Abstract
Dyslexia (or reading disability) and specific language impairment (or SLI) are common childhood disorders that show considerable co-morbidity and diagnostic overlaps and have been suggested to share some genetic aetiology. Recently, genetic risk variants have been identified for SLI and dyslexia enabling the direct evaluation of possible shared genetic influences between these disorders. In this study we investigate the role of variants in these genes (namely MRPL19/C20RF3, ROBO1, DCDC2, KIAA0319, DYX1C1, CNTNAP2, ATP2C2 and CMIP) in the aetiology of SLI and dyslexia. We perform case–control and quantitative association analyses using measures of oral and written language skills in samples of SLI and dyslexic families and cases. We replicate association between KIAA0319 and DCDC2 and dyslexia and provide evidence to support a role for KIAA0319 in oral language ability. In addition, we find association between reading-related measures and variants in CNTNAP2 and CMIP in the SLI families.
Electronic supplementary material
The online version of this article (doi:10.1007/s10519-010-9424-3) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Keywords: Dyslexia, Specific language impairment (SLI), Genetics, Association
Introduction
Dyslexia (or reading disability, RD) and specific language impairment (SLI) are two prevalent childhood learning disorders that show extensive co-morbidity. Dyslexia involves an unexpected deficit in the development of reading skills whereas SLI refers to an impairment in the acquisition of oral language (Pennington and Bishop 2009). In both disorders, a diagnosis is usually dependent upon the existence of normal non-verbal performance and the problems must be unexpected. As such neither dyslexia nor SLI are diagnosed in children who present with other co-occurring medical conditions (e.g. hearing loss for SLI) or neurological disorders (e.g. generalised learning disability) (Bishop 1997).
Like many neurodevelopmental conditions, SLI and dyslexia are considered to have a complex genetic aetiology, caused by multifaceted interactions between various genetic and environmental factors. Studies have repeatedly indicated that, for both disorders, relatives of affected individuals have a 30–50% chance of also developing the disorder (Snowling et al. 2007; Barry et al. 2007). This contrasts to a general population prevalence of approximately 5% (Snowling et al. 2007; Barry et al. 2007). Moreover, such investigations have found a significant co-morbidity between dyslexia and SLI. It is estimated that 43% of children with SLI are later diagnosed with reading disability (Snowling et al. 2000) and 55% of dyslexic children meet diagnostic criteria for SLI (McArthur et al. 2000). This observation has led to the suggestion that SLI and dyslexia may share some aetiological factors or, that they may represent different manifestations of the same underlying cognitive deficit (Bishop and Snowling 2004; Catts et al. 2005; Pennington and Bishop 2009).
Genetic linkage studies of dyslexia and SLI have identified several loci which may contribute to these disorders (Williams and O’Donovan 2006; Newbury et al. 2005). These include seven putative dyslexia loci (DYX1 on chromosome 15q21, DYX2 on chromosome 6p21, DYX3 on chromosome 2p, DYX5 on chromosome 3p, DYX6 on chromosome 18, DYX8 on chromosome 1p, and DYX9 on chromosome Xq27) and three SLI linkage loci (SLI1 on chromosome 16q, SLI2 on chromosome 19q and SLI3 on chromosome 13q). More recently, association studies of these linkage loci have led to the identification of specific genetic variants. To our knowledge, no genome-wide association studies have been published for either disorder. Below, we provide a summary of the genetic studies that led to the identification of candidate genes. Further details of these and other investigations are described in comprehensive recent reviews (Scerri and Schulte-Korne 2009; Newbury et al. 2010).
DYX1C1 (dyslexia susceptibility 1 candidate 1, also known as EKN1, OMIM#608716) was the first gene to be proposed as a candidate for dyslexia susceptibility (Taipale et al. 2003). This gene was identified by the breakpoint mapping of a chromosome translocation (t(2;15)(q11;q21)) co-segregating with reading disability in a single family (Taipale et al. 2003). In addition, two coding variants in DYX1C1, one that alters a putative transcription factor binding site and another that introduces a premature stop codon, were found to be associated with dyslexia in a larger sample (Taipale et al. 2003). Initial replication attempts met with little success (Wigg et al. 2004; Scerri et al. 2004; Bellini et al. 2005; Ylisaukko-Oja et al. 2005; Marino et al. 2005; Meng et al. 2005a) and it was suggested that this may be due to the low minor allele frequency of the SNPs described or differences in linkage disequilibrium patterns between samples. However, recent studies have reported significant association between DYX1C1 variants and both working memory (Marino et al. 2007) and reading measures (Dahdouh et al. 2009; Bates et al. 2009).
The ROBO1 (Roundabout 1, also known as DUTT1, OMIM#602430) gene was also identified as a candidate for dyslexia susceptibility through the breakpoint mapping of a chromosome translocation involving a dyslexia linkage locus (DYX5, chromosome 3) in a dyslexic individual (Nopola-Hemmi et al. 2001). Sequencing of this gene in members from a large multi-generational pedigree, in whom the DYX3 linkage was originally identified, revealed a rare SNP haplotype that segregated with dyslexia (Hannula-Jouppi et al. 2005). This haplotype was not observed in other families with dyslexia suggesting that ROBO1 may play a role only in sporadic cases (Hannula-Jouppi et al. 2005).
The DYX2 locus on chromosome 6 has produced the most consistent observations of association with dyslexia and significant associations have been described to two genes in this region; DCDC2 (Doublecortin domain containing, OMIM#605755) (Meng et al. 2005b; Schumacher et al. 2006; Lind et al. 2010; Rice et al. 2009) and KIAA0319 (OMIM#609269) (Francks et al. 2004; Cope et al. 2005; Ludwig et al. 2008; Rice et al. 2009). Dyslexia-associated alleles in KIAA0319 fall within a putative promoter region (Couto et al. 2009b) and a single variant has been shown to specifically cause a reduced expression of this gene (Dennis et al. 2009).
Lastly, in 2007, Anthoni et al. reported association to SNPs in two genes located within the DYX3 locus; MRPL19 and C2ORF3 (Anthoni et al. 2007). These two genes are in high linkage disequilibrium and appear to be co-regulated. These associations have yet to be replicated.
To date, only three associations have been described for SLI and these have yet to be independently verified. The first association to be reported was with CNTNAP2 (contactin-associated protein-like 2, OMIM#604569) on chromosome 7q (Vernes et al. 2008). This gene was initially identified as a candidate for SLI as it is regulated by FOXP2 (OMIM#605317). Disruption of the FOXP2 gene causes a severe form of language impairment in some rare cases (Lai et al. 2001). Subsequent investigation of CNTNAP2 variants demonstrated a significant association to performance on a task of phonological short-term memory in a language-impaired cohort (Vernes et al. 2008).
The other two SLI associations were identified by a high density SNP screen for association in the SLI1 region of linkage on chromosome 16 (Newbury et al. 2009). Across SLI1, two clusters of variants were observed to be significantly associated with phonological short-term memory in language-impaired families. The first cluster fell within the CMIP (C-MAF Inducing Protein, OMIM#610112) gene and the second within the ATP2C2 (ATPase, Ca++ transporting, type 2C, member 2, OMIM#613082) gene. Both associations were internally replicated in a sample selected from a population cohort on the basis of low language performance.
The identification of specific genetic variants allows the direct evaluation of the question of shared genetic influences between co-morbid disorders. For example, studies have found that genes that contribute to dyslexia or SLI are also associated to related but distinct neurodevelopmental disorders. CNTNAP2 is a prime example and has been associated with autism (Alarcon et al. 2008; Arking et al. 2008; Bakkaloglu et al. 2008; Rossi et al. 2008; Jackman et al. 2009; Poot et al. 2009) and ADHD (Elia et al. 2009), Gilles de la Tourette syndrome (Verkerk et al. 2003), schizophrenia and epilepsy (Strauss et al. 2006; Friedman et al. 2008) and mental retardation (Ballarati et al. 2009; Zweier et al. 2009). Similarly, variants in ATP2C2 and DCDC2 have been associated with ADHD (Lesch et al. 2008; Couto et al. 2009a) and ROBO1 has been shown to have reduced expression in autistic cases (Anitha et al. 2008). Rice et al. directly addressed the question of shared effects between SLI and dyslexia by performing association analyses of SNPs in DCDC2 and KIAA0319 in a sample of children affected by SLI (Rice et al. 2009). They found marginal association (0.05 > P > 0.01) with several SNPs across KIAA0319 and measures of reading, articulation, vocabulary and an omnibus test of language ability, indicating that this gene may have pleiotropic effects upon both reading and language measures or that neurodevelopmental pathways are shared between these traits.
In this paper, we further explore the possibility of shared genetic influences between SLI and dyslexia. We investigate variants in the DYX1C1, DCDC2, KIAA0319, MRPL19/C2ORF3, CNTNAP2, CMIP and ATP2C2 genes in groups of SLI and dyslexic subjects. We perform both case–control and quantitative association analyses using measures of oral and written language skills in both cohorts. The aims of this study were, firstly to provide a replication set for previously identified risk variants and secondly to investigate the possibility of shared genetic effects across disorders and traits. We replicate association between reading-related measures and DCDC2 and KIAA0319 and provide evidence for association between spoken language and KIAA0319 indicating that this gene may have shared effects across traits and disorders. We detect association to multiple variants in CNTNAP2 and CMIP with reading-measures but these findings were limited to the SLI cohort indicating that, in contrast to KIAA0319, the effects of these genes may be more specific.
Subjects and methods
Subjects
SLI cohort
The SLI families used in this study were provided by the SLI Consortium (SLIC) cohort which has previously been described in detail (SLIC 2002, 2004; Falcaro et al. 2008). This family-based sample included 780 individuals from 181 2-generation families and had a child male:female ratio of 1.6:1. All families were ascertained on the basis of a single proband with receptive and/or expressive language skills, either currently or in the past, more than 1.5 SD below the normative mean for their chronological age. The samples were assessed by one of five separate centres across the UK and were derived from both clinical and epidemiological studies. The Newcomen Centre at Guy’s Hospital, London, the Cambridge Language and Speech Project (CLASP—(Burden et al. 1996)), the Child Life and Health Department at the University of Edinburgh (Clark et al. 2007), the Department of Child Health at the University of Aberdeen and the Manchester Language Study (Conti-Ramsden and Botting 1999; Conti-Ramsden et al. 1997). Ethical approval for these studies was provided by local ethics committees. Any child reported to have a non-verbal IQ of below 80 was excluded from the study. Other exclusion criteria included monozygotic twinning, chronic illness requiring multiple hospital visits or admissions, deafness, a clinical diagnosis of autism, English being a second language, children with known neurological disorders and children under local authority care.
Whole blood (Guy’s Hospital and Edinburgh) or buccal swab (Cambridge, Aberdeen and Manchester) samples were collected from all available family members, regardless of language ability. DNA was extracted using standard protocols and all buccal swab DNAs were pre-amplified using either a PCR pre-amplification procedure (PEP (Zhang et al. 1992)) or a rolling circle whole genome amplification protocol (Genomiphi—GE Healthcare).
Language abilities of all available SLIC children (regardless of language ability) were assessed using the expressive (ELS, n = 392) and receptive (RLS, n = 392) scales of the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals (CELF-R) battery (Semel et al. 1992) and a 28-item nonword repetition (NWR, n = 451) test (Gathercole et al. 1994). Reading aptitude was measured using the single-word reading (Read, n = 312), single-word spelling (Spell, n = 310) and reading comprehension (Comp, n = 276) tests from the Wechsler Objective Reading Dimensions (WORD) (Rust et al. 1993). Verbal and non-verbal IQ were examined, primarily for exclusion purposes, using the Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children (WISC-III) (Wechsler 1992). Due to the age constraints of standardised tests, all phenotypic data were collected for children only. All scores were normalised for age effects. Due to logistical constraints, the reading tests were only performed for a subset of SLI individuals.
Correlations between all the measures analysed in the SLI family sample can be found in Electronic Supplementary Table S1.
All 181 families in the SLI cohort formed part of the sample previously used to identify association between language measures and variants in CNTNAP2 (Vernes et al. 2008), ATP2C2 and CMIP (Newbury et al. 2009). Although an identical cohort, this investigation included repeat genotyping data and the reported P-values may therefore differ from those previously described due to small differences in missing genotype information.
Dyslexia cohort
The collection of families used for quantitative trait association has been extensively described previously (Francks et al. 2004). Briefly, all probands and siblings from our complete Oxford set of 264 unrelated nuclear families (a total of 634 siblings with a male:female ratio of 1.5:1), were identified from the dyslexia clinic at the Royal Berkshire Hospital (Reading, U.K). Families were ascertained if the proband had a British Abilities Scales (BAS) single-word reading score >2 SDs below that predicted by their intelligence quotient (IQ) and if at least one other sibling had a history of reading problems. These criteria identified some probands with high IQ scores and BAS reading scores within the ‘normal’ range. Therefore, after collecting 173 UK families the ascertainment conditions were changed such that the only required criterion was that the probands’ difference in their BAS single-word reading score had to be ≥1 SD below the population mean for their age (and not IQ), along with an IQ ≥ 90. Probands were excluded if they had been diagnosed with co-occuring developmental disorders such as SLI, autism or attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).
We administered a battery of psychometric tests to all probands and siblings in each family, and we age-adjusted and standardized their scores against a normative control data set, as described elsewhere (Marlow et al. 2001; Fisher et al. 2002). These included measures of single-word reading ability (READ; n = 634) and spelling ability (SPELL; n = 603) from the British Ability Scales (BAS) (Elliot et al. 1983) or Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT-R) for children older than 14.5 (Jastak and Wilkson 1984), phonological decoding ability (PD; n = 629) (use of letter to sound relationship rules to read pseudowords) (Castles and Coltheart 1993), phonemic awareness (PA; n = 591) (awareness of the phonemic structure of language; test which required the individuals to orally move phonemes either within the same word or between words, also known as “spoonerism”) (Gallagher and Frederickson 1995), orthographic coding (OC-irreg; n = 625) (the ability to read real words that do not follow conventional spelling to sound rules e.g. yacht) (Castles and Coltheart 1993) and orthographic coding assessed by a forced word choice test (OC-choice; n = 548) (identification of a correctly spelt word from two phonologically equivalent options, e.g. rane vs rain) between (Olson et al. 1994). Tests of verbal and non-verbal reasoning were assessed using the BAS similarities (SIM; n = 620) or BAS matrices (MAT; n = 588) tests respectively (Elliot et al. 1983). The Similarities sub-scale of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scales (WAIS), which is analogous to the BAS similarities test, was used when age was >17.5 years (Wechsler 1981). Note that the measures of reading and spelling were derived from different tests of the same constructs to those utilized in the SLI cohort.
Correlations between all the measures analysed in the dyslexia family samples can be found in Electronic Supplementary Table S2.
The 264 families have been previously used to identify the KIAA0319 locus (Francks et al. 2004) and to study the DYX1C1 locus (Scerri et al. 2004). Therefore, some of the results are a repetition of data previously published. These results are clearly identified in the tables and text.
In addition to the dyslexia family-based sample, we analysed a collection of 331 UK unrelated cases which have not been investigated in previous studies for any of the loci under study here. These samples were recruited through the Dyslexia Research Centre clinics in Oxford and Reading, and the Aston Dyslexia and Development clinic in Birmingham. The cases are between 8 and 18.5 years of age, have a BAS2 single-word reading score ≤100 (at chronological age) and >1.5 SDs below that predicted by their IQ scores. Since these individuals were collected as a case cohort, we did not investigate quantitative measures for them.
Ethical approval for this study was acquired from the Oxfordshire Psychiatric Research Ethics Committee (OPREC O01.02). Written informed consent to participate in this study was obtained from all families and individuals.
Data cleaning and data handling
SNP selection was primarily achieved through a literature review with those highly significant and consistently associated to dyslexia or SLI being prioritised.
In total 31 SNPs were genotyped. These included four SNPs from MRPL19/C2ORF3, four SNPs from DCDC2, six SNPs from KIAA0319, two SNPs from DYX1C1, five SNPs from CNTNAP2, five SNPs from CMIP and five SNPs from ATP2C2 (Table 1). SLI and dyslexia individuals were genotyped in separate experiments, therefore the SNP data quality varied between these cohorts. In addition, control data were not available for all SNPs, therefore case–control analyses were not performed for all 31 SNPs.
Table 1.
Chr | Gene | Linkage region | SNP | Chr posn (B36) | Identified by | Alleles | CEPH MAF | Control MAF | Reported risk allele | Tested in SLIC | Tested in Dys |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
2 | MRPL19/C2ORF3 | DYX3 | rs1000585 | 75,676,670 | Anthoni et al. 2007 | G/A | 0.39 | 0.40 | Minor | Yes | Yes |
2 | MRPL19/C2ORF3 | DYX3 | rs917235 | 75,679,327 | Anthoni et al. 2007 | G/A | 0.48 | 0.48 | Minor | Yes | Yes |
2 | MRPL19/C2ORF3 | DYX3 | rs714939 | 75,688,615 | Anthoni et al. 2007 | A/G | 0.46 | 0.38 | Major | Yes | Yes |
2 | MRPL19/C2ORF3 | DYX3 | rs6732511 | 75,693,241 | Anthoni et al. 2007 | T/C | 0.16 | 0.17 | Major | Yes | Yes |
6 | DCDC2 | DYX2 | rs793862 | 24,315,179 | Schumacher et al. 2006 | A/G | 0.27 | 0.23 | Minor | Yes | No |
6 | DCDC2 | DYX2 | rs807701 | 24,381,770 | Schumacher et al. 2006 | C/T | 0.33 | 0.33 | Minor | Yes | Yes |
6 | DCDC2 | DYX2 | rs807724 | 24,386,848 | Meng et al. 2005b | C/T | 0.19 | 0.18 | NA | Yes | Yes |
6 | DCDC2 | DYX2 | rs1087266 | 24,463,129 | Meng et al. 2005b | T/C | NA | 0.43 | NA | Yes | Yes |
6 | KIAA0319 | DYX2 | rs4504469 | 24,696,863 | Cope et al. 2005 | T/C | 0.39 | 0.40 | Major | Yes | Yes |
6 | KIAA0319 | DYX2 | rs761100 | 24,740,621 | Harold et al. 2006 | A/C | 0.46 | 0.39 | Major | Yes | Yes |
6 | KIAA0319 | DYX2 | rs6935076 | 24,752,301 | Cope et al. 2005 | T/C | 0.40 | Minor | QTDT only | QTDT only | |
6 | KIAA0319 | DYX2 | rs3212236 | 24,756,434 | Harold et al. 2006 | G/A | 0.24 | 0.17 | Minor | Yes | Yes |
6 | KIAA0319 | DYX2 | rs9461045 | 24,757,040 | Dennis et al. 2009 | T/C | 0.22 | 0.18 | Minor | No | Yes |
6 | KIAA0319 | DYX2 | rs2143340 | 24,767,050 | Francks et al. 2004 | G/A | 0.21 | 0.16 | Minor | Yes | Yes |
15 | DYX1C1 | DYX1 | rs57809907a | 53,510,176 | Taipale et al. 2003 | T/G | NA | 0.09 | Minor | Yes | Yes |
15 | DYX1C1 | DYX1 | rs3743205b | 53,577,822 | Taipale et al. 2003 | T/C | 0.07 | 0.04 | Minor | Yes | Yes |
7 | CNTNAP2 | rs7794745 | 146,120,539 | Arking et al. 2008 | T/A | 0.31 | 0.32 | Major | Yes | Yes | |
7 | CNTNAP2 | rs10246256 | 147,185,740 | Vernes et al. 2008 | C/T | 0.35 | Major | QTDT only | No | ||
7 | CNTNAP2 | rs2710102 | 147,205,323 | Vernes et al. 2008 | A/G | 0.47 | 0.51 | Major | Yes | Yes | |
7 | CNTNAP2 | rs17236239 | 147,213,238 | Vernes et al. 2008 | G/A | 0.35 | Minor | QTDT only | No | ||
7 | CNTNAP2 | rs2710117 | 147,232,705 | Vernes et al. 2008 | T/A | 0.38 | Major | QTDT only | No | ||
16 | CMIP | SLI1 | rs12927866 | 80,209,823 | Newbury et al. 2009 | T/C | 0.47 | 0.41 | Major | Yes | Yes |
16 | CMIP | SLI1 | rs6564903 | 80,211,158 | Newbury et al. 2009 | C/T | 0.48 | 0.53 | Minor | Yes | Yes |
16 | CMIP | SLI1 | rs3935802 | 80,219,068 | Newbury et al. 2009 | C/G | 0.46 | 0.42 | Minor | Yes | Yes |
16 | CMIP | SLI1 | rs4265801 | 80,222,553 | Newbury et al. 2009 | T/G | 0.43 | 0.44 | Minor | Yes | Yes |
16 | CMIP | SLI1 | rs7201632 | 80,234,949 | Newbury et al. 2009 | C/T | 0.50 | 0.47 | Major | Yes | Yes |
16 | ATP2C2 | SLI1 | rs8053211 | 83,011,254 | Newbury et al. 2009 | G/A | 0.47 | Major | QTDT only | No | |
16 | ATP2C2 | SLI1 | rs11860694 | 83,014,948 | Newbury et al. 2009 | C/G | 0.49 | Minor | QTDT only | No | |
16 | ATP2C2 | SLI1 | rs16973771 | 83,018,079 | Newbury et al. 2009 | C/T | 0.43 | Major | QTDT only | No | |
16 | ATP2C2 | SLI1 | rs2875891 | 83,021,410 | Newbury et al. 2009 | T/C | 0.44 | 0.38 | Major | Yes | Yes |
16 | ATP2C2 | SLI1 | rs8045507 | 83,022,078 | Newbury et al. 2009 | A/G | 0.48 | 0.42 | Major | Yes | Yes |
Alleles are given with minor allele first. Reported risk alleles use ‘major’ or ‘minor’ as given in the allele column. Minor allele frequencies (MAF) are shown for HapMap CEPH individuals (CEPH MAF). If these were not available, allele frequencies are given for the European population from dbSNP. Otherwise NA. Where possible MAF are also given for the European control samples genotyped in the present study (Control MAF)
ars57809907 = 1249G>T in Taipale et al. 2003
brs3743205 = −3G>A in Taipale et al. 2003
SNPs were genotyped using the Sequenom iPLEX assay. Genotypes in the form of marker clusters were checked manually in the MassArray TyperAnalyser software. Any SNP with a success rate of <80% or any SNP that showed consistent bad inheritances (>10 errors after data clean-up) within each independent cohort were removed from the association analyses. Probabilities of Hardy–Weinberg Equilibrium (HWE) were calculated for all SNPs within PEDSTATS (Wigginton and Abecasis 2005) and any SNP with a HWE-P of <0.0001 was excluded.
Statistical analyses
The primary samples used for this study were family-based and thus the principal analysis consisted of a quantitative test of association within QTDT (Abecasis et al. 2000) using all available language and reading measures in each data set. In both cohorts, a number of unlinked SNP markers spread across the genome (n = 28 in the dyslexia cohort and n = 25 in the SLI cohort) were directly tested for population stratification effects within QTDT. These tests supported an assumption of no population stratification and we therefore applied the total association QTDT option across all analyses. This model jointly considers both within- and between-family variance and hence should provide more power to detect association provided there is no sample stratification. Identity by Descent (IBD) values (required as an input for QTDT) were calculated in MERLIN (Abecasis et al. 2002).
Since each cohort applied different measures of language and reading ability, the QTDT tests were performed independently within the SLI and dyslexia families.
This investigation included the repeat genotyping of some SNPs already assayed in these families. The reported P-values may differ from those previously reported due to small differences in missing genotype information.
Case–control analyses
In addition to the quantitative tests of association, we performed an allelic test of association within PLINK (Purcell et al. 2007). In these analyses, for the SLI cohort, the case was defined as the proband upon whom the family was ascertained (and therefore had receptive and/or expressive language skills either currently or in the past, more than 1.5 SD below the normative mean for their chronological age). This included additional individuals from singleton families providing a total of 213 cases. Note that this proband selection is different from that applied in our previous association paper, in which we specifically investigated NWR and therefore SLI cases were selected on the basis of a low NWR performance alone (Newbury et al. 2009). For the dyslexia cohort, this included 188 cases, selected on the basis of severe reading deficit. In addition, we analysed the collection of 331 unrelated UK dyslexic cases specifically recruited for case–control analyses as an independent case–control sample. Between 112 and 363 unselected European controls from the Human Random Control (HRC) panel of the European Collection of Cell Cultures (ECACC) were genotyped for each SNP and used as universal controls across all case–control analyses. Thus the control data were identical for each case–control analysis performed.
Results
In this study a large number of tests were performed. The application of a Bonferroni correction would therefore yield a very stringent P-value. Furthermore, since we tested a restricted and targeted selection of SNPs from each locus, these tests were not independent (due to both the proximity of the markers within each locus and the correlation between the phenotypes). Therefore we do not correct our P-value for multiple testing but rather aim to describe trends of association across markers and traits.
Association analysis of dyslexia loci (Tables 2 and 3)
Table 2.
Gene | SNP | SLI sample | Dyslexia sample | ||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Read | Comp | Spell | ELS | RLS | NWR | ‘Risk’ allele | READ | SPELL | OC-irreg | PD | OC-choice | PA | ‘Risk’ allele | ||
MRPL19/C2ORF3 | rs1000585 | 0.0217 | Major | ||||||||||||
MRPL19/C2ORF3 | rs917235 | ||||||||||||||
MRPL19/C2ORF3 | rs714939 | ||||||||||||||
MRPL19/C2ORF3 | rs6732511 | ||||||||||||||
DCDC2 | rs793862 | NT | NT | NT | NT | NT | NT | ||||||||
DCDC2 | rs807701 | ||||||||||||||
DCDC2 | rs807724 | ||||||||||||||
DCDC2 | rs1087266 | ||||||||||||||
KIAA0319 | rs4504469 | 0.0174 | Major | ||||||||||||
KIAA0319 | rs761100 | 0.0349 | Minor | ||||||||||||
KIAA0319 | rs6935076 | 0.0073 | Major | ||||||||||||
KIAA0319 | rs3212236 | 0.0223 | 0.0495 | 0.0038 | Minor | Minor | |||||||||
KIAA0319 | rs9461045 | NT | NT | NT | NT | NT | NT | 0.0256 | 0.0254 | 0.0307 | Minor | ||||
KIAA0319 | rs2143340 | 0.0130 | 0.0188 | 0.0084 | Minor | 0.0403 | Minor | ||||||||
DYX1C1 | rs57809907 | 0.0497 | Major | ||||||||||||
DYX1C1 | rs3743205 |
QTDT in all available SLI (n = 780 individuals from 181 families) and dyslexia families (n = 634 siblings from 264 families)
Only P < 0.05 are shown
The terms ‘major’ and ‘minor’ alleles refer to alleles as reported in Table 1
No SNPs listed in Table 2 had previously been investigated in the SLI cohort. The DCDC2, KIAA0319 and DYX1C1 loci had already been investigated in the dyslexia cohort (Francks et al. 2004; Harold et al. 2006; Dennis et al. 2009; Scerri et al. 2004)
SLI phenotypes: Read WORD single-word reading, Comp WORD reading comprehension, Spell WORD single-word spelling, ELS CELF expressive language score, RLS CELF receptive language score, NWR nonword repetition. Dyslexia phenotypes: READ single-word reading, SPELL single-word spelling, OC-irreg irregular word reading, PD phonological decoding (non-word reading), OC-choice orthographic coding assessed by a forced word choice test, PA phoneme awareness. NT not tested
Table 3.
Gene | SNP | Controls | SLI probands | Dyslexia probands | Independent dyslexia cases | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
MAF | MAF | χ 2 | P | OR | MAF | χ 2 | P | OR | MAF | χ 2 | P | OR | ||
MRPL19/C2ORF3 | rs1000585 | 0.40 | 0.42 | 0.40 | 0.40 | |||||||||
MRPL19/C2ORF3 | rs917235 | 0.48 | 0.51 | 0.48 | 0.47 | |||||||||
MRPL19/C2ORF3 | rs714939 | 0.38 | 0.37 | 0.40 | 0.41 | |||||||||
MRPL19/C2ORF3 | rs6732511 | 0.17 | 0.19 | 0.18 | 0.16 | |||||||||
DCDC2 | rs793862 | 0.23 | 0.26 | NT | NT | NT | NT | NT | NT | NT | NT | |||
DCDC2 | rs807701 | 0.33 | 0.33 | 0.40 | 4.85 | 0.028 | 1.35 | 0.35 | ||||||
DCDC2 | rs807724 | 0.18 | 0.22 | 0.24 | 4.86 | 0.027 | 1.42 | 0.22 | 4.42 | 0.036 | 1.33 | |||
DCDC2 | rs1087266 | 0.43 | 0.40 | 0.48 | 0.45 | |||||||||
KIAA0319 | rs4504469 | 0.40 | 0.39 | 0.38 | 0.37 | |||||||||
KIAA0319 | rs761100 | 0.39 | 0.45 | 0.46 | 0.41 | |||||||||
KIAA0319 | rs3212236 | 0.17 | 0.14 | 0.17 | 0.20 | |||||||||
KIAA0319 | rs9461045 | 0.18 | NT | NT | NT | NT | 0.18 | 0.20 | ||||||
KIAA0319 | rs2143340 | 0.16 | 0.11 | 4.38 | 0.036 | 0.68 | 0.14 | 0.17 | ||||||
DYX1C1 | rs57809907 | 0.09 | 0.05 | 6.37 | 0.012 | 0.51 | 0.08 | 0.10 | ||||||
DYX1C1 | rs3743205 | 0.07 | 0.04 | 0.05 | 0.07 |
SLI (n = 213) and dyslexia (n = 188) probands were selected as a single proband from each nuclear family genotyped. In addition, an independent set of 331 dyslexia cases was analysed. For these analyses, a set of up to 363 universal controls were used (see methods)
Allele frequencies are given for all SNPs but only results with P < 0.05 are shown. NT not tested
No SNPs listed in Table 3 had previously been analysed for case–control associations within either the SLI or dyslexia proband cohorts
MAF minor allele frequency given for the ‘minor’ allele as reported in Table 1
The results of all quantitative analyses of dyslexia loci in the SLI and dyslexia families can be found in Table 2. The results of the case–control analyses of the dyslexia loci are in Table 3.
Across all the dyslexia loci tested, the minimum P-value seen with measures of reading ability was 0.013, observed with rs2143340 (KIAA0319) and the single-word reading measure (Read) in the SLI families. The minimum P-value arising from the investigation of language measures was 0.004 between receptive language (RLS) and rs3212236 (KIAA0319) in the SLI families.
In the SLI families, two KIAA0319 markers (rs2143340 and rs3212236) and one MRPL19/C2ORF3 marker (rs1000585) showed trends of association with reading-related measures. No notable association was observed for either DCDC2 or DYX1C1 with any of the traits analysed in the SLI families.
In the dyslexia families we found a trend of association between orthographic and spelling measures and SNPs in KIAA0319, as previously reported (Francks et al. 2004; Harold et al. 2006; Dennis et al. 2009) and between a single SNP in DYX1C1 and orthographic choice.
In the case–control analyses of the dyslexia probands selected from the family samples, we observed a trend of association to two SNPs in DCDC2 (minimum P = 0.027). Association to one of these markers (rs807724) was also supported by case–control analyses in an independent cohort of 331 unrelated dyslexic cases (P = 0.036). In the SLI probands, we also observed marginal association to single SNPs in KIAA0319 (rs2143340, P = 0.036) and DYX1C1 (rs57809907, P = 0.012) but, in both cases, with opposite direction compared to the original reports (Francks et al. 2004; Cope et al. 2005; Harold et al. 2006; Taipale et al. 2003).
Association Analysis of SLI Loci (Table 4)
Table 4.
Gene | SNP | SLI sample | Dyslexia sample | ||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Read | Comp | Spell | ELS | RLS | NWR | ‘Risk’ allele | READ | SPELL | OC-irreg | PD | OC-choice | PA | ‘Risk’ allele | ||
CNTNAP2 | rs7794745 | 0.0425 | Minor | ||||||||||||
CNTNAP2 | rs10246256 | 0.0003 | 0.0016 | 0.0002 | 0.0062 | 0.0032 | 0.0008 | Major | NT | NT | NT | NT | NT | NT | |
CNTNAP2 | rs2710102 | 0.0354 | 0.0089 | 0.0200 | 0.0312 | 0.0005 | Major | ||||||||
CNTNAP2 | rs17236239 | 0.0272 | 0.0086 | 0.0071 | 0.0336 | 8 × 10−5 | Minor | NT | NT | NT | NT | NT | NT | ||
CNTNAP2 | rs2710117 | 0.0205 | 0.0067 | 0.0051 | 0.0189 | 0.0056 | 0.0010 | Major | NT | NT | NT | NT | NT | NT | |
CMIP | rs12927866 | 0.0046 | 0.0240 | 0.0451 | 0.0027 | 0.0113 | Major | ||||||||
CMIP | rs6564903 | 0.0002 | 0.0079 | 0.0110 | 0.0012 | 0.0007 | Minor | ||||||||
CMIP | rs3935802 | 0.0025 | 0.0277 | 0.0285 | 0.0033 | 0.0079 | Minor | ||||||||
CMIP | rs4265801 | ||||||||||||||
CMIP | rs7201632 | 0.0047 | 0.0137 | 0.0021 | 0.0157 | Major | |||||||||
ATP2C2 | rs8053211 | 0.0088 | 0.0004 | Major | NT | NT | NT | NT | NT | NT | |||||
ATP2C2 | rs11860694 | 0.0025 | 0.0381 | 0.0003 | Minor | NT | NT | NT | NT | NT | NT | ||||
ATP2C2 | rs16973771 | 0.0363 | 0.0011 | Major | NT | NT | NT | NT | NT | NT | |||||
ATP2C2 | rs2875891 | 0.0027 | Major | 0.0251 | Major | ||||||||||
ATP2C2 | rs8045507 | 0.0317 | 0.0028 | Major |
QTDT in all available SLI (n = 780 individuals from 181 families) and dyslexia families (n = 264 families)
Only P < 0.05 are shown
The terms ‘major’ and ‘minor’ alleles refer to alleles as reported in Table 1
All five SNPs in CNTNAP2 have previously been assessed for association with the ELS, RLS and NWR traits (Vernes et al. 2008)
All five SNPs in CMIP and all five SNPs in ATP2C2 have previously been assessed for association with the NWR trait (Newbury et al. 2009). In addition, the single SNPs rs16973771 (ATP2C2) and rs4265801 (CMIP) have previously been examined for association to all language and reading measures in Table 4 (Newbury et al. 2009)
None of the SNPs in this table have been previously analysed in the dyslexia cohort
SLI phenotypes: Read WORD single-word reading, Comp WORD reading comprehension, Spell WORD single-word spelling, ELS CELF expressive language score, RLS CELF receptive language score, NWR nonword repetition. Dyslexia phenotypes: READ single-word reading, SPELL single-word spelling, OC-irreg irregular word reading, PD phonological decoding (non-word reading), OC-choice orthographic coding assessed by a forced word choice test, PA phoneme awareness
Across the SLI loci investigated, the minimum P-value achieved was 8 × 10−5. This was between rs17236239 in CNTNAP2 and NWR in the SLI families and has been previously reported (Vernes et al. 2008). Also as previously reported, we observed strong evidence for association between SNPs in CMIP, CNTNAP2 and ATP2C2 and multiple language measures in the SLI families.
In addition, in the SLI families we identified novel evidence of association to reading-related measures (Read, Comp and Spell) for multiple SNPs in CNTNAP2 and CMIP. In contrast, in the SLI families, SNPs in ATP2C2 only showed association to measures of language ability (ELS, RLS and NWR).
In the dyslexia families at the SLI loci, we observed only trends of association to a single SNP in CNTNAP2 (rs7794745 and OC-choice, P = 0.0425), in the opposite orientation from that previously described and to a single SNP in ATP2C2 (rs2875891 and PA, P = 0.0251).
In our case–control analyses, we did not observe any evidence for association to any SLI locus in either the SLI or the dyslexia cases (minimum P = 0.084, data not shown).
Discussion
In this study, we performed a candidate gene association study in three independent samples, one consisting of SLI families, one of dyslexic families and one of dyslexic cases. We evaluated the effects of putative SLI and dyslexia risk variants upon language and reading measures in the two family cohorts and performed case–control analyses across all three sample sets. The aims of this study were twofold: firstly, to provide a replication set for previously identified risk variants and secondly to investigate the possibility of shared genetic effects across disorders and traits. We found consistent evidence for a trend of association between KIAA0319 and reading measures in the SLI cohort, thus replicating previous findings from both dyslexic individuals (Cope et al. 2005; Dennis et al. 2009; Francks et al. 2004; Harold et al. 2006; Paracchini et al. 2006), population cohorts (Paracchini et al. 2008; Luciano et al. 2007) and SLI families (Rice et al. 2009). In addition, we observed some association between KIAA0319 and expressive and receptive language skills further supporting the findings of Rice et al. (2009) who suggested that this gene may have pleiotropic effects across measures. We also found association to DCDC2 across two independent case–control analyses of dyslexic individuals providing consistent support for a role for this gene in susceptibility to reading deficits. Finally, we also detected novel and consistent associations between variants in CNTNAP2 and CMIP and reading-measures in SLI families. Association to all three language loci were, at best, sporadic in the dyslexia cohort, indicating that their effects may be specific to language-impaired individuals.
MRPL19/C2ORF3
To our knowledge, this is the first attempted replication analysis of the MRPL19/C2ORF3 locus. In the dyslexia families, we did not see any compelling evidence for association to reading or language measures in this gene. The strongest association was observed in the SLI cohort, where we found a trend of association to rs1000585 (P = 0.0217). However, this result was in the opposite direction to that previously reported (Anthoni et al. 2007) and was observed at a nominal level with only a single SNP. Thus this is likely to represent a false positive.
DCDC2
Variants in DCDC2 had not previously been explored in the SLI families investigated in this manuscript but had been analysed in the dyslexia families (Harold et al. 2006). At this locus, we did not observe any evidence for association in the family samples. Nonetheless, we did observe a consistent, if weak, trend of association to rs807724 across both the dyslexic samples tested in our case–control analyses (min P = 0.027). In these analyses, the minor allele occurred at a higher frequency in both groups of dyslexic probands (24 and 22%) when compared to that of controls (18%) (Table 3). These case–control data therefore provide compelling support for the involvement of DCDC2 in dyslexia susceptibility.
KIAA0319
The dyslexic family sample used here includes those families in which the KIAA0319 association was initially identified (Francks et al. 2004). However, variants at this locus had not previously been investigated in the SLI families described in this manuscript. Two SNPs (rs3212236 and rs2143340) were found to show a trend of association with reading-related measures, in the same direction as previously described (see Table 1) (Francks et al. 2004; Cope et al. 2005; Harold et al. 2006). Furthermore, these two SNPs were also found to be associated with receptive language ability, again with the same allelic trend (min P = 0.0038). Conversely, two additional KIAA0319 SNPs (rs761100 and rs6935076) were associated with the expressive language scores (min P = 0.0073), but this association was in an opposite orientation from that previously described for reading. Association between KIAA0319 and language ability has previously been suggested by a recent investigation of SLI subjects (Rice et al. 2009) which also found that reduced performance in a test of spoken language was associated with two SNPs in KIAA0319 including the minor allele of rs6935076 (i.e. in an opposite direction to that observed in the present study).
In the case–control analyses of the SLI probands, we observed a trend of association to the major allele of rs2143340 (Table 3, P = 0.036) but in these analyses this effect is in an opposite direction to the original findings for this variant (Table 1) (Francks et al. 2004).
Thus, our analyses provide further evidence that variants in KIAA0319 may not only contribute to reading ability but may also be relevant to the development of other language-related skills.
DYX1C1
We previously analysed DYX1C1 variants in the dyslexic families (Scerri et al. 2004) and described nominal association at rs57809907 in an opposite trend to the discovery sample where it was labelled 1249G→T (Taipale et al. 2003). Variants in DYX1C1 had not previously been explored in the SLI family sample. No quantitative association was observed in the SLI families but in the case–control analyses, we did see an increase in the frequency of the major allele of rs57809907 in SLI probands (Table 3, P = 0.012). Once more, this is a sporadic result in an opposite direction to that originally reported and so should be treated cautiously (Taipale et al. 2003).
Language Loci
Analysis of CNTNAP2, ATP2C2 and CMIP in the SLI families investigated here have previously been reported (Vernes et al. 2008; Newbury et al. 2009) but these loci had not been studied for association in the dyslexic families. In the dyslexia cohort we only observed weak and sporadic associations to these loci. Thus, our investigations do not support pleiotropic effects for the investigated SLI loci in the dyslexic spectrum. It is perhaps surprising therefore that the reading measures collected in the SLI sample yielded strong and consistent association across multiple SNPs in CMIP and CNTNAP2 (Table 4, minimum P = 0.0002). In our previous study, we did investigate the role of CMIP and ATP2C2 across multiple measures (including reading) and did not see any evidence for association outside expressive and receptive language and nonword repetition (Newbury et al. 2009). However, that study was limited to a single SNP from each gene (rs4265801 for CMIP and rs16973771 for ATP2C2). Ironically, rs4265801 is the only CMIP SNP in the present study which did not show association to reading measures (Table 4). One may postulate that the overlaps of association between the reading and language measures at the CNTNAP2 and CMIP loci merely reflect phenotypic correlations between these traits. In the SLI families, these traits are moderately correlated with each other (Electronic Supplementary Table S1). Nonetheless, the associations to ATP2C2 in these families appear to be specific to language measures (ELS, RLS and NWR) indicating that the results observed for CNTNAP2 and CMIP are not caused directly by correlation effects. Thus, our findings support a novel role for variants in CNTNAP2 and CMIP upon reading ability in SLI individuals. Since children with SLI were excluded from the dyslexia sample, the divergence of the results between cohorts may indicate that these genes represent modifier loci whose effects are strengthened in the presence of other variants which predispose individuals to language-impairment but not dyslexia and, as such, support the findings of Newbury et al. (2009). Alternatively, these data may just reflect sampling or other stochastic differences across the two cohorts studied here, or the use of differing phenotypic measures across samples. In particular, the use of alternative measures across samples forces the assumption that different tests of the same construct measure comparable underlying biological processes. This is a common problem in the replication of quantitative genetic investigations and a limitation of the current study. Such issues exemplify the complexity of the biological pathways underlying multifaceted phenotypes and demonstrate the danger of attempting to delineate a complex trait by mapping specific genetic effects to distinct behavioural components.
As mentioned in the results section, the proximity of the markers and the correlation between the phenotypes in replication studies can complicate the application of a suitable multiple testing correction method. It should therefore be noted that none of the results presented in this paper have been corrected for multiple testing and that such a correction would render much of our data non-significant. For example, a traditional Bonferroni correction would yield a 0.05 significance threshold of 1 × 10−4. Even if we consider the fact that the 31 SNPs investigated in the present study could be tagged by 19 markers (r2 > 0.6), the significance threshold would still be P < 2 × 10−4 and none of the novel data presented in this paper exceed this level of significance. Nonetheless, it should be noted that these correction methods rely upon both the number of tests presented in any one paper, rather than the total number of tests performed in any given sample over time, and the assumption that all phenotypes are completely independent. In the absence of an accurate multiple testing procedure for such a hypothesis-driven study, we choose to present uncorrected P-values and openly acknowledge that these may overestimate the significance. We suggest that when assessing the validity of any given data point, the reader should consider the consistency of that result across samples and phenotypes.
Another common issue in the interpretation of association data is the presence of flip-flop associations in which SNP-trait associations are replicated but follow an opposite trend in the replication cohort (Lin et al. 2007). In this investigation, we observed some such associations, many of which were sporadic results of nominal significance (e.g. MRPL19/C2ORF3) and thus are likely to represent Type I errors. In some cases, however, the reversed associations were found to be consistent across traits or SNPs (e.g. KIAA0319 rs761100 and rs6935076 and expressive language measures). It has been postulated that the inversion of association may be caused by interactive effects or variable patterns of linkage disequilibrium between causal variants and associated SNPs (Lin et al. 2007). These data therefore reinforce the importance of remembering that even a robust genetic association is unlikely to precisely identify a causal genetic variant.
In conclusion, our investigations of dyslexia loci provide some marginal evidence for the existence of shared genetic effects across SLI and dyslexia, particularly for variants in KIAA0319. We found that the minor alleles of rs3212236 and rs2143340 in KIAA0319 were associated with reduced reading and language-related ability in samples of SLI individuals. This finding provides an independent replication for the suggested role of KIAA0319 in reading and language abilities. We were also able to replicate association to DCDC2 in our case–control analyses across two independent dyslexic samples. Our investigations of CNTNAP2 and CMIP yielded association to both reading- and language-related traits but these were restricted to the SLI cohort providing further support that these loci are particularly relevant to this clinical group. Our data indicate that, within the SLI population, variants in CMIP and CNTNAP2 influence both reading- and language-related traits whilst those in ATP2C2 appear to be more specific to oral language skills.
Electronic supplementary material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank all the families and individuals who kindly agreed to participate in these studies. We are extremely grateful to the clinicians, speech and language therapists and nurses who contribute to data collection and to Kim Rochelle, who was instrumental in participant recruitment and data collection. This research was specifically funded by a Wellcome Trust programme grant (no. 076566) and more generally by a Wellcome Trust core award grant (no. 075491). Dianne Newbury is funded by a MRC Career Development Award. Open access of this manuscript is funded by the Wellcome Trust.
Open Access
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Noncommercial License which permits any noncommercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and source are credited.
Appendix
The SLI Consortium (SLIC) is a collaborative group of clinicians, psychologists, speech therapists and geneticists who work together to further our understanding of the aetiology of SLI. Members of SLIC are as follows: D.F. Newbury, L. Addis, L. Winchester, SE Fisher, AP Monaco (Wellcome Trust Centre for Human Genetics, University of Oxford); I.M. Goodyer (Cambridge Language and Speech Project); E. Simonoff, P.F. Bolton (Child & Adolescent Psychiatry Dept and MRC Centre for Social, Developmental & Genetic Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry); V. Slonims, G. Baird (Newcomen Centre, Guy’s Hospital); A. Everitt, E. Hennessy, M. Main, D. Shaw, P.J. Helms (Department of Child Health, University of Aberdeen); A.D. Kindley (The Raeden Centre and Grampian University Hospitals Trust); A. Clark, J. Watson (Speech and Hearing Sciences, Queen Margaret University); A. O’Hare (Department of Reproductive and Developmental Sciences, University of Edinburgh); J. Seckl (Molecular Medicine Centre, University of Edinburgh); H. Cowie (Department of Speech and Language Therapy, Royal Hospital for Sick Children, Edinburgh); W. Cohen (Department of Educational and Professional Studies, University of Strathclyde); J. Nasir (Clinical Developmental Sciences, St. George’s University of London); D.V.M. Bishop (Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford); Z. Simkin, G. Conti- Ramsden (Human Communication and Deafness, School of Psychological Sciences, University of Manchester); A. Pickles (Biostatistics Group, School of Epidemiology and Health Science, University of Manchester).
Footnotes
Edited by Elena Grigorenko and Brett Miller.
D. F Newbury and S. Paracchini contributed equally to this work.
D. F. Newbury, L. Winchester, L. Addis, A. P. Monaco are part of the SLI Consortium (SLIC). For a full list of members please see appendix.
References
- Abecasis GR, Cardon LR, Cookson WO. A general test of association for quantitative traits in nuclear families. Am J Hum Genet. 2000;66:279–292. doi: 10.1086/302698. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Abecasis GR, Cherny SS, Cookson WO, Cardon LR. Merlin—rapid analysis of dense genetic maps using sparse gene flow trees. Nat Genet. 2002;30:97–101. doi: 10.1038/ng786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alarcon M, Abrahams BS, Stone JL, Duvall JA, Perederiy JV, Bomar JM, Sebat J, Wigler M, Martin CL, Ledbetter DH, Nelson SF, Cantor RM, Geschwind DH. Linkage, association, and gene-expression analyses identify CNTNAP2 as an autism-susceptibility gene. Am J Hum Genet. 2008;82:150–159. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2007.09.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anitha A, Nakamura K, Yamada K, Suda S, Thanseem I, Tsujii M, Iwayama Y, Hattori E, Toyota T, Miyachi T, Iwata Y, Suzuki K, Matsuzaki H, Kawai M, Sekine Y, Tsuchiya K, Sugihara G, Ouchi Y, Sugiyama T, Koizumi K, Higashida H, Takei N, Yoshikawa T, Mori N. Genetic analyses of roundabout (ROBO) axon guidance receptors in autism. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2008;147B:1019–1027. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30697. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anthoni H, Zucchelli M, Matsson H, Muller-Myhsok B, Fransson I, Schumacher J, Massinen S, Onkamo P, Warnke A, Griesemann H, Hoffmann P, Nopola-Hemmi J, Lyytinen H, Schulte-Korne G, Kere J, Nothen MM, Peyrard-Janvid M. A locus on 2p12 containing the co-regulated MRPL19 and C2ORF3 genes is associated to dyslexia. Hum Mol Genet. 2007;16:667–677. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddm009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arking DE, Cutler DJ, Brune CW, Teslovich TM, West K, Ikeda M, Rea A, Guy M, Lin S, Cook EH, Chakravarti A. A common genetic variant in the neurexin superfamily member CNTNAP2 increases familial risk of autism. Am J Hum Genet. 2008;82:160–164. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2007.09.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bakkaloglu B, O’roak BJ, Louvi A, Gupta AR, Abelson JF, Morgan TM, Chawarska K, Klin A, Ercan-Sencicek AG, Stillman AA, Tanriover G, Abrahams BS, Duvall JA, Robbins EM, Geschwind DH, Biederer T, Gunel M, Lifton RP, State MW. Molecular cytogenetic analysis and resequencing of contactin associated protein-like 2 in autism spectrum disorders. Am J Hum Genet. 2008;82:165–173. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2007.09.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ballarati L, Recalcati MP, Bedeschi MF, Lalatta F, Valtorta C, Bellini M, Finelli P, Larizza L, Giardino D. Cytogenetic, FISH and array-CGH characterization of a complex chromosomal rearrangement carried by a mentally and language impaired patient. Eur J Med Genet. 2009;52:218–223. doi: 10.1016/j.ejmg.2009.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barry JG, Yasin I, Bishop DV. Heritable risk factors associated with language impairments. Genes Brain Behav. 2007;6:66–76. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2006.00232.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bates TC, Lind PA, Luciano M, Montgomery GW, Martin NG, Wright MJ. Dyslexia and DYX1C1: deficits in reading and spelling associated with a missense mutation. Mol Psychiatr. 2009;15:1190–1196. doi: 10.1038/mp.2009.120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bellini G, Bravaccio C, Calamoneri F, Donatella Cocuzza M, Fiorillo P, Gagliano A, Mazzone D, Del Giudice EM, Scuccimarra G, Militerni R, Pascotto A. No evidence for association between dyslexia and DYX1C1 functional variants in a group of children and adolescents from Southern Italy. J Mol Neurosci. 2005;27:311–314. doi: 10.1385/JMN:27:3:311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bishop DVM. Uncommon understanding. development and disorders of language comprehension in children. Hove: Psychology Press; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Bishop DV, Snowling MJ. Developmental dyslexia and specific language impairment: same or different? Psychol Bull. 2004;130:858–886. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.6.858. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burden V, Stott CM, Forge J, Goodyer I. The Cambridge language and speech project (CLASP). I. Detection of language difficulties at 36 to 39 months. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1996;38:613–631. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1996.tb12126.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Castles A, Coltheart M. Varieties of developmental dyslexia. Cognition. 1993;47:149–180. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(93)90003-E. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Catts HW, Adlof SM, Hogan TP, Weismer SE. Are specific language impairment and dyslexia distinct disorders? J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2005;48:1378–1396. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2005/096). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark A, O’hare A, Watson J, Cohen W, Cowie H, Elton R, Nasir J, Seckl J. Severe receptive language disorder in childhood—familial aspects and long-term outcomes: results from a Scottish study. Arch Dis Child. 2007;92:614–619. doi: 10.1136/adc.2006.101758. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conti-Ramsden G, Botting N. Characteristics of children attending language units in England: a national study of 7-year-olds. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 1999;34:359–366. doi: 10.1080/136828299247333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conti-Ramsden G, Crutchley A, Botting N. The extent to which psychometric tests differentiate subgroups of children with SLI. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1997;40:765–777. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4004.765. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cope N, Harold D, Hill G, Moskvina V, Stevenson J, Holmans P, Owen MJ, O’donovan MC, Williams J. Strong evidence that KIAA0319 on chromosome 6p is a susceptibility gene for developmental dyslexia. Am J Hum Genet. 2005;76:581–591. doi: 10.1086/429131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Couto JM, Gomez L, Wigg K, Ickowicz A, Pathare T, Malone M, Kennedy JL, Schachar R, Barr CL. Association of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder with a candidate region for reading disabilities on chromosome 6p. Biol Psychiatr. 2009;66:368–375. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.02.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Couto JM, Livne-Bar I, Huang K, Xu Z, Cate-Carter T, Feng Y, Wigg K, Humphries T, Tannock R, Kerr EN, Lovett MW, Bremner R, Barr CL. Association of reading disabilities with regions marked by acetylated H3 histones in KIAA0319. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2009;153B:447–462. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dahdouh F, Anthoni H, Tapia-Paez I, Peyrard-Janvid M, Schulte-Korne G, Warnke A, Remschmidt H, Ziegler A, Kere J, Muller-Myhsok B, Nothen MM, Schumacher J, Zucchelli M. Further evidence for DYX1C1 as a susceptibility factor for dyslexia. Psychiatr Genet. 2009;19:59–63. doi: 10.1097/YPG.0b013e32832080e1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dennis MY, Paracchini S, Scerri TS, Prokunina-Olsson L, Knight JC, Wade-Martins R, Coggill P, Beck S, Green ED, Monaco AP. A common variant associated with dyslexia reduces expression of the KIAA0319 gene. PLoS Genet. 2009;5:e1000436. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1000436. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elia J, Gai X, Xie HM, Perin JC, Geiger E, Glessner JT, D’arcy M, Deberardinis R, Frackelton E, Kim C, Lantieri F, Muganga BM, Wang L, Takeda T, Rappaport EF, Grant SF, Berrettini W, Devoto M, Shaikh TH, Hakonarson H, White PS. Rare structural variants found in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder are preferentially associated with neurodevelopmental genes. Mol Psychiatr. 2009;15:637–646. doi: 10.1038/mp.2009.57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elliot C, Murray D, Pearson L. British Abilities Scales. Windsor: NFER-Nelson; 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Falcaro M, Pickles A, Newbury DF, Addis L, Banfield E, Fisher SE, Monaco AP, Simkin Z, Conti-Ramsden G. Genetic and phenotypic effects of phonological short-term memory and grammatical morphology in specific language impairment. Genes Brain Behav. 2008;7:393–402. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2007.00364.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher SE, Francks C, Marlow AJ, Macphie IL, Newbury DF, Cardon LR, Ishikawa-Brush Y, Richardson AJ, Talcott JB, Gayan J, Olson RK, Pennington BF, Smith SD, Defries JC, Stein JF, Monaco AP. Independent genome-wide scans identify a chromosome 18 quantitative-trait locus influencing dyslexia. Nat Genet. 2002;30:86–91. doi: 10.1038/ng792. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Francks C, Paracchini S, Smith SD, Richardson AJ, Scerri TS, Cardon LR, Marlow AJ, Macphie IL, Walter J, Pennington BF, Fisher SE, Olson RK, Defries JC, Stein JF, Monaco AP. A 77-kilobase region of chromosome 6p22.2 is associated with dyslexia in families from the United Kingdom and from the United States. Am J Hum Genet. 2004;75:1046–1058. doi: 10.1086/426404. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friedman JI, Vrijenhoek T, Markx S, Janssen IM, Van Der Vliet WA, Faas BH, Knoers NV, Cahn W, Kahn RS, Edelmann L, Davis KL, Silverman JM, Brunner HG, Van Kessel AG, Wijmenga C, Ophoff RA, Veltman JA. CNTNAP2 gene dosage variation is associated with schizophrenia and epilepsy. Mol Psychiatr. 2008;13:261–266. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4002049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gallagher A, Frederickson N. The phonological assessment battery (PhAB): an initial assessment of its theoretical and practical utility. Educ Child Psychol. 1995;12:53–67. [Google Scholar]
- Gathercole SE, Willis CS, Baddeley AD, Emslie H. The children’s test of nonword repetition: a test of phonological working memory. Memory. 1994;2:103–127. doi: 10.1080/09658219408258940. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hannula-Jouppi K, Kaminen-Ahola N, Taipale M, Eklund R, Nopola-Hemmi J, Kaariainen H, Kere J. The axon guidance receptor gene ROBO1 is a candidate gene for developmental dyslexia. PLoS Genet. 2005;1:e50. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.0010050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harold D, Paracchini S, Scerri T, Dennis M, Cope N, Hill G, Moskvina V, Walter J., Richardson AJ, Owen MJ, Stein JF, Green ED, O’donovan MC, Williams J, Monaco AP (2006) Further evidence that the KIAA0319 gene confers susceptibility to developmental dyslexia. Mol Psychiatry 11:1085–1091, 1061 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Jackman C, Horn ND, Molleston JP, Sokol DK. Gene associated with seizures, autism, and hepatomegaly in an Amish girl. Pediatr Neurol. 2009;40:310–313. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2008.10.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jastak S, Wilkson G. Wide-range achievement test (revised) Wilmington: Jastak Associates; 1984. [Google Scholar]
- Lai CS, Fisher SE, Hurst JA, Vargha-Khadem F, Monaco AP. A forkhead-domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nature. 2001;413:519–523. doi: 10.1038/35097076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lesch KP, Timmesfeld N, Renner TJ, Halperin R, Roser C, Nguyen TT, Craig DW, Romanos J, Heine M, Meyer J, Freitag C, Warnke A, Romanos M, Schafer H, Walitza S, Reif A, Stephan DA, Jacob C. Molecular genetics of adult ADHD: converging evidence from genome-wide association and extended pedigree linkage studies. J Neural Transm. 2008;115:1573–1585. doi: 10.1007/s00702-008-0119-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin PI, Vance JM, Pericak-Vance MA, Martin ER. No gene is an island: the flip-flop phenomenon. Am J Hum Genet. 2007;80:531–538. doi: 10.1086/512133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lind PA, Luciano M, Wright MJ, Montgomery GW, Martin NG, Bates TC. Dyslexia and DCDC2: normal variation in reading and spelling is associated with DCDC2 polymorphisms in an Australian population sample. Eur J Hum Genet. 2010;18:668–673. doi: 10.1038/ejhg.2009.237. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luciano M, Lind PA, Duffy DL, Castles A, Wright MJ, Montgomery GW, Martin NG, Bates TC. A haplotype spanning KIAA0319 and TTRAP is associated with normal variation in reading and spelling ability. Biol Psychiatr. 2007;62:811–817. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ludwig KU, Roeske D, Schumacher J, Schulte-Korne G, Konig IR, Warnke A, Plume E, Ziegler A, Remschmidt H, Muller-Myhsok B, Nothen MM, Hoffmann P. Investigation of interaction between DCDC2 and KIAA0319 in a large German dyslexia sample. J Neural Transm. 2008;115:1587–1589. doi: 10.1007/s00702-008-0124-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marino C, Giorda R, Luisa Lorusso M, Vanzin L, Salandi N, Nobile M, Citterio A, Beri S, Crespi V, Battaglia M, Molteni M. A family-based association study does not support DYX1C1 on 15q21.3 as a candidate gene in developmental dyslexia. Eur J Hum Genet. 2005;13:491–499. doi: 10.1038/sj.ejhg.5201356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marino C, Citterio A, Giorda R, Facoetti A, Menozzi G, Vanzin L, Lorusso ML, Nobile M, Molteni M. Association of short-term memory with a variant within DYX1C1 in developmental dyslexia. Genes Brain Behav. 2007;6:640–646. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2006.00291.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marlow AJ, Fisher SE, Richardson AJ, Francks C, Talcott JB, Monaco AP, Stein JF, Cardon LR. Investigation of quantitative measures related to reading disability in a large sample of sib-pairs from the UK. Behav Genet. 2001;31:219–230. doi: 10.1023/A:1010209629021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mcarthur GM, Hogben JH, Edwards VT, Heath SM, Mengler ED. On the “specifics” of specific reading disability and specific language impairment. J Child Psychol Psychiatr. 2000;41:869–874. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meng H, Hager K, Held M, Page GP, Olson RK, Pennington BF, Defries JC, Smith SD, Gruen JR. TDT-association analysis of EKN1 and dyslexia in a Colorado twin cohort. Hum Genet. 2005;118:87–90. doi: 10.1007/s00439-005-0017-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meng H, Smith SD, Hager K, Held M, Liu J, Olson RK, Pennington BF, Defries JC, Gelernter J, O’reilly-Pol T, Somlo S, Skudlarski P, Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA, Marchione K, Wang Y, Paramasivam M, Loturco JJ, Page GP, Gruen JR. DCDC2 is associated with reading disability and modulates neuronal development in the brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2005;102:17053–17058. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0508591102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newbury DF, Bishop DV, Monaco AP. Genetic influences on language impairment and phonological short-term memory. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9:528–534. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newbury DF, Winchester L, Addis L, Paracchini S, Buckingham LL, Clark A, Cohen W, Cowie H, Dworzynski K, Everitt A, Goodyer IM, Hennessy E, Kindley AD, Miller LL, Nasir J, O’hare A, Shaw D, Simkin Z, Simonoff E, Slonims V, Watson J, Ragoussis J, Fisher SE, Seckl JR, Helms PJ, Bolton PF, Pickles A, Conti-Ramsden G, Baird G, Bishop DV, Monaco AP. CMIP and ATP2C2 modulate phonological short-term memory in language impairment. Am J Hum Genet. 2009;85:264–272. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2009.07.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newbury DF, Fisher SE, Monaco AP. Recent advances in the genetics of language impairment. Genome Med. 2010;2:6. doi: 10.1186/gm127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nopola-Hemmi J, Myllyluoma B, Haltia T, Taipale M, Ollikainen V, Ahonen T, Voutilainen A, Kere J, Widen E. A dominant gene for developmental dyslexia on chromosome 3. J Med Genet. 2001;38:658–664. doi: 10.1136/jmg.38.10.658. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olson R, Forsberg H, Wise B, Rack J (1994) In: Lyon GR (ed) Frames of reference for the assessment of learning disabilities: New views on measurement issues. Baltimore: Paul H Brookes
- Paracchini S, Thomas A, Castro S, Lai C, Paramasivam M, Wang Y, Keating BJ, Taylor JM, Hacking DF, Scerri T, Francks C, Richardson AJ, Wade-Martins R, Stein JF, Knight JC, Copp AJ, Loturco J, Monaco AP. The chromosome 6p22 haplotype associated with dyslexia reduces the expression of KIAA0319, a novel gene involved in neuronal migration. Hum Mol Genet. 2006;15:1659–1666. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddl089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paracchini S, Steer CD, Buckingham LL, Morris AP, Ring S, Scerri T, Stein J, Pembrey ME, Ragoussis J, Golding J, Monaco AP. Association of the KIAA0319 dyslexia susceptibility gene with reading skills in the general population. Am J Psychiatr. 2008;165:1576–1584. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07121872. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pennington BF, Bishop DV. Relations among speech, language, and reading disorders. Annu Rev Psychol. 2009;60:283–306. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poot M, Beyer V, Schwaab I, Damatova N, Van’t Slot R, Prothero J, Holder SE, Haaf T. Disruption of CNTNAP2 and additional structural genome changes in a boy with speech delay and autism spectrum disorder. Neurogenetics. 2009;11:81–89. doi: 10.1007/s10048-009-0205-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Purcell S, Neale B, Todd-Brown K, Thomas L, Ferreira MA, Bender D, Maller J, Sklar P, De Bakker PI, Daly MJ, Sham PC. PLINK: a tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. Am J Hum Genet. 2007;81:559–575. doi: 10.1086/519795. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rice ML, Smith SD, Gayan J. Convergent genetic linkage and associations to language, speech and reading measures in families of probands with specific language impairment. J Neurodev Disord. 2009;1:264–282. doi: 10.1007/s11689-009-9031-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rossi E, Verri AP, Patricelli MG, Destefani V, Ricca I, Vetro A, Ciccone R, Giorda R, Toniolo D, Maraschio P, Zuffardi O. A 12 Mb deletion at 7q33–q35 associated with autism spectrum disorders and primary amenorrhea. Eur J Med Genet. 2008;51:631. doi: 10.1016/j.ejmg.2008.06.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rust J, Golombok S, Trickey G. Wechsler objective reading dimensions. Sidcup: Psychological Corporation; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Scerri TS, Schulte-Korne G. Genetics of developmental dyslexia. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatr. 2009;19:179–197. doi: 10.1007/s00787-009-0081-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scerri TS, Fisher SE, Francks C, Macphie IL, Paracchini S, Richardson AJ, Stein JF, Monaco AP. Putative functional alleles of DYX1C1 are not associated with dyslexia susceptibility in a large sample of sibling pairs from the UK. J Med Genet. 2004;41:853–857. doi: 10.1136/jmg.2004.018341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schumacher J, Anthoni H, Dahdouh F, Konig IR, Hillmer AM, Kluck N, Manthey M, Plume E, Warnke A, Remschmidt H, Hulsmann J, Cichon S, Lindgren CM, Propping P, Zucchelli M, Ziegler A, Peyrard-Janvid M, Schulte-Korne G, Nothen MM, Kere J. Strong genetic evidence of DCDC2 as a susceptibility gene for dyslexia. Am J Hum Genet. 2006;78:52–62. doi: 10.1086/498992. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Semel EM, Wiig EH, Secord W. Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals—revised. San Antonio: Phychological Corporation; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- SLIC A genomewide scan identifies two novel loci involved in specific language impairment. Am J Hum Genet. 2002;70:384–398. doi: 10.1086/338649. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- SLIC Highly significant linkage to the SLI1 locus in an expanded sample of individuals affected by specific language impairment. Am J Hum Genet. 2004;74:1225–1238. doi: 10.1086/421529. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snowling M, Bishop DV, Stothard SE. Is preschool language impairment a risk factor for dyslexia in adolescence? J Child Psychol Psychiatr. 2000;41:587–600. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snowling MJ, Muter V, Carroll J. Children at family risk of dyslexia: a follow-up in early adolescence. J Child Psychol Psychiatr. 2007;48:609–618. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01725.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strauss KA, Puffenberger EG, Huentelman MJ, Gottlieb S, Dobrin SE, Parod JM, Stephan DA, Morton DH. Recessive symptomatic focal epilepsy and mutant contactin-associated protein-like 2. N Engl J Med. 2006;354:1370–1377. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa052773. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taipale M, Kaminen N, Nopola-Hemmi J, Haltia T, Myllyluoma B, Lyytinen H, Muller K, Kaaranen M, Lindsberg PJ, Hannula-Jouppi K, Kere J. A candidate gene for developmental dyslexia encodes a nuclear tetratricopeptide repeat domain protein dynamically regulated in brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2003;100:11553–11558. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1833911100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verkerk AJ, Mathews CA, Joosse M, Eussen BH, Heutink P, Oostra BA. CNTNAP2 is disrupted in a family with Gilles de la Tourette syndrome and obsessive compulsive disorder. Genomics. 2003;82:1–9. doi: 10.1016/S0888-7543(03)00097-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vernes SC, Newbury DF, Abrahams BS, Winchester L, Nicod J, Groszer M, Alarcon M, Oliver PL, Davies KE, Geschwind DH, Monaco AP, Fisher SE. A functional genetic link between distinct developmental language disorders. N Engl J Med. 2008;359:2337–2345. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa0802828. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. Wecshler adult intelligence scales (revised) San Antonio: Psychological Corporation; 1981. [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. Wechsler intelligence scale for children – third UK edn. London: Psychological Corporation; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Wigg KG, Couto JM, Feng Y, Anderson B, Cate-Carter TD, Macciardi F, Tannock R, Lovett MW, Humphries TW, Barr CL. Support for EKN1 as the susceptibility locus for dyslexia on 15q21. Mol Psychiatr. 2004;9:1111–1121. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wigginton JE, Abecasis GR. PEDSTATS: descriptive statistics, graphics and quality assessment for gene mapping data. Bioinformatics. 2005;21:3445–3447. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/bti529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams J, O’donovan MC. The genetics of developmental dyslexia. Eur J Hum Genet. 2006;14:681–689. doi: 10.1038/sj.ejhg.5201575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ylisaukko-Oja T, Peyrard-Janvid M, Lindgren CM, Rehnstrom K, Vanhala R, Peltonen L, Jarvela I, Kere J. Family-based association study of DYX1C1 variants in autism. Eur J Hum Genet. 2005;13:127–130. doi: 10.1038/sj.ejhg.5201272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang L, Cui X, Schmitt K, Hubert R, Navidi W, Arnheim N. Whole genome amplification from a single cell: implications for genetic analysis. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1992;89:5847–5851. doi: 10.1073/pnas.89.13.5847. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zweier C, De Jong EK, Zweier M, Orrico A, Ousager LB, Collins AL, Bijlsma EK, Oortveld MA, Ekici AB, Reis A, Schenck A, Rauch A. CNTNAP2 and NRXN1 are mutated in autosomal-recessive Pitt-Hopkins-like mental retardation and determine the level of a common synaptic protein in Drosophila. Am J Hum Genet. 2009;85:655–666. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2009.10.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.