Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2012 Jan 1.
Published in final edited form as: Environ Res. 2010 Oct 12;111(1):156–163. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2010.09.006

Elevated manganese and cognitive performance in school-aged children and their mothers

José A Menezes-Filho 1,2, Cristiane de O Novaes 2, Josino C Moreira 2, Paula N Sarcinelli 2, Donna Mergler 3
PMCID: PMC3026060  NIHMSID: NIHMS241936  PMID: 20943219

Abstract

Background

Growing evidence suggests that excess manganese (Mn) in children is associated with neurobehavioral impairments. In Brazil, elevated hair Mn concentrations were reported in children living near a ferro-manganese alloy plant.

Objectives

We investigated these children’s and caregivers’ cognitive function in relation to bioindicators of Mn exposure.

Methods

In this cross-sectional study, the WISC-III was administered to 83 children aged between 6 and 12 years; the Raven Progressive Matrix was administered to the primary caregivers (94% mothers), who likewise responded to a questionnaire on socio demographics and birth history. Mn in hair (MnH) and blood (MnB) and blood lead (PbB) were measured by graphite furnace atomic absorption spectrometry (GFAAS).

Results

Children’s mean MnB and MnH were 8.2 µg/L (2.7 – 23.4) and 5.83 µg/g (0.1 −86.68), respectively. Mean maternal MnH was 3.50 µg/g (0.10 – 77.45) and correlated to children’s MnH (rho=0.294, p=0.010). Children’s MnH was negatively related to Full-Scale Intelligence Quotient (IQ) and Verbal IQ; β coefficients for MnH were −5.78 (95% CI −10.71 to −0.21) and −6.72 (−11.81 to −0.63), adjusted for maternal education and nutritional status. Maternal MnH was negatively associated with performance on the Raven’s (β = −2.69, 95% CI −5.43 to 0.05), adjusted for education years, family income and age.

Conclusions

These findings confirm that high MnH in children is associated with poorer cognitive performance, especially in the verbal domain. Primary caregiver’s IQ is likewise associated to Mn exposure, suggesting that, in this situation, children’s cognition may be affected directly and indirectly by Mn exposure.

Keywords: Manganese, intelligence, children, neurobehavioral, alloy plant

Introduction

There is growing interest in environmental manganese exposure in children. Recent studies suggest that excess Mn may interfere with developing brain functions. In Bangladeshi children, Wasserman and associates (2004, 2006) observed a negative effect of high Mn levels in tube well water on cognitive capacities. In Quebec, Bouchard et al. (2007) reported a significant association between hair manganese (MnH) levels and hyperactive and oppositional behavior in children exposed to Mn through municipal well water. Decrements in IQ scores in Korean children were associated with elevated blood Mn levels in a population-based study, but Mn sources were not identified (Kim et al. 2009). Zoni et al. (2007), in a review of recent studies on neurobehavioral performance and manganese exposure across the lifespan, suggested that children’s cognitive functions might be particularly vulnerable to manganese.

The developing nervous system is a prime target for the disrupting effects of toxic chemicals (Faustman et al. 2000; Landrigan et al. 2005; Weiss and Landrigan, 2000; Rice and Barone 2000; Bellinger 2009). In a birth cohort study in France, cord blood Mn was negatively associated with attention and non-verbal memory and boys’ manual ability at 3 years, after adjusting for mother's educational level. (Takser et al. 2003). In a study in the United States, Mn was analyzed in the enamel of deciduous teeth. Its concentration in tissue formed during the intra-uterine phase, was significantly associated with disinhibitory behavior evaluated at 36 and 54 months of development (Ericson et al. 2007).

Among the many factors that influence children’s neurodevelopment, mothers’ education and/or IQ constitute a major determinant (To et al. 2004; Kordas et al. 2004; Andrade et al. 2005). In an exposure situation, parents may also be exposed and suffer some of the toxic effects of the polluting agent (Bellinger, 2009). In a non-occupational population, living in the vicinity of a ferro- and silico- manganese alloy plant, (Mergler etal. 1999) reported decreased memory and learning and poorer performance on motor tests among persons with elevated blood Mn levels. In Mexico, studies performed with communities exposed to dust from Mn mines and transformation plants showed an increased risk of deficient cognitive performance and an association between air Mn concentrations, but not MnB levels (Santos-Burgoa et al. 2001).

Children’s exposure has been investigated primarily with respect to ingested Mn through diet: baby formulas (Collip et al. 1983), water (He et al. 1994; Zhang et al. 1995;Wasserman et al. 2006; Bouchard et al. 2007) and for children with elevated Mn from total parenteral nutrition (Alves et al. 1997). One recent study of Mexican children living in an area with manganese mines and small transformation plants showed an inverse association between hair Mn (MnH) concentrations and IQ, with the strongest associations with verbal IQ (Riojas et al, 2010). We recently reported elevated MnH in children living in the vicinity of a ferro-manganese alloy plant (Menezes-Filho et al. 2009); MnH concentrations were significantly associated to the distance and position of their houses relative to wind direction. Mean air Mn concentration in the respirable fraction (PM2.5) sampled during the raining season was, on average, three times higher than the US EPA reference concentration (RfC 0.05 µg/m3) (US EPA, 1993).

The objectives of the present study were to investigate the associations between (i)biomarkers of Mn exposure (hair and blood) and neurobehavioral performance in children living in the vicinity of this ferro-maganese alloy plant; (ii) caregivers’ hair Mn concentrations and performance on a test of intelligence.

Material and Methods

Exposure

Manganese exposure of children from the village of Cotegipe, a small community of 620 people in the municipality of Simões Filho, 30 km from the city of Salvador, State of Bahia, Brazil (Figure 1), was characterized in 2007 (Menezes-Filho et al. 2009). The community resides within a 2-km radius from a ferro-manganese alloy plant and mostly in a downwind direction. The plant was inaugurated in 1970 and after two expansions, three ovens are currently in operation, with an annual production of SiMn and FeMn alloys of 280,000 tons. For the children living in this area, there is a gradient of exposure in relation to the child’s house distance and position with respect to the plant (Menezes-Filho et al. 2009)

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Schematic map of the Cotegipe village in Simões Filho town in the metropolitan area of Salvador, Bahia, Brazil.

Study design and population

A cross-sectional study design was used. Children aged 6 years to 11 years and 11 months, who attended the Cotegipe Elementary School and had lived in the community for at least one year, were invited to participate. The school principal provided us with a list of the 110 children, enrolled and regularly attending classes. We sent out invitations to the mothers or legal guardians of all of the children who were in the specified age range (N=80). All accepted to participate. Five other children, who lived on an isolated road on the northwest side and downwind of the plant, were also included because our previous study indicated that the children on this area presented the highest hair Mn concentrations. The area is separated from the village by the plant. The children attend an elementary school on the boundary of the town of Simões Filho. Thus, a total of 85 children were enrolled in the study. Two children were excluded from the study: one boy had a history of seizure and one girl had hearing problems and used a hearing aid. The final study group was comprised of 83 children.

The Simões Filho Education Department gave permission to use the school premises to set up the study and the school principal gave us full support and provided two rooms where we carried out the assessments activities.

The project was approved by the Federal University of Bahia Ethics Committee. All parents signed the informed consent forms.

Questionnaires

Three psychology students, with clinical and psycho-diagnostic experience, were trained and monitored by the lead psychologist (C.O.N.) to administer the caregivers’ interview questionnaires on socio-economic characteristics, family structure, child development, behavior and illnesses. A second questionnaire, translated and adapted from the HOME Inventory (Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment Scale, NLSY79 Child HOME-SF), comprised 20 items and was used to assess the quality of family environment. It included indicators of cognitive stimulation, parent-child interaction and general interpersonal interactions. A simple score was derived by summing the number of positive answers obtained for the twenty questions.

Anthropometry

A single person performed all weight and height measurements. Children took off their shoes but kept their clothes for both weight and height assessments. Weight was measured using an upright scale (CATSYS 2000 System®, Snerkkersten, Denmark) connected to a computer, with a capacity to weigh 150 kg in 100-g increments. Height was taken using a measuring board. Body mass index (BMI) was calculated by dividing the weight in kilograms by the square of the height in meters. The chronic nutritional index, height-for-age (HA) z-score, encompasses growth and stature, which is inversely related to protein, calcium and iron deficiency during early childhood (Abrams et al. 2005). It was calculated using the AnthroPlus software (WHO, 2009) based on the WHO reference population collected in 2007 for 5–19 years.

Blood measurements

Children’s venous blood samples were collected from the cubital vein into sodium–EDTA vacuum tubes proper for metal analysis (Vacutainer, Bencton Dickinson, USA). Thirteen children (15.7%) refused or were not available to provide blood samples. For blood analyses, we adapted the method described by Montes et al., (2002). The blood sample was diluted 1:5 with matrix modifier (1% ammonium-dihydrogenphosphate in 0.1% Triton X-100 solution). After homogenization in a vortex, it was centrifuged for 10 minutes at 14,000 rpm. Quality control of blood Mn analysis was assured by measuring human blood reference materials QMEQAS07B-03 and QMEQAS07B-06 (Centre de Toxicologie/INSPQ, Canada). Samples were measured in duplicate; every measurement consisted of two injections into graphite furnace, in all cases standard deviation was lower than 10%; if otherwise, sample was reanalyzed.

As lead (Pb) is an ubiquitous contaminant and a recognized neurotoxin, associated with effects on cognition and behavior in children at low PbB, it was also measured by GFAAS with Zeeman background correction (GTA-120, Varian Inc.).

An additional blood sample, collected with a no additive in the vacuum tube (Vacuntainer, Bencton Dickinson, USA) was obtained for serum iron (FeS) determination. After blood clotting, tubes were centrifuged 9000 rpm for twenty minutes. FeS was determined by automated method using a commercial kit (Roche Hitachi 747, Roche®) for 58 children; twelve samples were rejected due to some degree of hemolysis.

Hair measurements

For children, and caregivers, a tuft of hair of approximate 0.5 cm diameter was cut off as close as possible to the scalp in the occipital region, with a surgical stainless steel scissor. Detailed information on hair sampling, washing procedure and Mn determination by GFAAS are reported in Menezes-Filho et al (2009).

Caregivers of seventy-seven children provided hair samples. One mother refused for religious reasons, three mothers refused for personal reasons and a father of one child had hair too short to be sampled.

Psychological Measures

Children’s cognition

The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, version III -WISC-III (Wechsler, 1991), previously validated for Brazilian children (Figueiredo 2002) and suitable for children ≥ 6 years of age, was administered by the same psychologist (C.O.N.) over a period of four weeks in July 2008. Testing followed the procedure recommended in the WISC-III manual. Total testing time ranged between 50 to 80 minutes. The tester was unaware of children’s degree of Mn exposure.

Maternal Cognition

The Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices (Raven et al. 1983) was used to assess caregiver’s intelligence. This instrument is culture-free and has been validated for the Brazilian population. The test was applied individually to each caregiver following the procedure described in the manual. The test is composed of five sets of 12 item of increasing difficulty level. In each test item, the participant is asked to identify the missing item that completes a pattern. It was abbreviated between series, at the examiner’s discretion when it became evident that the response was purely made by guessing.

Statistical Analyses

Descriptive statistics were used to examine the distribution of socio-demographic information, bioindicators of Mn and Pb exposure and cognitive function scores. For normally distributed variables, the arithmetic mean (AM) was used, otherwise the geometric means (GM) are presented.

Pearson correlation tests were applied to evaluate bivariate correlations between co-variables and exposure bioindicators.

Since the distributions of hair and blood metal levels were skewed, data were log10 transformed for further analyses. Backward stepwise regression models were used to identify variables that were potentially associated with intellectual quotients (0.100 to enter; >0.05 to exclude). These variables were then included in linear regression models. The significance level of p=0.05 was used and residual analysis was performed to verify the model’s parameters. Possible associations of other potential confounders (gender, time in years mothers lived in the community prior to child birth, marital status, caregiver’s age, family income, children’s blood lead, serum iron and hemoglobin levels, growth index and nutritional status) of manganese biomarkers’ levels and cognitive functions were examined.

The residuals from the models were assessed in standardized residual vs predicted plot for heterocedasticity and non-linearity and in a half-normal plot for non-normality. All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS version 13 software.

Results

Sample Characteristics

Table 1 presents a summary of the demographic and anthropometric characteristics. The primary caregivers included mothers (94%), a father (1.2%) a grandmother (1.2%) and three stepmothers (3.6%). Children were from 55 families, and approximately half live in a structured family with mother and father. Eight families (9.8%) have only one child, 25.5% have two children, 27.7% have three children, 13.3% of the families have four children and 22.9% have five or more children. The ethnic composition is representative of the population around the Todos os Santos Bay area, comprising approximately 80% of Brazilian-Africans with black curly hair. The socio-economic status is very low; the main income source is from cultivating cassava and rudimental processing and commercialization of manioc flour. The average monthly income is U$168, ranging from 25 to 444 US dollars. Families who maintain children at school receive social monthly benefits. The low SES is reflected in the low nutritional status, the mean age-for-height z-score, which reflects chronic malnutrition, is −0.16 (range −2.39 to 2.54). Four boys and two girls (7.3%) could be classified as suffering from stunted growth, their HA z-scores were below −2.0 (WHO, 2006).

Table 1.

Socio-demographic characteristics and bioindicator levels in the study participants.

N Mean SD Min Max
Age (months) 83 106.1 19.8 74 147
Height (cm) 83 130.2 10.8 108 155
Weight (Kg) 83 26.8 6.9 16.2 49.9
BMI (kg/m2) 83 15.6 2.0 11.8 22.9
HA z-score 83 −0.16 1.21 −2.39 2.54
Boys 44 (53.0%)
Ethnicity
  African-Brazilian 62 (78.1%)
  Non African-Brazilian 21 (21.9%)
Parents living together 47 (56.6%)
Years of maternal education 82 6.9 4.0 0 14
Maternal age at birth 82 24.5 6.4 14.3 46.1
Mother’s Raven score 82 15.9 9.1 5 46
Family income (U$/month) 75 168 103 21 444
HOME Inventory (%) 82 51.8 15.2 6.7 80
Number of children in the home 82 3.6 2.3 1 15
Fe Serum 58 65.7 31.8 11 164
Mn hair (µg/g) 83 5.83a 11.5 0.10 86.68
Mn blood (µg/L) 70 8.2 b 3.6 2.7 23.4
PbB (µg/dL) 70 1.43 a 1.90 0.5 10.35
Mother’s MnH (µg/g) 77 3.50 a 12.76 0.10 77.45

U$ exchange rate=1.868 BRR (Brazilian Reais) on Aug.02.2009

a

geometric mean;

b

arithmetic mean

FeS concentration (N=58) is normally distributed with mean 65.6 µg/dL (range 11–165 µg/dL), with 41.4% below the normal range (55–120 µg/dL) and classified as iron deficient (Takemoto et al. 2004). Fe deficient children have a mean HA z-score of 0.13 versus −0.37 for those with normal FeS, but this difference does not reach statistical significance (p=0.06).

Children who provided blood samples (n=70) and those who were not available to donate blood (n=13) did not statistically differ in several aspects: age, stature, maternal education, family income, MnH levels and performances in IQ tests. Among those who did not provide blood 20.5% (n=9) were boys and 10.3% (n=4) were girls.

Exposure Characteristics

Table 1 also presents the distribution of the biomarkers of metal exposure. A large proportion of MnH concentrations (77.1%) surpass 3.0 µg/g, the upper cutoff limit that has been associated with hyperactive behavior (Bouchard et al. 2007). Blood manganese levels observed in this study (mean 8.2±3.6 µg/L) are similar to the levels observed in other studies with children in the same age range (Rollin et al. 2004 and Kim et al. 2009). Only 4% of blood Mn are above 14 µg/L. PbB is above 2 µg/dL for 51% (n=36) and only one child had PbB slightly above 10 µg/dL. MnH levels are not significantly correlated with age.

Table 2 presents the correlations between biomarkers. No relation was observed between MnB, PbB and FeS. Children with iron deficiency present similar MnB compared to those with normal FeS (8.7±4.52 µg Mn/L vs 7.9±3.24 µgMn/L; p=0.50), however, those with iron deficiency have higher MnH (15.94±19.68 µg/g) compared to those with FeS in the normal range (8.69±8.23 µg Mn /g); difference reached borderline statistical significance (p = 0.059).

Table 2.

Pearson correlation coefficient matrix among metal biomarkers in children and caregivers along with cognitive functions. Data are coefficients, p-values and N.

Log
MnH
LogMnB PbB Maternal
LogMnH
FeS HOME Raven
Score
FS-Q Verbal
IQ
Perform.
IQ
LogMnH 1.00 0.053 0.205 0.202 −0.142 −0.020 −0.153 −0.184 −0.210 −0.054
0.660 0.089 0.079 0.289 0.862 0.167 0.096 0.057 0.625
70 70 77 58 82 83 83 83 83

LogMnB 1.00 0.219 0.307* 0.056 −0.273* −0.190 0.072 0.139 −0.052
0.072 0.012 0.688 0.022 0.114 0.555 0.252 0.672
68 66 54 70 70 70 70 70

PbB 1.00 −0.077 0.028 0.213 −0.213 −0.130 −0.103 −0.173
0.540 0.845 0.076 0.077 0.284 0.397 0.151
52 70 70 70 70 70

Maternal MnH 1.00 −0.140 −0.274* −0.205 −0.144 −0.150 −0.113
0.300 0.016 0.073 0.210 0.194 0.329
77 77 77 77 77

FeS 1.00 0.009 0.009 0.008 −0.006 −0.014
0.945 0.947 0.955 0.964 0.919
58 58 58 58

HOME 1.00 0.245* 0.074 0.024 0.141
0.026 0.507 0.833 0.206
82 82 82

Raven Score 1.00 0.288** 0.218* 0.342**
0.008 0.048 0.002
83 83

FS−IQ 1.00 0.889** 0.872**
0.000 0.000
83 83

Verbal IQ 1.00 0.598**
0.000
83

Performance
IQ
1.00
**

p = 0.01 (2-tailed)

*

p = 0.05 (2-tailed).

Children’s intellectual function

Table 3 presents the summary of the WISC scores. No significant difference was observed between boys and girls. In bivariate analyses, maternal education is significantly correlated with children’s Full-Scale IQ (r=0.300, p=0.006) and Performance IQ, (r=0.364, p=0.001), but not with Verbal IQ. Maternal score on the Raven’s Progressive Matrix is significantly correlated with children’s Full-Scale, Performance and Verbal IQ’s (r=0.311 p=0.004; r=0.341 p=0.002; r=0.221 p=0.044, respectively). Height-for-age z-score is positively correlated with Verbal IQ (r=0.239, p=0.030) and a tendency is observed for Full-Scale IQ (r=0.209, p=0.068). Family income is positively correlated with Full-Scale IQ (r=0.232, p=0.045) and Performance IQ (r=0.229, p=0.044). None of the IQ scores are significantly related to the HOME inventory score.

Table 3.

Summary of the Wechsler test scores of all 83 children according to gender

IQ scores Gender Mean SD Min Max
Full-Scale Boys 84.8 13.16 60 121
Girls 85.3 14.10 50 115

Verbal Boys 89.3 12.97 57 120
Girls 90.6 15.63 55 127

Performance Boys 82.6 13.83 57 117
Girls 80.4 12.74 45 106

Biomarkers of Mn exposure and children’s IQ

In bivariate analyses MnB or MnH levels are not significantly correlated with any of the children’s IQ scores. In multiple regression analyses, MnB does not enter significantly into any of the models that were tested. On the other hand, negative associations are observed with children’s MnH (Table 4). MnH is inversely associated with adjusted Full-Scale and Verbal IQ. Figure 2 presents the adjusted IQ scores with respect to MnH levels. We also tested the association between MnH levels with cognitive indices only of those children who provided blood samples (n=70). There were no significant statistical changes in the coefficients. The beta coefficient of the Verbal IQ (n=70) was −6.65 and 95% CI (12.58 to −0.72). No confounding effects were observed for PbB or low serum iron levels.

Table 4.

Summary of the linear multiple regression models for children’s IQ (n=83)

Intelligence Quotient Crude
Coefficients (95% CI)
Adjusted
Coefficients (95% CI)
Full-scale IQ
  Intercept 88.60 (83.46 to 93.73) 82.80 (76.34 to 89.75)
  LogMnH −4.66 (−10.18 to 0.85) −5.78 (−10.71 to −0.21)
  Maternal Education (Years) 0.992 (0.23 to 1.64)
  Height-for-age z-score 2.47 (−0.53 to 5.48)
Verbal IQ
  Intercept 94.03 (88.67 to 99.38) 90.10 (82.90 to 97.18)
  LogMnH −5.58 (−11.34 to 0.19) −6.72 (−11.81 to −0.63)
  Maternal Education (Years) 0.75 (−0.06 to 1.45)
  Height-for-age z-score 2.45 (−0.39 to 6.00)
Performance IQ
  Intercept 82.56 (77.43 to 87.69) 74.90 (69.37 to 82.32)
  LogMnH −1.36 (−6.87 to 4.16) −2.41(−7.39 to 2.75)
  Maternal Education (Years) 1.23 (0.45 to 1.81)
  Height-for-age z-score 3.04 (0.15 to 5.95)

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Scatter plot of children’s Full-Scale (a), Verbal (b) IQ scores versus Mn hair levels, adjusted for maternal education and nutrition status.

MnH and caregivers’ Intelligence

The caregivers’ mean raw score on the Raven’s Progressive Matrix was 15.9 (SD=9.1, range 5–46). Bivariate analyses show significantly and positive correlations with years of formal education (Pearson Coefficient r=0.540, p<0.001), family income (r=0.378, p=0.001) and the HOME score (r=0.245, p=0.026). On the other hand, it is negatively correlated with age (r=−0.358, p=0.001) and with the log of maternal MnH levels (rho=−0.288, p=0.011). Maternal MnH is also significantly associated with HOME score (Pearson Coefficient r=−0.273, p=0.022) (Table 2). In the multiple regression model, these variables explain 43.2% of the variance in the mother’s score.

Discussion

The findings of this study indicate that for these children living in the vicinity of a manganese alloy production plant, there is a negative association between hair Mn and Full-Scale and Verbal IQ . In addition, primary caregivers cognitive performance was likewise inversely associated with Mn hair level, suggesting that in the vicinity of this plant, Mn exposure is impacting both children and adults.

The MnH concentrations observed here are considerably higher than those reported in other studies; 76.8% are above the 3 µg/g cut-off that was used by Bouchard et al (2007) in their study of children exposed to Mn through well water. MnH in the present study are, on average, six times higher than the levels reported for the general Brazilian population (0.25 −1.15 µg/g) (Miekeley et al. 1998). They are also considerably higher than those observed in children living near a waste site in the USA (mean 0.47 µg/g, range 0.89 – 2.15 µg/g) (Wright et al. 2006). On the other hand, MnB levels are very comparable to levels observed in other studies with children in the same age range. In South African children, MnB levels were 6.7 (1.6 to 32.8) µg/L and 9,8 (3.6 to 26.5) µg/L in Cape Town and Johannesburg, respectively, the authors attribute this difference to a Mn based gasoline additive (Rollin et al. 2005). Nonetheless, MnB concentrations in the present study are lower than those reported by Wasserman et al. 2006 Mn (12.8±3.2 µg Mn/L) for children exposed to Mn drinking from well water in Bangladesh.

In the present study hair, but not blood Mn, was associated with IQ deficits. Riojas et al (2010) had similar findings for school-age children living in a manganese mining region in Mexico. Wasserman et al (2006) did not observe an association between MnB and cognitive function in Bangladeshi children; poorer performance was associated with Mn content in well water. Wright et al. (2006) reported decrements in verbal functions associated with MnH in children living near a toxic waste site and Bouchard et al. (2007) reported a positive relation between hair Mn and behavioral problems in children exposed to Mn through drinking water. There are however some studies that have reported relations between MnB and neurobehavioral performance in infants. Takser et al (2003) found that cord blood manganese in Parisian newborns was associated with lower attention and nonverbal memory at 3 years. Claus Henn et al (2010) observed an inverted U shape doseresponse curve between MnB levels at age of one year of age and a mental development index . Kim et al. (2009), who studied Korean children aged 8–11 years, reported that full-scale IQ and verbal IQ of the children showed significant associations with blood Pb, when blood Mn concentration was above 14 µg/L, but not when Mn was below 14 µg/L, suggesting an effect modification between Pb and Mn. It is difficult to generalize at this stage because blood Mn concentrations vary with age, decreasing from birth onwards (Kirchgessner et al., 1981).

In the present study, the two bioindicators of MnB and MnH were not correlated, which raises the question of what each represents in terms of Mn toxicity. It is possible that MnB may be subjected to greater homeostatic control, while MnH may represent excess Mn. In this region, dominated by a manganese alloy production plant, MnH varies with distance from the plant (Menezes-Filho et al. 2009a).

Similar to the present study, Wasserman et al. (2006), Wright et al. (2006) and Kim et al. (2009), reported more pronounced deficits in the verbal domain. In the present study, a ten-fold increase of Mn in children’s hair is associated with a loss of 6.7 point loss in Verbal IQ score. Verbal IQ reflects crystallized intelligence, related to general knowledge, it demonstrates the extension and depth of information acquired normally through school. Generally it is used to solve problems similar to those experienced in the past (Primi et al. 2001). Cognitive deficits have been reported in adults exposed to airborne Mn (for review see Zoni et al. 2007), but it is only recently that the mechanisms of action have been examined. In non-human primates exposed to Mn by intravenous injection and inhalation, Schneider et al. (2006) observed that chronic manganese exposure was associated with cognitive deficits, such as impaired spatial working memory and behavioral alterations consisting of compulsive-like behaviors. Burton and Guilarte (2009), in a review article of their non-human primate studies suggest that changes in gene expression (i.e. tumor suppressor p53, amyloid beta precursor-like protein (1APLP1)) and markers of neurodegeneration in the frontal cortex (i.e. copper homeostasis dysregulation and extracellular accumulation of toxic peptide beta amyloid (Aβ)) may explain subtle cognitive deficits and other early manifestations of Mn neurotoxicity in humans related to working memory and neuropsychiatric behaviors.

Children’s development is strongly influenced by their families and by the social forces and cultural values in the society they live. Early maternal as well as paternal influences are crucial in children’s development (Barros et al. 2009). In our study the quality of family environment was not associated with children’s cognition, even though it was significantly correlated with maternal intelligence score. In other studies on Mn exposure in children, maternal education was an important covariable in the association between children’s exposure to Mn and neuro-psychological outcomes in France (Takser et al. 2003), in the USA (Wright et al. 2006) and in Bangladesh (Wassermann et al. 2006). In an investigation carried out in the outskirts of Salvador, Brazil on the association between social factors and intelligence among children from low income Families, Andrade et al. (2005) reported a positive association between the quality of family stimulation and children’s cognitive performance. They concluded that a better pattern of stimulation was observed among children who live with their parents and whose mothers have better education, a job and a partner involved in the family environment.

In the present study, cognitive function was markedly affected by the nutritional status of the child. Malnutrition is a well recognized risk factor for intellectual deficit. Niehaus et al. (2002) observed that height-for-weight z-score was positively correlated with the TONI-III (Test of Non-verbal Intelligence) score; Wasserman et al. (2006) reported that anthropometric parameters, such as stature and head circumference, which likewise reflect nutritional status, explained 17.7% of the Verbal IQ in Mn exposed children. Fonseca et al. (2008) also reported significant correlations between height-forage z-scores and several WISC-III subtests in Amazonian children exposed to methyl mercury. The association between height and cognitive outcomes was also observed in a study with Pb exposed Mexican children (Kordas et al. 2004). The authors commented that the association of height-for-age z-score with cognitive performance might be explained to some extent by parent and teacher perceptions of ability in taller children.

This is the first investigation to demonstrate not only that children’s intellectual impairment, but also mothers’ cognition can be affected by Mn exposure. About 40% of the mothers lived in the community since childhood and the alloy plant has been emitting high levels of Mn in the air for almost four decades. Women’s environmental exposure to Mn in reproductive age has been little investigated. Mn blood levels increase during pregnancy, independently of iron status, peaking in the third trimester (Tholin et al. 1995). High MnB levels at delivery were reported by Takser et al. (2003) in pregnant Quebecoise women living near farms where pesticides were sprayed. Ljung et al. (2009) observed high MnB levels, similar to occupationally exposed groups, in pregnant Bangladeshi women exposed to Mn from tube well water.

There are few reports of the effects of environmental Mn on adults. In Mexico, in a pilot study of 72 persons, the majority women (86%), living in a community exposed to high air levels of Mn from Mn mining and transformation activities, the authors reported a twelvefold increase in risk for deficient cognitive performance (cutoff <17) in the Minimental Test score (Santos-Burgoa et al. 2001). A recent study in the same region detected a significant association between elevated Mn air levels and attention deficit in adults of the region; no gender differences were observed (Sollis et al. 2009). In Quebec, Canada, persons living in the vicinity of a manganese alloy plant had higher blood Mn than those loving elsewhere and blood Mn was associated with several neurobehavioral deficits, particularly among the older residents (Mergler et al, 1999)

Although it is not possible to determine the direction of the associations between mother’s cognitive capactities, her educational level and the home environment, the results of our study suggest that there is an association between mothers’ exposure and her performance of the Raven’s. Thus, the children from Cotegipe village may be affected not only through the direct effects of Mn on their own brain, but also, possibly indirectly, as consequence of Mn on their caregiver’s cognition, resulting in a diminished ability to provide better stimulating environment.

The IQ levels observed in Cotegipe’s children are very low, in the range of one SD lower than normal (Figueiredo, 2002). Walker et al. (2007) reviewed the proximal risk factors for child development in the developing countries. Among the most consistent in the literature, inadequate cognitive stimulation was the most important psychosocial determinant along with maternal depression and exposure to violence. The authors also mention other risk factors for impaired child development are biological (stunting, iodine and iron deficiencies, malaria, intrauterine growth retardation) and exposure to metals. The Cotegipe community has a very low socio-economic status and is needy of attention for sanitary and education interventions. There is a high unemployment index and many families depend on governmental social assistance. It is well known that cognitive deficit has multi factorial causation, and yet, despite many adverse factors we were able to observe associations with MnH.

This study bears the limitations of a cross-sectional study and causal inferences cannot be made on the relationship of Mn exposure and cognitive deficits. Moreover the HOME inventory adaptation has not been validated previously and FeS measurements were not available for all of the children. Only bioindicators of manganese and lead exposure were measured, it is possible that other neurotoxic chemicals are present in this environment. On the other hand, we were able to test all of the children in the desired age range attending the only local elementary school, thus avoiding selection bias.

In the face of the evidence presented here, we strongly recommend cognitive strategy interventions. These interventions are designed to improve performance through compensatory procedures or through more efficient functioning of weak or deficient processes (Morris and Mather, 2008). An intervention program with mothers in the Northeastern, Brazil, observed positive associations in cognitive and motor development in children whose mothers were included in the intervention group (Eickmann et al. 2003). Barros et al. (2009) demonstrated a clear interaction between stimulation and maternal schooling, indicating not only that stimulation has a stronger effect in children of lesseducated mothers, but also that by effectively stimulating these children they can achieve a very similar result to those who are more stimulated with better-schooled mothers.

Table 5.

Linear regression model for caregivers’ cognition (n=70).

Variable Crude Coefficients
(95% CI)
Adjusted Coefficients
(95% CI)
Intercept 17.1 (14.5 to 19.6) 12.7 (3.8 to 21.5)
LogMnH −2.73 (−5.72 to 0.27) −2.69 (−5.43 to 0.05)
Education Years 0.970 (0.50 to 1.44)
Age −0.176 (−0.40 to 0.05)
Family Income 0.02 (0.01 to 0.04)

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to Cotegipe’s children and their parents. We acknowledge the collaboration of Genésia Lopes Braga, the principal of Cotegipe Elementary School. Menezes-Filho, is an Irving J. Selikoff International Scholar of the Mount Sinai School of Medicine. This work was supported in part by grant award number D43TW00640 from the Fogarty International Center. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the Fogarty International Center or the National Institutes of Health.

Footnotes

This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Declaration: The authors declare that there are no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Abrams SA, Griffin IJ, Hawthorne KM, Liang L. Height and Height Z-Score Are Related to Calcium Absorption in Five to Fifteen-Year-Old Girls. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2005;90:5077–5081. doi: 10.1210/jc.2005-0537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Alves G, Thiebot J, Tracqui A, Delangre T, Guedon C, Lerebours E. Neurologic disorders due to brain manganese deposition in a jaundiced patient receiving longterm parenteral nutrition. J Parenter Enteral Nutr. 1997;21:41–45. doi: 10.1177/014860719702100141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Andrade SJ, Santos DN, Bastos AC, Pedromônico MRM, Almeida-Filho N, Barreto ML. Family environment and child’s cognitive development: an epidemiological approach. Rev Saude Pública. 2005;39(4):1–6. doi: 10.1590/s0034-89102005000400014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Antonini JM, Santamaria AB, Jenkins NT, Albini E, Lucchini A. Fate of manganese associated with the inhalation of welding fumes: Potential neurological effects. NeuroToxicology. 2006;27:304–310. doi: 10.1016/j.neuro.2005.09.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Aschner JL, Aschner M. Nutritional aspects of manganese homeostasis. Mol Aspects Med. 2005;26:353–362. doi: 10.1016/j.mam.2005.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Barlow PJ. A pilot study on the metal levels in the hair of hyperactive children. Med Hypotheses. 1983;11:309–318. doi: 10.1016/0306-9877(83)90094-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Barros AJD, Matijasevich M, Santos I, Halpern R. Child development in a birth cohort: effect of child stimulation is stronger in less educated mothers. Int J of Epidemiol. 2009;39(1):1–10. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyp272. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Brenneman KA, Wong BA, Bucellato MA, Costa ER, Gross EA, Dorman DC. Direct olfactory transport of inhaled manganese (54MnCl2) to the rat brain: toxikinetic investigations in a unilateral nasal occlusion model. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol. 2000;169:238–248. doi: 10.1006/taap.2000.9073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bouchard M, Laforest F, Vandelac L, Bellinger D, Mergler Donna. Hair Manganese and Hyperactive Behaviors: Pilot Study of School-Age Children Exposed through Tap Water. Environ Health Perspect. 2007;15(1):122–127. doi: 10.1289/ehp.9504. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Burton NC, Guilarte TR. Manganese neurotoxicity: lessons learned from longitudinal studies in nonhuman primates. Environ Health Perspect. 2009;117(3):325–332. doi: 10.1289/ehp.0800035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) [Accessed on Oct. 26 2009];Atlanta, GA: Preventing Lead Poisoning in Young Children: A Statement by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. 1991 Available at: < http://www.cdc.gov/nceh/lead/publications/books/plpyc/contents.htm.
  12. Claus Henn B, Ettinger AS, Schwartz J, Téllez-Rojo MM, Lamadrid-Figuero H, Hernández-Avila M, Schnaas L, Amarasiriwardena C, Bellinger DC, Hu H, Wright RO. Early Postnatal Blood Manganese Levels and Children’s Neurodevelopment. Epidemiology. 2010;21(4):433–439. doi: 10.1097/ede.0b013e3181df8e52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Collip PJ, Chen SY, Maitinsky CS. Manganese in infant formulas and learning disability. Ann Nutr Metab. 1983;27:488–494. doi: 10.1159/000176724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Coutinho G, Mattos P, Malloy-Diniz LF. Neuropsychological differences between attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and control children and adolescents referred for academic impairment. Rev Bras Psiquiatr. 2009;31(2):141–144. doi: 10.1590/s1516-44462009000200011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Dorman DC, Struve MF, Marshall MW, Parkinson CU, James RA, Wong BA. Tissue manganese concentrations in young male rhesus monkeys following subchronic manganese sulfate inhalation. Toxicol Sci. 2006;92(1):201–210. doi: 10.1093/toxsci/kfj206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Eickmann SH, Lima AC, Guerra MQ, Lima MC, Lira PIC, Huttly SRA, Ashworth Al. Improved cognitive and motor development in a community-based intervention of psychosocial stimulation in northeast Brazil. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2003;45:536–541. doi: 10.1017/s0012162203000987. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Erikson KM, Thompson K, Aschner J, Aschner M. Manganese neurotoxicity: A focus on the neonate. Pharmacol Therapeut. 2007;113(2):369–377. doi: 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2006.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Figueiredo VLM. Casa do Psicólogo. São Paulo: 2002. WISC-III: Escala de Inteligência Wechsler para Crianças: Manual. Adaptação e Padronização de uma amostra Brasileira. [Google Scholar]
  19. Fitsanakis V, Zhang Na, Avison MJ, Gore JC, Aschner JL, Aschner M. The use of magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) in the study of manganese neurotoxicity. NeuroToxicol. 2006;27(5):798–806. doi: 10.1016/j.neuro.2006.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Fonseca LC, Tedrus GMAS, Pacheco EMC, Berretta MFB, Campregher AA, Costa DM. Benign childhood epilepsy with centro-temporal pikes - Correlation between clinical, cognitive and EEG aspects. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2007;65(3-A):569–575. doi: 10.1590/s0004-282x2007000400004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Fonseca MF, Dórea JG, Bastos WR, Marques RC, Torres JPM, Malm O. Poor psychometric scores of children living in isolated riverine and agrarian communities and fish–methylmercury exposure. NeuroToxicol. 2008;29:1008–1015. doi: 10.1016/j.neuro.2008.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Golub MS, Hogrefe CE, Germann SL, Tran TT, Beard JL, Crinella FM, Lonnerdal B. Neurobehavioral evaluation of rhesus monkey infants fed cow’s milk formula, soy formula, or soy formula with added manganese. Neurotox Teratol. 2005;27(4):615–627. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2005.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Graumann R, Paris I, Martinez-Alvar Oxidation of dopamine to aminochrome as a mechanism for neurodegeneration of dopaminergic systems in Parkinson’s disease. Possible neuroprotective role of DT-diaphorase. Pol J Pharmacol. 2002;54:573–579. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gulson B, Mizona K, Taylor A, Korsch M, Stauber J, Davis JM, Louie H, Wu M, Swan H. Changes in manganese and lead in the environment and Young children associated with the introduction of methylcyclopentadienyl manganese tricarbonyl in gasoline—preliminary results. Environ Res. 2006;100:100–114. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2005.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. He P, Liu DH, Zhang GQ. Effects of high-level-manganese sewage irrigation on children's neurobehavior. Zhonghua Yu Fang Yi Xue Za Zhi (Chin J Prevent Med) 1994;28(4):216–218. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Jardim-Botelho A, Raff S, Rodrigues RA, Hoffman HJ, Diemert JD, Oliveira RC, Bethony JM, Gazzinelli MF. Hookworm, Ascaris lumbricoides infection and polyparasitism associated with poor cognitive performance in Brazilian schoolchildren. Trop Med Int Health. 2008;13(8):994–1004. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3156.2008.02103.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Kirchgessner M, Weigand E, Schwartz FJ. Absorption of zinc, manganese in relation to age. In: Howell J McC, Gawthorne JM., editors. Trace element metabolism in man and animals. Canberra: Australian Academy of Sciences; 1981. p. 715. [Google Scholar]
  28. Kim Y, Kim BN, Hong YC, Shin MS, Yoo HJ, Kim JW, Bhang SY, Cho SC. Co-exposure to environmental lead and manganese affects the intelligence of school-aged children. NeuroTox. 2009;30:564–571. doi: 10.1016/j.neuro.2009.03.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kondakis XG, Makris N, Leotsinidis M, Prinou M, Papapetropoulos T. Possible health effects of high manganese concentration in drinking water. Arc Environ Health. 1989;44(3):175–178. doi: 10.1080/00039896.1989.9935883. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Kordas K, Lopez P, Rosado JL, Garcia Vargas G, Rico JA, Ronquillo D, Cebrian ME, Stoltzfus RJ. Blood Lead, Anemia, and Short Stature Are Independently Associated with Cognitive Performance in Mexican School Children. J Nut. 2004;134(2):363–371. doi: 10.1093/jn/134.2.363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Krachler M, Rossipal E, Micetic-Turk D. Trace element transfer from the mother to the newborn-- investigations on triplets of colostrum, maternal and umbilical cord sera. Eur J Clin Nutr. 1999;53(6):486–494. doi: 10.1038/sj.ejcn.1600781. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Larsen NA, Pakkenberg H, Damsgaard E, Heydorn K. Topographical distribution of arsenic, manganese, and selenium in the normal human brain. J Neural Sci. 1979;42:407–416. doi: 10.1016/0022-510x(79)90173-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Landrigan PJ, Sonawane B, Butler RN, Trasande L, Callan R, Droller D. Early environmental origins of neurodegenerative disease in later life. Environ Health Perspect. 2005;113(9):1230–1233. doi: 10.1289/ehp.7571. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Lewis SJ, Dove A, Robbins TW, Barker RA, Owen AM. Striatal contributions to working memory: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study in humans. Eur J Neurosci. 2004;19:755–760. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03108.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Lezak M. Neuropsychological Assessment. EUA: Oxford University Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  36. Mena I, Horiuchi K, Burke K, et al. Chronic manganese poisoning: Individual susceptibility and absorption of iron. Neurology. 1969;19:1000–1006. doi: 10.1212/wnl.19.10.1000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Menezes-Filho JAa, Paes CR, Pontes AC, Moreira JC, Sarcinelli PN, Mergler D. High levels of hair manganese in children living in the vicinity of a ferro-manganese alloy production plant. NeuroTox. 2009;30(6):1207–1213. doi: 10.1016/j.neuro.2009.04.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Menezes-Filho JAb, Bouchard M, Sarcinelli PN, Moreira JC. Manganese exposure and neuropsychological effect on children and adolescents: A review. The Pan-Am J Pub Health. 2009;26(6):541–548. doi: 10.1590/s1020-49892009001200010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Mergler D, Baldwin M. Early manifestations of manganese neurotoxicity in humans: an update. Environ Res. 1997;73:92–100. doi: 10.1006/enrs.1997.3710. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Mergler D, Baldwin M, Bélanger S, Larribe F, Beuler A, Bowler R, Panisset M, Edwards R, de Geoffroy A, Sassine MP, Hudnell HK. Manganese neurotoxicity: a continuum of dysfunction: results from a community based study. NeuroTox. 1999;20:327–342. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Miekeley N, Carneiro MTWD, Da Silveira CLP. How reliable are human hair reference intervals for trace elements? Sci Total Environ. 1998;218:9–17. doi: 10.1016/s0048-9697(98)00185-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Niehaus MD, Moore SR, Patrick PD, Derr LL, Lorntz B, Lima AA, Guerrant RL. Early childhood diarrhea is associated with diminished cognitive Function 4 to 7 years later in children in a northeast Brazilian shantytown. Am J Trop Med Hyg. 2002;66(5):590–593. doi: 10.4269/ajtmh.2002.66.590. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Pihl RO, Parkes M. Hair element content in learning disabled children. Science. 1977;198:204–206. doi: 10.1126/science.905825. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Primi R, Santos AAA, Vendramini CM, et al. Competências e habilidades cognitivas: diferentes definições dos mesmos construtos. Psic: Teoria e Pesq. 2001;17:151–159. [Google Scholar]
  45. Raven JC, Court JH, Raven J. Manual for Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales (Section 3) - Standard Progressive Matrices. London: Lewis; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  46. Rektor J, Bares M, Brazdil M, Kanovsky P, Rektorova I, Sochurkova D, Kubová D, Kuba R, Daniel P. Cognitive-and-movement-related potentials recorded in human basal ganglia. Mov Disord. 2005;20:562–568. doi: 10.1002/mds.20368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Rice D, Barone S., Jr Critical periods of vulnerability for the developing nervous system: evidence from humans and animal models. Environ Health Perspect. 2000;108(3):511–533. doi: 10.1289/ehp.00108s3511. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Riojas-Rodríguez H, Solís-Vivanco R, Schilmann A, Montes S, Rodríguez S, Ríos C, Rodríguez-Agudelo Y. Intellectual Function in Mexican Children Environmentally Exposed to Manganese Living in a Mining. AreaHoracio. 2010 doi: 10.1289/ehp.0901229. Online 01 Jun 2010 | doi:10.1289/ehp.0901229. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Rollin H, Mathee A, Levin J, Theodorou P, Wewersc F. Blood manganese concentrations among first-grade schoolchildren in two South African cities. Environ Res. 2005;97:93–99. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2004.05.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Santos-Burgoa C, Rios C, Mercado LA, et al. Exposure to manganese: health effects on the general population, a pilot study in central Mexico. Environ Res. 2001;85:90–104. doi: 10.1006/enrs.2000.4108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Smargiassi A, Takser L, Masse A, Sergerie M, Mergler D, et al. A comparative study of manganese and lead levels in human umbilical cords and maternal blood from two urban centers exposed to different gasoline additives. Sci Total Environ. 2002;290:157–164. doi: 10.1016/s0048-9697(01)01071-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Schneider JS, Decamp E, Koser AJ, Fritz S, Gonczi H, Syversen T, et al. Effects of chronic manganese exposure on cognitive and motor functioning in non-human primates. Brain Res. 2006;118:222–231. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.08.054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Takeda A. Manganese action in brain function. Brain Res Rev. 2003;41:79–87. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(02)00234-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Takemoto C, Holding J, Kraus D. Pediatric Dosage handbook. 11th ed. Hudson, OH: LexiComp; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  55. Takser L, Mergler D, Hellier G, Sahuquillo J, Huel G. Manganese, Monoamine Metabolite Levels at Birth, and Child Psychomotor Development. NeuroTox. 2003;24:667–674. doi: 10.1016/S0161-813X(03)00058-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Tholin K, Sandstrom B, Palm R, Hallmans G. Changes in blood manganese levels during pregnancy in iron supplemented and non supplemented women. J Trace Elem Med Biol. 1995;9(1):13–17. doi: 10.1016/S0946-672X(11)80003-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. To T, Guttmann A, Dick PT, Rosenfield JD, Parkin PC, Tassoudji M, Vydykhan TN, Cao H, Harris JK. Risk markers for poor developmental attainment in young children: results from a longitudinal national survey. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2004;158:643–649. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.158.7.643. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Walker SP, Wachs TD, Gardner JM, Lozoff B. Child development: risk factors for adverse outcomes in developing countries. Lancet. 2007;369:145–157. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)60076-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Wasserman GA, Liu X, Parvez F, Ahsan H, Factor-Litvak P, van Geen A, et al. Water arsenic exposure and children’s intellectual function in Araihazar, Bangladesh. Environ Health Perspect. 2004;12:1329–1333. doi: 10.1289/ehp.6964. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Wasserman GA, Liu X, Parvez F, Ahsan H, Levy D, Factor-Litvak P, et al. Water manganese exposure and children’s intellectual function in Araihazar, Bangladesh. Environ Health Perspect. 2006;114(1):124–129. doi: 10.1289/ehp.8030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Wechsler D. Manual for the WISC-III. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  62. WHO Multicentre Growth Reference Study Group. WHO Child Growth Standards: Length/height-for-age, weight-for-age, weight-for-length, weight-for-height and body mass index-for-age: Methods and development. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2006. p. 312. [Google Scholar]
  63. WHO. Geneva: WHO; AnthroPlus for personal computers Manual: Software for assessing growth of the world'schildren and adolescents. 2009 ( http://www.who.int/growthref/tools/en/).
  64. Woolf A, Wright R, Amarasiriwardena C, Bellinger D. A Child with Chronic Manganese Exposure from Drinking Water. Environ Health Perspect. 2002;110(6):613–616. doi: 10.1289/ehp.02110613. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Wright RO, Amarasiriwardena C, Woolf AD, Jim R, Bellinger DC. Neuropsychological correlates of hair arsenic, manganese, and cadmium levels in school-age children residing near a hazardous waste site. NeuroTox. 2006;27:210–216. doi: 10.1016/j.neuro.2005.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Zhang G, Liu D, He P. A preliminary study of the effects of manganese on learning abilities primary school pupils. Zhonghua Yu Fang Yi Xue Za Zhi (Chin J Prevent Med) 1995;29(3):156–158. 157. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Zoni S, Albini E, Lucchini R. Neuropsychological testing for the assessment of manganese neurotoxicity: A review and a proposal. Am J Ind Med. 2007;50(11):812–830. doi: 10.1002/ajim.20518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES