Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2012 Feb 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Behav Med. 2010 Sep 1;34(1):53–63. doi: 10.1007/s10865-010-9286-4

Personal attributions for melanoma risk in melanoma-affected patients and family members

Jennifer Hay a, Marco DiBonaventura b, Raymond Baser a, Nancy Press c, Jeanne Shoveller d, Deborah Bowen e
PMCID: PMC3026094  NIHMSID: NIHMS254794  PMID: 20809355

Abstract

Personal attributions for cancer risk involve factors that individuals believe contribute to their risk for developing cancer. Understanding personal risk attributions for melanoma may dictate gene-environment melanoma risk communication strategies. We examined attributions for melanoma risk in a population-based sample of melanoma survivors, first degree family members, and family members who are also parents (N=939). We conducted qualitative examination of open-ended risk attributions and logistic regression examining predictors (demographics, family member type, perceived risk) of the attributions reported (ultraviolet radiation [UVR] exposure, heredity/genetics, phenotype, personal melanoma history, miscellaneous). We found a predominance of risk attributions to UVR and heredity/genetics (80% and 45% of the sample, respectively). Those reporting higher education levels were more likely to endorse attributions to heredity/genetics, as well as to phenotype, than those of lower education levels. First-degree relatives and parent family members were more likely to endorse heredity/genetic attributions than melanoma survivors; melanoma survivors were more likely to endorse personal history of melanoma attributions compared to first-degree relatives and parent family members. These findings inform the development of risk communication interventions for melanoma families.

Keywords: risk perceptions, risk attributions, melanoma families

Background

Personal attributions for illness risk involve factors that individuals believe contribute to their risk for developing an illness such as cancer (Weinstein, 1984, 1987). Perceptions of cancer risk have been heavily researched, as they are theoretically (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980; Cameron, 2007; Cameron & Leventhal, 2003; Janz & Becker, 1984; Rogers & Mewborn, 1976; Sutton, 1982) and empirically (Edwards, et al., 2006; McCaul, Branstetter, Schroeder, & Glasgow, 1996) important in motivating cancer screening and risk reduction behaviors, but nevertheless are relatively resistant to change over time (Weinstein, Sandman, & Hallman, 1994; Weinstein, Sandman, & Roberts, 1991). Little is known about the attributions through which risk perceptions are formed (French & Hevey, 2008; Lipkus, et al., 2005). Such processes may involve an amalgam of a person s knowledge of personally relevant cancer risk factors as well as subjective beliefs about these risk factors. Understanding cancer risk attributions could shed light on why cancer risk perceptions might be difficult to change, clarify beliefs that may modify response to risk communication interventions, and inform communication strategies to at-risk populations that keep pace with our rapidly developing understanding of the genetic, environmental, and lifestyle risk factors associated with the development of many cancers (Rimer & Glassman, 1999).

Personal attributions for cancer risk may be a particularly fruitful area for exploration among individuals who are at risk for developing melanoma, a cancer with a number of established risk factors. Incident melanoma is expected in approximately 69,000 individuals in the United States in 2009 (American Cancer Society, 2009). Ultraviolet radiation exposure (Armstrong & Kricker, 2001; Gandini, et al., 2005), genetically-affected phenotypes such as mole number, red hair and light skin (Ford, et al., 1995) as well as genetic mutations or common genetic polymorphisms (Nelson & Tsao, 2009) can all play a role in increasing risk. Family history of melanoma also increases risk; it likely comprises all the risk elements mentioned above.

Two small studies have examined personal risk attributions for skin cancer. In 40 college students, exposure to ultraviolet radiation was noted by 20 students and genetics, skin or hair color (all coded as “genetic factors” by investigators) were noted by 14 students as important factors in determining their skin cancer risk (French & Hevey, 2008). In a second study of 31 individuals who had survived melanoma at least five years, most (58%) reported that they held a belief in a specific cause of their melanoma diagnosis. The most frequently reported attribution was sun exposure (n = 11; 35% of participants), followed by childhood sunburns (n = 2; 6%), heredity (n = 2; 6%), stress (n = 1; 3%), household chemicals (n = 1; 3%), and occupational hazards (n = 1; 3%). While sun exposure was the most likely attribution, it is important to note that the assertion of no specific cause (n = 15; 48%) was the most common response to the attribution question (Dirksen, 1995). These small studies raise important unanswered questions concerning the extent to which melanoma attributions vary across demographic groups, risk groups (i.e., melanoma survivors and first-degree relatives) and levels of perceived risk. In the current study, we examine personal attributions for melanoma risk in a large, population-based sample of individuals affected by melanoma, as well as their family members. We examine prevalence of melanoma attributions and patterns of attributions across demographic, family member type, and perceived risk groups.

Methods

Data for this study was drawn from baseline assessment in a randomized trial of a web-based intervention (“SunTalk,” R01 CA107430, Bowen, PI). The purpose of SunTalk was to increase knowledge, awareness, and communication about melanoma prevention in families affected by this disease.

Sample

The recruitment process has been described in more detail elsewhere (Bowen, et al., in press,Bowen, et al., 2010). Briefly, melanoma cases were recruited through a population-based cancer registry (Anton-Culver, et al., 2003), the Northwest Cancer Genetics Network. This registry is funded by the National Cancer Institute to act as an infrastructure for studies of cancer genetic susceptibility. Participants in the Northwest Cancer Genetics Network are people with cancer identified by state and national cancer registries, first-degree relatives of cancer cases, people who self-referred to the Network by responding to a media campaign, and controls recruited from a random population sample. Informed Consent was obtained from prospective study participants after the nature of the procedure had been fully explained to them. Cases diagnosed with a first primary melanoma between April 1st 1998 and October 1st 2001 were identified for inclusion. The melanoma case, one first-degree relative of the case, and one parent of a child aged 18 or younger were included in the study. The eligibility criteria for the family members were as follows: 1) The Case was an individual diagnosed with melanoma, aged 18 years or older, 2) The First-Degree Relative was defined as a parent, child or sibling of the case, related biologically and/or socially. This meant biological, adoptive, half or step relationships qualified but spouses did not. 3) The Parent must have had primary responsibility for the child s health and the child must live with the parent at least 50% of the time. Both parent and child could have been related to each other and to the case via either a biological or social relationship. This means biological, adoptive, half or step relationships qualified; spouses of cases did not qualify. Parents could also be related via marriage to the case, but not on the spouse s side (for example, case s sister s husband qualifies as parent, case s spouse s sister does not qualify as parent). The parent group was included in the intervention study because of the intervention focus on the entire family unit, and the fact that a melanoma diagnosis in the family may influence cognitions and behaviors through the family system, beyond first-degree relatives.

The opportunities afforded by a cancer diagnosis may lead to increases in risk appreciation, as well as information-seeking about risk, in first-degree relatives as well as other relatives. In the case of melanoma risk, in particular, sun exposure in childhood is likely to be more highly related to subsequent melanoma diagnosis than adulthood sun exposure (Veierod, Adami, Lund, Armstrong, & Weiderpass, 2010; Whiteman, Whiteman, & Green, 2001), so inclusion of relatives who are in a position to encourage and support sun protection in young children in the family is likely informative in planning for a family-focused intervention for melanoma risk reduction. We include the parent group in the current analyses given that a melanoma diagnosis in the family may impact personal risk attributions in different family members in similar or dissimilar ways, but this has not been examined in prior studies. All study participants had to have access to the Internet from a place that would be comfortable for accessing the study website. If the case was eligible and interested, project staff then collected relative names, relationships, contact information, and permission to contact all possible first-degree relatives and parents. Specifically, we told participating families that we were recruiting them to an intervention study to help them reduce their melanoma risk. Overall, 939 participants (313 families) completed a baseline survey and were included in the present study and family member type involved case, first-degree relative, or parent for each study participant.

We conducted a baseline telephone survey administered by trained interviewers prior to randomization and intervention delivery. All measures were self-report and those relevant to the current paper are described below.

Measures

Personal risk perceptions and attributions for melanoma risk. We asked participants to report their risk for developing melanoma compared to others of the same age and sex with the following question, “Compared to other people your age and sex, your chances of having melanoma in the future are…” (Response categories: Much lower [1]; A little lower [2]; Average [3]; A little higher [4], Much higher [5]…than other people your age and sex (Weinstein, 1980; Weinstein & Lachendro, 1982), and then to report, in an open-ended response, those personal attributions for increased melanoma risk, using the following question, “Overall, what do you think contributes to your risk for developing melanoma?” (Weinstein, 1984, 1987). We allowed them to report up to four separate personal attributions for their melanoma risk.

We measured risk perceptions as comparative risk because it is a widely utilized measure of risk perception in the cancer context and early work examining the link between perceived risk and causal attributions used comparative risk perceptions as an initial “prompt” for causal attributions (Weinstein, 1984, 1987), and because responses on comparative risk measures tend to be somewhat more normally distributed and less positively skewed than measures of absolute likelihood (see Hay, Coups, & Ford, 2006), which is particularly important for examining risk perceptions regarding cancers with low actual incidence rates such as melanoma.

We collected background information from all participants using standard questions on age, gender, ethnic/racial background (White/Caucasian, Black or African-American, Native American or Aleut or Eskimo, Asian, Pacific Islander, Hawaiian, Spanish, Hispanic or Latino, other), education level (8 years or less, some high school, high school graduate, some college or technical school, graduated from college or beyond), and marital status (never married, married or living as married, separated, divorced, widowed), income (less than $15k, $15k–$30k, $31k–$50k, $51k–$70k, $71k–$99k, $100k or above). Information obtained from the Northwest Cancer Genetics Network database for each case included diagnosis date and stage at diagnosis.

Analyses

Our analysis plan involved qualitative and quantitative examination of personal risk attributions. First, we report descriptive statistics for all demographic variables (See Table 1). Then we conducted a thematic analysis that allowed us to code all personal risk attributions into 14 categories (See Table 2). We use these 14 qualitatively-generated categories of personal risk attributions to construct more global attribution types. Next, we quantitatively examine predictors of each category, using univariate Chi-Square (Table 3) and multivariable logistic regression models (Table 4). Finally, we examine interactions between predictors for each attribution type. In order to incorporate the potential correlations introduced within family clusters, we used generalized estimating equations to derive all significance tests and logistic regression parameters.

Table 1.

Demographic information (N = 939)

Demographic variable n %
Family member type
 Case 313 33.3
 First-degree relative 313 33.3
 Parent 313 33.3
Gender
 Female 579 61.7
 Male 360 38.3
Age
 Under 50 451 48.0
 50 and older 488 52.0
Incomea
 < $15K 21 2.2
 $15K – $30K 48 5.1
 $31K – $50K 138 14.7
 $51K – $70K 192 20.4
 $71K – $99K 185 19.7
 >$99K 296 31.5
Raceb
 White 907 96.6
 Bi- or Multi-racial 28 3.0
Hispanic origin
 No 934 99.5
 Yes 5 0.5
Educationc
 < High school (HS) 3 0.3
 Some HS 6 0.6
 HS/GED 82 8..7
 Some college 268 28.5
 College degree 579 61.7
Marital status
 Never married 62 6.6
 Married/living as married 764 81.4
 Separated 9 1.0
 Divorced 76 8.1
 Widowed 28 3.0
SEER summary stage
 Not applicable 626 67.7
 In situ 38 4.0
 Localized 247 26.3
 Regional, direct extension only 2 0.2
 Regional, regional lymph nodes only 17 1.8
 Unstaged 2 0.2
Year of diagnosis
 Not applicable 626 66.7
 1998 44 4.7
 1999 64 6.8
 2000 70 7.5
 2001 41 4.4
 2005 94 10.0
a

Seven participants (0.7%) reported that did not know their income and 52 participants (5.5%) refused to answer.

b

Four participants (0.4%) refused to provide their race.

c

One participant (0.1%) reported that they did not know their highest level of education.

d

Four participants (0.4%) did not have any tumor information and two participants (0.2%) had non-skin melanoma.

Table 2.

Frequencies of risk attribution categories organized by response order (N = 939)

Risk Attribution Category 1st Response 2nd Response 3rd Response 4th Response Total Responses Total Participants

n % n % n % n % n % n %
Sun exposure 372 40.0 212 32.8 62 24.5 19 24.4 665 34.9 574 61.1
“I work outside in the yard a lot and haven’t been good about covering up in the past”
Heredity/genetics 232 24.9 145 22.4 56 22.1 11 14.1 444 23.3 418 44.5
“Melanoma runs on both sides of my family”
Sun exposure as a child 86 9.2 86 13.3 36 14.2 7 9.0 215 11.3 205 21.8
“I was raised on a lake and ran around all day without sunscreen”
Phenotype 70 7.5 64 9.9 21 8.3 7 9.0 162 8.5 155 16.5
“Blonde hair, blue eyes”
Personal history of melanoma 69 7.4 31 4.8 18 7.1 1 1.3 119 6.2 114 12.1
“The fact that I’ve already had it”
Stress 6 0.6 8 1.2 - - - - 14 0.7 14 1.5
“Stress in my life is a big factor”
Many moles 15 1.6 17 2.6 15 5.9 8 10.3 55 2.9 54 5.8
“I have tons of moles”
Combination of risks 31 3.3 19 2.9 5 2.0 4 5.1 59 3.1 54 5.8
“Melanoma in the family and many precancerous moles removed”
Many tanning experiences 6 0.6 - - - - - - 6 0.3 6 .6
“Too much tanning; I love the feeling of sun on my skin”
Use of tanning booths or beds 3 0.3 5 0.8 1 0.4 1 1.3 10 0.5 10 1.1
“Definitely my use of tanning booths increases risk”
Age 2 0.2 1 0.2 3 1.2 - - 6 0.3 6 .6
“My age”
Miscellaneous comments 16 1.7 47 7.3 29 11.5 16 20.5 108 5.7 93 9.9
“Pollution is my biggest concern; paint etc”
Not at risk 13 1.4 3 0.5 2 0.8 - - 18 0.9 16 1.7
“I do not feel I’m at risk.”
Don t know/no response 9 1.0 9 1.4 5 2 4 5.1 27 1.4 21 2.2

Total attributions for each response 930 647 253 78 1908

Table 3.

Frequencies of attributions by predictor (N =939)

Predictor UVR Exposure Heredity Phenotype Personal Hx Melanoma Miscellaneous

n % n % N % n % n %
Family member type
 Case 242 77.3 76 24.3a 79 25.2 90 28.7a 43 13.7a
 FDR* 253 80.8 180 57.5b 64 20.4 a 18 5.7b 35 11.2
 Parent 256 81.8 184 58.8b 87 27.8 b 6 1.9c 26 8.3b
Gender
 Female 465 80.3 297 51.3a 164 28.3a 75 12.9 71 12.3
 Male 286 79.4 143 39.7b 66 18.3b 39 10.8 33 9.1
Age
 Below 50 362 80.3 254 56.3a 124 27.5a 35 7.8a 45 10.0
 50 and older 389 79.7 186 38.1b 106 21.7b 79 16.2b 59 12.1
Education
 Up to HS 72 79.1 27 29.7a 11 12.1a 10 11.0 9 9.9
 Some college 207 77.2 127 47.4b 73 27.2b 32 11.9 33 12.3
 Graduate 472 81.5 286 49.4b 146 25.2b 71 12.3 61 10.5
Marital status
 Not married 137 78.3 81 46.3 42 24.0 18 10.3 22 12.6
 Married 614 80.4 359 47.0 188 24.6 96 12.6 82 10.7
Income
 Up to 70K 315 78.9 176 44.1 102 25.6 52 13.0 45 11.3
 Over 70K 396 82.3 233 48.4 115 23.9 55 11.4 51 10.6
Perceived risk
 Low risk 365 80.0 167 36.6a 81 17.8a 39 8.5a 51 11.2
 High risk 379 80.3 267 56.6b 148 31.4b 73 15.5b 51 10.8

Notes:

*

First-degree relative. Within each predictor, significant differences (p < .05) between levels are denoted by different letters (levels with the same letter are not significantly different). Significance tests calculated using GEE to correct for clustering.

Table 4.

Logistic regression results of the demographic predictors regressed on the five attribution types

Predictor UVR Exposure Heredity Phenotype Personal HX Melanoma Miscellaneous

OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI)
Family member type
 FDR* vs Case 1.26 (0.83–1.89) 5.77 (3.92–8.49)* 0.74 (0.50–1.12) 0.16 (0.09–0.28)* 0.86 (0.51–1.48)
 Parent vs Case 1.24 (0.80–1.93) 4.78 (3.08–7.42)* 0.97 (0.62–1.50) 0.04 (0.01–0.10)* 0.58 (0.30–1.12)
Gender
 Female vs Male 1.09 (0.75–1.59) 1.38 (1.00–1.91) 1.53 (1.07–2.18)* 1.64 (1.02–2.66)* 1.38 (0.83–2.27)
Age
 Under 50 vs 0.96 (0.65–1.42) 1.22(0.87–1.69) 1.17 (0.81–1.70) 0.86 (0.50–1.48) 0.92 (0.54–1.58)
 50 and over
Education
 ≤ High School vs College graduate 0.85 (0.47–1.54) 0.42 (0.25–0.71)* 0.40 (0.21–0.79)* 0.61 (0.28–1.32) 0.92 (0.42–2.00)
 Some college vs College graduate 0.77 (0.52–1.14) 0.76 (0.54–1.08) 1.07 (0.76–1.55) 1.10 (0.63–1.93) 1.17 (0.72–1.90)
Marital status
 Not married vs Married 0.92 (0.59–1.46) 0.89 (0.60–1.32) 0.89 (0.57–1.39) 0.52 (0.28–0.95)* 1.13 (0.64–1.99)
Income
 ≤ 70K vs > 70K 0.87 (0.60–1.26) 0.88 (0.65–1.21) 1.17 (0.84–1.64) 1.08 (0.68–1.71) 0.93 (0.57–1.50)
Perceived risk 1.08 (0.80–1.34) 1.84 (1.55–2.18)* 1.57 (1.31–1.89)* 1.61 (1.24–2.09)* 1.07 (0.82–1.39)

Notes:

*

First-degree relative. Each three-level categorical variable (family member type and education) was recoded into a pair of dichotomous variables prior to their inclusion into the regression models to reflect the most relevant comparisons. The direction of each dichotomous variable is noted below, with the reference category listed second.

Results

Of the 939 participants in the study, more than half (61.7%) were female, about half (48%) were under age 50, most (96.6%) were white and most (81.4%) were married. They were highly educated, with most (61.7%) reporting a college degree or more. They were equally distributed over income levels. Most of the cases had been diagnosed with localized disease. Most participants (60.1%) reported that their risk for developing melanoma was a little higher or much higher than others of their age and sex. Risk perceptions differed between all family member types (F(2,935)=25.18; p<.001), with cases reporting the highest risk perceptions, followed by parents, followed by first-degree relatives (Ms =3.87; 3.44; 3.25 respectively on 5-point scales); all ps<.05 as assessed through Fisher s least significant difference tests.

We first examined the four open-ended risk attribution responses provided by each participant. To make the analyses more manageable, we thematically organize the raw text (1908 responses) into one of the following 14 descriptive categories: “sun exposure”, “sun exposure as a child”, “heredity/genetics”, “phenotype”, “many moles”, “personal history of melanoma”, “many tanning experiences”, “use of tanning booths or beds”, “stress”, “age”, “combination of risks”, “miscellaneous comments”, “don t feel at risk”, or “unknown” (see Table 2). Two coders separately coded each of the open-ended responses using the 14 categories above with good reliability (1st response: κ = 0.92; 2nd response: κ = 0.86; 3rd response: κ = 0.74; 4th response: κ = 0.81). Discrepancies were resolved by a third coder. All but nine participants (2 cases; 3 first-degree relatives, 4 parents) responded to the open-ended risk attribution questions.

We created five dichotomous attribution types, which identified whether participants (at any point in their open-ended responses) attributed their risk to a particular type of attribution. The five types of attributions included: (1) UVR exposure (consisting of attributions related to sun exposure, sun exposure as a child, many tanning experiences, use of tanning booths or beds); (2) heredity (consisting of attributions related to heredity/genetics); (3) phenotype (consisting of attributions related to phenotype and many moles); (4) personal history of melanoma, and (5) miscellaneous (consisting of attributions related to stress, combination of risks, age, miscellaneous, not at risk, and unknown). For example, participants who provided an attribution that was coded as a “UVR exposure” attribution were assigned a “1” (presence) for the dichotomous UVR exposure attribution variable. If they did not, then they were given a “0” (absence) for this variable. Similarly, the original fourteen categories were also used to establish presence or absence of heredity attributions, phenotype attributions, personal melanoma history attributions, and miscellaneous attributions. Concordance between the two coders for these five dichotomous attribution variables was excellent (UVR exposure: κ = 0.94; heredity: κ = 0.95; phenotype: κ = 0.90; personal melanoma history = κ 0.98; miscellaneous: κ = 0.88). Discrepancies were resolved by a third coder.

We then re-examined the responses of 54 participants whose open-ended responses were coded as a “combination of risks” (e.g., because they may have attributed their melanomas to “both family history and sun exposure”). In total, responses from 49 participants were recoded and reclassified as either “UVR exposure,” “heredity,” or “phenotype” where appropriate.

We then investigated which factors best predicted the participants endorsements of these five types of attributions (UVR exposure, heredity, phenotype, personal melanoma history and miscellaneous, see Table 3). Predictors included: family member type, gender, age (dichotomized using a median split of below 50 and above 50), education (trichotomized into “up to and including a high school education”, “some college or technical school”, and “college graduate or graduate education”), marital status (dichotomized into “married” or “not married’), income (dichotomized into either “up to $70,000” or “over $70,000”), and perceived risk (dichotomized into above or below a median split of the standardized perceived risk score).

None of the predictor variables were associated with UVR exposure which may reflect a “ceiling effect,” as approximately 80% of the overall sample attributed their melanoma risk to some type of UVR exposure (by far the most common attribution). With so few other people (approximately 20%) not attributing their risk to UVR exposure, it may have been difficult to isolate any underlying associations with our predictors.

Family member type, gender, age, education, and perceived risk were all significantly associated with making a heredity attribution for melanoma, although cases were significantly less likely to make such an attribution compared to first-degree relatives and parents. This is understandable because all first-degree relatives, by definition, have a family history since they are related to the cases. The cases, on the other hand, may or may not have a family history. We also found that both females and those younger than 50 were more likely to make a heredity attribution. Those with a high school education were significantly less likely to attribute their risk to heredity compared to those with a college or graduate education. Finally, we found that those reporting a low risk for melanoma were less likely to attribute heredity than those reporting a high risk. This seems to be because those with a family history are likely to both report higher perceived risk and attribute their risk to heredity.

Several factors were found to predict phenotype attributions. Family member type, gender, age, education, and perceived risk were all significantly related to making a phenotype attribution for melanoma. Specifically, females, those under age 50, and those with higher perceived risk were more likely to report that their phenotype contributed to their melanoma risk. First-degree relatives were significantly less likely to attribute their risk to their phenotype compared to cases and parents. Those with a high school education or less were also significantly less likely to attribute their risk to their phenotype compared to those with a college or graduate education.

Next, we examined the relationship between our set of predictors and making an attribution to personal melanoma history. Not surprisingly, we found that cases were more likely to attribute melanoma risk to their personal melanoma history. Those in the 50 and older age group, as well as those with higher perceived risk, were also more likely to attribute to a history of having melanoma. Finally, we examined the relationship between our set of predictors and making a miscellaneous attribution. Only family member type was significant. Cases were significantly more likely to provide a miscellaneous attribution than parents.

Next, in a series of five logistic regressions, we investigated the simultaneous effect of our predictors on each of our five attributional categories (see Table 4). Non-dichotomous categorical variables (e.g., study type, education) were dummy coded, age was also coded dichotomously (under 50 versus 50 and older) while perceived risk was left as continuous. Since none of the univariate crosstab analyses were significant, it is not surprising that the overall model predicting UVR exposure attribution was not significant. The overall model predicting heredity was significant, with family member type, education, and perceived risk as the only significant predictors. The overall model predicting phenotype attributions was also significant with gender, education, and perceived risk as significant predictors. The overall model predicting personal melanoma history attributions was also significant, with family member type, gender, marital status, and perceived risk as significant predictors. Of note, we also ran the same model among cases only, and this was not significant, given that attributions to personal melanoma history were almost universally limited to cases, rather than first-degree relatives or parents. The overall model predicting miscellaneous attributions was not significant.

Finally, we examined whether attribution type (UVR exposure, heredity, phenotype, personal melanoma history, miscellaneous) differed based on interactions between family member type (case, first-degree relative, parent) and gender, age, education, marital status, income or perceived risk level, and found two significant interactions (p <.05) out of 30 interactions such that family member type interacted with gender and marital status. Accordingly, female parents were more likely than male parents to make heredity attributions; female and male cases and first-degree relatives did not differ in their heredity attributions. Additionally, unmarried parents were more likely to make miscellaneous attributions than married parents; no marital status effect was evident for first-degree relatives and cases. There were no significant interactions between family member type and personal melanoma history attributions. We also examined whether attributions (UVR exposure, heredity, phenotype, personal melanoma history, miscellaneous) differed based on interactions between perceived risk level (lower, higher) and gender, age, education, marital status and income, and found two significant interactions (p<.05) out of 25 interactions. Here, age modified perceived risk such that among those who perceived higher melanoma risk, those in the younger age group (<50) were more likely to endorse phenotypic attributions than older (≥50) participants; those who perceived lower melanoma risk did not differ by age on phenotypic attributions. Income modified the relation of perceived risk on personal melanoma history such that lower income participants with higher perceived melanoma risk were much more likely to endorse personal melanoma history attributions, but this relationship did not exist among those in the higher income group.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first study of personal risk attributions and predictors of risk attributions for melanoma in a large general population sample of families affected by melanoma. Overall, our findings indicate a predominance of melanoma risk attributions to sun exposure, with about 80% of participants endorsing that their UVR exposure histories or current behavior increases their risk of developing melanoma. Melanoma risk attributions to hereditary or genetic factors were common as well, with slightly less than half of participants (45%) noting that heredity increased their melanoma risk. About a quarter (24%) endorsed phenotypic risks.

While infrequent, it is important to highlight endorsement of melanoma risk attributions to factors that lack epidemiological evidence to date–including stress, pollution, and occupational exposures such as paint fumes–as well as assertions of a lack of knowledge or denial of risk attributions. Studies examining risk attributions for other cancers, including breast cancer (Arman, Rehnsfeldt, Carlsson, & Hamrin, 2001; Baider & Sarell, 1983; Friedman, et al., 2007; Glinder & Compas, 1999; Kulik & Kronfeld, 2005; Lavery & Clarke, 1996; Lowery, Jacobsen, & DuCette, 1993; Rabin & Pinto, 2006; Stewart, et al., 2001; Taylor, Lichtman, & Wood, 1984; Wold, Byers, Crane, & Ahnen, 2005) have revealed beliefs about similar attributions, and dictate the importance of proactively addressing the lack of evidence for the role of these risk factors for cancer where such evidence exists. While uncommon, these beliefs may impede individuals efficacy in addressing actual risks via lifestyle behavior change (via sun protection and increased screening efforts), especially if these beliefs lead to increased fatalism or reduced perceived control over risk for melanoma.

There were some consistent predictors of endorsement of melanoma risk attributions across type of attribution. Specifically, those with higher levels of educational attainment (but not higher income), and those with higher perceptions of risk were more likely to endorse hereditary or phenotypic attributions for melanoma risk, and women were more likely than men to report phenotypic attributions. These findings likely reflect documented knowledge gaps in cancer risk awareness where those with lower education levels are less aware of skin cancer risk factors and methods of prevention (Hay, Coups, Ford, & DiBonaventura, 2009), as well as documented gender effects in illness risk perceptions showing heightened risk awareness in women (Finucane, 2000) and gender effects regarding awareness of the role of genetic factors in cancer (Parrott, Silk, & Condit, 2003). The current investigation documents that more highly educated individuals are more likely to link these risk factors to their own personal risk. Furthermore, younger participants with higher perceived risk tended to endorse phenotypic attributions; this relationship was not as pronounced for those aged 50 and older, which may reflect greater knowledge of the melanoma risk-sensitive phenotype relationship in younger family members. We expected there to be greater salience of all attributions in those with higher perceptions of risk given the role of attributions in shaping level of perceived risk (Kelly, et al., 2005; Lipkus, Biradavolu, Fenn, Keller, & Rimer, 2001; Lipkus, et al., 2005; Ponder, Lee, Green, & Richards, 1996). It is likely that heredity was a particularly salient attribution among first-degree relatives because of their position as a melanoma first-degree relative, and knowledge of this as a risk factor for melanoma, and that personal history of melanoma was a salient attribution among cases because of their recent diagnosis of melanoma. We did not identify significant predictors of miscellaneous attributions.

The inclusion of the parent family member type - those family members who had primary responsibility for the health of a child in the family who was also related to the case - has not been assessed in prior studies of personal risk attributions for melanoma risk. Parents beliefs were distinct in the following ways. First, parents were as likely to report personal attributions to heredity as were first-degree relatives, and this effect was largely due to the heredity attributions of female parents. Second, parents were more likely to endorse phenotypic attributions than were first-degree relatives. Third, parents risk perceptions for melanoma exceeded levels reported by first-degree relatives, although both groups reported significantly lower melanoma risk perceptions compared to cases. Taken together, these findings indicate that parents were aware of the heightened risk status of their family and may even overestimate their heredity attributions. While parents attributions for personal risk may differ from the risk attributions they have for their children, these findings may potentially be useful in guiding their personal sun protection choices for their families, and should be addressed in the development of melanoma family interventions. These findings highlight the potential impact of a family melanoma diagnosis on family members beyond first-degree relatives, and that interventions to inform accurate risk perceptions and prevention opportunities may be well-received by these family members.

These findings specifically inform the development of risk communication interventions for melanoma survivors, melanoma first-degree relatives, and other members of families that have been affected by melanoma in the following ways. First, the role of UVR exposure as a risk attribution was noted in such a high proportion of participants in melanoma-affected families that it may not be necessary to focus on it in risk communication interventions targeted to these families. Second, attributions to heredity, while common, were only noted by 58% of first-degree relatives in this population, making this a useful focus for attention in risk communication interventions with this at-risk population. This may be because they are unaware that family history or genetic factors are risk factors for melanoma (Kasparian, Butow, Meiser, & Mann, 2008) or because they do not necessarily appreciate their own heightened risk due to family history (Bergenmar & Brandberg, 2001), especially when they do not share the same skin type as the index patient (Hay, et al., 2008). At the same time, given that a large proportion of first-degree relatives who are aware that their family history increases their risk, strategies for risk communication may usefully focus on issues of risk overestimation as well, as it may be important to point out to these first-degree relatives that their relative likely does not have a hereditary melanoma that would dictate dramatically increased risk. Third, while most of those who attributed their risk to their personal history of melanoma were indeed cases, only 29% of cases made this attribution, indicating that a large proportion of these population-based melanoma cases may not be aware that their melanoma history places them at increased melanoma risk. Finally, attributions to phenotype may be an important area to address in intervention as many factors were reported–including frequency of moles, and skin, eye, and hair color - but these factors were reported much less frequently than sun exposure per se, raising concern that those who may have sun-sensitive phenotypes may underappreciate these risk factors that may increase risk even if they are not chronically sun exposed. Intervention strategies that involve elicitation of these melanoma risk attributions will help participants identify and overcome tacit beliefs that may moderate reactions to risk communication interventions. The intervention priorities outlined above may be usefully integrated into existing skin cancer risk reduction interventions.

While our findings are most relevant for populations at-risk for melanoma, they may also be useful for a broader segment of the general population, as well. In prior work, (French & Hevey, 2008) examined personal attributions for skin cancer in 40 college students unselected for risk status, and found that half (20 students) reported attributions to UVR exposure, and a third (14 students) endorsed either heredity, genetic, or phenotypic characteristics as attributions. These rates of endorsement are lower than for the comparable attributions reported in our at-risk families, accordingly in a general population sample we would anticipate that overall rates of personal attributions would be lower, with higher endorsement of no personal attributions, and very few personal attributions to heredity. We would also anticipate that UVR exposure is likely a well-understood personal risk attribution that may best translate to general population samples.

Our findings document that these individuals have multiple attributions with which they justify their melanoma risk that represent an amalgam of prior knowledge, their own family and medical experiences with the disease, as well as individual differences in the salience of qualitatively different risks (Hay, et al., 2007). Knowledge of the causes of cancer is likely an important determinant of personal risk attributions, with documented low levels of cancer risk factor knowledge in the general population (Breslow, Sorkin, Frey, & Kessler, 1997; Goldman, et al., 2006; Keighley, et al., 2004). Yet even though individuals may know the actual risk factors for the type of cancer in question, they may also resist acknowledging that those factors are important in influencing their risk (Cerully, Klein, & McCaul, 2006; Shiloh, Drori, Orr-Urtreger, & Friedman, 2009; Weinstein, 1980; Weinstein, 1984, 1987; Weinstein & Lachendro, 1982). Exposure to colorectal cancer risk communication interventions that aim to increase individuals risk perceptions have not been fully successful (Blalock, DeVellis, Afifi, & Sandler, 1990; Lipkus, et al., 2005; Lipkus, et al., 2004). Accordingly, our results highlight the need to address risk attributions that may be subjectively important to the formulation of risk perceptions, rather than to impose accurate information with the hopes that it will trump the subjective perceptions of risk. A clear understanding of the risks as well as one s subjective beliefs about the applicability of risks to one s personal circumstances is a necessary (albeit insufficient) condition to support behavior change related to melanoma risk reduction. However, we have confidence in our findings since the study was conducted in a large, general population sample, and also because self-referred individuals tend to have greater knowledge of cancer risk factors than the general public (Henrikson, Harris, & Bowen, 2007).

The inclusion of qualitative and quantitative research methods employed is an important strength of the study. Qualitative assessment of open-ended responses to risk attribution questions tap beliefs that come to mind with ease; this inductive approach imposes fewer a priori assumptions concerning attribution content and thus may be most relevant to individuals risk perceptions, awareness of health risks, and spontaneous behavioral decision-making. Alternatively, checklist methods provide participants with an a priori list of causal attributions where participants are asked to endorse factors from the list, or rate their level of agreement via a Likert-type item (Moss-Morris, et al., 2002; Weinman, 1996). Closed-ended approaches may overestimate the frequency of attributional beliefs and may not tap a full range of participants responses (Ostroff, Hay, Schantz, & Maher, 2000). In future work, the use of multiple assessment strategies in studies examining cancer risk attributions and behavioral outcomes may provide the clearest picture concerning which attributions are most salient, or the most motivating, in the melanoma context.

There are some study limitations that are important to note. First, our study was cross-sectional. We suggest that future work to examine whether melanoma risk attributions change over time, could provide valuable insights into the stability (or not) of these notions–something that may be particularly important to understand better within the realm of intervention research in this area. Additionally, heredity attribution rates might have been inflated in this study due to the salience of the study rationale (to help them reduce melanoma risk); individuals with comparable risk profiles in the general population may have lower levels of appreciation of their heredity melanoma risks than we report in this sample, and this requires further study.

In conclusion, in work showing that risk factor information did not consistently affect risk attributions for colorectal cancer, Lipkus noted, “It may be premature to try to affect risk perception via colorectal risk attributions without a more comprehensive understanding of these processes”(Lipkus, et al., 2004). In our future research, we will examine changes in attributions prospectively in these groups (cases, first-degree relatives, parents) as they are affected by the web-based educational intervention. Further work examining the nature of subjective risk perceptions for various cancers–including within-subject variation in these attributions across time and in the context of intervention - will provide important information concerning novel approaches to intervening on cancer risk perceptions, and highlight basic processes that contribute to the stability of individuals beliefs about their cancer risk.

Acknowledgments

This manuscript was completed under the grant support of R01 CA107430 to Deborah Bowen, and K07 CA98106 to Jennifer Hay. We also acknowledge the support of John Cardinale, Kira Farberov, and Ollie Ganz in completing the manuscript.

References

  1. Ajzen I, Fishbein M. Understanding Attitudes and Predicting Behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall; 1980. [Google Scholar]
  2. American Cancer Society. 2009 Cancer Facts and Figures. 2009. [Google Scholar]
  3. Anton-Culver H, Ziogas A, Bowen D, Finkelstein D, Griffin C, Hanson J, et al. The Cancer Genetics Network: recruitment results and pilot studies. Community Genet. 2003;6(3):171–177. doi: 10.1159/000078165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Arman M, Rehnsfeldt A, Carlsson M, Hamrin E. Indications of change in life perspective among women with breast cancer admitted to complementary care. European Journal of Cancer Care. 2001;10:192–200. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2354.2001.00252.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Armstrong BK, Kricker A. The epidemiology of UV induced skin cancer. J Photochem Photobiol B. 2001;63(1–3):8–18. doi: 10.1016/s1011-1344(01)00198-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Baider L, Sarell M. Perceptions and causal attributions of Israeli women with breast cancer concerning their illness: The effects of ethnicity and religiosity. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics. 1983;39:136–143. doi: 10.1159/000287734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bergenmar M, Brandberg Y. Sunbathing and sun-protection behaviors and attitudes of young Swedish adults with hereditary risk for malignant melanoma. Cancer Nurs. 2001;24(5):341–350. doi: 10.1097/00002820-200110000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Blalock SJ, DeVellis BM, Afifi RA, Sandler RS. Risk perceptions and participation in colorectal cancer screening. Health Psychology. 1990;9(6):792–806. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.9.6.792. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bowen Hay, Mayer Kuniyuki, Meischke Harris, et al. Recruiting melanoma families into a cancer prevention intervention project. Community Genomics 2010 in press. [Google Scholar]
  10. Breslow RA, Sorkin JD, Frey CM, Kessler LG. Americans’ knowledge of cancer risk and survival. Preventive Medicine. 1997;26(2):170–177. doi: 10.1006/pmed.1996.0136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cameron LD. Illness risk representations and motivations to engage in protective behavior: The case of skin cancer risk. Psychology and Health. 2007:1–22. doi: 10.1080/14768320701342383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cameron LD, Leventhal H. The self-regulation of health and illness behaviour. London; New York: Routledge; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  13. Cerully JL, Klein WM, McCaul KD. Lack of acknowledgment of fruit and vegetable recommendations among nonadherent individuals: associations with information processing and cancer cognitions. J Health Commun. 2006;11(Suppl 1):103–115. doi: 10.1080/10810730600637491. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Dirksen SR. Search for meaning in long-term cancer survivors. J Adv Nurs. 1995;21(4):628–633. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2648.1995.21040628.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Edwards AG, Evans R, Dundon J, Haigh S, Hood K, Elwyn GJ. Personalised risk communication for informed decision making about taking screening tests. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2006;(4):CD001865. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD001865.pub2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Finucane ML, Slovic P, Mertz CK, Flynn J, Satterfield TA. Gender, race, and perceived risk: the white male’ effect. Health, Risk & Society. 2000;2(2):159–172. [Google Scholar]
  17. Ford D, Bliss JM, Swerdlow AJ, Armstrong BK, Franceschi S, Green A, et al. Risk of cutaneous melanoma associated with a family history of the disease. The International Melanoma Analysis Group (IMAGE) International Journal of Cancer. 1995;62(4):377–381. doi: 10.1002/ijc.2910620403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. French DP, Hevey D. What do people think about when answering questionnaires to assess unrealistic optimism about skin cancer? A think aloud study. Psychol Health Med. 2008;13(1):63–74. doi: 10.1080/13548500701243959. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Friedman LC, Romero C, Elledge R, Chang J, Kalidas M, Dulay MF, et al. Attribution of blame, self-forgiving attitude and psychological adjustment in women with breast cancer. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2007 doi: 10.1007/s10865-007-9108-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Gandini S, Sera F, Cattaruzza MS, Pasquini P, Zanetti R, Masini C, et al. Meta-analysis of risk factors for cutaneous melanoma: III. Family history, actinic damage and phenotypic factors. Eur J Cancer. 2005;41(14):2040–2059. doi: 10.1016/j.ejca.2005.03.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Glinder JG, Compas BE. Self-blame attributions in women with newly diagnosed breast cancer: a prospective study of psychological adjustment. Health Psychol. 1999;18(5):475–481. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.18.5.475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Goldman RE, Barbeau E, Hunt MK, Acevedo-Garcia D, Emmons KM, Gagne J, et al. Perceptions of health promotion and cancer prevention among adults in working-class occupations and neighborhoods. Health Education & Behavior. 2006 doi: 10.1177/1090198106291375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hay J, Coups EJ, Ford J, DiBonaventura M. Exposure to mass media health information, skin cancer beliefs, and sun protection behaviors in a United States probability sample. J Am Acad Dermatol. 2009 doi: 10.1016/j.jaad.2009.04.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Hay J, Shuk E, Brady M, Berwick M, Ostroff J, Halpern A. Family communication after melanoma diagnosis. Archives of Dermatology. 2008;144(4):553–554. doi: 10.1001/archderm.144.4.553. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Hay JL, Coups E, Ford J. Predictors of perceived risk for colon cancer in a national probability sample in the United States. Journal of Health Communication. 2006;11(Suppl1):71–92. doi: 10.1080/10810730600637376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hay JL, Meischke HW, Bowen DJ, Mayer J, Shoveller J, Press N, et al. Anticipating dissemination of cancer genomics in public health: a theoretical approach to psychosocial and behavioral challenges. Ann Behav Med. 2007;34(3):275–286. doi: 10.1007/BF02874552. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Henrikson NB, Harris JN, Bowen DJ. Predictors of self-referral into a cancer genetics registry. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2007;16(7):1387–1392. doi: 10.1158/1055-9965.EPI-06-0009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Janz NK, Becker MH. The Health Belief Model: a decade later. Health education quarterly. 1984;11(1):1–47. doi: 10.1177/109019818401100101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kasparian NA, Butow PN, Meiser B, Mann GJ. High- and average-risk individuals’ beliefs about, and perceptions of, malignant melanoma: an Australian perspective. Psychooncology. 2008;17(3):270–279. doi: 10.1002/pon.1230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Keighley MR, O’morain C, Giacosa A, Ashorn M, Burroughs A, Crespi M, et al. Public awareness of risk factors and screening for colorectal cancer in Europe. European Journal of Cancer Prevention. 2004;13(4):257–262. doi: 10.1097/01.cej.0000136575.01493.9b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kelly K, Leventhal H, Andrykowski M, Toppmeyer D, Much J, Dermody J, et al. Using the common sense model to understand perceived cancer risk in individuals testing for BRCA1/2 mutations. Psychooncology. 2005;14:34–48. doi: 10.1002/pon.805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kulik L, Kronfeld M. Adjustment to breast cancer: The contribution of resources and causal attributions regarding the illness. Social Work in Health Care. 2005;41(2):37–57. doi: 10.1300/J010v41n02_03. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Lavery JF, Clarke VA. Causal attributions, coping strategies, and adjustment to breast cancer. Cancer Nursing. 1996;19(1):20–28. doi: 10.1097/00002820-199602000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Lipkus IM, Biradavolu M, Fenn K, Keller P, Rimer BK. Informing women about their breast cancer risks: Truth and consequences. Health Communication. 2001;13(2):205–226. doi: 10.1207/S15327027HC1302_5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Lipkus IM, Skinner CS, Dement J, Pompeii L, Moser B, Samsa GP, et al. Increasing colorectal cancer screening among individuals in the carpentry trade: test of risk communication interverntions. Preventive Medicine. 2005;40(5):489–501. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2004.09.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Lipkus IM, Skinner CS, Green LS, Dement J, Samsa GP, Ransohoff D. Modifying attributions of colorectal cancer risk. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2004;13(4):560–566. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Lowery BJ, Jacobsen B, DuCette J. Causal attribution, control, and adjustment to breast cancer. Journal of Psychosocial Oncology. 1993;10(4):37–53. [Google Scholar]
  38. McCaul KD, Branstetter AD, Schroeder DM, Glasgow RE. What is the relationship between breast cancer risk and mammography screening? A meta-analytic review. Health Psychol. 1996;15(6):423–429. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.15.6.423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Moss-Morris R, Weinman J, Petrie K, Horne R, Cameron L, Buick D. The Revised Illness Perception Questionnaire (IPQ-R) Psychology & Health. 2002;17(1):1–16. [Google Scholar]
  40. Nelson AA, Tsao H. Melanoma and genetics. Clin Dermatol. 2009;27(1):46–52. doi: 10.1016/j.clindermatol.2008.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Ostroff JS, Hay JL, Schantz SP, Maher MM. A survey of smoking status and cancer risk perceptions among participants attending a hospital-based head and neck screening program. Psychology and Health. 2000;14:979–990. doi: 10.1080/08870440008407362. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Parrott RL, Silk KJ, Condit C. Diversity in lay perceptions of the sources of human traits: genes, environments, and personal behaviors. Soc Sci Med. 2003;56(5):1099–1109. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(02)00106-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Ponder M, Lee J, Green J, Richards M. Family history and perceived vulnerability to some common diseases: a study of young people and their parents. Journal of Medical Genetics. 1996;33(6):485–492. doi: 10.1136/jmg.33.6.485. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Rabin C, Pinto B. Cancer-related beliefs and health behavior change among breast cancer survivors and their first-degree relatives. Psychooncology. 2006;15:701–712. doi: 10.1002/pon.1000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Rimer BK, Glassman B. Is there a use for tailored print communications in cancer risk communication? Journal of the National Cancer Institute. 1999;25(9) doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.jncimonographs.a024190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Rogers RW, Mewborn CR. Fear appeals and attitude change: effects of a threat’s noxiousness, probability of occurrence, and the efficacy of coping responses. Journal of Personality And Social Psychology. 1976;34(1):54–61. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.34.1.54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Shiloh S, Drori E, Orr-Urtreger A, Friedman E. Being ’at-risk’ for developing cancer: cognitive representations and psychological outcomes. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2009;32(2):197–208. doi: 10.1007/s10865-008-9178-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Stewart DE, Cheung AM, Duff S, Wong F, McQuestion M, Cheng T, et al. Attributions of cause and recurrence in long-term breast cancer survivors. Psychooncology. 2001;10(2):179–183. doi: 10.1002/pon.497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Sutton SR. Fear-arousing communications: a critical examination of theory and research. In: Eiser JR, editor. Social Psychology and Behavioral Medicine. New York: John Wiley & Sons Ltd; 1982. pp. 303–338. [Google Scholar]
  50. Taylor SE, Lichtman RR, Wood JV. Attributions, beliefs about control, and adjustment to breast cancer. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1984;46(3):489–502. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.46.3.489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Veierod MB, Adami HO, Lund E, Armstrong BK, Weiderpass E. Sun and solarium exposure and melanoma risk: effects of age, pigmentary characteristics, and nevi. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2010;19(1):111–120. doi: 10.1158/1055-9965.EPI-09-0567. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Weinman J, Petrie K, Moss-Morris R, Horne R. The illness perception questionnaire: A new method for assessing the cognitive representation of illness. Psychology & Health. 1996;11(3):431–445. [Google Scholar]
  53. Weinstein ND. Unrealistic Optimism About Future Life Events. Journal Of Personality And Social Psychology. 1980;39(5):806–820. [Google Scholar]
  54. Weinstein ND. Why it won’t happen to me: perceptions of risk factors and susceptibility. Health Psychol. 1984;3(5):431–457. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.3.5.431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Weinstein ND. Unrealistic optimism about susceptibility to health problems: Conclusions from a community-wide sample. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 1987;10:481–500. doi: 10.1007/BF00846146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Weinstein ND, Lachendro E. Egocentrism as a source of unrealistic optimism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 1982;8:195–200. [Google Scholar]
  57. Weinstein ND, Sandman PM, Hallman WK. Testing a visual display to explain small probabilities. Risk Anal. 1994;14(6):895–897. doi: 10.1111/j.1539-6924.1994.tb00053.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Weinstein ND, Sandman PM, Roberts NE. Perceived susceptibility and self-protective behavior: a field experiment to encourage home radon testing. Health Psychol. 1991;10(1):25–33. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.10.1.25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Whiteman DC, Whiteman CA, Green AC. Childhood sun exposure as a risk factor for melanoma: a systematic review of epidemiologic studies. Cancer Causes Control. 2001;12(1):69–82. doi: 10.1023/a:1008980919928. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Wold KS, Byers T, Crane LA, Ahnen D. What do cancer survivors believe causes cancer? (United States) Cancer Causes and Control. 2005;16:115–123. doi: 10.1007/s10552-004-2414-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES