Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Dec 1.
Published in final edited form as: Clin Psychol (New York). 2010 Dec;17(4):272–280. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2850.2010.01219.x

Developmental Psychopathology and the Diagnosis of Mental Health Problems among Youth

Deborah A G Drabick 1, Philip C Kendall 1
PMCID: PMC3019616  NIHMSID: NIHMS233746  PMID: 21243110

Abstract

Recent years have evidenced a tremendous increase in research using a developmental psychopathology framework to examine clinical diagnoses among youth. Despite this increase, a relative dearth of literature systematically examines the development of co-occurring conditions among youth. In this introduction to the Special Issue on comorbidity among youth, we suggest that a developmental psychopathology perspective can provide an important foundation for the diagnosis of mental health problems among youth. As a potential framework for future investigations, we consider several developmental psychopathology principles that can inform assessment and diagnosis among youth psychological disorders. We use these principles as a foundation for considering co-occurring psychological disorders and provide potential explanations for comorbidity that can be addressed in future research that uses a developmental psychopathology perspective.


Research using a developmental psychopathology perspective has surged in recent decades and provides an important framework for diagnoses among youth (Beauchaine, 2003; Drabick, 2009a; Holmbeck & Kendall, 2002; Jensen & Hoagwood, 1997; Rutter & Sroufe, 2000; Steinberg, 2002). In this paper, we describe several ways in which a developmental psychopathology perspective can inform diagnostic decision-making. We discuss the relations between, and potential influence of, a developmental psychopathology perspective on comorbidity or disorder co-occurrence among youth. We describe several potential explanations for disorder co-occurrence and link the developmental psychopathology framework to these explanations. We intend to be illustrative and not exhaustive of the many explanations that may underlie co-occurring conditions, and in so doing, hope to provide suggestions and a framework for future investigations.

Use of a Developmental Psychopathology Emphasis

The value of a developmental psychopathology perspective can be seen in research involving a wide range of psychological conditions, including attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), oppositional defiant disorder (ODD), conduct disorder (CD), anxiety disorders, depressive disorders, bipolar disorder, autism, and schizophrenia (Beauchaine, 2001; Bubier & Drabick, 2008; Carlson & Meyer, 2006; Dodge, 2009; Garber, Keiley, & Martin, 2002; Jensen et al., 1997; Miklowtiz, 2004; Pine, Guyer, & Leibenluft, 2008; Silk et al., 2007; Viding & Blakemore, 2007). However, the influence of the developmental psychopathology perspective extends well beyond the consideration of diagnoses among youth. Rather, this perspective has had a broader and more complex influence on research considering issues such as contextual factors, categorical and dimensional models, risk and resilience, and developmental pathways (Beauchaine, 2003; Cicchetti & Rogosch, 1996; Drabick, 2009a, 2009b; Hart & Marmorstein, 2009; Holmbeck & Kendall, 2002; Jensen & Hoagwood, 1997; Rutter & Sroufe, 2000; Silk et al., 2007; Steinberg & Avenevoli, 2000; Steinberg, 2002). We consider several developmental psychopathology principles that we believe are particularly relevant to diagnoses next.

Normative vs. Atypical Development

The developmental psychopathology perspective emphasizes that normative development provides a crucial and necessary comparison for determining whether youth behavior is atypical or problematic (Drabick, 2009a; Kendall & Comer, 2010; Rutter & Sroufe, 2000; Steinberg, 2002). For example, recognizing that although youth noncompliance among toddlers and adolescents may be distressing for parents, yet developmentally expected, facilitates the appropriate diagnosis of ODD only when the behavior occurs more frequently than would be observed among individuals of the same age and/or developmental level (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Similarly, separation anxiety exhibited by a toddler, but not a preadolescent, is seen as developmentally appropriate. However, the wide range of what is considered “typical” often makes it challenging to differentiate normative and atypical behavior, such as when we have to distinguish between youth overactivity and behavior consistent with the syndrome of ADHD. In this case, DSM indicates that impairment from symptoms or behaviors must be present in at least two settings (APA, 2000); thus, the child who engages in impulsive and hyperactive behavior at both home and school would be more likely to meet diagnostic criteria than a child who is hyperactive at home only. The range of behaviors considered typical among different developmental periods also reflects the fact that individual differences exist among youth in terms of their cognitive and emotion regulation abilities, as well as our expectations that these abilities will change over time.

Identification of CD is potentially more challenging in adolescence. Ample developmental research indicates that the shift from childhood to adolescence is accompanied by increases in risk taking and sensation seeking, particularly in the presence of peers (Steinberg, 2008). Thus, engaging in behaviors consistent with CD in adolescence may be expected among some youth, but determining at what point these behaviors cause “clinically significant impairment” may be difficult. The requirement of three or more symptoms to meet diagnostic criteria for CD, as well as other-age related factors (e.g., staying out late and truancy must begin before age 13), can help us to distinguish among youth who are engaging in typical, though potentially distressing and impairing, risk-taking or sensation-seeking behavior from youth who qualify for a diagnosis of CD. Thus, the phrasing of many DSM specifiers and symptoms permits us to take developmentally expected behavior into account, though access to and knowledge of developmental norms is critically important for appropriate diagnostic decision-making.

Categorical vs. Dimensional Models

A second developmental psychopathology principle relevant to diagnoses among youth is the consideration of both categorical and dimensional models, given the relative dearth of evidence suggesting that diagnostic categories represent discrete syndromes (Beauchaine, 2003; Drabick, 2009a; Kazdin & Kagan, 1994; Rutter & Sroufe, 2000). Although DSM uses a categorical model for conceptualizing diagnoses, many symptoms cause clinically significant impairment at subthreshold levels (Angold, Costello, Farmer, Burns, & Erkanli, 1999b; Maser et al., 2009), reinforcing the importance of evaluating psychological symptoms using both categorical and dimensional approaches. Moreover, a dimensional approach could facilitate identification of children who are at risk for more severe or pernicious courses prior to the full presentation of symptoms (Bubier & Drabick, 2008, 2009; Drabick, 2009a, 2009b; Ezpeleta, Keeler, Erkanli, Costello, & Angold, 2001). Thus, consideration of categorical syndromes is important for a variety of reasons, including epidemiology, communication among professionals, treatment research, and obtaining services, yet dimensional models can facilitate acquisition of complementary information regarding risk, resilience, etiology, prevention, and intervention (Kazdin & Kagan, 1994; Rutter & Sroufe, 2000). Joint consideration of categorical and dimensional models would facilitate our understanding of the development, maintenance, prognosis, treatment, and course of a variety of psychological conditions, as well as the development of co-occurring psychological symptoms and comorbid syndromes (e.g., Bubier & Drabick, 2009).

Contextual Influences

Although there is wide agreement that contextual influences (e.g., parent-child, family, peer, neighborhood) are critical to consider in research involving psychopathology among youth (e.g., Boyce et al., 1998; Deater-Deckard, 2001; Hart & Marmorstein, 2009; Silk et al., 2007; Steinberg & Avenevoli, 2000), there is relatively little attention given to these influences in research involving DSM diagnoses. Indeed, the DSM has been criticized for a tendency to conceptualize disorders as existing within the individual (Beauchaine, 2003; Jensen & Hoagwood, 1997; Kazdin & Kagan, 1994; Rutter & Sroufe, 2000). The developmental psychopathology framework provides several possible research routes involving contextual factors. To illustrate, contextual factors have been considered as predictors of risk, resilience, symptom severity, course, prognosis, and treatment outcomes, and thus in the roles of correlates, mediators, and outcomes (Bubier, Drabick, & Breiner, 2009; Deater-Deckard, 2001; Dodge, 2009; Holmbeck & Kendall, 2002; Jensen & Hoagwood, 1997; Miklowitz, 2004; Steinberg, 2002; Steinberg & Avenevoli, 2000).

An alternative way to construe contextual factors in the development and/or maintenance of psychological symptoms is through the consideration of Individual × Context interactions. Specifically, individuals engage in reciprocal and transactional relations with their contexts. For example, youth who exhibit a difficult temperament may be more likely to elicit negative or coercive responses from parents and peers. Over time, these difficulties in interpersonal reactions may contribute to an increase in the likelihood that youth with difficult temperaments will develop CD and/or depression (Patterson, 1993). Thus, it is the combination of individual and contextual factors that may lead to a particular outcome, as opposed to factors operating in isolation.

The importance of considering Individual × Context interactions has been demonstrated in research using a developmental psychopathology perspective, including work illustrating that parental behaviors interact with child temperament (Bates, Pettit, Dodge, & Ridge, 1998; Colder, Lochman, & Wells, 1997) or child autonomic functioning (Bubier et al., 2009) to predict conduct problems (e.g., ODD, CD behaviors) among youth. Similarly, child temperament interacts with parental overprotection (Colder et al., 1997; Oldehinkel, Veenstra, Ormel, De Winter, & Verhulst, 2006) and with peer rejection (Brendgen, Wanner, Morin, & Vitaro, 2005) to predict youth depressive symptoms. As a final set of examples, the interaction of genetic polymorphisms and adverse environmental experiences (Dodge, 2009), maternal insensitivity (Bakermans-Kranenburg & van Ijzendoorn, 2006), and neighborhood factors (Hart & Marmorstein, 2009) predict conduct problems among youth. This empirical support buttresses the recognition that contexts do not have a uniform effect among youth. Indeed, negative contextual influences may lead to a variety of outcomes, including youth emotional or behavioral problems, as well as adaptive functioning. This possibility is consistent with the developmental psychopathology principle of multifinality, that is, the expectation that a single risk factor can lead to a variety of outcomes based on other aspects of the system (e.g., context) (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 1996). The related principle of equifinality, or the possibility that different processes can lead to the same outcome (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 1996), provides an important foundation for our consideration of disorder co-occurrence. That is, although youth may meet diagnostic criteria for multiple conditions, the means by which they arrived at these diagnoses are likely highly variable and potentially consistent with a variety of explanations for the co-occurrence of disorder.

Comorbidity among Youth Disorders

Epidemiological research indicates that comorbidity, or disorder co-occurrence, is quite common among youth psychological disorders (Angold, Costello, & Erkanli, 1999a; Copeland, Shanahan, Costello, & Angold, 2009; Costello, Mustillo, Erkanli, Keeler, & Angold, 2003; Crawford et al., 2008; Kendall, Brady, & Verduin, 2002; Kessler & Wang, 2008; Moffitt et al., 2007). In general, the presence of a comorbid condition is associated with increased symptom severity and impairment, more frequent and severe negative correlates and sequelae, differential treatment response, and distinct courses. Thus, consideration of comorbidity is critical for informing psychopathology models relevant to etiology, nosology, and intervention (Angold et al., 1999a; Bubier & Drabick, 2009; Clarkin & Kendall, 1992; Costello et al., 2003; Drabick, 2009b; Drabick et al., 2006; Verduin & Kendall, 2003). Although application of the term “comorbidity” within a medical disease model suggests a common underlying disease entity (Lilienfeld, Waldman, & Israel, 1994), the etiological mechanisms responsible for psychological disorders likely involve complex interactions among risk and resilience variables from multiple domains, consistent with a developmental psychopathology perspective. Thus, it is difficult to apply the medical disease model definition of comorbidity to mental health problems. Instead, we focus on disorder co-occurrence at the syndrome levels and present shared etiological processes as just one of many possible explanations for comorbidity among youth disorders.

Not surprisingly, the range of explanations for disorder co-occurrence is varied and broad (e.g., Angold et al., 1999a; Caron & Rutter, 1991; Drabick, 2009b; Klein & Riso, 1993). However, there is little systematically organized knowledge considering why these elevated levels of comorbidity exist. One impetus for this Special Issue was to highlight current knowledge regarding disorder co-occurrence across a range of conditions, relying on the expertise of individuals who have conducted research among youth with multiple psychological conditions. To provide a foundation for these papers, we describe several explanations that have been offered to account for comorbidity among youth disorders. We begin by describing several artifactual and methodological explanations for comorbidity, which suggest that comorbidity may be due to chance, sampling and assessment issues, or problems with the diagnostic system. We then turn to potential causal explanations, which suggest that comorbidity may arise because one disorder confers risk for another condition, or shared risk factors account for disorder co-occurrence. We reference principles of developmental psychopathology where appropriate, with the goal of illustrating the use of this perspective for informing diagnostic decision-making and disorder co-occurrence among youth.

Artifactual and Methodological Explanations

Chance

One artifactual explanation is that the comorbidity of two conditions is due to chance. Specifically, if the occurrence of two disorders is high, the chance of their co-occurrence also will be high. Epidemiological studies provide an unbiased estimate of comorbidity (e.g., compared to self-selected or clinic samples), and are most amenable to addressing questions related to base rates and chance. Although there is some variation based on the pairs of disorders considered in terms of rates of comorbidity, each paper in the Special Issue addresses comorbid conditions that have been shown to co-occur at greater than chance rates based on epidemiological studies (Angold et al., 1999a; Copeland et al., 2009; Costello et al., 2003; Crawford et al., 2008; Kessler & Wang, 2008; Moffitt et al., 2007; Nock et al., 2007).

Methodological issues

A second artifactual explanation is sampling bias. The probability of seeking treatment is greater among individuals with more than one disorder (Berkson’s bias). Accordingly, clinic samples may produce artificially elevated rates of comorbidity (Angold et al., 1999a; Klein & Riso, 1993). Using comorbidity rates from epidemiological studies reduces concern with this issue. Nevertheless, as Caron and Rutter (1991) argued, as long as elevated rates of comorbidity have been demonstrated in community samples, clinic data can be useful and practical for examining the relations between disorders, and thus the papers in this Special Issue consider a range of data for understanding disorder co-occurrence. It is also possible that biases associated with the methodologies used to assess psychological syndromes (e.g., strategies, reporters, study designs) could account for elevated rates of their co-occurrence. To rule out methodological biases, researchers should demonstrate that the comorbidity of two conditions is not a function of, or specific to, the assessment strategies (e.g., semi-structured and structured interviews, checklists); reporters (parents, teachers, youth); samples (community, clinic, epidemiological); or study designs (cross-sectional, prospective). The pairs of comorbid conditions considered in this Special Issue have been identified across a range of methodologies and thus do not appear to be a function of these biases.

Population stratification

Population stratification refers to the possibility that two disorders have separate sets of risk factors that are common in the same subgroup of the population, thereby resulting in an apparent but artifactual comorbidity within that subgroup (Klein & Riso, 1993). For instance, maternal depression has been linked repeatedly to childhood depression (Frye & Garber, 2005; Hammen, Shih, & Brennan, 2004; Rice, Harold, & Thapar, 2005), and conduct problems are often related to family discord, which is more common among families where a parent suffers from depression (Patterson, DeBaryshe, & Ramsey, 1989; Patterson, 1993). Thus, because mothers with depression are more likely to experience marital discord, child depression and conduct problems may co-occur in samples with a disproportionate representation of mothers who are depressed. A developmental psychopathology approach that includes attention to issues of selection, sample generalizability, and contextual variables can minimize concerns related to population stratification.

Symptom overlap

Another artifactual explanation for comorbidity involves symptom overlap across diagnostic categories (Angold et al., 1999a; Klein & Riso, 1993). For example, because symptoms of inattention may occur as part of ADHD, anxiety, and mood disorders, a child who presents with symptoms of inattention may be more likely to meet diagnostic criteria for multiple conditions because this symptom appears in multiple categories. Increasing recognition of this issue has led researchers to examine rates of comorbidity with and without similar symptoms to try to unpack whether this artifactual explanation may underlie disorder co-occurrence, though symptom omission obviously modifies the defined syndrome under consideration (e.g., Gadow & Nolan, 2002; Gadow et al., 2004).

The comorbid condition is an atypical form of one condition

Another artifactual explanation for disorder co-occurrence is that the comorbid condition is a variant or atypical form of one condition (Klein & Riso, 1993). Because disorders can be manifested heterogeneously and include symptoms often associated with other disorders, observed comorbidity may be a function of alternative symptom expression. For instance, the now-dated notion that conduct disordered behaviors were a form of “masked depression” (Kovacs et al., 1988; McCracken, Cantwell, & Hanna, 1993) is consistent with this explanation. According to the masked depression model, depression in childhood is not manifest in the same way among adults because of homotypic changes in symptoms across development. Instead, disruptive behavior was hypothesized to be a vehicle for acting out intense affect stemming from underlying depression among children. The utility of this explanation can be evaluated by comparing individuals with comorbid conditions (e.g., ADHD and CD) to individuals with only one condition (e.g., ADHD only or CD only) on etiological processes, risk and resilience factors, developmental pathways, and distal outcomes. This line of inquiry could address whether the comorbid condition is more similar to one of the pure or monomorbid conditions, and whether these similarities change as a function of the developmental period considered, and thereby rule out or provide support for this comorbidity explanation.

Causal Explanations

A potentially “causal” explanation for co-occurring conditions is simply that having one disorder causes or confers risk for the other. This explanation fits within a developmental psychopathology perspective that attends to developmental pathways, risk and resilience factors from multiple domains, and the transactional relations among individuals and their contexts. For example, youth who exhibit ADHD, anxiety, or CD may be at risk for developing depression. One model for these associations is that the primary psychological condition (e.g., ADHD, generalized anxiety disorder, or CD) may lead to difficulties in interpersonal and academic functioning. Over time, these difficulties in domains in which we would like youth to experience success may lead to the development of depression. Similarly, youth who exhibit ADHD may have difficulty attending to demands at home and school; with continued difficulties in these settings, these youth may subsequently meet diagnostic criteria for ODD (Angold et al., 1999a; Costello et al., 2003; Drabick et al., 2006; Gadow & Nolan, 2002; Gadow et al., 2004; Moffitt et al., 2007; Patterson et al., 1989).

Another possible explanation for disorder comorbidity is that each condition is associated with overlapping and unique factors, and comorbidity stems from shared risk factors or etiological processes (Angold et al., 1999a; Caron & Rutter, 1991; Klein & Riso, 1993). An approach that considers multifinality, similarities in pathogenesis, and risk factors drawn from multiple domains and using multiple levels of analysis would be useful for testing this explanation. For example, a number of psychological conditions have been linked to genetic mechanisms (Dodge, 2009; Hart & Marmorstein, 2009), child temperament (Muris & Ollendick, 2005), autonomic functioning (Beauchaine, 2001), peer rejection (Deater-Deckard, 2001), parental psychological difficulties (Loeber, Farrington, Stouthamer-Loeber, & Van Kammen, 1998), and parent-child conflict (Loeber et al., 1998; Patterson et al., 1989), suggesting that some processes may act as shared risk factors or etiological processes for multiple conditions and thus may underlie comorbidity. Given the likelihood that shared risk or etiological processes are moderated or modified by additional risk or resilience variables, examining psychopathology at different points and through different transitions in development is crucial for addressing this comorbidity explanation. Nevertheless, few studies have compared these comorbidity explanations concurrently and within the same sample, or have used a developmental psychopathology perspective in examining co-occurring clinical diagnoses. Thus, it is unclear which explanations are most applicable to specific co-occurring conditions during different periods of life.

Concluding Thoughts

Although we believe that it is critical for research to consider multiple explanations for co-occurring conditions, we also believe that it is important for such research to use a developmental psychopathology perspective and consider normative vs. atypical development, categorical and dimensional (subdiagnostic) models, the role of context, and developmental pathways that may lead to disorder co-occurrence. The paucity of research exploring the role of developmental processes in co-occurring conditions poses a crucial limitation. As children age, they experience a variety of expected developmental changes/challenges in terms of cognitive and emotion regulation abilities, sensation seeking and risk taking, and associations with peers, many of which are underpinned by neurobiological changes (e.g., Holmbeck & Kendall, 2002; Steinberg, 2002, 2008; Viding & Blakemore, 2007). These developmental changes often correspond to changes (increases, decreases) in symptom frequency or severity (Loeber et al., 1998), the addition of novel symptoms or behaviors to pre-existing problematic behaviors (i.e., heterotypic continuity; Angold et al., 1999a; Patterson, 1993), and the experience of comorbidity among youth. Moreover, factors may differentially confer risk or resilience, depending on the timing of their onset, presence or carry-forward effects of other risk factors, and their interactions with stressors associated with different developmental periods (Deater-Deckard et al., 1998; Rutter & Sroufe, 2000; Steinberg, 2002).

Consideration of the interactions among biological, psychological, and social factors within a developmental framework may seem onerous, but we suggest that this framework is important for evaluating the emergence and maintenance of co-occurring symptoms and disorders. Determining whether certain disorders are risk factors for, or emerge secondarily to, other conditions could suggest different etiological models and intervention efforts (Angold et al., 1999a; Clarkin & Kendall, 1992; Drabick, 2009a; Pine et al., 2008). Similarly, research that examines whether the monomorbid and comorbid versions of a disorder differ in terms of etiological processes, risk factors, course, treatment response, and outcomes is critical. Moreover, consideration of relevant developmental tasks and stressors, the developmental timing of risk and resilience processes, Child × Context interactions, and developmental pathways is important for advancing our understanding of the co-occurrence of psychological symptoms and disorders. Such efforts could extend the burgeoning literature that considers biological and social contexts in risk and resilience for psychological outcomes (e.g., Dodge, 2009; Hart & Marmorstein, 2009; Silk et al., 2007; Viding & Blakemore, 2007) to augment our understanding of disorder co-occurrence among youth. We are grateful that the authors of the papers in this Special Issue embraced various aspects of our recommendations, and thus illustrate their applicability to common co-occurring conditions among youth. It is our hope that the papers in the Special Issue can frame additional research designed to improve and thus advance our understanding of comorbidity among youth.

Acknowledgments

Preparation of this manuscript was supported in part by NIMH grants 1K01MH073717-01A2 awarded to Deborah A. G. Drabick and MH MH080788 awarded to Philip C. Kendall.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, Fourth edition-Text revision (DSM-IV-TR) Washington, DC: Author; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  2. Angold A, Costello EJ, Erkanli A. Comorbidity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1999a;40:57–87. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Angold A, Costello EJ, Farmer E, Burns B, Erkanli A. Impaired but undiagnosed. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1999b;38:129–137. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199902000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van Ijzendoorn MH. Gene-environment interaction of the dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) and observed maternal insensitivity predicting externalizing behavior in preschoolers. Developmental Psychobiology. 2006;48:406–409. doi: 10.1002/dev.20152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bates J, Pettit G, Dodge K, Ridge B. Interaction of temperamental resistance to control and restrictive parenting in the development of externalizing behavior. Developmental Psychology. 1998;34:982–995. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.34.5.982. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Beauchaine TP. Vagal tone, development, and Gray’s motivational theory: Toward an integrated model of autonomic nervous system functioning in psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology. 2001;13:183–214. doi: 10.1017/s0954579401002012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Beauchaine TP. Taxometrics and developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology. 2003;15:501–527. doi: 10.1017/s0954579403000270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Boyce WT, Frank E, Jensen PS, Kessler RC, Nelson CA, Steinberg L, et al. Social context in developmental psychopathology: Recommendations for future research from the MacArthur Network on Psychopathology and Development. Development and Psychopathology. 1998;10:143–164. doi: 10.1017/s0954579498001552. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Brendgen M, Wanner B, Morin AJS, Vitaro F. Relations with parents and with peers, temperament, and trajectories of depressed mood during early adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2005;33:579–594. doi: 10.1007/s10802-005-6739-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bubier J, Drabick DA. Affective decision-making and externalizing behaviors: The role of autonomic activity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2008;36:941–953. doi: 10.1007/s10802-008-9225-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bubier J, Drabick DA. Co-occurring anxiety and disruptive behavior disorders: The roles of anxious symptoms, reactive aggression, and shared risk processes. Clinical Psychology Review. 2009;29:658–669. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.08.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bubier J, Drabick DA, Breiner T. Autonomic functioning moderates the relation between contextual factors and externalizing behaviors among inner-city children. Journal of Family Psychology. 2009;23:500–510. doi: 10.1037/a0015555. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Carlson GA, Meyer SE. Phenomenology and diagnosis of bipolar disorder in children, adolescents, and adults: Complexities and developmental issues. Development and Psychopathology. 2006;18:939–969. doi: 10.1017/S0954579406060470. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Caron C, Rutter M. Comorbidity in child psychopathology: Concepts, issues, and research strategies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1991;32:1063–1080. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1991.tb00350.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA. Equifinality and multifinality in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology. 1996;8:597–600. [Google Scholar]
  16. Clarkin JF, Kendall PC. Comorbidity and treatment planning: Summary and future directions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1992;60:904–908. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Colder CR, Lochman JE, Wells KC. The moderating effects of children’s fear and activity level on relations between parenting practices and childhood symptomatology. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1997;25:251–263. doi: 10.1023/a:1025704217619. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Copeland WE, Shanahan L, Costello J, Angold A. Childhood and adolescent psychiatric disorders as predictors of young adult disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2009;66:764–772. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.85. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Costello EJ, Mustillo S, Erkanli A, Keeler G, Angold A. Prevalence and development of psychiatric disorders in childhood and adolescence. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2003;60:837–844. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.8.837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Crawford TN, Cohen P, First MB, Johnson JG, Kasen S, Skodol AE. Comorbid Axis I and Axis II disorders in early adolescence: Outcomes 20 years later. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2008;65:641–648. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.6.641. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Deater-Deckard K. Annotation: Recent research examining the role of peer relationships in the development of psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2001;42:565–579. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Dodge KA. Mechanisms of the gene-environment interaction effects in the development of conduct disorder. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2009;4:408–414. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01147.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Drabick DA. Can a developmental psychopathology perspective facilitate a paradigm shift toward a mixed categorical-dimensional classification system? Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. 2009a;16:41–49. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2850.2009.01141.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Drabick DA. Depression comorbidity among children: Potential explanations, shared risk factors, and directions for future research. In: Naylor BT, editor. Depression in children. Huntington, NY: Nova Science; 2009b. pp. 1–23. [Google Scholar]
  25. Drabick DA, Gadow KD, Sprafkin J. Co-occurrence of conduct disorder and depression in a clinic-based sample of boys with ADHD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2006;47:766–774. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01625.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Ezpeleta L, Keeler G, Erkanli A, Costello EJ, Angold A. Epidemiology of psychiatric disability in childhood and adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2001;42:901–914. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Frye AA, Garber J. The relations among maternal depression, maternal criticism, and adolescents’ externalizing and internalizing symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2005;33:1–11. doi: 10.1007/s10802-005-0929-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Gadow KD, Drabick DA, Loney J, Sprafkin J, Salisbury H, Azizian A, Schwartz J. Comparison of ADHD symptom subtypes as source-specific syndromes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2004;45:1135–1149. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00306.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Gadow KD, Nolan EE. Differences between preschool children with ODD, ADHD, and ODD+ADHD symptoms. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2002;43:191–201. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Garber J, Keiley MK, Martin NC. Developmental trajectories of adolescents’ depressive symptoms: Predictors of change. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2002;70:79–95. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.70.1.79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hammen C, Shih J, Brennan P. Intergenerational transmission of depression: Test of an interpersonal stress model in a community sample. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2004;72:511–522. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.3.511. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hart D, Marmorstein NR. Neighborhoods and genes and everything in between: Understanding adolescent aggression in social and biological contexts. Development and Psychopathology. 2009;21:961–973. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409000510. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Holmbeck GN, Kendall PC. Introduction to the special section on clinical adolescent psychology: Developmental psychopathology and treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2002;70:3–5. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Jensen PS, Hoagwood K. The book of names: DSM-IV in context. Development and Psychopathology. 1997;9:231–249. doi: 10.1017/s0954579497002034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Jensen P, Mrazek D, Knapp P, Steinberg L, Pfeffer C, Schowalter J, Shapiro T. Evolution and revolution in child psychiatry: ADHD as a disorder of adaptation. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1997;36:1672–1679. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199712000-00015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kazdin A, Kagan J. Models of dysfunction in developmental psychopathology. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. 1994;1:35–52. [Google Scholar]
  37. Kendall PC, Brady EU, Verduin TL. Comorbidity in childhood anxiety disorders and treatment outcome. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2001;40:787–794. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200107000-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kendall PC, Comer J. Childhood disorders. 2. London: Taylor and Francis; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  39. Kessler RC, Wang PS. The descriptive epidemiology of commonly occurring mental disorders in the United States. Annual Review of Public Health. 2008;29:115–129. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.29.020907.090847. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Klein DN, Riso LP. Psychiatric disorders: Problems of boundaries and comorbidity. In: Costello CG, editor. Basic issues in psychopathology. New York: Guilford; 1993. pp. 19–66. [Google Scholar]
  41. Kovacs M, Paulauskas S, Gatsonis C, Richards C. Depressive disorders in childhood: III. A longitudinal study of comorbidity with and risk for conduct disorders. Journal of Affective Disorders. 1988;15:205–217. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(88)90018-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Lilienfeld SO, Waldman ID, Israel AC. A critical examination of the use of the term and concept of comorbidity in psychopathology research. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. 1994;1:71–83. [Google Scholar]
  43. Loeber R, Farrington DP, Stouthamer-Loeber M, Van Kammen WB. Multiple risk factors for multiproblem boys: Co-occurrence of delinquency, substance use, attention deficit, conduct problems, physical aggression, covert behavior, depressed mood, and shy/withdrawn behavior. In: Jessor R, editor. New perspectives on adolescent risk behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1998. pp. 90–149. [Google Scholar]
  44. Maser J, Norman S, Zisook S, Everall I, Stein M, Schettler P, Judd L. Psychiatric nosology is ready for a paradigm shift in DSM-V. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. 2009;16:24–40. [Google Scholar]
  45. Maughan B, Rowe R, Messer J, Goodman R, Meltzer H. Conduct disorder and oppositional defiant disorder in a national sample: Developmental epidemiology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2004;45:609–621. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00250.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Miklowitz DJ. The role of family systems in severe and recurrent psychiatric disorders: A developmental psychopathology view. Development and Psychopathology. 2004;16:667–688. doi: 10.1017/s0954579404004729. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Moffitt TE, Harrington H, Caspi A, Kim-Cohen J, Goldberg D, Gregory AM, et al. Depression and generalized anxiety disorder: Cumulative and sequential comorbidity in a birth cohort followed prospectively to age 32 years. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2007;64:651–660. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.6.651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Muris P, Ollendick TH. The role of temperament in the etiology of child psychopathology. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 2005;8:271–289. doi: 10.1007/s10567-005-8809-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Oldehinkel AJ, Veenstra R, Ormel J, De Winter AF, Verhulst FC. Temperament, parenting, and depressive symptoms in a population sample of preadolescents. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2006;47:684–695. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01535.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Patterson GR. Orderly change in a stable world: The antisocial trait as a chimera. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1993;61:911–919. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.61.6.911. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Patterson GR, DeBaryshe BD, Ramsey E. A developmental perspective on antisocial behavior. American Psychologist. 1989;44:329–335. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.44.2.329. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Pine DS, Guyer AE, Leibenluft E. Functional magnetic resonance imaging and pediatric anxiety. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2008;47:1217–1221. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e318185dad0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Rice F, Harold GT, Thapar A. The link between depression in mothers and offspring: An extended twin analysis. Behavior Genetics. 2005;35:565–577. doi: 10.1007/s10519-005-5432-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Rowe R, Maughan B, Pickles A, Costello EJ, Angold A. The relationship between DSM-IV oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder: Findings from the Great Smoky Mountains Study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2002;43:365–373. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Rutter M, Sroufe L. Developmental psychopathology: Concepts and challenges. Development and Psychopathology. 2000;12:265–296. doi: 10.1017/s0954579400003023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Silk JS, Vanderbilt-Adriance E, Shaw DS, Forbes EE, Whalen DJ, Ryan ND, Dahl RE. Resilience among children and adolescents at risk for depression: Mediation and moderation across social and neurobiological context. Development and Psychopathology. 2007;19:841–865. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407000417. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Steinberg L. Clinical adolescent psychology: What it is, and what it needs to be. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2002;70:124–128. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.70.1.124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Steinberg L. A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking. Developmental Review. 2008;28:78–106. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Steinberg L, Avenevoli S. The role of context in the development of psychopathology: A conceptual framework and some speculative propositions. Child Development. 2000;71:66–74. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Verduin T, Kendall PC. Differential occurrence of comorbidity within childhood anxiety disorders. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2003;32:290–295. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3202_15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Viding E, Blakemore S. Endophenotype approach to developmental psychopathology: Implications for autism research. Behavior Genetics. 2007;37:51–60. doi: 10.1007/s10519-006-9105-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES