liu et al., 2004; van den Bergh et al., 2006; Winstanley et al., 2005), an effect blocked by antagonism of 5-HT1A receptors (Winstanley et al., 2003b). When administered alone, however, 5-HT1A receptor antagonists have no effect on impulsive choice, nor do 5-HT1B receptor agonists (van den Bergh et al., 2006). Impulsive choice is decreased, however, following administration of the 5-HT2C antagonist SER082 (Winstanley et al., 2004). Therefore, 5-HT1A and 5-HT2C receptors appear to have opposing roles in impulsive choice.
In contrast to impulsive choice, a role for 5-HT1A/1B receptors has not been found in impulsive action. Instead, impulsive action is increased following systemic and intra-OFC administration of the 5-HT2A/2C receptor agonist (±)-2, 5-dimethoxy-4-iodoamphetamine (DOI; Hadamitzky and Koch, 2009; Koskinen et al., 2000; Koskinen and Sirvio 2001), and decreased following both systemic and intra-mPFC administration of 5-HT2A antagonists (Fletcher et al., 2007; Higgins et al., 2003; Passetti et al., 2003; Robinson et al., 2008a; Ruotsalainen et al., 1997; Winstanley et al., 2003b). Blockade of 5-HT2C receptors also increases impulsive action in the 5CSRT task (Fletcher et al., 2007; Robinson et al., 2008a). The differential roles of 5-HT1A/1B and 5-HT2A/2C subtypes in impulsive choice and impulsive action, respectively, suggests that selective pharmacotherapies may be useful for treating different facets of impulsive behavior.
4.3 Norepinephrine (NE)
Central NE neurons are localized in brainstem nuclei and project diffusely to almost every part of the brain, including PFC. NE cell bodies in locus coeruleus receive reciprocal innervation from mPFC (Heidbreder and Groenewegen, 2003). NE release into the synapse is deactivated by rapid uptake by the NE transporter (NET) into presynaptic terminals, followed by MAO metabolism (Bonisch and Bruss, 2006; Dostert et al., 1989). Three families of receptors are activated by NE, including α1-, α2-, and β-adrenergic receptors (Bylund et al., 1994). α2 NE receptors have received the most research focus, because ADHD is proposed to involve a deficit in PFC α2 NE activity (Arnsten et al., 2000). α2 NE agonists reduce impulsive choice, impulsive action and inattention in mice (Franowicz et al., 2002), rats (Tanila et al., 1996), monkeys (Arnsten et al., 1988; Rama et al., 1996) and humans (Jakala et al., 1999a; Jakala et al., 1999b).
NE plays a role in both impulsive choice and impulsive action. While impulsive choice is decreased by elevating extracellular NE via NET inhibition (Blondeau and Dellu-Hagedorn, 2007; Robinson et al., 2007), it is increased by direct stimulation of the α2 receptors with clonidine (van Gaalen et al., 2006). These contradictory results likely reflect a preferential activation of presynaptic α2 autoreceptors by clonidine, which may decrease NE release, thus resulting in an increase in impulsivity similar to NET inhibition. In any case, results from these studies suggest that α2 receptors, rather than α1 or β receptors, have a key role in impulsive choice.
Similar to impulsive choice, α2 NE receptors appear to play a role in impulsive action. Saporin-induced lesions of NE afferents to PFC do not alter impulsive action (Milstein et al., 2007). However, NET inhibition decreases impulsive action on 5CSRT and stop-signal tasks (Robinson et al., 2007; van Gaalen et al., 2006). Consistent with these preclinical observations, the NET inhibitor atomoxetine decreases stop-signal reaction time in healthy human volunteers (Chamberlain et al., 2006). Inhibition of NE transport would be expected to increase activation of postsynaptic NE receptors, thus leading to the observed reduction in impulsive action. The reduction in impulsive action likely reflects α2 NE receptor stimulation, as the α2 NE antagonist yohimbine increases premature responding in the 5CSRT task, as well as increasing phosphorylation of cyclic adenosine monophosphate response element binding (CREB) protein in OFC (Sun et al., 2010). Further, impulsive action in the 5CSRT task is increased by α2 NE antagonists, but not by α1 NE antagonists (Koskinen et al., 2003), indicating a selective involvement of α2 receptors. However, the α1 NE antagonist prazosin reduces impulsive action induced by methylphenidate, and the β-adrenoceptor antagonist propranolol eliminates methylphenidate-induced increases in impulsive action (Milstein et al., 2010). Combined, these results suggest that NE α2 receptors, perhaps in OFC, play a primary role in both types of impulsivity under normal conditions, but that α1 and β NE receptors also may contribute in the presence of high extracellular DA levels induced by stimulant drugs.
4.4 Glutamate
Glutamate is a major excitatory neurotransmitter in PFC, including both corticostriatal and corticothalamic efferents (Bromberg et al., 1981; Fonnum et al., 1981a; Fonnum et al., 1981b). Excitatory glutamatergic neurons from PFC control DA release in VTA and nucleus accumbens (Del Arco and Mora, 2008; Krystal et al., 2003). An imbalance in glutamate homeostasis (synaptic and non-synaptic glutamate balance), which impairs communication between PFC and nucleus accumbens, has been linked to impulsive drug seeking behavior and drug abuse (Hyman et al., 2006; Kalivas, 2009). Recent clinical research also reveals a positive correlation between ACC glutamate levels and self-reported levels of impulsivity measured on the Barrett scale (Hoerst et al., 2010).
Glutamate acts via stimulation of both ionotropic glutamate receptors (iGluRs) and metabotropic receptors (mGluRs). iGluRs, including N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA), kainate and α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid (AMPA) receptors, are ligand-gated nonselective cation channels, which allow K+, Na+ and sometimes Ca2+ flux in response to glutamate binding to its recognition site on the protein (Tikhonov and Magazanik, 2009). NMDA receptors and the associated specific subunits NR1–3 have been studied in detail, since these are thought to be involved in learning and memory (Dingledine, 1983). mGluRs, which include mGlu1–8 receptors, are a heterogeneous family of G-protein-coupled receptors which function to modulate brain excitability via presynaptic, postsynaptic and glial mechanisms (Conn and Pin, 1997; Schoepp, 2001).
Impulsivity is modulated by NMDA receptors, specifically the NR2B subunit. Impulsive choice is increased after administration of ketamine, an NMDA antagonist (Floresco, 2007). Similarly, impulsive action is increased by NMDA antagonists administered either systemically or into mPFC (Higgins et al., 2003; Mirjana et al., 2004; Murphy et al., 2005; Paine et al., 2007). The selective NR2B NMDA receptor antagonist Ro63–1908 increases impulsive action (Higgins et al., 2003).
mGluRs also have a role in impulsivity, particularly mGluR1 and mGluR5 subtypes. Blockade of mGluR1 receptors in rats improves working memory and reduces impulsive choice (Sukhotina et al., 2008). In contrast, administration of mGluR1 receptor antagonists increases impulsive action (Sukhotina et al., 2008). Antagonism of the mGluR5 receptor has similar effects (i.e., increased impulsive action); however, antagonism of mGluR2/3 receptors has no effect on impulsive action (Semenova and Markou, 2007). Collectively, these findings indicate that inhibition of glutamate neurotransmission increases both impulsive choice and impulsive action, although little is known about the PFC subregions involved beyond mPFC.
4.5 GABA
About one fourth of all PFC neurons utilize GABA, the prime inhibitory synaptic transmitter and most abundant of all neurotransmitters in the CNS. PFC GABA neurons generally do not project directly the reward-relevant nucleus accumbens (Christie et al., 1987); however, subcortical regions can be affected indirectly due to the inhibitory role of GABA in several PFC regions (Brailowsky et al., 1986; Matsumura et al., 1992). There are two types of GABA receptors: GABAA and GABAB. GABAA is the postsynaptic receptor and GABAB has been identified as mainly an autoreceptor involved in the self-regulation of GABA. GABAB receptors are widely distributed in several brain areas related to drug seeking, where they modulate both excitatory and inhibitory effects pre-and postsynaptic mechanisms (Thompson and Gahwiler, 1992). In PFC, GABAB receptors are localized on the presynaptic terminals of the glutamatergic nerve endings and/or cell bodies, (Margeta-Mitrovic et al., 1999) and on GABA neurons where they act as autoreceptors (Calver et al., 2002).
Few studies have investigated the role of GABA in impulsivity, although some evidence implicates a role for GABAA receptors. Local injections of the GABAA antagonist bicuculline into nonhuman primate PFC increases impulsive action (Sawaguchi et al., 1988). In human subjects, oral administration of benzodiazepines, which allosterically augment GABAA receptor function and increase extracellular concentrations of GABA in brain, is positively correlated with increased self-reports of impulsivity (Deakin et al., 2004; Lane et al., 2005). However, controlled laboratory studies evaluating the effects of benzodiazepines on impulsivity are mixed. One report showed that diazepam does not alter impulsive choice or impulsive action (Reynolds et al., 2004), while another found that triazolam increases impulsive action in a stop-signal task (Fillmore et al., 2001). In rats, benzodiazepines are reported to either increase or produce no change in impulsive choice (Cardinal et al., 2001; Charrier and Thiebot, 1996; Evenden and Ryan, 1996). Thus, more research is necessary to elucidate the effects of benzodiazepines on impulsivity.
The GABAA agonist muscimol decreases lever pressing for preferred food and increases consumption of a less preferred food, a possible measure of impulsive choice (Farrar et al., 2008). In addition, microinjection of muscimol into median raphe increases impulsive action and reinstates alcohol seeking in alcohol-dependent rats (Le et al., 2008), indicating an interaction between GABA and 5-HT systems. Thus, GABAA antagonists may represent a novel target for intervention of impulsive behaviors involved in reinstatement of drug seeking.
4. Age- and sex-related differences in prefrontal cortex
4.1. Developmental differences
Initiation of drug use occurs most often during adolescence and early adulthood (Spear, 2000). One of the most important features that characterizes the transition from childhood to adolescence is a shift away from parental control to social bonding with peers. The move away from a parent-centered life to a peer-centered life is necessary for reproductive fitness and mating. During this developmental period, it is not surprising that risk-related behaviors become more prominent, with hedonic limbic structures being more dominant than inhibitory PFC structures. Although many regions of the brain undergo structural changes during this period, mPFC and superior temporal sulcus sculpt the social brain (Blakemore, 2008). The functional consequence of this anatomical maturation includes changes in face recognition and mental-state attribution (Blakemore, 2008), with each of these processes playing a role in social interaction.
Considerable work with laboratory animals shows that synaptogenesis during development initially yields an excess of potential synaptic connections, but that many nonfunctional connections are pruned during maturation (Cragg, 1974; 1975; Zecevic et al., 1989). In humans, several longitudinal and cross-sectional neuroimaging studies have captured the changes in white and gray matter volumes in various brain regions from childhood to young adulthood (Giedd et al., 1999; Gogtay et al., 2004; Olson et al., 2009; Paus et al., 1999; Sowell et al., 2003). A general conclusion from these studies is that PFC white matter increases linearly across this age period, whereas gray matter decreases. Studies using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) show that activity in mPFC is highest during the adolescent period, which coincides with synaptic pruning leading to a higher signal-to-noise ratio for PFC networks in the mature brain (Blakemore, 2008). In rat, mPFC also shows a loss of neurons from adolescence to adulthood (Markham et al., 2007), consistent with the idea of streamlining of PFC connectivity across this transitional developmental period.
Although adolescents show superior performance on various inhibitory tasks compared to children (Lamm et al., 2006), evidence indicates that PFC connections are not developed fully until early adulthood (Cunningham et al., 2002; Gogtay et al., 2004). Galvan et al. (2006) examined event-related fMRI activity of OFC and nucleus accumbens in children, adolescents and young adults participating in a task that measured choice of varying reward values. Across these age groups, adult-like patterns of accumbal activity occurred earlier in development than OFC activity. A direct comparison between adolescents and adults indicated that OFC activity was lower in adolescents than adults at the anticipatory stage of the task. Similarly, other work has shown that adolescents have lower OFC activation than adults at the decision point of a risky economic choice (Eshel et al., 2007). To the extent that the nucleus accumbens mediates reward seeking and OFC mediates behavioral inhibition, these findings provide neural evidence that attraction to rewarding stimuli during adolescence is not counterbalanced by the type of inhibitory control that characterizes most adults.
The reduced PFC activity noted in children and adolescents compared to adults does not generalize across all tasks. For example, in contrast to results obtained with risky choice tasks, children show greater mPFC, lPFC, ACC and OFC activity than adults when instructed to suppress their reactions to emotional stimuli (Levesque et al., 2004). One interpretation of this finding is that emotional self-regulation in children requires the recruitment of more connections within the immature PFC compared to more practiced adults. This interpretation is consistent with work showing that adolescents with conduct disorder recruit relatively more PFC tissue than controls in order to normalize their performance on the Stroop test (Banich et al., 2007). However, an alternative interpretation of the greater PFC activation in children is that this age-related difference may be secondary to differences in amygdala activity, since PFC and amygdala have reciprocal connections (Lewis and Stieben, 2004).
In addition to maturation of emotional self-regulatory processes, PFC development across the periadolescent period is thought to play a vital role in memory processing. Ofen et al. (2007) investigated declarative memory in children and adults in a neuroimaging study. Subjects studied various scenes and were asked to remember the scenes for later view. When subsequently asked whether the scenes were old or new, recognition memory improved with age and this age-related improvement in memory was accompanied by greater activation in various PFC regions. These results suggest that the immature PFC in children hinders the ability to retrieve old information, thus making previously viewed stimuli appear as novel. Interestingly, there is considerable evidence suggesting that individuals who are most responsive to novel stimuli (i.e., high novelty seekers) are also more responsive to drugs of abuse (Bardo et al., 1996). To the extent that an immature PFC enhances the relative novelty of various stimuli that motivate behavior, this may represent an important factor to explain why periadolescents are at increased risk for drug use.
Environmental influences during development may alter the trajectory of PFC connectivity and functionality. Among the most important influences is the amount of social and environmental enrichment experienced during development. Adolescent rats reared in an enriched environment with social cohorts and novel objects show an accelerated cortical maturation compared to isolated rats raised without cohorts or objects (Renner and Rosenzweig, 1987). Enriched rats also show decreased vulnerability to stimulant self-administration (Bardo et al., 2001), which is associated with decreased DA transporter function in mPFC (Zhu et al., 2005). While exposure to novel objects is thought to play a role in the enrichment effects, a strong influence of social factors is also important. In particular, maternal interactions early in life have long-lasting consequences for PFC function during young adulthood (Denenberg, 1964; Meaney et al., 1996). Bock et al. (2008) found that early social experience was especially important in promoting dendritic spine density in ACC.
Environment-dependent neurobehavioral changes also have been observed in non-human primates. Lyons et al. (2002) separated young squirrel monkeys from their mothers on intermittent occasions from 13 to 21 weeks of age. These separations provoked repetitive peep-calls, agitated locomotion and elevated cortisol levels in the offspring, effects indicative of distress. When evaluated as young adults, separated monkeys had a larger ventromedial PFC, consisting of portions of both mPFC and lPFC, compared to non-separated controls. This cytoarchitectural change was asymmetric, being evident in the right hemisphere only. This asymmetry may be important because negative emotional experiences are thought to be linked to the right hemisphere in humans (Ahern et al., 1994).
Clinical studies in children with ADHD show an altered developmental trajectory in PFC maturation. Anomalous PFC functioning has been observed in children with ADHD compared to controls (Bush et al., 1999; Ernst et al., 2003). An interesting feature of ADHD, however, is that a substantial portion of children with ADHD show a normalization of behavior with age. To determine if there is an anatomical dissociation among ADHD children with positive or negative clinical trajectories, Shaw et al. (2006) conducted a longitudinal study that examined cortical thickness in various PFC regions from approximately 8 to 15 years of age. At the younger age, individuals with ADHD had a thinned mPFC and ACC compared to controls, an effect that persisted across the study. Children with the worst clinical outcomes had thinner left mPFC at baseline compared to children with a better clinical outcome. However, it is not clear to what extent this difference in mPFC thickness reflects an alteration in synaptic density related to attention or impulsivity.
4.2. Sex differences
Notable sex differences exist in brain structure (Luders et al., 2004), basal measures of brain functioning (Andreason et al., 1994; Gur et al., 1995) and functional measures of neural responding (Cahill et al., 2001). Sex differences also exist in cognitive functioning and working memory on tasks that require PFC regions (Overman, 2004). Furthermore, following exposure to stress, there is a sexual dimorphism in performance on behavioral tasks and associated neurochemical changes in PFC (Bowman et al., 2003; Bowman et al., 2009; Lin et al., 2009).
Among the various tasks used to assess sex differences in decision-making and cognitive impulsivity in humans, the Iowa Gambling Task has been studied the most widely. This task requires participants to choose from decks of cards that result in either high reward with infrequent but high sporadic losses (disadvantageous decks) or low reward with more frequent but low sporadic losses (advantageous decks). While control participants learn to choose the advantageous decks, this winning strategy is impaired among patients with PFC damage (Bechara et al., 1994; Bechara et al., 1997; Clark et al., 2008), drug abusers (Grant et al., 2000), violent offenders (Fishbein, 2000), individuals with eating disorders (Liao et al., 2009), and pathological gamblers (Cavedini et al., 2002; Forbush et al., 2008). Interestingly, normal males also choose the advantageous decks more than females (Crone et al., 2005; Garon and Moore, 2004; Kerr and Zelazo, 2004; Overman, 2004; Reavis and Overman, 2001), likely due to a sex difference in the strategy employed (Overman, 2004). While males learn to maximize monetary outcomes in the long run by choosing only advantageous decks, females tend to choose decks that have the lowest probability of resulting in a loss, even if the loss is large (as it is with disadvantageous decks). This suggests that males are more sensitive to long-term monetary gains, while females are more sensitive to immediate loss.
Sex differences in PFC activity during performance on the Iowa Gambling Task accompany the sex differences in performance on this task. In males, brain activation is more lateralized to the right hemisphere, and males show more activation in right lPFC and right lateral OFC than females (Bolla et al., 2004b). While females show activation in both hemispheres, more activation in left lPFC also occurs in females than in males (Bolla et al., 2004b). Previous studies have shown that right PFC damage results in impairment on the Iowa Gambling Task and decision-making in real life, whereas left PFC damage has no effect (Bechara et al., 2000a; Bechara et al., 2000b; Manes et al., 2002; Tranel et al., 2002). Taken together, these results suggest that increased activity in right lPFC and/or lateral OFC could underlie the propensity of males to select cards from advantageous decks.
Other measures of decision-making and impulsivity have yielded inconsistent sex differences in both performance and brain activation. No reliable sex differences in performance are observed on impulsive action measured by the stop signal reaction time task, even though sex differences in brain activation may occur (Li et al., 2006). In the Stroop task, no reliable sex differences in brain activation are observed (Bolla et al., 2004b). On other measures of risk taking or impulsivity, discrepant reports have shown males to be more impulsive (Kirby and Marakovic, 1996; Rosenblitt et al., 2001; Van Leijenhorst et al., 2008; Whiteside and Lynam, 2003), less impulsive (Reynolds et al., 2006; Wallace, 1979) or equally impulsive (Fillmore and Weafer, 2004; Reynolds et al., 2006; Skinner et al., 2004), compared to females. Experimental conditions seem to play an important role in these mixed results, possibly because males and females use different strategies on the various tasks employed. For example, females discount delayed hypothetical reinforcers at a higher rate than males (i.e., show greater impulsive choice), but when real reinforcers were offered, males discount at a higher rate than females (Heyman and Gibb, 2006). Understanding sex differences in making risky or impulsive decisions may have important implications for maximizing prevention and treatment strategies targeted toward either males or females.
5. Translation to applied interventions
5.1. Prevention
Drug abuse prevention interventions commonly consist of educational, family- or peer-based programs aimed at middle and high school age adolescents (Botvin and Griffin, 2007; Sussman et al., 2004; Velleman et al., 2005). Some interventions, termed universal interventions, are designed for use among all members of a population. Other ‘targeted’ interventions are designed specifically for a subset of individuals characterized by some common endophenotype associated with risk, such as high sensation seekers or adolescents with conduct disorder (Fishbein et al., 2006). With both universalistic and targeted approaches, prevention can result from teaching refusal skills, as well as from reinforcing choices that promote healthier lifestyles. A major hurdle in conducting prevention science relates to the high cost of evaluation studies. As a case in point, the National Youth Anti-Drug Media Campaign sponsored by the U.S. Office of National Drug Control Policy took 5 years to develop and test, at a total cost of $2 billion (Palmgreen et al., 2007). Thus, the use of controlled laboratory-based experiments that establish proof-of-concept results are desirable prior to implementing time-consuming and expensive interventions.
One way to establish proof-of-concept results is to use neuroimaging results to determine if a novel prevention intervention engenders PFC activation patterns associated with self-controlled decisions and desired behavioral outcomes. For example, development of televised public service announcements (PSAs) may benefit from neuroimaging technologies (Ray et al., 2008). Mass media campaigns that target high sensation seekers with high arousal messages reliably decrease risky decision-making involving drug use and unsafe sex (Palmgreen and Donohew, 2003). Recent neuroimaging studies have examined the activation patterns of PFC and related structures following presentation of arousing stimuli. For example, Joseph et al. (2009) examined brain activation patterns during an emotional induction task in high and low sensation seekers. When exposed to emotional images, high sensation seekers displayed a rapid and pronounced activation of insula, a brain region implicated in arousal (Craig, 2005) and drug craving (Naqvi and Bechara, 2008). Insula activation was followed rapidly by ACC activation. In contrast to high sensation seekers, however, low sensation seekers showed an early response in ACC, but a minimal response in insula, which may explain why they have greater emotional control than high sensation seekers. ACC activation may reflect a compensatory mechanism that inhibits the initial arousal response in insula.
Likewise, high and low sensation seekers show differences in brain anatomy (Martin et al., 2007) and brain function in a novelty detection task (Jiang et al., 2009; Smith et al., 2008). Jiang et al. (2009) measured cortical evoked potentials following exposure to a series of familiar or novel visual stimuli. High sensation seekers had reduced ventral PFC responses to novel stimuli compared to low sensation seekers. These results are consistent with a report by De Pascalis et al. (2009) showing that subjects high on impulsivity measured on the Zuckerman-Kuhlman Personality Questionnaire show delayed event-related neural potentials. These formative results illustrate how neuroimaging techniques may be used to identify pre-existing differences in brain function associated with sensation-seeking status. Moreover, neuroimaging technologies may prove to be useful for screening various intervention stimulus materials prior to the design and implementation of expensive and time-consuming field-based prevention efficacy trials.
Although high message sensation value PSAs may be important for attracting the attention of high sensation seekers, there is some evidence that “deep” processing of message content may be more effective when delivered with low message sensation value. Langleben et al. (2009) examined brain activity and recognition memory in regular tobacco smokers exposed to anti-smoking PSAs. Although it is not clear whether the subjects used in this study were high or low sensation seekers, low sensation value PSAs were recognized more accurately and produced greater PFC activation than high sensation value PSAs. These results suggest that attention-intensive formats may compete with message information for cognitive resources. Thus, when combined with previous results (Palmgreen et al., 2001), it is possible that PSAs that initially attract attention with high arousal stimuli and then transition into a low arousal persuasive message for deep processing may be most effective.
Since at-risk adolescents are thought to be impulsive due to immature PFC function, training exercises that specifically engage PFC areas involved in decision-making may offer a useful prevention intervention strategy. Conrod et al. (2006) implemented a prevention intervention that targeted adolescents high in either sensation seeking or anxiety. The intervention was a multi-dimensional cognitive-behavioral approach that included lessons on the consequences of coping strategies that centered around high-risk personality dimensions such as sensation seeking. Subjects completed activities designed to recognize automatic thoughts and engaged in group exercises based on real-life scenarios. Compared to a non-intervention control group, impulsive binge drinking was reduced by the intervention. It would be interesting to determine if the reduction in binge drinking was associated with increased activity in PFC regions.
Although training exercises designed to engage PFC inhibitory processes may seem like an appealing idea to reduce drug abuse vulnerability, there may be a paradoxical risk to such a strategy. While the ability to exert inhibitory control over thoughts, emotions and actions is a central process of human existence, self-control is a limited resource that can be exhausted under certain situations. This phenomenon has been demonstrated in laboratory experiments in which subjects that are required to exert inhibitory control in one task show a deficit in inhibitory control in a second task that requires the same resource (Baumeister and Heatherton, 1996; Muraven et al., 1998). This finding may have important implications for prevention interventions aimed at enhancing inhibitory control over drug use. Rather than being protective, intervention strategies that drain self-control resources immediately prior to the opportunity to use or refuse drugs may actually exacerbate the problem of drug use. This “boomerang” effect may be negated by insuring that training exercises in self-control are not implemented shortly before entering a situation where drugs are available.
The depletion of self-control resources that occurs with repeated exertion involves ACC. Inzlicht and Gutshell (2007) monitored electroencephalographic waveforms associated with ACC in subjects that were instructed to suppress their emotions while viewing a movie depicting animals that were suffering. Subjects subsequently performed the Stroop test in which good performance required suppression of the tendency to read a word (red or green) in order to rapidly name the color used to print the word. Subjects instructed to suppress their emotions in the movie performed worse on the Stroop test compared to control subjects. This deficit in performance was associated with a diminution of ACC activity, suggesting a depletion of inhibitory function in this region.
Engaging in physical activity also may be an effective strategy for promoting PFC-mediated inhibitory processes. Using a confirmatory multivariate analysis in adolescents, Wills et al. (2007) found that self-control constructs are related to both dietary intake and physical activity. Individuals who were poor at self-control (i.e., impulsive) were least likely to engage in physical activity, thus raising the risk for drug use. Consistent with this, children showing good aerobic fitness between the ages of 7 and 12 years have better Stroop test performance (Buck et al., 2008), while obese children show less inhibitory control compared to lean counterparts (Nederkoorn et al., 2006).
Preclinical evidence also indicates that physical activity has direct neurobehavioral effects on PFC functioning. Rats given access to a running wheel display an increase in glioneogenesis in mPFC, including greater numbers of both astrocytes and oligodendrocytes that support neuronal activity (Mandyam et al., 2007). Similarly, rats raised during the periadolescent period in an enriched environment that promotes physical activity display a decrease in DA transporter function in mPFC (Zhu et al., 2005), which presumably leads to an increase in extracellular DA available in this region. Physical activity induced by access to a running wheel or exposure to enrichment decreases cocaine and amphetamine self-administration (Bardo et al., 2001; Cosgrove et al., 2002; Smith et al., 2008), as well as decreasing impulsive choice on a delay discounting task (Perry et al., 2008). Thus, physical activity and enriched environments appear to protect against PFC-mediated impulsivity and drug abuse vulnerability, at least in preclinical models.
One of the most salient examples of how an understanding of PFC dysfunction may inform prevention intervention strategies rests with work conducted in maltreated children raised in foster care. Children raised in foster care display a deficit in salivary cortisol levels measured in the early morning and this deficit may be normalized with suitable interventions (Fisher et al., 2000; Fisher et al., 2007; Gunnar and Fisher, 2006). mPFC is an important component of the neurocircuitry that regulates pituitary secretion of adrenocorticotropic hormone via the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus (Gunnar and Fisher, 2006). This raises the possibility that maltreated children have a deficit in mPFC inhibitory processes involved in the hypothalamic-pituitary adrenal axis, as well as in self-control. To the extent that an intervention can normalize the blunted cortisol response, this may serve as a marker for determining the effectiveness of intervention strategies targeting stress-related risky behaviors.
Among the various predisposing psychopathological factors that predict risk for drug abuse, the presence of ADHD is thought to be a significant contributor. Adolescents with ADHD are impulsive in a variety of tasks (Solanto, 2002) and stimulant drugs such as amphetamine and methylphenidate are effective in treating ADHD. While there is some controversy in the field, evidence suggests that non-medicated ADHD children are at increased risk for drug abuse, and this risk factor may be mitigated by medication (Winstanley et al., 2006a). ADHD is thought to stem, at least in part, from PFC dysfunction (Castellanos and Tannock, 2002), suggesting that stimulant drugs may decrease risk by normalizing inhibitory control in PFC.
5.2. Treatment
A more complete understanding of PFC involvement in behavioral inhibition also may have important implications for drug abuse treatment. Impulsivity confers greater risk for initiation and escalation of drug use among adolescents and is known to accompany substance use disorders. While it is difficult to disentangle whether impulsivity precedes or results from drug use, controlled studies using laboratory animals indicate that drug use can decrease inhibitory control, thus leading to greater impulsive choice and impulsive action. However, in a delay discounting task, impulsive choice is decreased by methamphetamine (Richards et al., 1999) and increased by nicotine (Dallery and Locey, 2005), indicating that general conclusions cannot be made across different stimulant drugs. Nonetheless, repeated administration of amphetamine, methamphetamine or nicotine enhances impulsive choice, even when rats are tested in a drug free state following withdrawal (Dallery and Locey, 2005; Richards et al., 1999). These latter findings indicate that chronic exposure to stimulant drugs produces lasting deficits in inhibitory control.
Similar to the effects observed with experimenter-administered stimulants, repeated self-administration of stimulants in rats increases impulsive choice in a delay discounting task (Gipson and Bardo, 2009). Rats learning to self-administer cocaine show an initial loss of inhibitory control, although tolerance to this effect is observed across repeated testing (Winstanley, 2007). Upon termination of the cocaine self-administration, a deficit in inhibitory control re-emerges, suggesting a long-term neural adaptation due to the cocaine self-administration regimen. OFC may be altered by cocaine self-administration, as fMRI studies with cocaine-abusing humans tested during a period of abstinence show hypoactivity in this PFC region (Volkow et al., 2004).
Medication is a viable treatment intervention approach (Vocci, 2008). Among the various pharmacotherapeutic options, perhaps the most relevant for altering PFC function are drugs that work selectively on monoamine and amino acid neurotransmission. As described previously, these neurotransmitter systems subserve PFC circuitry and impulsivity (Pattij and Vanderschuren, 2008). Preclinical evidence indicates that impulsive action in the 5CSRT task is decreased by the NE reuptake inhibitor atomoxetine (Robinson et al., 2008b) and increased by the NE autoreceptor agonist clonidine (van Gaalen et al., 2006). In humans, atomoxetine is used to treat ADHD and shows promise for treatment of stimulant abuse (Sofuoglu and Sewell, 2008; Stoops et al., 2008). Buspirone may also be useful, as it a partial agonist at 5-HT autoreceptors and it is currently used to treat anxiety. Consistent with this, repeated administration of buspirone lowers 5-HT levels in mPFC and decreases impulsive choice (Liu et al., 2004). Finally, antagonism of mGluRs may also have utility because recent preclinical evidence indicates that blockade of these receptors decreases impulsive action (Sukhotina et al., 2008).
An alternative neurobehavioral approach to reduce risky behavior involves the application of direct current stimulation to PFC. High-frequency transcortical magnetic stimulation of the skull overlying the right PFC reduces craving for cocaine (Camprodon et al., 2007). While the precise mechanisms and extent of brain subregions affected need to be characterized more fully, the decrease in craving likely relates to enhanced cortical excitability of critical inhibitory areas of right lPFC in proximity to the stimulation site. In contrast to the decrease in drug craving, however, low-frequency transcortical magnetic stimulation of the right lPFC increases risky behavior in a decision-making task (Knoch et al., 2006), an outcome that would be disadvantageous for promoting abstinence.
Simultaneous application of excitatory anodal current to the right lPFC and inhibitory cathodal current to the left lPFC also has been investigated. Fecteau et al. (2007) examined the effect of this asymmetrical transcortical technique on performance among healthy young adults in a gambling task involving rewards and penalties. Treated subjects showed a significant increase in choosing low-risk options compared to sham-stimulated subjects, as well as compared to subjects who were stimulated with a reverse procedure (i.e., anodal current to left lPFC and cathodal current to right lPFC). These results suggest that excitation of the right PFC and/or inhibition of the left PFC enhances inhibitory control. While this noninvasive and safe treatment is innovative, the treatment is limited primarily to surface brain structures. It would be interesting to determine if selective stimulation of deeper brain regions, such as OFC, would alter risk-taking.
Individuals also can learn to control fMRI activation of selected brain regions using feedback training. Posse et al. (2003) used real-time fMRI feedback in subjects exposed to sad and neutral faces. Subjects given immediate feedback of amygdala activation to reinforce mood induction displayed heightened left-side amygdala activation, which correlated with the intensity of self-rated sadness, indicating that individuals may be able to control discrete limbic brain regions. More recent evidence indicates that PFC regions also may be trained by feedback control. deCharms et al. (2005) used real-time fMRI to train chronic pain patients to alter activity of right ACC, a structure involved in pain perception. Patients trained to control right ACC brain activation reported a reduction in ongoing pain. Perhaps similar technology could be used to train individuals to recruit discrete PFC regions involved in inhibitory control in order to reduce impulsive and unhealthy behaviors.
Finally, craving among drug abusing subjects may be especially sensitive to treatments designed to alter PFC activity. When drug abusing individuals are exposed to cues associated previously with cocaine, such as a crack pipe or syringe, there is an increase in subject-rated and physiological craving. Although many cortical and subcortical brain regions subserve craving, PFC areas play a prominent role (Bonson et al., 2002; Childress et al., 1999; Grant et al., 1996; Wang et al., 1999). For example, Childress et al. (1999) reported that detoxified male cocaine users exposed to cocaine-related videos experienced craving and showed limbic activation, including ACC. Bonson et al. (2002) extended these findings by showing that cue-elicited cocaine craving also involves OFC activation (defined by increased glucose metabolism measured with positron emission tomography) and that this activation is correlated with craving intensity. Cue-elicited craving was also associated with mPFC deactivation, suggesting that craving involves a neural network of co-activation and deactivation of multiple PFC regions. Future research should determine what specific therapeutic approaches are most effective in reducing drug craving mediated by PFC circuits.
6. Final comment
While this review has emphasized the role of PFC in inhibitory control related to drug abuse and other risk-related behaviors, much information about PFC function has been ignored for the sake of clarity. The large human PFC, which accounts for 30% of cortical mass, is inextricably tied to many functions that define the whole human experience, including emotion, attention, reward, rule learning and memory formation. While a long-standing idea is that mPFC and OFC are associated with behavioral inhibition and lPFC is associated with attention and memory (Goldman-Rakic, 1987), this demarcation is more heuristic than precise. Nonetheless, there is a general view that PFC serves as a “top-down” modulator of underlying neural circuits, rather than being in series with underlying neural circuits (Miller and Cohen, 2001). This view suggests that PFC exerts influence on other brain structures, thus integrating behaviorally relevant information that maximizes an adaptive capacity which prevents an overreliance on fixed action patterns, prepotent thoughts and feelings. Without PFC, compulsive behaviors driven by structures in the brainstem (e.g., VTA) would dominate. In the case of drug abuse vulnerability, these well-established neural pathways would be those that subserve addictive behaviors, such as ascending mesocorticolimbic DA projections. Therefore, strengthening the descending projections from the prefrontal cortex that are responsible for inhibitory control could decrease drug abuse vulnerability or the likelihood of relapse, and may be especially important for at-risk individuals. As mentioned in the previous section, a number of strategies for promoting PFC-mediated inhibitory control are emerging. It would be beneficial for these strategies to be employed in a manner that appeals to or targets vulnerable populations (e.g., high sensation seekers, adolescents). Thus, a major challenge in drug abuse prevention and treatment rests with identifying individuals at-risk due to deficits in PFC function and improving intervention strategies aimed at strengthening PFC inhibitory systems among these individuals.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by NIH grants P50 DA05312, R01 DA012964 and R21 DA024401. J. L. Perry was supported by an NIH training grant (T32 DA007304). We acknowledge the assistance of Emily Denehy in preparing the manuscript and Kenn Minter in preparing the anatomical drawings.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Ahern GL, Herring AM, Tackenberg JN, Schwartz GE, Seeger JF, Labiner DM, Weinand ME, Oommen KJ. Affective self-report during the intracarotid sodium amobarbital test. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1994;16:372–376. doi: 10.1080/01688639408402647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alia-Klein N, Goldstein RZ, Tomasi D, Zhang L, Fagin-Jones S, Telang F, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Volkow ND. What is in a word? No versus Yes differentially engage the lateral orbitofrontal cortex. Emotion. 2007;7:649–659. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.3.649. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amiez C, Joseph JP, Procyk E. Reward encoding in the monkey anterior cingulate cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2006;16:1040–1055. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andreason PJ, Zametkin AJ, Guo AC, Baldwin P, Cohen RM. Gender-related differences in regional cerebral glucose metabolism in normal volunteers. Psychiatry Res. 1994;51:175–183. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(94)90037-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andrucci GL, Archer RP, Pancoast DL, Gordon RA. The relationship of MMPI and Sensation Seeking Scales to adolescent drug use. J Pers Assess. 1989;53:253–266. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa5302_4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnsten AF, Cai JX, Goldman-Rakic PS. The alpha-2 adrenergic agonist guanfacine improves memory in aged monkeys without sedative or hypotensive side effects: evidence for alpha-2 receptor subtypes. J Neurosci. 1988;8:4287–4298. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.08-11-04287.1988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnsten AF, Goldman-Rakic PS. Noise stress impairs prefrontal cortical cognitive function in monkeys: evidence for a hyperdopaminergic mechanism. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55:362–368. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.4.362. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnsten AF, Murphy B, Merchant K. The selective dopamine D4 receptor antagonist, PNU-101387G, prevents stress-induced cognitive deficits in monkeys. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2000;23:405–410. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(00)00133-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnsten AF. The Emerging Neurobiology of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: The Key Role of the Prefrontal Association Cortex. J Pediatr. 2009;154 doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2009.01.018. I-S43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aron AR, Poldrack RA. Cortical and subcortical contributions to Stop signal response inhibition: role of the subthalamic nucleus. J Neurosci. 2006;26:2424–2433. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4682-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baeg EH, Jackson ME, Jedema HP, Bradberry CW. Orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate cortex neurons selectively process cocaine-associated environmental cues in the rhesus monkey. J Neurosci. 2009;29:11619–11627. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3206-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baler RD, Volkow ND. Drug addiction: the neurobiology of disrupted self-control. Trends Mol Med. 2006;12:559–566. doi: 10.1016/j.molmed.2006.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ball SA. Personality Traits, Disorders, and Substance Abuse. New York: Elsevier; 2004. Vol. [Google Scholar]
- Balleine BW, Dickinson A. Goal-directed instrumental action: contingency and incentive learning and their cortical substrates. Neuropharmacology. 1998;37:407–419. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3908(98)00033-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Banich MT, Crowley TJ, Thompson LL, Jacobson BL, Liu X, Raymond KM, Claus ED. Brain activation during the Stroop task in adolescents with severe substance and conduct problems: A pilot study. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2007;90:175–182. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.03.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barbas H, Pandya DN. Architecture and intrinsic connections of the prefrontal cortex in the rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol. 1989;286:353–375. doi: 10.1002/cne.902860306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bardo MT, Donohew RL, Harrington NG. Psychobiology of novelty seeking and drug seeking behavior. Behav Brain Res. 1996;77:23–43. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(95)00203-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bardo MT, Klebaur JE, Valone JM, Deaton C. Environmental enrichment decreases intravenous self-administration of amphetamine in female and male rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2001;155:278–284. doi: 10.1007/s002130100720. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnes NM, Sharp T. A review of central 5-HT receptors and their function. Neuropharmacology. 1999;38:1083–1152. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3908(99)00010-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bates JF, Goldman-Rakic PS. Prefrontal connections of medial motor areas in the rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol. 1993;336:211–228. doi: 10.1002/cne.903360205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baumeister RF, Heatherton TR. Self-regulation failure: an overview. Psychol Inq. 1996;7:1–15. [Google Scholar]
- Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW. Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition. 1994;50:7–15. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90018-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Damasio AR. Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science. 1997;275:1293–1295. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5304.1293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bechara A, Damasio H, Damasio AR. Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2000a;10:295–307. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.3.295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H. Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain. 2000b;123((Pt 11)):2189–2202. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.11.2189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bechara A. Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: a neurocognitive perspective. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:1458–1463. doi: 10.1038/nn1584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beck LH, Bransome ED, Jr, Mirsky AF, Rosvold HE, Sarason I. A continuous performance test of brain damage. J Consult Psychol. 1956;20:343–350. doi: 10.1037/h0043220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berridge KC, Robinson TE. What is the role of dopamine in reward: hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience? Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 1998;28:309–369. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(98)00019-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blakemore SJ. The social brain in adolescence. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:267–277. doi: 10.1038/nrn2353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blondeau C, Dellu-Hagedorn F. Dimensional analysis of ADHD subtypes in rats. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;61:1340–1350. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.06.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bock J, Murmu RP, Ferdman N, Leshem M, Braun K. Refinement of dendritic and synaptic networks in the rodent anterior cingulate and orbitofrontal cortex: critical impact of early and late social experience. Dev Neurobiol. 2008;68:685–695. doi: 10.1002/dneu.20622. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bolla K, Ernst M, Kiehl K, Mouratidis M, Eldreth D, Contoreggi C, Matochik J, Kurian V, Cadet J, Kimes A, Funderburk F, London E. Prefrontal cortical dysfunction in abstinent cocaine abusers. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2004a;16:456–464. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.16.4.456. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bolla KI, Eldreth DA, London ED, Kiehl KA, Mouratidis M, Contoreggi C, Matochik JA, Kurian V, Cadet JL, Kimes AS, Funderburk FR, Ernst M. Orbitofrontal cortex dysfunction in abstinent cocaine abusers performing a decision-making task. Neuroimage. 2003;19:1085–1094. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00113-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bolla KI, Eldreth DA, Matochik JA, Cadet JL. Sex-related differences in a gambling task and its neurological correlates. Cereb Cortex. 2004b;14:1226–1232. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh083. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bolla KI, Eldreth DA, Matochik JA, Cadet JL. Neural substrates of faulty decision-making in abstinent marijuana users. Neuroimage. 2005;26:480–492. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bonisch H, Bruss M. The norepinephrine transporter in physiology and disease. Handb Exp Pharmacol. 2006:485–524. doi: 10.1007/3-540-29784-7_20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bonson KR, Grant SJ, Contoreggi CS, Links JM, Metcalfe J, Weyl HL, Kurian V, Ernst M, London ED. Neural systems and cue-induced cocaine craving. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;26:376–386. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00371-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Botvin GJ, Griffin KW. School-based programmes to prevent alcohol, tobacco and other drug use. Int Rev Psychiatry. 2007;19:607–615. doi: 10.1080/09540260701797753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowman RE, Beck KD, Luine VN. Chronic stress effects on memory: sex differences in performance and monoaminergic activity. Horm Behav. 2003;43:48–59. doi: 10.1016/s0018-506x(02)00022-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowman RE, Micik R, Gautreaux C, Fernandez L, Luine VN. Sex-dependent changes in anxiety, memory, and monoamines following one week of stress. Physiol Behav. 2009;97:21–29. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.01.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brailowsky S, Knight RT, Blood K, Scabini D. gamma-Aminobutyric acid-induced potentiation of cortical hemiplegia. Brain Res. 1986;362:322–330. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(86)90457-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Breiter HC, Gollub RL, Weisskoff RM, Kennedy DN, Makris N, Berke JD, Goodman JM, Kantor HL, Gastfriend DR, Riorden JP, Mathew RT, Rosen BR, Hyman SE. Acute effects of cocaine on human brain activity and emotion. Neuron. 1997;19:591–611. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80374-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brody AL, Mandelkern MA, Olmstead RE, Jou J, Tiongson E, Allen V, Scheibal D, London ED, Monterosso JR, Tiffany ST, Korb A, Gan JJ, Cohen MS. Neural substrates of resisting craving during cigarette cue exposure. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62:642–651. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.10.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bromberg MB, Penney JB, Jr, Stephenson BS, Young AB. Evidence for glutamate as the neurotransmitter of corticothalamic and corticorubral pathways. Brain Res. 1981;215:369–374. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(81)90519-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown VJ, Bowman EM. Rodent models of prefrontal cortical function. Trends Neurosci. 2002;25:340–343. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(02)02164-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buck SM, Hillman CH, Castelli DM. The relation of aerobic fitness to stroop task performance in preadolescent children. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2008;40:166–172. doi: 10.1249/mss.0b013e318159b035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bush G, Frazier JA, Rauch SL, Seidman LJ, Whalen PJ, Jenike MA, Rosen BR, Biederman J. Anterior cingulate cortex dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder revealed by fMRI and the Counting Stroop. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;45:1542–1552. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00083-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bylund DB, Eikenberg DC, Hieble JP, Langer SZ, Lefkowitz RJ, Minneman KP, Molinoff PB, Ruffolo RR, Jr, Trendelenburg U. International Union of Pharmacology nomenclature of adrenoceptors. Pharmacol Rev. 1994;46:121–136. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cahill L, Haier RJ, White NS, Fallon J, Kilpatrick L, Lawrence C, Potkin SG, Alkire MT. Sex-related difference in amygdala activity during emotionally influenced memory storage. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2001;75:1–9. doi: 10.1006/nlme.2000.3999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calver AR, Davies CH, Pangalos M. GABA(B) receptors: from monogamy to promiscuity. Neurosignals. 2002;11:299–314. doi: 10.1159/000068257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Camprodon JA, Martinez-Raga J, Alonso-Alonso M, Shih MC, Pascual-Leone A. One session of high frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) to the right prefrontal cortex transiently reduces cocaine craving. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2007;86:91–94. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Capriles N, Rodaros D, Sorge RE, Stewart J. A role for the prefrontal cortex in stress- and cocaine-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2003;168:66–74. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1283-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardinal RN, Pennicott DR, Sugathapala CL, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. Impulsive choice induced in rats by lesions of the nucleus accumbens core. Science. 2001;292:2499–2501. doi: 10.1126/science.1060818. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carli M, Robbins TW, Evenden JL, Everitt BJ. Effects of lesions to ascending noradrenergic neurones on performance of a 5-choice serial reaction task in rats; implications for theories of dorsal noradrenergic bundle function based on selective attention and arousal. Behav Brain Res. 1983;9:361–380. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(83)90138-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carmichael ST, Price JL. Architectonic subdivision of the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex in the macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol. 1994;346:366–402. doi: 10.1002/cne.903460305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Castellanos FX, Tannock R. Neuroscience of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: the search for endophenotypes. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3:617–628. doi: 10.1038/nrn896. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Castner SA, Vosler PS, Goldman-Rakic PS. Amphetamine sensitization impairs cognition and reduces dopamine turnover in primate prefrontal cortex. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57:743–751. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cavedini P, Riboldi G, Keller R, D'Annucci A, Bellodi L. Frontal lobe dysfunction in pathological gambling patients. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;51:334–341. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01227-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chamberlain SR, Muller U, Blackwell AD, Clark L, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. Neurochemical modulation of response inhibition and probabilistic learning in humans. Science. 2006;311:861–863. doi: 10.1126/science.1121218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Charrier D, Thiebot MH. Effects of psychotropic drugs on rat responding in an operant paradigm involving choice between delayed reinforcers. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1996;54:149–157. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(95)02114-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Childress AR, Mozley PD, McElgin W, Fitzgerald J, Reivich M, O'Brien CP. Limbic activation during cue-induced cocaine craving. Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156:11–18. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.1.11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christie MJ, Summers RJ, Stephenson JA, Cook CJ, Beart PM. Excitatory amino acid projections to the nucleus accumbens septi in the rat: a retrograde transport study utilizing D[3H]aspartate and [3H]GABA. Neuroscience. 1987;22:425–439. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(87)90345-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chudasama Y, Muir JL. Visual attention in the rat: a role for the prelimbic cortex and thalamic nuclei? Behav Neurosci. 2001;115:417–428. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chudasama Y, Passetti F, Rhodes SE, Lopian D, Desai A, Robbins TW. Dissociable aspects of performance on the 5-choice serial reaction time task following lesions of the dorsal anterior cingulate, infralimbic and orbitofrontal cortex in the rat: differential effects on selectivity, impulsivity and compulsivity. Behav Brain Res. 2003a;146:105–119. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2003.09.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chudasama Y, Robbins TW. Dissociable contributions of the orbitofrontal and infralimbic cortex to pavlovian autoshaping and discrimination reversal learning: further evidence for the functional heterogeneity of the rodent frontal cortex. J Neurosci. 2003b;23:8771–8780. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-25-08771.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark L, Bechara A, Damasio H, Aitken MR, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW. Differential effects of insular and ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions on risky decision-making. Brain. 2008;131:1311–1322. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conde F, Maire-Lepoivre E, Audinat E, Crepel F. Afferent connections of the medial frontal cortex of the rat. II. Cortical and subcortical afferents. J Comp Neurol. 1995;352:567–593. doi: 10.1002/cne.903520407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conn PJ, Pin JP. Pharmacology and functions of metabotropic glutamate receptors. Annu Rev Pharmacol Toxicol. 1997;37:205–237. doi: 10.1146/annurev.pharmtox.37.1.205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conrod PJ, Stewart SH, Comeau N, Maclean AM. Efficacy of cognitive-behavioral interventions targeting personality risk factors for youth alcohol misuse. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2006;35:550–563. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3504_6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cools R, Clark L, Owen AM, Robbins TW. Defining the neural mechanisms of probabilistic reversal learning using event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci. 2002;22:4563–4567. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-11-04563.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cosgrove KP, Hunter RG, Carroll ME. Wheel-running attenuates intravenous cocaine self-administration in rats: sex differences. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2002;73:663–671. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(02)00853-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coutureau E, Killcross S. Inactivation of the infralimbic prefrontal cortex reinstates goal-directed responding in overtrained rats. Behav Brain Res. 2003;146:167–174. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2003.09.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cragg BG. Plasticity of synapses. Br Med Bull. 1974;30:141–144. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.bmb.a071184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cragg BG. The development of synapses in the visual system of the cat. J Comp Neurol. 1975;160:147–166. doi: 10.1002/cne.901600202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craig AD. Forebrain emotional asymmetry: a neuroanatomical basis? Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9:566–571. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crawford AM, Pentz MA, Chou CP, Li C, Dwyer JH. Parallel developmental trajectories of sensation seeking and regular substance use in adolescents. Psychol Addict Behav. 2003;17:179–192. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.17.3.179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crone EA, Bunge SA, Latenstein H, van der Molen MW. Characterization of children's decision making: sensitivity to punishment frequency, not task complexity. Child Neuropsychol. 2005;11:245–263. doi: 10.1080/092970490911261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cunningham MG, Bhattacharyya S, Benes FM. Amygdalo-cortical sprouting continues into early adulthood: implications for the development of normal and abnormal function during adolescence. J Comp Neurol. 2002;453:116–130. doi: 10.1002/cne.10376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dallery J, Locey ML. Effects of acute and chronic nicotine on impulsive choice in rats. Behav Pharmacol. 2005;16:15–23. doi: 10.1097/00008877-200502000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dalley JW, Theobald DE, Eagle DM, Passetti F, Robbins TW. Deficits in impulse control associated with tonically-elevated serotonergic function in rat prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;26:716–728. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00412-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dalley JW, Cardinal RN, Robbins TW. Prefrontal executive and cognitive functions in rodents: neural and neurochemical substrates. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2004;28:771–784. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.09.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dalley JW, Fryer TD, Brichard L, Robinson ES, Theobald DE, Laane K, Pena Y, Murphy ER, Shah Y, Probst K, Abakumova I, Aigbirhio FI, Richards HK, Hong Y, Baron JC, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Nucleus accumbens D2/3 receptors predict trait impulsivity and cocaine reinforcement. Science. 2007;315:1267–1270. doi: 10.1126/science.1137073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dawe S, Loxton NJ. The role of impulsivity in the development of substance use and eating disorders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2004;28:343–351. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Pascalis V, Arwari B, D'Antuono L, Cacace I. Impulsivity and semantic/emotional processing: an examination of the N400 wave. Clin Neurophysiol. 2009;120:85–92. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.10.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Wit H, Richards JB. Dual determinants of drug use in humans: reward and impulsivity. Nebr Symp Motiv. 2004;50:19–55. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deakin J, Aitken M, Robbins T, Sahakian BJ. Risk taking during decision-making in normal volunteers changes with age. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10:590–598. doi: 10.1017/S1355617704104104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- deCharms RC, Maeda F, Glover GH, Ludlow D, Pauly JM, Soneji D, Gabrieli JD, Mackey SC. Control over brain activation and pain learned by using real-time functional MRI. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005;102:18626–18631. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0505210102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Del Arco A, Mora F. Prefrontal cortex-nucleus accumbens interaction: in vivo modulation by dopamine and glutamate in the prefrontal cortex. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2008;90:226–235. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2008.04.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delatour B, Gisquet-Verrier P. Involvement of the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex in temporal behavioral sequencing: subregional analysis of the medial prefrontal cortex in rat. Behav Brain Res. 2001;126:105–114. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(01)00251-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Denenberg VH. Critical Periods, Stimulus Input, And Emotional Reactivity: A Theory Of Infantile Stimulation. Psychol Rev. 1964;71:335–351. doi: 10.1037/h0042567. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Depue RA, Collins PF. Neurobiology of the structure of personality: dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion. Behav Brain Sci. 1999;22:491–517. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x99002046. discussion 518–469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diergaarde L, Pattij T, Poortvliet I, Hogenboom F, de Vries W, Schoffelmeer AN, De Vries TJ. Impulsive choice and impulsive action predict vulnerability to distinct stages of nicotine seeking in rats. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:301–308. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.07.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dietrich A, Allen JD. Functional dissociation of the prefrontal cortex and the hippocampus in timing behavior. Behav Neurosci. 1998;112:1043–1047. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.112.5.1043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dingledine R. N-methyl aspartate activates voltage-dependent calcium conductance in rat hippocampal pyramidal cells. J Physiol. 1983;343:385–405. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1983.sp014899. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donohew L, Lorch EP, Palmgreen P. Applications of a theoretic model of information exposure to health interventions. Hum Commun Res. 1998;24:454–468. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2958.1998.tb00425.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donohew RL, Lorch EP, Palmgreen P. In: Persuasive communication and drug abuse prevention. Donohew L, Sypher H, Bukoski, editors. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1991. pp. 209–226. Vol. [Google Scholar]
- Dostert PL, Strolin Benedetti M, Tipton KF. Interactions of monoamine oxidase with substrates and inhibitors. Med Res Rev. 1989;9:45–89. doi: 10.1002/med.2610090104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eagle DM, Robbins TW. Inhibitory control in rats performing a stop-signal reaction-time task: effects of lesions of the medial striatum and d-amphetamine. Behav Neurosci. 2003;117:1302–1317. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.117.6.1302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eagle DM, Tufft MR, Goodchild HL, Robbins TW. Differential effects of modafinil and methylphenidate on stop-signal reaction time task performance in the rat, and interactions with the dopamine receptor antagonist cis-flupenthixol. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2007;192:193–206. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-0701-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eldreth DA, Matochik JA, Cadet JL, Bolla KI. Abnormal brain activity in prefrontal brain regions in abstinent marijuana users. Neuroimage. 2004;23:914–920. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ernst M, Kimes AS, London ED, Matochik JA, Eldreth D, Tata S, Contoreggi C, Leff M, Bolla K. Neural substrates of decision making in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:1061–1070. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.6.1061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eshel N, Nelson EE, Blair RJ, Pine DS, Ernst M. Neural substrates of choice selection in adults and adolescents: development of the ventrolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:1270–1279. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evenden JL, Ryan CN. The pharmacology of impulsive behaviour in rats: the effects of drugs on response choice with varying delays of reinforcement. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1996;128:161–170. doi: 10.1007/s002130050121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evenden JL, Ryan CN. The pharmacology of impulsive behaviour in rats VI: the effects of ethanol and selective serotonergic drugs on response choice with varying delays of reinforcement. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1999;146:413–421. doi: 10.1007/pl00005486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Everitt BJ, Hutcheson DM, Ersche KD, Pelloux Y, Dalley JW, Robbins TW. The orbital prefrontal cortex and drug addiction in laboratory animals and humans. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2007;1121:576–597. doi: 10.1196/annals.1401.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farrar AM, Font L, Pereira M, Mingote S, Bunce JG, Chrobak JJ, Salamone JD. Forebrain circuitry involved in effort-related choice: Injections of the GABAA agonist muscimol into ventral pallidum alter response allocation in food-seeking behavior. Neuroscience. 2008;152:321–330. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.12.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fecteau S, Knoch D, Fregni F, Sultani N, Boggio P, Pascual-Leone A. Diminishing risk-taking behavior by modulating activity in the prefrontal cortex: a direct current stimulation study. J Neurosci. 2007;27:12500–12505. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3283-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fillmore MT, Rush CR, Kelly TH, Hays L. Triazolam impairs inhibitory control of behavior in humans. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2001;9:363–371. doi: 10.1037//1064-1297.9.4.363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fillmore MT, Rush CR. Impaired inhibitory control of behavior in chronic cocaine users. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2002;66:265–273. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(01)00206-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fillmore MT. Drug abuse as a problem of impaired control: current approaches and findings. Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev. 2003;2:179–197. doi: 10.1177/1534582303257007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fillmore MT, Weafer J. Alcohol impairment of behavior in men and women. Addiction. 2004;99:1237–1246. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2004.00805.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fillmore MT, Ostling EW, Martin CA, Kelly TH. Acute effects of alcohol on inhibitory control and information processing in high and low sensation-seekers. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2009;100:91–99. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.09.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fishbein D. The importance of neurobiological research to the prevention of psychopathology. Prev Sci. 2000;1:89–106. doi: 10.1023/a:1010090114858. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fishbein DH, Eldreth DL, Hyde C, Matochik JA, London ED, Contoreggi C, Kurian V, Kimes AS, Breeden A, Grant S. Risky decision making and the anterior cingulate cortex in abstinent drug abusers and nonusers. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2005;23:119–136. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.12.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fishbein DH, Hyde C, Eldreth D, Paschall MJ, Hubal R, Das A, Tarter R, Ialongo N, Hubbard S, Yung B. Neurocognitive skills moderate urban male adolescents' responses to preventive intervention materials. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2006;82:47–60. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2005.08.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fishbein DH, Krupitsky E, Flannery BA, Langevin DJ, Bobashev G, Verbitskaya E, Augustine CB, Bolla KI, Zvartau E, Schech B, Egorova V, Bushara N, Tsoy M. Neurocognitive characterizations of Russian heroin addicts without a significant history of other drug use. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2007;90:25–38. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.02.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher PA, Gunnar MR, Chamberlain P, Reid JB. Preventive intervention for maltreated preschool children: impact on children's behavior, neuroendocrine activity, and foster parent functioning. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2000;39:1356–1364. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200011000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher PA, Stoolmiller M, Gunnar MR, Burraston BO. Effects of a therapeutic intervention for foster preschoolers on diurnal cortisol activity. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2007;32:892–905. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.06.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fletcher PJ, Tampakeras M, Sinyard J, Higgins GA. Opposing effects of 5-HT(2A) and 5-HT(2C) receptor antagonists in the rat and mouse on premature responding in the five-choice serial reaction time test. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2007;195:223–234. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-0891-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Floresco SB. Dopaminergic regulation of limbic-striatal interplay. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2007;32:400–411. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Floresco SB, Tse MT, Ghods-Sharifi S. Dopaminergic and glutamatergic regulation of effort- and delay-based decision making. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:1966–1979. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Floyd NS, Price JL, Ferry AT, Keay KA, Bandler R. Orbitomedial prefrontal cortical projections to distinct longitudinal columns of the periaqueductal gray in the rat. J Comp Neurol. 2000;422:556–578. doi: 10.1002/1096-9861(20000710)422:4<556::aid-cne6>3.0.co;2-u. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Floyd NS, Price JL, Ferry AT, Keay KA, Bandler R. Orbitomedial prefrontal cortical projections to hypothalamus in the rat. J Comp Neurol. 2001;432:307–328. doi: 10.1002/cne.1105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fonnum F, Soreide A, Kvale I, Walker J, Walaas I. Glutamate in cortical fibers. Adv Biochem Psychopharmacol. 1981a;27:29–41. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fonnum F, Storm-Mathisen J, Divac I. Biochemical evidence for glutamate as neurotransmitter in corticostriatal and corticothalamic fibres in rat brain. Neuroscience. 1981b;6:863–873. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(81)90168-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forbush KT, Shaw M, Graeber MA, Hovick L, Meyer VJ, Moser DJ, Bayless J, Watson D, Black DW. Neuropsychological characteristics and personality traits in pathological gambling. CNS Spectr. 2008;13:306–315. doi: 10.1017/s1092852900016424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frankle WG, Lombardo I, New AS, Goodman M, Talbot PS, Huang Y, Hwang DR, Slifstein M, Curry S, Abi-Dargham A, Laruelle M, Siever LJ. Brain serotonin transporter distribution in subjects with impulsive aggressivity: a positron emission study with [11C]McN 5652. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162:915–923. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.5.915. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franklin TR, Acton PD, Maldjian JA, Gray JD, Croft JR, Dackis CA, O'Brien CP, Childress AR. Decreased gray matter concentration in the insular, orbitofrontal, cingulate, and temporal cortices of cocaine patients. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;51:134–142. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01269-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franowicz JS, Kessler LE, Borja CM, Kobilka BK, Limbird LE, Arnsten AF. Mutation of the alpha2A–adrenoceptor impairs working memory performance and annuls cognitive enhancement by guanfacine. J Neurosci. 2002;22:8771–8777. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-19-08771.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frysztak RJ, Neafsey EJ. The effect of medial frontal cortex lesions on respiration, "freezing," and ultrasonic vocalizations during conditioned emotional responses in rats. Cereb Cortex. 1991;1:418–425. doi: 10.1093/cercor/1.5.418. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuchs RA, Evans KA, Ledford CC, Parker MP, Case JM, Mehta RH, See RE. The role of the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, basolateral amygdala, and dorsal hippocampus in contextual reinstatement of cocaine seeking in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30:296–309. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuster JM. The prefrontal cortex. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2008. Vol. [Google Scholar]
- Galvan A, Hare TA, Parra CE, Penn J, Voss H, Glover G, Casey BJ. Earlier development of the accumbens relative to orbitofrontal cortex might underlie risk-taking behavior in adolescents. J Neurosci. 2006;26:6885–6892. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1062-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garavan H, Ross TJ, Murphy K, Roche RA, Stein EA. Dissociable executive functions in the dynamic control of behavior: inhibition, error detection, and correction. Neuroimage. 2002;17:1820–1829. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garavan H, Kaufman JN, Hester R. Acute effects of cocaine on the neurobiology of cognitive control. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2008;363:3267–3276. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garon N, Moore C. Complex decision-making in early childhood. Brain Cogn. 2004;55:158–170. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00272-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giedd JN, Blumenthal J, Jeffries NO, Castellanos FX, Liu H, Zijdenbos A, Paus T, Evans AC, Rapoport JL. Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:861–863. doi: 10.1038/13158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gipson CD, Bardo MT. Effect of varied access to d-amphetamine self-administration on impulsive choice in a delay discounting task in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2009;207:391–400. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1667-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gogtay N, Giedd JN, Lusk L, Hayashi KM, Greenstein D, Vaituzis AC, Nugent TF, 3rd, Herman DH, Clasen LS, Toga AW, Rapoport JL, Thompson PM. Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101:8174–8179. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0402680101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldman-Rakic PS. Motor control function of the prefrontal cortex. Ciba Found Symp. 1987;132:187–200. doi: 10.1002/9780470513545.ch12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldstein LE, Rasmusson AM, Bunney BS, Roth RH. Role of the amygdala in the coordination of behavioral, neuroendocrine, and prefrontal cortical monoamine responses to psychological stress in the rat. J Neurosci. 1996;16:4787–4798. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-15-04787.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant S, London ED, Newlin DB, Villemagne VL, Liu X, Contoreggi C, Phillips RL, Kimes AS, Margolin A. Activation of memory circuits during cue-elicited cocaine craving. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1996;93:12040–12045. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.21.12040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant S, Contoreggi C, London ED. Drug abusers show impaired performance in a laboratory test of decision making. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38:1180–1187. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(99)00158-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Groenewegen HJ. Organization of the afferent connections of the mediodorsal thalamic nucleus in the rat, related to the mediodorsal-prefrontal topography. Neuroscience. 1988;24:379–431. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(88)90339-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Groenewegen HJ, Wright CI, Uylings HB. The anatomical relationships of the prefrontal cortex with limbic structures and the basal ganglia. J Psychopharmacol. 1997;11:99–106. doi: 10.1177/026988119701100202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gunnar MR, Fisher PA. Bringing basic research on early experience and stress neurobiology to bear on preventive interventions for neglected and maltreated children. Dev Psychopathol. 2006;18:651–677. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gur RC, Mozley LH, Mozley PD, Resnick SM, Karp JS, Alavi A, Arnold SE, Gur RE. Sex differences in regional cerebral glucose metabolism during a resting state. Science. 1995;267:528–531. doi: 10.1126/science.7824953. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hadamitzky M, Koch M. Effects of acute intra-cerebral administration of the 5-HT(2A/C) receptor ligands DOI and ketanserin on impulse control in rats. Behav Brain Res. 2009;204:88–92. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.05.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hampton AN, Bossaerts P, O'Doherty JP. The role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in abstract state-based inference during decision making in humans. J Neurosci. 2006;26:8360–8367. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1010-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heidbreder CA, Groenewegen HJ. The medial prefrontal cortex in the rat: evidence for a dorso-ventral distinction based upon functional and anatomical characteristics. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2003;27:555–579. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2003.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Helen CF, Keri LB, Peihua G, Rajita S. Interactive effects of cumulative stress and impulsivity on alcohol consumption. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2010;34:1376–1385. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01221.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heyman GM, Gibb SP. Delay discounting in college cigarette chippers. Behav Pharmacol. 2006;17:669–679. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e3280116cfe. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Higgins GA, Enderlin M, Haman M, Fletcher PJ. The 5-HT2A receptor antagonist M100,907 attenuates motor and 'impulsive-type' behaviours produced by NMDA receptor antagonism. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2003;170:309–319. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1549-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoerst M, Weber-Fahr W, Tunc-Skarka N, Ruf M, Bohus M, Schmahl C, Ende G. Correlation of Glutamate Levels in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex With Self-reported Impulsivity in Patients With Borderline Personality Disorder and Healthy Controls. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010 doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holroyd CB, Coles MG. The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychol Rev. 2002;109:679–709. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.109.4.679. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hornak J, O'Doherty J, Bramham J, Rolls ET, Morris RG, Bullock PR, Polkey CE. Reward-related reversal learning after surgical excisions in orbito-frontal or dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in humans. J Cogn Neurosci. 2004;16:463–478. doi: 10.1162/089892904322926791. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoyer D, Hannon JP, Martin GR. Molecular, pharmacological and functional diversity of 5-HT receptors. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2002;71:533–554. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(01)00746-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hutcheson DM, Everitt BJ. The effects of selective orbitofrontal cortex lesions on the acquisition and performance of cue-controlled cocaine seeking in rats. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2003;1003:410–411. doi: 10.1196/annals.1300.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hyman SE, Malenka RC, Nestler EJ. Neural mechanisms of addiction: the role of reward-related learning and memory. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2006;29:565–598. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.113009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Inzlicht M, Gutsell JN. Running on empty: neural signals for self-control failure. Psychol Sci. 2007;18:933–937. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02004.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ito S, Stuphorn V, Brown JW, Schall JD. Performance monitoring by the anterior cingulate cortex during saccade countermanding. Science. 2003;302:120–122. doi: 10.1126/science.1087847. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Izquierdo A, Suda RK, Murray EA. Bilateral orbital prefrontal cortex lesions in rhesus monkeys disrupt choices guided by both reward value and reward contingency. J Neurosci. 2004;24:7540–7548. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1921-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jakala P, Riekkinen M, Sirvio J, Koivisto E, Riekkinen P., Jr Clonidine, but not guanfacine, impairs choice reaction time performance in young healthy volunteers. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1999a;21:495–502. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(99)00048-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jakala P, Sirvio J, Riekkinen M, Koivisto E, Kejonen K, Vanhanen M, Riekkinen P., Jr Guanfacine and clonidine, alpha 2-agonists, improve paired associates learning, but not delayed matching to sample, in humans. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1999b;20:119–130. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(98)00055-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jentsch JD, Taylor JR. Impulsivity resulting from frontostriatal dysfunction in drug abuse: implications for the control of behavior by reward-related stimuli. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1999;146:373–390. doi: 10.1007/pl00005483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jentsch JD, Olausson P, De La Garza R, 2nd, Taylor JR. Impairments of reversal learning and response perseveration after repeated, intermittent cocaine administrations to monkeys. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;26:183–190. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00355-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jiang Y, Lianekhammy J, Lawson A, Guo C, Lynam D, Joseph JE, Gold BT, Kelly TH. Brain responses to repeated visual experience among low and high sensation seekers: role of boredom susceptibility. Psychiatry Res. 2009;173:100–106. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.09.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joel D, Doljansky J, Roz N, Rehavi M. Role of the orbital cortex and of the serotonergic system in a rat model of obsessive compulsive disorder. Neuroscience. 2005a;130:25–36. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.08.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joel D, Doljansky J, Schiller D. ‘Compulsive’ lever pressing in rats is enhanced following lesions to the orbital cortex, but not to the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala or to the dorsal medial prefrontal cortex. Eur J Neurosci. 2005b;21:2252–2262. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04042.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joseph JE, Liu X, Jiang Y, Lynam D, Kelly TH. Neural correlates of emotional reactivity in sensation seeking. Psychol Sci. 2009;20:215–223. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02283.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalivas PW, Volkow ND. The neural basis of addiction: a pathology of motivation and choice. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162:1403–1413. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.8.1403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalivas PW, Peters J, Knackstedt L. Animal models and brain circuits in drug addiction. Mol Interv. 2006;6:339–344. doi: 10.1124/mi.6.6.7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalivas PW. The glutamate homeostasis hypothesis of addiction. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10:561–572. doi: 10.1038/nrn2515. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelley AE, Berridge KC. The neuroscience of natural rewards: relevance to addictive drugs. J Neurosci. 2002;22:3306–3311. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-09-03306.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelly TH, Robbins G, Martin CA, Fillmore MT, Lane SD, Harrington NG, Rush CR. Individual differences in drug abuse vulnerability: d-amphetamine and sensation-seeking status. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2006;189:17–25. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0487-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kennerley SW, Wallis JD. Reward-dependent modulation of working memory in lateral prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 2009;29:3259–3270. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5353-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kerns JG, Cohen JD, MacDonald AW, 3rd, Cho RY, Stenger VA, Carter CS. Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science. 2004;303:1023–1026. doi: 10.1126/science.1089910. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kerr A, Zelazo PD. Development of 'hot' executive function: the children's gambling task. Brain Cogn. 2004;55:148–157. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00275-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Killcross S, Coutureau E. Coordination of actions and habits in the medial prefrontal cortex of rats. Cereb Cortex. 2003;13:400–408. doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.4.400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirby KN, Marakovic NN. Delay-Discounting probabilistic rewards: Rates decrease as amounts increase. Psychon Bull Rev. 1996;3:100–104. doi: 10.3758/BF03210748. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kita H, Kitai ST. Amygdaloid projections to the frontal cortex and the striatum in the rat. J Comp Neurol. 1990;298:40–49. doi: 10.1002/cne.902980104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knoch D, Gianotti LR, Pascual-Leone A, Treyer V, Regard M, Hohmann M, Brugger P. Disruption of right prefrontal cortex by low-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation induces risk-taking behavior. J Neurosci. 2006;26:6469–6472. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0804-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koski L, Paus T. Functional connectivity of the anterior cingulate cortex within the human frontal lobe: a brain-mapping meta-analysis. Exp Brain Res. 2000;133:55–65. doi: 10.1007/s002210000400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koskinen T, Ruotsalainen S, Puumala T, Lappalainen R, Koivisto E, Mannisto PT, Sirvio J. Activation of 5-HT2A receptors impairs response control of rats in a five-choice serial reaction time task. Neuropharmacology. 2000;39:471–481. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3908(99)00159-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koskinen T, Sirvio J. Studies on the involvement of the dopaminergic system in the 5-HT2 agonist (DOI)-induced premature responding in a five-choice serial reaction time task. Brain Res Bull. 2001;54:65–75. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(00)00425-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koskinen T, Haapalinna A, Sirvio J. Alpha-adrenoceptor-mediated modulation of 5-HT2 receptor agonist induced impulsive responding in a 5-choice serial reaction time task. Pharmacol Toxicol. 2003;92:214–225. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0773.2003.920504.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kosten TA, Ball SA, Rounsaville BJ. A sibling study of sensation seeking and opiate addiction. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1994;182:284–289. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199405000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krettek JE, Price JL. The cortical projections of the mediodorsal nucleus and adjacent thalamic nuclei in the rat. J Comp Neurol. 1977;171:157–191. doi: 10.1002/cne.901710204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kringelbach ML, Rolls ET. The functional neuroanatomy of the human orbitofrontal cortex: evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychology. Prog Neurobiol. 2004;72:341–372. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2004.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krystal JH, D'Souza DC, Mathalon D, Perry E, Belger A, Hoffman R. NMDA receptor antagonist effects, cortical glutamatergic function, and schizophrenia: toward a paradigm shift in medication development. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2003;169:215–233. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1582-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lambe EK, Krimer LS, Goldman-Rakic PS. Differential postnatal development of catecholamine and serotonin inputs to identified neurons in prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkey. J Neurosci. 2000;20:8780–8787. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-23-08780.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lamm C, Zelazo PD, Lewis MD. Neural correlates of cognitive control in childhood and adolescence: disentangling the contributions of age and executive function. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44:2139–2148. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.10.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lane SD, Tcheremissine OV, Lieving LM, Nouvion S, Cherek DR. Acute effects of alprazolam on risky decision making in humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005;181:364–373. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-2265-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Langleben DD, Loughead JW, Ruparel K, Hakun JG, Busch-Winokur S, Holloway MB, Strasser AA, Cappella JN, Lerman C. Reduced prefrontal and temporal processing and recall of high "sensation value" ads. Neuroimage. 2009;46:219–225. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lasseter HC, Ramirez DR, Xie X, Fuchs RA. Involvement of the lateral orbitofrontal cortex in drug context-induced reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behavior in rats. Eur J Neurosci. 2009;30:1370–1381. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06906.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Le AD, Funk D, Harding S, Juzytsch W, Li Z, Fletcher PJ. Intra-median raphe nucleus (MRN) infusions of muscimol, a GABA-A receptor agonist, reinstate alcohol seeking in rats: role of impulsivity and reward. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2008;195:605–615. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-0943-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lebron K, Milad MR, Quirk GJ. Delayed recall of fear extinction in rats with lesions of ventral medial prefrontal cortex. Learn Mem. 2004;11:544–548. doi: 10.1101/lm.78604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levesque J, Joanette Y, Mensour B, Beaudoin G, Leroux JM, Bourgouin P, Beauregard M. Neural basis of emotional self-regulation in childhood. Neuroscience. 2004;129:361–369. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.07.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewis MD, Stieben J. Emotion regulation in the brain: conceptual issues and directions for developmental research. Child Dev. 2004;75:371–376. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00680.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li CS, Huang C, Constable RT, Sinha R. Gender differences in the neural correlates of response inhibition during a stop signal task. Neuroimage. 2006;32:1918–1929. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.05.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liao PC, Uher R, Lawrence N, Treasure J, Schmidt U, Campbell IC, Collier DA, Tchanturia K. An examination of decision making in bulimia nervosa. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2009;31:455–461. doi: 10.1080/13803390802251378. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lidov HG, Molliver ME. Immunohistochemical study of the development of serotonergic neurons in the rat CNS. Brain Res Bull. 1982;9:559–604. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(82)90164-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin Y, Ter Horst GJ, Wichmann R, Bakker P, Liu A, Li X, Westenbroek C. Sex differences in the effects of acute and chronic stress and recovery after long-term stress on stress-related brain regions of rats. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19:1978–1989. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lishman WA. Cerebral beriberi (Wernicke's encephalopathy) J Psychosom Res. 1998;44:631–632. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(98)00037-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu X, Powell DK, Wang H, Gold BT, Corbly CR, Joseph JE. Functional dissociation in frontal and striatal areas for processing of positive and negative reward information. J Neurosci. 2007;27:4587–4597. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5227-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu YP, Wilkinson LS, Robbins TW. Effects of acute and chronic buspirone on impulsive choice and efflux of 5-HT and dopamine in hippocampus, nucleus accumbens and prefrontal cortex. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2004;173:175–185. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1726-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luders E, Narr KL, Thompson PM, Rex DE, Jancke L, Steinmetz H, Toga AW. Gender differences in cortical complexity. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7:799–800. doi: 10.1038/nn1277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lyons DM, Afarian H, Schatzberg AF, Sawyer-Glover A, Moseley ME. Experience-dependent asymmetric variation in primate prefrontal morphology. Behav Brain Res. 2002;136:51–59. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(02)00100-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mai JK, Assheuer J, Paxinos G. Atlas of the Human Brain. Vol. San Diego: Academic Press; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Mamounas LA, Mullen CA, O'Hearn E, Molliver ME. Dual serotoninergic projections to forebrain in the rat: morphologically distinct 5-HT axon terminals exhibit differential vulnerability to neurotoxic amphetamine derivatives. J Comp Neurol. 1991;314:558–586. doi: 10.1002/cne.903140312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mandyam CD, Wee S, Eisch AJ, Richardson HN, Koob GF. Methamphetamine self-administration and voluntary exercise have opposing effects on medial prefrontal cortex gliogenesis. J Neurosci. 2007;27:11442–11450. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2505-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manes F, Sahakian B, Clark L, Rogers R, Antoun N, Aitken M, Robbins T. Decision-making processes following damage to the prefrontal cortex. Brain. 2002;125:624–639. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maren S, Quirk GJ. Neuronal signalling of fear memory. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5:844–852. doi: 10.1038/nrn1535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Margeta-Mitrovic M, Mitrovic I, Riley RC, Jan LY, Basbaum AI. Immunohistochemical localization of GABA(B) receptors in the rat central nervous system. J Comp Neurol. 1999;405:299–321. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(19990315)405:3<299::aid-cne2>3.0.co;2-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markham JA, Morris JR, Juraska JM. Neuron number decreases in the rat ventral, but not dorsal, medial prefrontal cortex between adolescence and adulthood. Neuroscience. 2007;144:961–968. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin SB, Covell DJ, Joseph JE, Chebrolu H, Smith CD, Kelly TH, Jiang Y, Gold BT. Human experience seeking correlates with hippocampus volume: convergent evidence from manual tracing and voxel-based morphometry. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:2874–2881. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.05.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Masaki D, Yokoyama C, Kinoshita S, Tsuchida H, Nakatomi Y, Yoshimoto K, Fukui K. Relationship between limbic and cortical 5-HT neurotransmission and acquisition and reversal learning in a go/no-go task in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2006;189:249–258. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0559-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matochik JA, London ED, Eldreth DA, Cadet JL, Bolla KI. Frontal cortical tissue composition in abstinent cocaine abusers: a magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroimage. 2003;19:1095–1102. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00244-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matsumura M, Sawaguchi T, Kubota K. GABAergic inhibition of neuronal activity in the primate motor and premotor cortex during voluntary movement. J Neurophysiol. 1992;68:692–702. doi: 10.1152/jn.1992.68.3.692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McFarland K, Kalivas PW. The circuitry mediating cocaine-induced reinstatement of drug-seeking behavior. J Neurosci. 2001;21:8655–8663. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-21-08655.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McFarland K, Davidge SB, Lapish CC, Kalivas PW. Limbic and motor circuitry underlying footshock-induced reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behavior. J Neurosci. 2004;24:1551–1560. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4177-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLaughlin J, See RE. Selective inactivation of the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex and the basolateral amygdala attenuates conditioned-cued reinstatement of extinguished cocaine-seeking behavior in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2003;168:57–65. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1196-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meaney MJ, Diorio J, Francis D, Widdowson J, LaPlante P, Caldji C, Sharma S, Seckl JR, Plotsky PM. Early environmental regulation of forebrain glucocorticoid receptor gene expression: implications for adrenocortical responses to stress. Dev Neurosci. 1996;18:49–72. doi: 10.1159/000111395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meunier M, Bachevalier J, Mishkin M. Effects of orbital frontal and anterior cingulate lesions on object and spatial memory in rhesus monkeys. Neuropsychologia. 1997;35:999–1015. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(97)00027-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller EK, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2001;24:167–202. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Milstein JA, Lehmann O, Theobald DE, Dalley JW, Robbins TW. Selective depletion of cortical noradrenaline by anti-dopamine beta-hydroxylase-saporin impairs attentional function and enhances the effects of guanfacine in the rat. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2007;190:51–63. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0594-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Milstein JA, Dalley JW, Robbins TW. Methylphenidate-induced impulsivity: pharmacological antagonism by beta-adrenoreceptor blockade. J Psychopharmacol. 2010;24:309–321. doi: 10.1177/0269881108098146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mirjana C, Baviera M, Invernizzi RW, Balducci C. The serotonin 5-HT2A receptors antagonist M100907 prevents impairment in attentional performance by NMDA receptor blockade in the rat prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:1637–1647. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300479. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Monterosso JR, Aron AR, Cordova X, Xu J, London ED. Deficits in response inhibition associated with chronic methamphetamine abuse. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2005;79:273–277. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2005.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morgan MA, Romanski LM, LeDoux JE. Extinction of emotional learning: contribution of medial prefrontal cortex. Neurosci Lett. 1993;163:109–113. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(93)90241-c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morgan MA, LeDoux JE. Differential contribution of dorsal and ventral medial prefrontal cortex to the acquisition and extinction of conditioned fear in rats. Behav Neurosci. 1995;109:681–688. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.109.4.681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muir JL, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. The cerebral cortex of the rat and visual attentional function: dissociable effects of mediofrontal, cingulate, anterior dorsolateral, and parietal cortex lesions on a five-choice serial reaction time task. Cereb Cortex. 1996;6:470–481. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.3.470. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muraven M, Tice DM, Baumeister RF. Self-control as limited resource: regulatory depletion patterns. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1998;74:774–789. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.74.3.774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy BL, Arnsten AF, Goldman-Rakic PS, Roth RH. Increased dopamine turnover in the prefrontal cortex impairs spatial working memory performance in rats and monkeys. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1996;93:1325–1329. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.3.1325. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy BL, Roth RH, Arnsten AF. Clozapine reverses the spatial working memory deficits induced by FG7142 in monkeys. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1997;16:433–437. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(97)00019-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy ER, Dalley JW, Robbins TW. Local glutamate receptor antagonism in the rat prefrontal cortex disrupts response inhibition in a visuospatial attentional task. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005;179:99–107. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-2068-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray EA, O'Doherty JP, Schoenbaum G. What we know and do not know about the functions of the orbitofrontal cortex after 20 years of cross-species studies. J Neurosci. 2007;27:8166–8169. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1556-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Naqvi NH, Bechara A. The hidden island of addiction: the insula. Trends Neurosci. 2008 doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.09.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nederkoorn C, Braet C, Van Eijs Y, Tanghe A, Jansen A. Why obese children cannot resist food: the role of impulsivity. Eat Behav. 2006;7:315–322. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2005.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ofen N, Kao YC, Sokol-Hessner P, Kim H, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Gabrieli JD. Development of the declarative memory system in the human brain. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10:1198–1205. doi: 10.1038/nn1950. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olausson P, Jentsch JD, Krueger DD, Tronson NC, Nairn AC, Taylor JR. Orbitofrontal cortex and cognitive-motivational impairments in psychostimulant addiction: evidence from experiments in the non-human primate. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2007;1121:610–638. doi: 10.1196/annals.1401.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olson EA, Collins PF, Hooper CJ, Muetzel R, Lim KO, Luciana M. White matter integrity predicts delay discounting behavior in 9- to 23-year-olds: a diffusion tensor imaging study. J Cogn Neurosci. 2009;21:1406–1421. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ongur D, Price JL. The organization of networks within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys and humans. Cereb Cortex. 2000;10:206–219. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.3.206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Overman WH. Sex differences in early childhood, adolescence, and adulthood on cognitive tasks that rely on orbital prefrontal cortex. Brain Cogn. 2004;55:134–147. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00279-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paine TA, Tomasiewicz HC, Zhang K, Carlezon WA., Jr Sensitivity of the five-choice serial reaction time task to the effects of various psychotropic drugs in Sprague-Dawley rats. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62:687–693. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.11.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palmgreen P, Donohew L, Lorch EP, Hoyle RH, Stephenson MT. Television campaigns and adolescent marijuana use: tests of sensation seeking targeting. Am J Public Health. 2001;91:292–296. doi: 10.2105/ajph.91.2.292. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palmgreen P, Donohew L. Effective mass media strategies for drug abuse prevention campaigns. In: Bukoski WJ, Sloboda Z, editors. Handbook of drug abuse theory, science and practice. Vol. New York: Plenum; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Palmgreen P, Lorch EP, Stephenson MT, Hoyle RH, Donohew L. Effects of the Office of National Drug Control Policy's Marijuana Initiative Campaign on high-sensation-seeking adolescents. Am J Public Health. 2007;97:1644–1649. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2005.072843. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Papez JW. Visceral brain, its component parts and their connections. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1958;126:40–56. doi: 10.1097/00005053-195801000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Passetti F, Chudasama Y, Robbins TW. The frontal cortex of the rat and visual attentional performance: dissociable functions of distinct medial prefrontal subregions. Cereb Cortex. 2002;12:1254–1268. doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.12.1254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Passetti F, Dalley JW, Robbins TW. Double dissociation of serotonergic and dopaminergic mechanisms on attentional performance using a rodent five-choice reaction time task. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2003;165:136–145. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1227-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pattij T, Vanderschuren LJ. The neuropharmacology of impulsive behaviour. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 2008;29:192–199. doi: 10.1016/j.tips.2008.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paulus MP, Hozack N, Frank L, Brown GG. Error rate and outcome predictability affect neural activation in prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate during decision-making. Neuroimage. 2002;15:836–846. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.1031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paulus MP, Hozack N, Frank L, Brown GG, Schuckit MA. Decision making by methamphetamine-dependent subjects is associated with error-rate-independent decrease in prefrontal and parietal activation. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;53:65–74. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01442-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paus T, Zijdenbos A, Worsley K, Collins DL, Blumenthal J, Giedd JN, Rapoport JL, Evans AC. Structural maturation of neural pathways in children and adolescents: in vivo study. Science. 1999;283:1908–1911. doi: 10.1126/science.283.5409.1908. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paxinos G, Watson C. The Rat Brain In Stereotaxic Coordinates. Vol. Amsterdam: Academic Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Perry JL, Larson EB, German JP, Madden GJ, Carroll ME. Impulsivity(delay discounting) as a predictor of acquisition of IV cocaine self-administration in female rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005;178:193–201. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-1994-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perry JL, Carroll ME. The role of impulsive behavior in drug abuse. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2008 doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1173-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perry JL, Nelson SE, Carroll ME. Impulsive choice as a predictor of acquisition of IV cocaine self- administration and reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behavior in male and female rats. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2008;16:165–177. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.16.2.165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peters J, Vallone J, Laurendi K, Kalivas PW. Opposing roles for the ventral prefrontal cortex and the basolateral amygdala on the spontaneous recovery of cocaine-seeking in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2008;197:319–326. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-1034-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petrides M, Pandya DN. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: comparative cytoarchitectonic analysis in the human and the macaque brain and corticocortical connection patterns. Eur J Neurosci. 1999;11:1011–1036. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00518.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Posner MI, Rothbart MK, Sheese BE, Tang Y. The anterior cingulate gyrus and the mechanism of self-regulation. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2007;7:391–395. doi: 10.3758/cabn.7.4.391. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Posse S, Fitzgerald D, Gao K, Habel U, Rosenberg D, Moore GJ, Schneider F. Real-time fMRI of temporolimbic regions detects amygdala activation during single-trial self-induced sadness. Neuroimage. 2003;18:760–768. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00004-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Preuss TM. Do rats have prefrontal cortex? The Rose-Woolsey-Akert program reconsidered. J Cogn Neurosci. 1995;7:1–24. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1995.7.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Price JL. Beyond the primary olfactory cortex: olfactory-related areas in the neocortex, thalamus, and hypothalamus. Chem Senses. 1985;10:235–258. [Google Scholar]
- Price JL. Definition of the orbital cortex in relation to specific connections with limbic and visceral structures and other cortical regions. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2007;1121:54–71. doi: 10.1196/annals.1401.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Puumala T, Sirvio J. Changes in activities of dopamine and serotonin systems in the frontal cortex underlie poor choice accuracy and impulsivity of rats in an attention task. Neuroscience. 1998;83:489–499. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(97)00392-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quirk GJ, Russo GK, Barron JL, Lebron K. The role of ventromedial prefrontal cortex in the recovery of extinguished fear. J Neurosci. 2000;20:6225–6231. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-16-06225.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ragozzino ME, Detrick S, Kesner RP. Involvement of the prelimbic-infralimbic areas of the rodent prefrontal cortex in behavioral flexibility for place and response learning. J Neurosci. 1999;19:4585–4594. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-11-04585.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rama P, Linnankoski I, Tanila H, Pertovaara A, Carlson S. Medetomidine, atipamezole, and guanfacine in delayed response performance of aged monkeys. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1996;55:415–422. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(96)00111-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ray JP, Price JL. The organization of the thalamocortical connections of the mediodorsal thalamic nucleus in the rat, related to the ventral forebrain-prefrontal cortex topography. J Comp Neurol. 1992;323:167–197. doi: 10.1002/cne.903230204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ray R, Loughead J, Wang Z, Detre J, Yang E, Gur R, Lerman C. Neuroimaging, genetics and the treatment of nicotine addiction. Behav Brain Res. 2008;193:159–169. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.05.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reavis R, Overman WH. Adult sex differences on a decision-making task previously shown to depend on the orbital prefrontal cortex. Behav Neurosci. 2001;115:196–206. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.115.1.196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Renner MJ, Rosenzweig MR. The golden-mantled ground squirrel (Spermophilus lateralis) as a model for the effects of environmental enrichment in solitary animals. Dev Psychobiol. 1987;20:19–24. doi: 10.1002/dev.420200106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reynolds B, Richards JB, Dassinger M, de Wit H. Therapeutic doses of diazepam do not alter impulsive behavior in humans. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2004;79:17–24. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2004.06.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reynolds B, Richards JB, de Wit H. Acute-alcohol effects on the Experiential Discounting Task (EDT) and a question-based measure of delay discounting. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2006;83:194–202. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2006.01.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richards JB, Sabol KE, de Wit H. Effects of methamphetamine on the adjusting amount procedure, a model of impulsive behavior in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1999;146:432–439. doi: 10.1007/pl00005488. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ridderinkhof KR, Ullsperger M, Crone EA, Nieuwenhuis S. The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science. 2004a;306:443–447. doi: 10.1126/science.1100301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ridderinkhof KR, van den Wildenberg WP, Segalowitz SJ, Carter CS. Neurocognitive mechanisms of cognitive control: the role of prefrontal cortex in action selection, response inhibition, performance monitoring, and reward-based learning. Brain Cogn. 2004b;56:129–140. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.09.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson DS, Gilmor ML, Yang Y, Moonsammy G, Azzaro AJ, Oren DA, Campbell BJ. Treatment effects of selegiline transdermal system on symptoms of major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis of short-term, placebo-controlled, efficacy trials. Psychopharmacol Bull. 2007;40:15–28. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson ES, Dalley JW, Theobald DE, Glennon JC, Pezze MA, Murphy ER, Robbins TW. Opposing roles for 5-HT2A and 5-HT2C receptors in the nucleus accumbens on inhibitory response control in the 5-choice serial reaction time task. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008a;33:2398–2406. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson ES, Eagle DM, Mar AC, Bari A, Banerjee G, Jiang X, Dalley JW, Robbins TW. Similar effects of the selective noradrenaline reuptake inhibitor atomoxetine on three distinct forms of impulsivity in the rat. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008b;33:1028–1037. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301487. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rose JE, Woolsey CN. The orbitofrontal cortex and its connections with the mediodorsal nucleus in rabbit, sheep and cat. Res Publ Assoc Res Nerv Ment Dis. 1948;27(1 vol.):210–232. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenblitt JC, Soler H, Johnson SE, Quadagno DM. Sensation seeking and hormones in men and women: exploring the link. Horm Behav. 2001;40:396–402. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.2001.1704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Runyan JD, Dash PK. Intra-medial prefrontal administration of SCH-23390 attenuates ERK phosphorylation and long-term memory for trace fear conditioning in rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2004;82:65–70. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2004.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruotsalainen S, Sirvio J, Jakala P, Puumala T, MacDonald E, Riekkinen P. Differential effects of three 5-HT receptor antagonists on the performance of rats in attentional and working memory tasks. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 1997;7:99–108. doi: 10.1016/s0924-977x(96)00389-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sawaguchi T, Matsumura M, Kubota K. Delayed response deficit in monkeys by locally disturbed prefrontal neuronal activity by bicuculline. Behav Brain Res. 1988;31:193–198. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(88)90023-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schilman EA, Uylings HB, Galis-de Graaf Y, Joel D, Groenewegen HJ. The orbital cortex in rats topographically projects to central parts of the caudate-putamen complex. Neurosci Lett. 2008;432:40–45. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2007.12.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoenbaum G, Chiba AA, Gallagher M. Neural encoding in orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala during olfactory discrimination learning. J Neurosci. 1999;19:1876–1884. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-05-01876.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoenbaum G, Setlow B, Nugent SL, Saddoris MP, Gallagher M. Lesions of orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala complex disrupt acquisition of odor-guided discriminations and reversals. Learn Mem. 2003;10:129–140. doi: 10.1101/lm.55203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoenbaum G, Roesch M. Orbitofrontal cortex, associative learning, and expectancies. Neuron. 2005;47:633–636. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.07.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoenbaum G, Roesch MR, Stalnaker TA. Orbitofrontal cortex, decision-making and drug addiction. Trends Neurosci. 2006;29:116–124. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2005.12.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoenbaum G, Shaham Y. The role of orbitofrontal cortex in drug addiction: a review of preclinical studies. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:256–262. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.06.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoepp DD. Unveiling the functions of presynaptic metabotropic glutamate receptors in the central nervous system. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2001;299:12–20. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schweimer J, Hauber W. Dopamine D1 receptors in the anterior cingulate cortex regulate effort-based decision making. Learn Mem. 2006;13:777–782. doi: 10.1101/lm.409306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Semenova S, Markou A. The effects of the mGluR5 antagonist MPEP and the mGluR2/3 antagonist LY341495 on rats' performance in the 5-choice serial reaction time task. Neuropharmacology. 2007;52:863–872. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2006.10.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shackman AJ, McMenamin BW, Maxwell JS, Greischar LL, Davidson RJ. Right Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortical Activity and Behavioral Inhibition. Psychol Sci. 2009 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02476.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaham Y, Shalev U, Lu L, De Wit H, Stewart J. The reinstatement model of drug relapse: history, methodology and major findings. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2003;168:3–20. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1224-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaw P, Lerch J, Greenstein D, Sharp W, Clasen L, Evans A, Giedd J, Castellanos FX, Rapoport J. Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and clinical outcome in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63:540–549. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.5.540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simmons JM, Richmond BJ. Dynamic changes in representations of preceding and upcoming reward in monkey orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18:93–103. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skinner MD, Aubin HJ, Berlin I. Impulsivity in smoking, nonsmoking, and ex-smoking alcoholics. Addict Behav. 2004;29:973–978. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.02.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith MA, Schmidt KT, Iordanou JC, Mustroph ML. Aerobic exercise decreases the positive-reinforcing effects of cocaine. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2008;98:129–135. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.05.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sofuoglu M, Sewell RA. Norepinephrine and stimulant addiction. Addict Biol. 2008 doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2008.00138.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Solanto MV. Dopamine dysfunction in AD/HD: integrating clinical and basic neuroscience research. Behav Brain Res. 2002;130:65–71. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(01)00431-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sowell ER, Peterson BS, Thompson PM, Welcome SE, Henkenius AL, Toga AW. Mapping cortical change across the human life span. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6:309–315. doi: 10.1038/nn1008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spear LP. The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2000;24:417–463. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(00)00014-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steinbusch HW. Distribution of serotonin-immunoreactivity in the central nervous system of the rat-cell bodies and terminals. Neuroscience. 1981;6:557–618. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(81)90146-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoops WW, Lile JA, Robbins CG, Martin CA, Rush CR, Kelly TH. The reinforcing, subject-rated, performance, and cardiovascular effects of d-amphetamine: influence of sensation-seeking status. Addict Behav. 2007;32:1177–1188. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.08.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoops WW, Blackburn JW, Hudson DA, Hays LR, Rush CR. Safety, tolerability and subject-rated effects of acute intranasal cocaine administration during atomoxetine maintenance. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2008;92:282–285. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.07.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sukhotina IA, Dravolina OA, Novitskaya Y, Zvartau EE, Danysz W, Bespalov AY. Effects of mGlu1 receptor blockade on working memory, time estimation, and impulsivity in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2008;196:211–220. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-0953-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sun H, Green TA, Theobald DE, Birnbaum SG, Graham DL, Zeeb FD, Nestler EJ, Winstanley CA. Yohimbine increases impulsivity through activation of cAMP response element binding in the orbitofrontal cortex. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;67:649–656. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.11.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sussman S, Earleywine M, Wills T, Cody C, Biglan T, Dent CW, Newcomb MD. The motivation, skills, and decision-making model of "drug abuse" prevention. Subst Use Misuse. 2004;39:1971–2016. doi: 10.1081/ja-200034769. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swanson LW. Tracing central pathways with the autoradiographic method. J Histochem Cytochem. 1981;29:117–124. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanila H, Rama P, Carlson S. The effects of prefrontal intracortical microinjections of an alpha-2 agonist, alpha-2 antagonist and lidocaine on the delayed alternation performance of aged rats. Brain Res Bull. 1996;40:117–119. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(96)00026-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomas KL, Everitt BJ. Limbic-cortical-ventral striatal activation during retrieval of a discrete cocaine-associated stimulus: a cellular imaging study with gamma protein kinase C expression. J Neurosci. 2001;21:2526–2535. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-07-02526.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomas KL, Arroyo M, Everitt BJ. Induction of the learning and plasticity-associated gene Zif268 following exposure to a discrete cocaine-associated stimulus. Eur J Neurosci. 2003;17:1964–1972. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02617.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson SM, Gahwiler BH. Effects of the GABA uptake inhibitor tiagabine on inhibitory synaptic potentials in rat hippocampal slice cultures. J Neurophysiol. 1992;67:1698–1701. doi: 10.1152/jn.1992.67.6.1698. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tierney PL, Degenetais E, Thierry AM, Glowinski J, Gioanni Y. Influence of the hippocampus on interneurons of the rat prefrontal cortex. Eur J Neurosci. 2004;20:514–524. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03501.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tikhonov DB, Magazanik LG. Origin and molecular evolution of ionotropic glutamate receptors. Neurosci Behav Physiol. 2009;39:763–773. doi: 10.1007/s11055-009-9195-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Torregrossa MM, Quinn JJ, Taylor JR. Impulsivity, compulsivity, and habit: the role of orbitofrontal cortex revisited. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:253–255. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.11.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tranel D, Bechara A, Denburg NL. Asymmetric functional roles of right and left ventromedial prefrontal cortices in social conduct, decision-making, and emotional processing. Cortex. 2002;38:589–612. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70024-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tremblay L, Schultz W. Modifications of reward expectation-related neuronal activity during learning in primate orbitofrontal cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2000;83:1877–1885. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.83.4.1877. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van De Werd HJ, Rajkowska G, Evers P, Uylings HB. Cytoarchitectonic and chemoarchitectonic characterization of the prefrontal cortical areas in the mouse. Brain Struct Funct. 2010;214:339–353. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0247-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van den Bergh FS, Bloemarts E, Groenink L, Olivier B, Oosting RS. Delay aversion: effects of 7-OH-DPAT, 5-HT1A/1B–receptor stimulation and D-cycloserine. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2006;85:736–743. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2006.11.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Gaalen MM, van Koten R, Schoffelmeer AN, Vanderschuren LJ. Critical involvement of dopaminergic neurotransmission in impulsive decision making. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60:66–73. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Leijenhorst L, Westenberg PM, Crone EA. A developmental study of risky decisions on the cake gambling task: age and gender analyses of probability estimation and reward evaluation. Dev Neuropsychol. 2008;33:179–196. doi: 10.1080/87565640701884287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Velleman RD, Templeton LJ, Copello AG. The role of the family in preventing and intervening with substance use and misuse: a comprehensive review of family interventions, with a focus on young people. Drug Alcohol Rev. 2005;24:93–109. doi: 10.1080/09595230500167478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vertes RP. Differential projections of the infralimbic and prelimbic cortex in the rat. Synapse. 2004;51:32–58. doi: 10.1002/syn.10279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vertes RP. Interactions among the medial prefrontal cortex, hippocampus and midline thalamus in emotional and cognitive processing in the rat. Neuroscience. 2006;142:1–20. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.06.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vocci FJ. Cognitive remediation in the treatment of stimulant abuse disorders: A research agenda. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2008;16:484–497. doi: 10.1037/a0014101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ. The addicted human brain viewed in the light of imaging studies: brain circuits and treatment strategies. Neuropharmacology. 2004;47 Suppl 1:3–13. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2004.07.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vollm BA, de Araujo IE, Cowen PJ, Rolls ET, Kringelbach ML, Smith KA, Jezzard P, Heal RJ, Matthews PM. Methamphetamine activates reward circuitry in drug naive human subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:1715–1722. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wager TD, Davidson ML, Hughes BL, Lindquist MA, Ochsner KN. Prefrontal-subcortical pathways mediating successful emotion regulation. Neuron. 2008;59:1037–1050. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.09.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wallace CJ. The effects of delayed rewards, social pressure, and frustration on the responses of opiate addicts. NIDA Res Monogr. 1979:6–25. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wallis JD. Orbitofrontal cortex and its contribution to decision-making. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2007;30:31–56. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walton ME, Bannerman DM, Rushworth MF. The role of rat medial frontal cortex in effort-based decision making. J Neurosci. 2002;22:10996–11003. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-24-10996.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walton ME, Bannerman DM, Alterescu K, Rushworth MF. Functional specialization within medial frontal cortex of the anterior cingulate for evaluating effort-related decisions. J Neurosci. 2003;23:6475–6479. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-16-06475.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang GJ, Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Cervany P, Hitzemann RJ, Pappas NR, Wong CT, Felder C. Regional brain metabolic activation during craving elicited by recall of previous drug experiences. Life Sci. 1999;64:775–784. doi: 10.1016/s0024-3205(98)00619-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weissenborn R, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. Effects of medial prefrontal or anterior cingulate cortex lesions on responding for cocaine under fixed-ratio and second-order schedules of reinforcement in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1997;134:242–257. doi: 10.1007/s002130050447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whiteside SP, Lynam DR. Understanding the role of impulsivity and externalizing psychopathology in alcohol abuse: application of the UPPS impulsive behavior scale. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2003;11:210–217. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.11.3.210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wills TA, Vaccaro D, McNamara G. Novelty seeking, risk taking, and related constructs as predictors of adolescent substance use: an application of Cloninger's theory. J Subst Abuse. 1994;6:1–20. doi: 10.1016/s0899-3289(94)90039-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wills TA, Windle M, Cleary SD. Temperament and novelty seeking in adolescent substance use: convergence of dimensions of temperament with constructs from Cloninger's theory. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1998;74:387–406. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.74.2.387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wills TA, Isasi CR, Mendoza D, Ainette MG. Self-control constructs related to measures of dietary intake and physical activity in adolescents. J Adolesc Health. 2007;41:551–558. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.06.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson MA, Molliver ME. The organization of serotonergic projections to cerebral cortex in primates: regional distribution of axon terminals. Neuroscience. 1991a;44:537–553. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(91)90076-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson MA, Molliver ME. The organization of serotonergic projections to cerebral cortex in primates: retrograde transport studies. Neuroscience. 1991b;44:555–570. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(91)90077-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winstanley CA, Chudasama Y, Dalley JW, Theobald DE, Glennon JC, Robbins TW. Intra-prefrontal 8-OH-DPAT and M100907 improve visuospatial attention and decrease impulsivity on the five-choice serial reaction time task in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2003a;167:304–314. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1398-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winstanley CA, Dalley JW, Theobald DE, Robbins TW. Global 5-HT depletion attenuates the ability of amphetamine to decrease impulsive choice on a delay-discounting task in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2003b;170:320–331. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1546-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winstanley CA, Theobald DE, Cardinal RN, Robbins TW. Contrasting roles of basolateral amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex in impulsive choice. J Neurosci. 2004;24:4718–4722. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5606-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winstanley CA, Theobald DE, Dalley JW, Robbins TW. Interactions between serotonin and dopamine in the control of impulsive choice in rats: therapeutic implications for impulse control disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30:669–682. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300610. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winstanley CA, Eagle DM, Robbins TW. Behavioral models of impulsivity in relation to ADHD: translation between clinical and preclinical studies. Clin Psychol Rev. 2006a;26:379–395. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.01.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winstanley CA, Theobald DE, Dalley JW, Cardinal RN, Robbins TW. Double dissociation between serotonergic and dopaminergic modulation of medial prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex during a test of impulsive choice. Cereb Cortex. 2006b;16:106–114. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winstanley CA. The orbitofrontal cortex, impulsivity, and addiction: probing orbitofrontal dysfunction at the neural, neurochemical, and molecular level. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2007;1121:639–655. doi: 10.1196/annals.1401.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winstanley CA, LaPlant Q, Theobald DE, Green TA, Bachtell RK, Perrotti LI, DiLeone RJ, Russo SJ, Garth WJ, Self DW, Nestler EJ. DeltaFosB induction in orbitofrontal cortex mediates tolerance to cocaine-induced cognitive dysfunction. J Neurosci. 2007;27:10497–10507. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2566-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winstanley CA, Green TA, Theobald DE, Renthal W, LaPlant Q, DiLeone RJ, Chakravarty S, Nestler EJ. DeltaFosB induction in orbitofrontal cortex potentiates locomotor sensitization despite attenuating the cognitive dysfunction caused by cocaine. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2009;93:278–284. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2008.12.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winstanley CA, Olausson P, Taylor JR, Jentsch JD. Insight into the relationship between impulsivity and substance abuse from studies using animal models. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2010;34:1306–1318. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01215.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wise RA. Drug-activation of brain reward pathways. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1998;51:13–22. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(98)00063-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wrase J, Kahnt T, Schlagenhauf F, Beck A, Cohen MX, Knutson B, Heinz A. Different neural systems adjust motor behavior in response to reward and punishment. Neuroimage. 2007;36:1253–1262. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yalachkov Y, Kaiser J, Naumer MJ. Brain regions related to tool use and action knowledge reflect nicotine dependence. J Neurosci. 2009;29:4922–4929. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4891-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zahrt J, Taylor JR, Mathew RG, Arnsten AF. Supranormal stimulation of D1 dopamine receptors in the rodent prefrontal cortex impairs spatial working memory performance. J Neurosci. 1997;17:8528–8535. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-21-08528.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zecevic N, Bourgeois JP, Rakic P. Changes in synaptic density in motor cortex of rhesus monkey during fetal and postnatal life. Brain Res Dev Brain Res. 1989;50:11–32. doi: 10.1016/0165-3806(89)90124-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeeb FD, Floresco SB, Winstanley CA. Contributions of the orbitofrontal cortex to impulsive choice: interactions with basal levels of impulsivity, dopamine signalling, and reward-related cues. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2010;211:87–98. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-1871-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhu J, Apparsundaram S, Bardo MT, Dwoskin LP. Environmental enrichment decreases cell surface expression of the dopamine transporter in rat medial prefrontal cortex. J Neurochem. 2005;93:1434–1443. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2005.03130.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zuckerman M. Sensation seeking: beyond the optimal level of arousal. Vol. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1979. [Google Scholar]
- Zuckerman M. Behavioral expressions and biosocial bases of sensation seeking. Vol. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1994. [Google Scholar]