Abstract
This paper investigates the double standard in attitudes toward courtship and family formation behaviors of sons and daughters. We argue there are strong theoretical reasons to expect that the magnitude of this double standard varies across substantive domains, as well as amongst parents and non-parents. We also argue key methodological limitations of previous studies likely produce an under-estimate of the gender double standard. We provide empirical estimates of the gender double standard that overcome these limitations, including a random assignment experiment explicitly designed to control the effects of social desirability. These estimates demonstrate variability in the double standard across domains and reveal key factors contributing to the magnitude of the double standards in parenting attitudes held by individuals.
Keywords: Gender double standard, courtship/family formation attitudes, parenting attitudes
Gender differences in social behaviors are among the most highly studied topics in the social sciences. Research emphasizes changes over time, causes, and consequences related to the gender segregation in social roles. Over the last half of the 20th century social research documented wide spread changes in attitudes about the appropriate social roles for men and women, uniformly toward more gender egalitarian attitudes (Brewster and Padavic 2000; Brooks and Bolzendhal 2004; Bumpass 1990; Cherlin and Walters 1981; Mason and Lu 1988; Thornton 1989; Thornton and Young-DeMarco 2001; Veroff, Douvan and Kulka 1981). However, recent research documents the persistence of substantial gender differences in key family formation behaviors over the same period. This includes gender differences in sexual, cohabiting, marital, and childbearing behaviors (Barber 2001a; Laumann et al. 1994; Marini 1978; Michael and Tuma 1985; Rindfuss, Morgan and Swicegood 1988; Smock 2000; Thornton, Axinn and Xie 2007; Waite and Goldscheider 1991). In this paper we investigate a set of attitudes that may contribute to these substantial gender differences in family behaviors.
We argue that ideational factors may be an important reason for gender differences in family behaviors in spite of the well known trend toward more egalitarian gender role attitudes. Other attitudes may be at work. For example, a substantial body of research demonstrates that courtship and family formation attitudes and beliefs are strongly correlated with subsequent cohabiting, marital, and childbearing behaviors in the United States and Europe (Axinn and Thornton 1992, 1993; Barber 2000, 2001b; Barber and Axinn 2004; Clarkberg, Stolzenberg and Waite 1995; Lesthaeghe 1983, 2002; Lesthaeghe and Surkyn 1988; Plotnick 1992). This research points toward parents’ attitudes as a particularly powerful force in shaping their children’s behaviors with respect to sex, cohabitation, marriage and childbearing (Axinn and Thornton 1993; Barber 2000). Moreover, this same body of research shows that men and women respond in similar ways to their parents’ attitudes, so there are not substantial gender differences in correlation between parental attitudes and children’s subsequent behavior (Axinn and Thornton 2003; Barber 2000). However, we argue that there may be substantial gender based differences in the parental or prospective parental attitudes about courtship and family formation known to predict children’s subsequent behavior.
Unfortunately, such gender based differences in parental or prospective attitudes towards their children’s behaviors have received very little scientific attention. We investigate gender differences in parental preferences for children’s courtship and family formation behaviors, or what we refer to as the gender double standard in parenting attitudes. We begin by formulating a conceptual framework for the study of this double standard that identifies the breadth of the double standard in parenting values among parents and non-parents. Next, we explain why the magnitude of this double standard is likely to vary across topics, even within the domain of courtship and family formation. Then we identify the key methodological issues limiting our ability to detect these double standards. Finally, we use measures from a unique random-assignment experiment to separate social desirability effects on responses from the true parenting gender double standard. This approach allows us to document the magnitude of both the social desirability effect and the underlying gender double standard in parenting values. These unusual measures also provide the means to examine differences in these parenting double standards across substantive domains. Together these advances create substantial new insight into the processes producing gender double standards in parenting values.
Theoretical and Methodological Background
At the foundation of our conceptual framework is a great deal of previous research on the origins of gender differences in attitudes and behavior. Gender differences in social life have been a focus of sociological and psychological theory throughout the history of these disciplines. Of key interest is whether and to what degree the social differences observed between men and women (e.g., education, occupation, marriage) are influenced by the way the sexes are differentially treated in various social environments and across the life course. For example, many scholars contend that the unequal treatment of men and women can be traced all the way back to different environments at birth, suggesting that the gender differences we observe later in life are a direct result of the distinct behavior, attitudes, and expectations we maintain toward infant boys and girls (Grieshaber 1998; Mondschein, Adolph and Tamis-LeMonda 2000; Pomerleau et al. 1990; Seavey, Katz and Zalk 1975; Sidorowicz and Lunney 1980). Throughout childhood, others observe that historically defined masculine behaviors tend to be enforced or permitted in boys and more feminine behaviors are encouraged in girls (Antill 1987; Crowley et al. 2001; Fagot 1974; Hoffman 1977; Martin 1998; Price-Bonham and Skeen 1982). Even when there is no evidence of gender enforced behavior or attitudes, many studies nevertheless reveal that mothers and fathers retain some distinct gender role attitudes or parenting styles that may vary in magnitude depending on the sex of the child (Conrade and Ho 2001; Grieshaber 1998; McGovern 1990; Ricks 1985; Rossi 1984; Rothbart and Maccoby 1966) and that parents frequently differentiate between their male and female children leading to unequal treatment (Antill 1987; Rossi 1984). Building on these studies, we construct a framework below that defines a gender double standard in parenting values, considers variations in those double standards across subjects, and addresses the fundamental methodological issues involved in studying these double standards.
Defining the Gender Double Standard
We focus on the gender double standard in attitudes toward courtship and family formation behavior. A double standard “implies that two things which are the same are measured by different standards” (Eichler 1980:15). Thus a gender double standard suggests that we evaluate the same behavior of men and women differently; what is acceptable or appropriate for one may not be equally so for the other. However, the gender double standard, applicable towards any type of behavior, has largely been discussed in previous research within the context of the acceptability of premarital sex (Modell 1989; Reiss 1960, 1964, 1967). The original operationalization offered by Reiss (1960, 1964, 1967) describes the double standard as the different standards of sexual permissiveness for women and men.
Since his work, the evidence regarding the existence of a gender double standard has been inconclusive. Some findings reveal that women who engage in premarital sex are more negatively evaluated than are men for the same behavior (Crawford and Popp 2003; Galper and Luck 1980; Harrison, Bennett and Globetti 1969; Milhausen and Herold 1999; Oliver and Sedikides 1992; Reiss 1967; Sheeran et al. 1996; Sprecher and Hatfield 1996; Treboux and Busch-Rossnagel 1990). Other scholars, however, contend that there is little to no evidence for the existence of a gender double standard (DeLameter and MacCorquodale 1979; Gentry 1998; Jacoby and Williams 1985; King, Balswick and Robinson 1977; Mark and Miller 1986; O’Sullivan 1995; Peplau, Rubin and Hill 1977; Sprecher 1989). And some have either been unable to find evidence for or against its existence (Istvan and Griffitt 1980) or believe that the nature of this double standard has evolved so that detecting it has become much more difficult (Sprecher, McKinney and Orbuch 1987). Interestingly, one study by Milhausen and Herold (1999) found that regardless of whether a gender double standard actually remains, the women in their study overwhelming believed that a double standard for sexual behavior (where it is more acceptable for men to have more sexual partners than it is for women) does exist in society.
We believe a number of factors contribute to these inconsistent findings regarding the gender double standard. They include differences in the subject matter of the gender double standard, generality of the gender comparison, the growing social desirability of presenting no double standard, and limitations of, or differences in the sample designs employed in previous research.
Variations in Gender Double Standards across Topics
We argue there are strong theoretical reasons to expect the gender double standard to vary across subject matter. We focus our investigation on courtship and family formation processes, but even within this domain we expect the gender double standard to vary across topic. We differentiate dating, sex, and premarital cohabitation from marital and childbearing behavior to investigate differences within the courtship and family formation domain.
Many factors point toward especially strong gender double standards in parental attitudes about their children’s courtship. Historically the United States has been characterized by stronger prohibitions on women’s participation in premarital sex (Modell 1989; Tolman 1991), so we expect tolerance of early dating, early sex, and premarital cohabitation will be significantly higher for sons than for daughters (Laumann et al. 1994; Moore and Stief 1991; Thornton and Young-DeMarco 2001; Thornton et al. 2007). In part, this difference may be purely the product of gender based ideologies of protecting daughters’ sexual purity (Ferree 1990). For example, Treboux and Busch-Rossnagel (1990) find that the message sons and daughters receive from parental discussions differ such that parents appear accepting of sexuality in their sons, “The message conveyed to daughters may be ‘Don’t–and if you do, we don’t want to know about it,” (Treboux and Busch-Rossnagel 1990:185). Research on teenage sexuality indicates parents communicate more factual information and moral discussion with their daughters about sex and dating than with sons (Nolin and Petersen 1992; Wood et al. 2002). Some scholars even argue that teenage girls are taught to repress or silence their sexual feelings or desire, a finding that is consistent with the popular view of women not engaging in sexual behavior outside of marriage (Tolman 1994).
Of course the difference in attitudes about sons and daughters’ sexuality may also be based on parental concerns regarding the potential consequences of a daughter’s pregnancy (Schalet 2000). The evidence of long term consequences for young women of pregnancy and birth before marriage or at young ages has been a matter of substantial debate (Geronimus and Korenman 1992; Hoffman 1998; Klepinger, Lundberg and Plotnick 1995). But for the parents of young women the short term consequences may be clearer. Young unwed mothers are highly likely to live with their parents and parents are quite likely to make financial contributions to the care of such grandchildren (Aquilino 1996; Sigle-Rushton and McLanahan 2002). The potential burden of a daughter becoming pregnant may be more likely to fall on the parents than the burden of a son causing a pregnancy. As a result there may be cost/benefit reasons for this gender double standard in addition to potential ideological reasons.
Parental double standards regarding their children’s courtship may also vary by the nature of the courtship. Although we do not explicitly evaluate the gender double standard based on the seriousness or stage of a relationship, previous research has shown that the gender double standard is stronger the more casual the relationship (Reiss 1967; Roche 1986; Sprecher and Hatfield 1996). Therefore we expect that the double standard will be more pronounced for sex and dating than for premarital cohabitation.
The historical context of ideas about men’s and women’s behavior in the United States is likely to shape family formation double standards in the opposite direction. The United States is characterized by a longstanding acceptance of earlier marriage for women, so we expect Americans to hold lower ideal ages for women to marry and to place less importance on women working before marriage than men (Thornton and Young-DeMarco 2001). So even though double standards in courtship are in the direction of later courtship for women, we expect the double standards in marriage to be toward early marriage for women. This contrast is consistent with both ideological differences in the concern of sexual purity of sons and daughters and potential gender based differences in the parental burden associated with a child becoming pregnant or causing a pregnancy. Later courtship and early marriage mean daughters are less likely to have sex or become pregnant while unmarried.
Finally, separate from the timing of marriage, we expect little gender based double standard in respondents’ ideas about their children eventually marrying and having children. In part this is because marriage and childrearing continue to be strongly endorsed among both men and women (Moore and Stief 1991; Rossi 1984; Thornton and Young-DeMarco 2001). Likewise, remaining childless and unmarried has been unpopular for both men and women (Blake 1979; Polit 1978; Thornton 1989; Veroff et al. 1981). Thus we have little basis to expect to find gender differences in this domain.
Methodological Issues in Studying the Gender Double Standard
Previous research documenting the gender double standard may not reveal its full extent for three key methodological reasons. First, research in this area has often been limited to the evaluation of a general, unspecified “other” – a fictional woman or man who is occasionally defined as a college student or classmate (Jacoby and Williams 1985; Mark and Miller 1986; O’Sullivan 1995; Reiss 1967; Sheeran et al. 1996; Sprecher 1989; Sprecher et al. 1987; Sprecher et al. 1988), and less often limited to a hypothetical friend, future date, or future spouse (Milhausen and Herold 1999; Oliver and Sedikides 1992). We argue the double standard is most clearly revealed when asking about someone with strong social ties, even if the person is hypothetical. We believe this is especially true when those ties are one’s own children, whether those children exist already or may be present sometime in the future. Although few studies indicate the magnitude of a sexual double standard among parents, it is clear from previous research on parenting that parents do indeed hold a gender double standard for their children in other domains such as gender role attitudes and the appropriateness of non-sexual behaviors (Crowley et al. 2001; Pomerleau et al. 1990) and may even convey different messages to daughters and sons about certain behavior such as sex (Treboux and Busch-Rossnagel 1990).
Second, many studies are based on samples with limited representation, quite often focusing on college students (Galper and Luck 1980; Hendrick and Hendrick 1987; Hong 1983; Jacoby and Williams 1985; Kinnaird and Gerrard 1986; Maranell et al. 1970; Mark and Miller 1986; Sprecher and Hatfield 1996; Sprecher et al. 1987). College students are known to hold attitudes that differ systematically from those in the general adult population (see Sears 1986 for a discussion). Both the processes of selection into institutions of higher education and the social environments of these institutions are likely to produce smaller gender double standards (Alwin et al. 1991). We argue that the adult population of the United States is more likely to possess a strong gender double standard than would college students.
Third, all research involving human subjects may be affected whenever the conditions or topic of the study can potentially affect the respondent to want to present him or herself more positively to the researcher. Social desirability, or “the tendency of people to deny socially undesirable traits or qualities and to admit socially desirable ones” (Phillips and Clancy 1972: 923), is a well-known threat to the measurement of behavior and attitudes in many domains (Belli et al. 1999; Bishop, Tuchfarber and Oldendick 1986; Crowne and Marlowe 1964; Phillips and Clancy 1972; Press and Townsley 1998; Presser 1990; Rossiter and Robertson 1975; Theriault and Holmberg 1998). This area of research reveals that social desirability tends to be a problem for accurate measurement regardless of how the information is collected, with self-reports or mail-in surveys probably yielding the lowest amount of this kind of bias (Press and Townsley 1998) and face-to-face interviews yielding more (Aquilino and Lo Sciuto 1990; Finkel, Guterbock and Borg 1991; Krysan et al. 1994). In addition, the level of social desirability bias present is also affected when what is being measured is of a slightly controversial or sensitive nature such as racial attitudes or premarital sexual behavior (Aquilino and Lo Sciuto 1990; Krysan 1998; MacCorquodale and DeLamater 1979). As gender role attitudes have become more egalitarian in the United States there is growing recognition that it is socially desirable to present no gender double standard when asked about courtship and family formation choices (Thornton and Young-DeMarco 2001). Without explicit steps designed to address the issue of social desirability in responses, research is likely to under-estimate the magnitude of the gender double standard.
To address these three obstacles to the measurement of the gender double standard we use three specific strategies. First, we focus on gender double standards for the behavior of one’s own children (both actual and hypothetical) in order to measure the double standard for a group to whom the individual has extremely close social ties. Second, we measure the double standard in an adult general population sample. Third, we use a random assignment experiment to measure the level of social desirability and to account for that social desirability in our estimates of the magnitude of the gender double standard.
Data and Methods
Data for our analysis comes from a systematic, population sample of white 31-year olds. These respondents were chosen from a randomized sample of 1961 birth records from the Detroit Metropolitan area, and interviewed in 1993 when they were all approximately 31 and a half years of age. This sample design has the advantage of eliminating variance in age, place of birth, and race to assist in focusing on variance in other key factors such as gender, parenthood, and education. Of course inference cannot be made to groups not represented in the sample, including those of other ages, other places of birth, or other races. However, compared to previous research on these topics using college student based samples or other convenience samples, this birth record based sample has the advantage that the only other selection criteria is the occurrence of the individual’s birth itself.
Although the sample respondents were all born in one metropolitan area, by the time they reached adulthood most were living elsewhere, scattered throughout Michigan as well as the rest of the United States. Previous analyses concerning background characteristics, attitudes, and behaviors of these young adults have yielded results very similar to those derived from national samples (Thornton and Axinn 1996). Response rates for this group are excellent. Accounting for both initial selection in 1961 and tracking from 1962 to 1993, the response rate for this sample is approximately 83%, calculated by the number of 1993 interviews divided by the number of possible interviews in 1993 (defined as 1962 respondents minus the number who had died or were permanently ill). This yields 904 respondents.
These 904 respondents are, of course, diverse with respect to many of the key factors of interest. Approximately 51% are female, 59% have experienced parenthood, and educational attainment ranges from 8 years to 17 or more years. Of those who are parents, the maximum number of children is 6, the mean age of the oldest child is nearly 6 years old, and approximately 67 percent have at least one daughter. Table 2, which is explained in more detail below, presents descriptive statistics for all the measures included in our analyses separately for males and females and pooled together. Note that because the children of respondents who are currently parents are quite young, responses to questions about dating, sex, cohabitation, marriage, and childbearing reflect feelings about possible future behaviors of children among both parents and non-parents in this sample.
Table 2.
All Respondents | Males | Females | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | Std. Dev | Min | Max | Mean | Std. Dev | Mean | Std. Dev | |
Attitudes Towards Courtship | ||||||||
Dating Young OK | 3.73 | 1.66 | 1 | 8 | 3.95 | 1.83 | 3.51 | 1.44 |
Sex Young OK | 3.95 | 1.99 | 1 | 8 | 4.25 | 2.04 | 3.65 | 1.90 |
Cohabitation OK | 2.83 | 1.13 | 1 | 4 | 2.88 | 1.11 | 2.79 | 1.15 |
Timing of Marriage | ||||||||
Ideal Age at Marriage (Years) | 26.07 | 2.86 | 18 | 35 | 25.53 | 2.88 | 26.58 | 2.75 |
Work Before Marriage Important | 3.75 | 0.57 | 1 | 4 | 3.69 | 0.63 | 3.80 | 0.50 |
Marriage | ||||||||
OK if No Marriage | 2.77 | 1.04 | 1 | 4 | 2.91 | 1.01 | 2.64 | 1.06 |
OK if No Children | 2.67 | 1.03 | 1 | 4 | 2.78 | 0.99 | 2.57 | 1.06 |
Child’s Gender | ||||||||
If Son Record | .50 | .50 | 0 | 1 | .50 | .50 | .50 | .50 |
CONTROLS | ||||||||
Social Desirability | ||||||||
If Daughter Asked 1st | .51 | .50 | 0 | 1 | .51 | .50 | .50 | .50 |
Respondent Gender | ||||||||
If Respondent is Male | .49 | .50 | 0 | 1 | ||||
Respondent Family Formation Experience | ||||||||
Currently Married | .66 | .47 | 0 | 1 | .63 | .48 | .68 | .47 |
Previously Married | .10 | .30 | 0 | 1 | .08 | .27 | .13 | .34 |
Ever Cohabited | .51 | .50 | 0 | 1 | .50 | .50 | .51 | .50 |
Ever a Parent | .59 | .49 | 0 | 1 | .53 | .50 | .64 | .48 |
Had Sex by age 18 | .60 | .49 | 0 | 1 | .63 | .48 | .58 | .49 |
Has Daughters (Parents Only) | .67 | .47 | 0 | 1 | .63 | .48 | .70 | .46 |
Age of Oldest Child (Parents Only) | 5.98 | 3.83 | 0 | 17 | 5.63 | 3.71 | 6.27 | 3.90 |
# of Children Ever Born (Parents Only) | 1.90 | .86 | 1 | 6 | 1.84 | .87 | 1.94 | .85 |
Respondent Education, Work, Income | ||||||||
Education (years) | 13.99 | 2.10 | 8 | 17 | 13.92 | 2.22 | 14.06 | 1.97 |
Work (% of last 6 months full-time) | 77.73 | 36.49 | 0 | 100 | 92.73 | 20.62 | 63.26 | 42.19 |
Income (log 1993 family income) | 10.53 | 1.44 | 0 | 13.38 | 10.55 | 1.24 | 10.51 | 1.60 |
Respondent Religious Background | ||||||||
Fundamentalist Protestant | .11 | .31 | 0 | 1 | .09 | .28 | .13 | .33 |
Catholic | .54 | .50 | 0 | 1 | .57 | .50 | .52 | .50 |
Jewish, Other, None | .12 | .32 | 0 | 1 | .14 | .35 | .09 | .28 |
Religious Service Attendance | 3.21 | 1.50 | 1 | 6 | 3.04 | 1.50 | 3.37 | 1.49 |
The 1993 interview included a set of child-rearing measures specifically designed to tap into the respondent’s attitudinal differences based on the gender of a child. Table 1 describes the actual wording and coding schemes associated with each measure. Each respondent, regardless of parental status, was asked to indicate how much he/she would be bothered by his/her child dating or having sexual intercourse at various ages, living with an opposite sex partner outside of marriage, not ever marrying, and not ever having children. The least restrictive views about a behavior were given the highest score possible (4 or 8), while the most restrictive attitudes were given a score of 1 (the lowest score possible). Respondents were also asked the ideal age for their daughter/son to marry. The actual age supplied by the respondent was used for the response code. Additionally, respondents were asked to rate the importance of working full time before marriage. This time the answer reflecting later marriage (extremely important) was scored highest, and the answer consistent with early marriage (not at all important) was scored lowest.
Table 1.
Attitudes Towards Courtship | Question Wording and Coding |
---|---|
Dating Young OKa | If a daughter/son of yours starts dating at 16/12, would that bother you a great deal, some, a little, or not at all? 1=A great deal at 16, 2=Some at 16, 3=A little at 16, 4=Not at all at 16, 5=A great deal at 12, 6=some at 12, 7=A little at 12, 8=Not at all at 12 |
Sex Young OKb | If a daughter/son of yours has sexual intercourse at age 20/16, would that bother you a great deal, some, a little, or not at all? 1=A great deal at 20, 2=Some at 20, 3=A little at 20, 4=Not at all at 20, 5=A great deal at 16, 6=some at 16, 7=A little at 16, 8=Not at all at 16 |
Cohabitation OK | Suppose that after she/he has grown up she/he decides to live with a man/woman in an intimate relationship without being married to her/him? Would that bother you a great deal, some, a little, or not at all? 1=A great deal, 2=Some, 3=A little, 4=Not at all |
Timing of Marriage | |
Ideal Age (Years) | If a daughter/son of yours does get married, and if it were just up to you, what do you think would be the ideal age for her/him to get married? Code actual age |
Work Before Marriage Important | What about work – how important do you think it would be for a daughter/son of yours to work full time for a year or two before she/he gets married – would you say very important, somewhat important, not very important, or not at all important? 1=Not at all important, 2=Not very important, 3=Somewhat important, 4=Very important |
Marriage and Children | |
OK if No Marriage | Suppose that things turn out so that a daughter/son of yours does not marry, would that bother you a great deal, some, a little, or not at all? 1=A great deal, 2=Some, 3=A little, 4=Not at all |
OK if No Children | Suppose that things turn out so that she/he does not have any children, would that bother you a great deal, some, a little, or not at all? 1=A great deal, 2=Some, 3=A little, 4=Not at all |
Respondents were initially asked how much it would bother them if their daughter/son began dating at age 14. Those answering “a great deal” or “some” were asked an identical set of questions about dating at age 16. Those answering “a little” or “not at all” were asked an identical set of questions about dating at age 12. The dependent variable was then created by recoding the second response.
Respondents were initially asked how much it would bother them if their daughter/son had sexual intercourse at age 18. Those answering “a great deal” or “some” were asked an identical set of questions about intercourse at age 20. Those answering “a little” or “not at all” were asked an identical set of questions about intercourse at age 16. The dependent variable was then created by recoding the second response.
Note that questions about dating actually began with a question about dating at age 14 and questions about sex began with a question about sex at age 18. Those who replied that dating at age 14 would bother them a great deal or some were asked about dating at age 16, those who replied that dating at age 14 would bother them a little or not at all were asked about dating at age 12. Figure 1 presents these questions and response categories as they were in the questionnaire to demonstrate how the measures are linked and then coded as a single item. Similarly, those who replied that sex at age 18 would bother them a great deal or some were asked about sex at age 20, those who replied that sex at age 18 would bother them a little or not at all were asked about sex at age 16. Thus the items for age of dating and first sex in Table 1 represent the combination of respondents’ initial answers about a central age and respondents’ secondary answers to questions designed to create a wider distribution as shown in Figure 1.
To check the sensitivity of our results, in addition to the analyses of the items presented in Table 1 and discussed throughout the text, we also analyzed the responses to the initial items on central ages (age 14 for dating and age 18 for sex). The correlations between responses to these initial items and the full scales are actually quite high (Pearson’s r = .81 for dating and .89 for sex), so it is no surprise analyses of these initial items yielded results substantively identical to those presented in the text. Of course these metrics for dating at 14 and sex by age 18 are different than those presented in Table 1. Responses to these initial questions are coded to range from 1–4, whereas the full range used in Table 1 varies from 1–8. This greater range produces some minor differences in coefficient sizes and specific estimates. However, for all the analyses presented in Tables 3–5 use of the initial item for central ages produces exactly the same conclusions about direction, standardized magnitude, and statistical significance as the full range measures actually presented in Tables 3–5.
Table 3.
All Respondents N=904 | Non-Parents N=366 | Parents N=538 | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Son | Dau | Diff | Son | Dau | Diff | Son | Dau | Diff | |
Courtship | |||||||||
Dating Young OK | 4.02 | 3.43 | 0.59*** | 4.32 | 3.74 | .58*** | 3.81 | 3.22 | .59*** |
Sex Young OK | 4.24 | 3.65 | 0.59*** | 4.61 | 4.04 | .58*** | 4.00 | 3.34 | .61*** |
Cohabitation OK | 2.96 | 2.71 | 0.25*** | 3.19 | 3.00 | .19*** | 2.80 | 2.51 | .28*** |
Timing of Marriage | |||||||||
Ideal Age at Marriage (Years) | 26.49 | 25.64 | 0.85*** | 27.39 | 26.36 | 1.02*** | 25.88 | 25.15 | .74*** |
Work before Marriage Important | 3.82 | 3.67 | 0.15*** | 3.82 | 3.67 | .14*** | 3.83 | 3.67 | .16*** |
Marriage and Children | |||||||||
OK if No Marriage | 2.80 | 2.74 | 0.06** | 3.04 | 2.88 | .16*** | 2.64 | 2.64 | .00 |
OK if No Children | 2.69 | 2.66 | 0.02 | 3.01 | 3.01 | .00 | 2.46 | 2.42 | .04* |
p<.05,
p<.01,
p<.001} one-tail
Table 5.
Attitudes Towards Courtship | Asked About Son 1st | Asked About Son 2nd | Difference |
---|---|---|---|
Dating Young OK | 4.41 | 3.64 | 0.77*** |
Sex Young OK | 4.34 | 4.15 | 0.19 |
Cohabitation OK | 3.12 | 2.80 | 0.32*** |
Timing of Marriage | |||
Ideal Age at Marriage (Years) | 26.85 | 26.15 | 0.70*** |
Work before Marriage Important | 3.75 | 3.90 | −0.15*** |
Marriage and Children | |||
OK if No Marriage | 2.88 | 2.74 | 0.14* |
OK if No Children | 2.75 | 2.62 | 0.13* |
p <.05,
p< .01,
p <.001} two-tail
These questions were administered to each respondent twice, once pertaining to male children and once about female children. The respondents were randomly assigned to two groups – one group was asked about male children first and then about female children and the second group responded to questions about female children first. This random assignment experiment was designed to reveal the extent to which respondents altered their answers in the second series of questions depending on their answers to the first series of questions1. Preliminary analyses of these data indicated that the measures were affected by the order in which they appeared in the protocol, that is, respondents tended to adjust the second set of responses based upon how they had answered the first set. Hence, a dichotomous measure was created to indicate which gender was asked about first – the daughter or son. The two sets of responses were analyzed as within family daughter/son comparisons as well as in pooled record formats where each respondent contributed a maximum of two records, one per gender of children.
Previous research demonstrates other factors such as age, education, marital status, parenthood and religion have strong effects on these attitudes toward courtship and family formation (Alwin, Cohen and Newcomb 1991; Cassidy and Warren 1996; Cunningham et al. 2005; Fan and Marini 2000; Gervai, Turner and Hinde 1995; Kiecolt and Acock 1988; Kinnaird and Gerrard 1986; Morgan and Waite 1987; Polit 1978; Roche 1986; Singh 1980; Stephan and Corder 1985; Thornton, Alwin and Camburn 1983; Trent and South 1992). With regard to sex and cohabitation, it has been well documented that women, less educated, older, and more religious persons in particular tend to have less permissive attitudes toward premarital sex and premarital cohabitation (Hendrick and Hendrick 1987; Hong 1983; Laner and Housker 1980; Maranell, Dodder and Mitchell 1970; Oliver and Hyde 1993; Sheeran et al. 1993; Sheeran et al. 1996; Singh 1980; Sprecher et al. 1988). Therefore our multivariate models also control for other aspects of respondents’ backgrounds. In the family formation domain we use dichotomous measures of the young adults’ own experiences with marriage (both current and previous), cohabitation (ever), parenthood (ever) and early sexual initiation (before age 18). For those who are parents our models sometimes include measures of whether or not the respondent has a daughter (1, 0), the age of the oldest child (in years), and the total number of children. In the socioeconomic domain we use educational attainment measured in years, the percent of time spent in full-time work during the last six months, and 1993 family income expressed in its natural log. Finally, in the religious domain we measure the respondent’s 1985 religious affiliation coded into a set of three dummy variables (Fundamentalist Protestant, Catholic, Jewish/Other/None), with Non-Fundamentalist Protestant serving as the reference group. We also measure the frequency of the respondent’s attendance at religious services with an ordinal variable coded 1 if the respondent never attends services, 2 if he/she attends less than once a month, 3 if the respondent attends once a month, 4 if he/she attends a few times a month, 5 if attendance occurs once a week, and 6 if the respondent attends several times a week or more. The univariate statistics for all these measures are presented in Table 2.
Results
Univariate Estimates of the Gender Double Standard
Our analysis begins with a simple comparison of attitudes toward sons’ and daughters’ courtship and family formation behavior. In order to make this comparison we computed a mean response for answers to each of the questions in the seven different subjects we investigate. We calculated the means separately for responses regarding sons and responses regarding daughters. Finally we compute the difference between the son and daughter means and use a simple difference of means test to determine the statistical significance of the gender differences in responses for each of the seven different subjects. Our sample includes both 31-year olds who are parents and 31-year olds who have never been parents, so we perform these calculations three times – once for the total sample, once for non-parents only, and once for parents only. Table 3 displays these results.
In the total sample (first column of Table 3) we find that respondents’ attitudes towards courtship behavior (dating, sex, cohabitation) and the timing of marriage (ideal age at marriage, work before marriage) differ significantly by the sex of the child. On average, respondents are more permissive toward a son dating (4.02), engaging in sex (4.24) and cohabiting (2.96) than they are toward a daughter participating in the same behaviors (3.43, 3.65, 2.71 respectively)2. These differences are all statistically significant at a p-value of .001. On the other hand, the average ideal age at marriage for a son (26.49) is significantly higher than the average ideal age at marriage for a daughter (25.64) and the importance of work before marriage is also significantly higher for a son (3.82) than for a daughter (3.67). So parents and potential parents tolerate their sons engaging in courtship behavior earlier than their daughters, but prefer their daughters to marry earlier than their sons. Attitudes toward a son remaining single or childless are also somewhat more permissive than for a daughter, but only the difference in attitudes toward remaining single is statistically significant. This higher tolerance of sons remaining single appears consistent with the higher tolerance of sons marrying later.
In Column 2 of Table 3 we examine these same gender differences, but now only among those who have never been a parent. Surprisingly, the gender differences in attitudes about hypothetical sons and daughters among those who have never been parents are virtually the same as the gender differences in attitudes among those who are parents. These similarities are particularly strong in attitudes about courtship behaviors. Column 3 of Table 3 presents the gender differences in attitudes among only those who are parents, revealing that in the courtship domain the gender differences calculated in Column 2 are almost identical to those calculated in Column 3. These gender differences are also quite similar in the domain of the timing of marriage. Non-parents have a much larger gender difference in attitudes about their sons and daughters remaining single and parents have a much larger gender difference in attitudes about their sons and daughters remaining childless. So, although we predict a stronger gender difference in attitudes about the behavior of those who are socially close – one’s own children – we find within the domains of courtship and marriage timing virtually no differences between those who actually have children and those who have no children. In the domain of marriage and childbearing the contrast between parents and non-parents runs in opposite directions for specific measures. Note that among the parents their children are on average quite young, so that the courtship and family formation behaviors at the focus of these questions loom in the distant future. This may explain some of the similarities between parents and non-parents in this study.
Overall these findings are consistent with our expectations. The measures from this general population sample reveal substantial gender differences in parental attitudes toward their children’s dating, premarital sex, cohabitation, and marriage. The fact that these gender differences run in opposite directions for courtship and the timing of marriage is consistent with historical gender differences in the United States. The fact that attitudes toward children never producing their own offspring do not, overall, vary by gender of the child is consistent with our expectation that the gender double standard varies greatly across subject matter. Finally, the fact that the differences we observe for courtship and marriage timing are so strong is consistent with our focus on one’s own children – a group to whom the respondent has (or will have) extremely close social ties. This appears to be just as true for those answering about hypothetical future children as for those who already have children.
We also compared the size of the gender double standard across the topics represented by our measures. The metric of our measures varies greatly across topics, so we conducted this comparison by recalculating Table 3 for standard deviation scores, or z-scores. This comparison reveals the double standard in attitudes toward dating to be the largest, with a gender difference of more than a third of a standard deviation (.355). The double standard in early sex is .296 standard deviations, cohabitation is .221 standard deviations, age at marriage is .297, work before marriage is .265, not marrying is .058, and no children is .019. Similar to Table 3, results of this analysis also demonstrate much larger doubles standards for marriage timing and courtship than for not marrying or not having children. To estimate the statistical significance of these differences across domains, we then conducted difference-in-difference tests for each pair of domains. The results are presented in Table 4. Virtually every pair is significantly different, with two notable exceptions – the large gender double standard between attitudes about sons’ vs. daughters’ sex and dating is not significantly different and the very small (statistically insignificant) double standard between attitudes about sons’ and daughters’ not marrying and not having children is not statistically different. That is, within the domain of sex and dating and within the domain of not marrying or having children there is no significant difference in gender double standards. All the across domain comparisons reveal statistically significant differences.
Table 4.
Courtship | Timing of Marriage | Marriage and Children | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Dating | Sex | Cohabit | Age at Mar | Work before Marriage | No Marriage | No Children | |
Courtship | |||||||
Dating Young OK | --- | ||||||
Sex Young OK | 0.10*** | --- | |||||
Cohabitation OK | 0.04 | .14*** | --- | ||||
Timing of Marriage | |||||||
Ideal Age at Marriage (Years) | −0.58*** | −0.68*** | −0.53*** | --- | |||
Work before Marriage Important | 0.38*** | 0.48*** | 0.34*** | −.20*** | --- | ||
Marriage and Children | |||||||
OK if No Marriage | −0.30*** | −0.19*** | −0.34*** | −0.88*** | −0.67*** | --- | |
OK if No Children | −0.27*** | −0.18*** | 0.32*** | −0.86*** | 0.66*** | −0.02 | --- |
p<.05,
p<.01,
p<.001} one-tail
Next we examine the effect of social desirability on responses. We accomplish this by focusing exclusively on answers about sons, but by comparing answers when respondents were asked about sons first to those when respondents were asked about sons second. The results in Table 5 reveal that when respondents are asked about the courtship behavior of sons after being asked about daughters (i.e., asked about sons second), they systematically adjust their responses about sons to be less permissive, making these responses more like their answers about daughters. For example, when asked about the timing of a son’s first dating, the mean among respondents who had not been asked about a daughter was 4.41 but the mean among those who had already been asked about a daughter was only 3.64 (first row of Table 5). On average respondents gave significantly less permissive answers about a daughter’s dating behavior (Table 3), and when asked about a daughter first they adjust their responses about a son to be more similar to their responses about a daughter. This difference is consistent with social desirability aimed at reflecting similar attitudes toward both sons and daughters, so that respondents give a different answer about their sons if they have already answered about a daughter than if they have not yet been asked about a daughter. The direction of this difference is the same for dating, premarital sex, and premarital cohabitation, but the differences were only statistically significant for dating and cohabitation.
Respondents who were asked about a son’s timing of marriage and a son not marrying or having children after being asked about a daughter also systematically adjusted their responses to be more similar to responses about daughters. All of these differences are statistically significant at a p-value of .05 or higher. Social desirability also affects responses about a son’s work before marriage, but here the effect is in the opposite direction. In this case, being asked about daughters first leads people to respond with answers about sons that are less similar to those about daughters. Although the direction of this effect was unexpected, it is consistent with concerns that social desirability may bias measures of the gender double standard. Overall these results indicate that the social desirability effect permeates a broader range of attitudes than the gender double standard estimated in Table 3.
Although one might expect symmetry in this social desirability, we actually find that respondents do not adjust their answers about their daughters to match answers about their sons in the same way that they adjusted their answers about their sons to match their answers about their daughters. Put another way, respondents’ restrictive attitudes toward daughters are not subject to the same level of social desirability effects as are their permissive attitudes toward their sons. These findings come from a comparison of those who were asked about daughters first to those who were asked about daughters second that is not displayed in the Tables. We find no statistically significant difference between the mean attitudes toward the courtship behavior or childbearing decisions of daughters (data not shown in tables). Difference in means for timing of marriage (−0.58 {ideal age} and 0.08 {importance of work}) and decisions to marry (0.21) were statistically significant (p-value of .05 or higher). Thus there is some evidence for social desirability effects among responses about daughters, but they are limited to the domain of marital behavior and do not include courtship or childbearing behavior. In the courtship domain respondents are more likely to hold fixed beliefs about what is considered appropriate behavior for daughters and tend to be more flexible about their attitudes toward sons. We find a similar result for attitudes toward remaining childless. Below we examine multivariate models to adjust our estimates of the gender double standard for this social desirability effect.
Multivariate Models of Parents’ Attitudes toward Their Children’s Behavior
We now turn to estimates from multivariate models that control for the social desirability effect while estimating effects on parental attitudes about children’s behavior, including the influence of the gender of the child. These data have been transformed so that each respondent contributes two observations to the analyses; one record speaks to attitudes about sons while the other addresses attitudes about daughters. We analyze multiple records per respondent, and records from the same respondent are likely to be similar, so we estimate these multivariate models using multilevel estimation techniques (two reports clustered within a single individual)3. The two-level models allow us to make appropriate adjustments for the error variance correlation between the two reports from the same respondent (Raudenbush and Bryk 2002). As discussed above, we have good reason to expect that a number of other personal characteristics and previous experiences influence individuals’ attitudes about each of these courtship and family formation behaviors. Therefore in these multivariate models we also add measures of these characteristics and experiences as controls. Our results are displayed in Table 6.
Table 6.
Courtship | Timing of Marriage | Marriage and Children | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Dating Young OK | Sex Young OK | Cohabit OK | Ideal Age (Years) | Work Before Important | OK if No Marriage | OK if No Children | |
Child’s Gender | |||||||
Son | .58*** (13.29) | .58*** (12.85) | .25*** (10.09) | .89*** (15.28) | .16*** (9.28) | .07** (2.59) | .03 (1.27) |
CONTROLS | |||||||
Social Desirability | |||||||
If Daughter Asked 1st | −.32*** (3.18) | −.04 (.34) | −.13* (2.09) | −.53*** (3.20) | .10** (2.85) | .04 (.57) | −.04 (.58) |
Respondent Gender | |||||||
If Respondent Male | .46*** (4.17) | .36** (2.80) | −.05 (.74) | −1.30*** (7.00) | −.17*** (4.17) | .18** (2.42) | .06 (.84) |
Respondent Family Formation Experience | |||||||
Currently Married | −.16 (1.02) | −.62*** (3.52) | −.31*** (3.19) | −1.15*** (4.49) | −.03 (.56) | −.16 (1.53) | −.24** (2.38) |
Previously Married | .12 (.60) | .14 (.59) | −.03 (.20) | −.49 (1.44) | −.09 (1.25) | .03 (.20) | −.15 (1.16) |
Ever Cohabited | .21* (1.94) | .64*** (5.12) | .69*** (9.97) | .55** (3.03) | .01 (.38) | .02 (.31) | .17** (2.39) |
Ever a Parent | −.33** (2.55) | −.21 (1.41) | −.20** (2.46) | −.71*** (3.32) | −.01 (.31) | −.19* (2.17) | −.39*** (4.62) |
Had Sex by Age 18 | .10 (.88) | .43*** (3.31) | .05 (.74) | .29 (1.56) | .03 (.83) | .04 (.52) | .04 (.58) |
Respondent Education, Work, Income | |||||||
Education (years) | .10*** (3.63) | −.01 (.33) | .002 (.10) | .22*** (4.79) | −.01 (1.28) | −.01 (.33) | .01 (.36) |
Work (% last 6 months full-time) | −.002 (1.48) | .002 (.97) | .002 (1.52) | .002 (.55) | .000 (.08) | .002* (2.18) | .003** (2.99) |
Income (log 1993 family income) | .04 (.88) | .09* (1.90) | .04 (1.35) | .02 (.33) | .01 (.44) | .01 (.42) | −.01 (.52) |
Respondent Religion | |||||||
Fundamental Protestant | −.31 (1.63) | −.35 (1.63) | −.21* (1.74) | −.61* (1.93) | .04 (.67) | −.14 (1.09) | −.17 (1.42) |
Catholic | −.29** (2.37) | −.14 (.95) | .01 (.17) | .20 (.97) | .10* (2.21) | −.13 (1.53) | −.13 (1.61) |
Jewish, Other, None | −.01 (.05) | ..06 (.27) | −.06 (.55) | .73** (2.33) | .04 (.54) | −.14 (1.17) | −.17 (1.42) |
Religious Service Attendance | −.04 (.90) | −.27*** (6.11) | −.17*** (6.98) | −.04 (1.56) | −.02 (1.52) | −.05* (1.86) | −.03 (1.12) |
N | 1658 | 1654 | 1657 | 1652 | 1661 | 1659 | 1658 |
p<.05,
p<.01,
p<.001} one-tail
Each column in Table 6 displays the results for a model of each specific attitude toward courtship, marriage timing, and remaining unmarried and childless. Coefficients in row 1 of Table 6 reflect fully controlled child gender effects on respondent attitudes toward courtship and family formation behaviors. These gender effects remain consistent and statistically significant even after the estimates control for social desirability as well as other known predictors of these attitudes4. Respondents continue to be significantly more receptive to earlier ages at dating and sexual initiation for sons, and more accepting of sons’ cohabitation, older ages at marriage, working before marriage, and remaining single. Differences in responses concerning the acceptability of not having children remain non-significant. All of these estimates are completely consistent with the bivariate results presented in Table 35. All in all, these findings provide strong evidence of a substantial gender double standard in parenting attitudes toward children’s courtship and marriage behavior, although the extent of this double standard varies across topics.
Table 6 controls reveal important insights as well. Note that this formulation of the model focuses on the distribution of parents’ answers about their children, not the gender difference in those answers. As discussed in the Table 5 social desirability analyses, when questions referring to daughter dating or cohabitation behaviors are administered first, responses are significantly less accepting than if the questions are asked after the respondent has answered about sons. Social desirability also has significant effects on attitudes about ideal age at marriage and working full-time before marriage. One of the most interesting aspects to note is that, in this formulation of the model, the order in which it is asked does little to influence what parents say about children’s age at sexual initiation. Considering the strong positive effects of “if son record” on this attitude, the willingness to display candor provides some evidence for the argument that an open sexual double standard exists in the domain of sexual behavior. At least in this formulation of the model, which focuses on parents attitudes not the gender difference in those attitudes, parental attitudes toward sex are not affected greatly by social desirability.
Analyses displayed in Table 6 verify the mostly significant effects of parental gender on endorsements of child courtship and family formation behaviors. Men report acceptance for younger ages at dating as well as younger ages at sexual initiation, and are less bothered than women if their offspring do not marry. Men also, on average, report a lower ideal age for marriage and are less likely to feel strongly about their children working full-time before marriage.
A respondent’s own experiences with marriage and family formation also affect attitudes towards children’s courtship and marriage behaviors. Currently married people or people who have already had children are less accepting of younger ages at dating, younger ages at initiation of sexual activity, and cohabitation without marriage. As one might expect, respondents who have cohabited without marriage are significantly more accepting of those same behaviors for their children. Anyone with current marital or parental experience endorses a younger ideal age at marriage, while those who have cohabited express an older ideal age. Currently married respondents or those who have had children have significantly less tolerance for their children not having their own offspring. Being a parent has significant negative effects on acceptance for children not marrying. People who have had cohabitation experience indicate that they are more accepting of their children choosing not to become parents. Finally, those who had sex by age 18 are significantly more accepting of their children engaging in premarital sex.
Education, work, and income play roles in shaping some of the attitudes within the courtship, marriage, and childbearing domains. Those with more years of formal education endorse younger ages at dating and are proponents of older ages at marriage. These findings are both statistically significant. Higher percentages of time spent in full-time work activities are associated with slightly accepting attitudes toward remaining single and remaining childless. Those with higher income also show somewhat more accepting attitudes toward premarital sex.
Respondents’ religious affiliations also have a significant effect on their attitudes toward their children’s courtship and family formation behaviors. Fundamentalist Protestants are somewhat more restrictive in their views concerning age at dating and sexual initiation than non-Fundamentalist Protestants, and significantly less accepting of cohabitation than non-Fundamentalist Protestants. Fundamentalists also prefer younger ideal ages at marriage than non-Fundamentalists. On average Catholics are significantly less accepting of early dating and place a higher importance on working before marriage than non-Fundamentalist Protestants. Those classified in the Jewish/Other/None category endorse significantly older ages at marriage than non-Fundamentalist Protestants.
Religiosity, as measured by attendance at religious services, has particularly strong effects on attitudes about children’s sexual and cohabiting behavior. Respondents with a high level of religious service attendance are significantly less likely to approve of either early sexual behavior or premarital cohabitation. More religious respondents are also somewhat less likely to be tolerant of their child deciding not to marry. Religiosity did not have a significant effect on the other courtship and family formation attitudes.
We also estimated these same models among the more restricted sub-sample of only those respondents who actually had children of their own. This restriction allowed us to investigate the influence of characteristics of the children on these same parenting attitudes. The results of this investigation are presented in Table 7. Just as in the bivariate results presented in Table 3, the effects of the child’s gender are virtually the same in the parent-only sub-sample (Table 7) as they were in the total sample (Table 6). Important differences limit any comparison of results in Tables 6 and Tables 7 – including both the smaller sample size and added explanatory factors in the models displayed in Table 7 – nonetheless, many of the other significant effects displayed in Table 6 are also replicated in Table 7. The biggest differences involve the addition of measures of childbearing experiences among these parents. Parents with daughters report significantly less tolerance of early dating than parents with no daughters (row 7, column 1, Table 7). Parents with older children reported lower ideal ages for marriage than parents with younger children (row 8 of Table 7). All the parents in this study were age 31 at the time of the interview, so those with older children themselves began family formation at younger ages than the other parents, which may explain this result. Parents with older children also reported significantly higher importance of working before marriage than parents with younger children. Parents with many children reported lower ideal ages at marriage and less importance of work before marriage. Clearly characteristics of these 31-year old parents’ actual children do shape their responses to some of these questions about parenting attitudes.
Table 7.
Courtship | Timing of Marriage | Marriage and Children | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Dating Young OK | Sex Young OK | Cohabit OK | Ideal Age (Years) | Work Before Important | OK if No Marriage | OK if No Children | |
Child’s Gender | |||||||
If Son Record | .59*** (10.15) | .60*** (10.19) | .29*** (8.99) | .75*** (11.35) | .16*** (6.89) | .01 (.18) | .04* (1.67) |
CONTROLS | |||||||
Social Desirability | |||||||
If Daughter Asked 1st | −.19 (1.52) | .18 (1.25) | −.03 (.34) | −.71*** (3.54) | .11** (2.51) | .03 (.33) | −.08 (.95) |
Respondent Gender | |||||||
If Respondent Male | .33* (2.20) | .36* (1.99) | −.11 (1.01) | −1.51*** (6.13) | −.15** (2.86) | .33*** (3.09) | .13 (1.22) |
Respondent Family Formation Experience | |||||||
Currently Married | .00 (0.00) | −.67* (1.65) | −.23 (.99) | .17 (.30) | .02 (.21) | .10 (.42) | .00 (.02) |
Previously Married | .01 (.01) | −.25 (.57) | −.13 (.49) | .38 (.62) | −.03 (.22) | .30 (1.15) | −.09 (.36) |
Ever Cohabited | .21 (1.54) | .50** (2.98) | .66*** (6.77) | .54** (2.37) | −.03 (.62) | −.02 (.20) | .19* (1.98) |
Has Daughters | −.37** (2.76) | −.11 (.68) | .01 (.09) | .33 (1.47) | .00 (.08) | .01 (.10) | .03 (.32) |
Age of Oldest Child | −.01 (.34) | .02 (.81) | .01 (.77) | −.08* (2.17) | .02** (2.37) | .02 (1.11) | .01 (.79) |
# of Children Ever Born Or Adopted | −.02 (.28) | −.02 (.15) | −.07 (1.20) | −.27* (1.84) | −.09** (2.93) | .05 (.82) | .02 (.40) |
Had Sex by Age 18 | .12 (.83) | .52** (2.97) | .14 (1.38) | .25 (1.05) | .01 (.17) | .06 (.54) | .09 (.91) |
Respondent Education, Work, Income | |||||||
Education (years) | .03 (.87) | −.08* (1.86) | −.04 (1.57) | .08 (1.26) | −.02 (1.22) | −.01 (.39) | .00 (.17) |
Work (% last 6 months full-time) | −.001 (.56) | .003 (1.23) | .002 (1.52) | .01 (1.53) | .00 (.96) | .002* (1.70) | .003** (2.33) |
Income (log 1993 family income) | .09* (1.75) | .10 (1.60) | .05 (1.36) | −.10 (1.10) | .02 (1.24) | .03 (.88) | .00 (.07) |
Respondent Religion | |||||||
Fundamental Protestant | −.20 (.91) | −.28 (1.06) | −.23 (1.51) | −.57 (1.60) | .06 (.75) | −.30* (1.97) | −.21 (1.34) |
Catholic | −.26* (1.70) | .02 (.10) | .05 (.48) | .48* (1.91) | .13** (2.40) | −.09 (.86) | −.09 (.85) |
Jewish, Other, None | −.09 (.38) | .09 (.31) | −.09 (.53) | .56 (1.37) | −.04 (.44) | −.30* (1.69) | −.21 (1.19) |
Religious Service Attendance | .01 (.26) | −.22*** (3.76) | −.14*** (4.07) | −.08 (1.05) | −.02 (1.05) | −.06* (1.84) | −.02 (.68) |
N | 993 | 989 | 993 | 990 | 994 | 994 | 993 |
p<.05,
p<.01,
p<.001 } one-tail
Conclusion
The evidence we present here is consistent with the conclusion that there is a substantial gender double standard in attitudes about children’s courtship and family formation in the adult population. By focusing on attitudes toward courtship and family formation behavior among those with extremely close social ties – one’s own children – our measures reveal a considerable gender double standard. Our experimental design reveals important consequences of social desirability for measurement of the gender double standard, but in this substantive domain, in reference to one’s own children, the double standard is strong enough to be observed even without controlling for social desirability effects. Controlling for social desirability, the gender double standard is large and robust even in multivariate models controlling for many other known determinants of these attitudes. We conclude this gender double standard in parenting values is substantial and deserves careful research attention.
Research on the gender double standard will confront important obstacles. Our investigation demonstrates that both the magnitude and the direction of these double standards vary by subject matter. Even though we focus on subjects of courtship and family formation, the direction of this double standard varies across specific domains of courtship and family formation, with people preferring later courtship but earlier marriage for their daughters. Given these differences within the courtship and family formation domain investigators will need to be vigilant for substantial variation in the magnitude and direction of the gender double standard across other domains. Likewise, social desirability also appears to affect measurement of this double standard, so that failure to mitigate the social desirability effect may produce under-estimates of the true gender double standard. As a result any research on gender double standards will need to give careful attention to the possible effects of social desirability.
These double standards in parents’ values and preferences for their sons and daughters may hold one key to understanding persistent gender differences in courtship and family behavior. For example, here we document a tendency to hold significantly more permissive attitudes toward a future son’s courtship experiences than a future daughter’s courtship experiences. We find this substantial gender double standard in the 1990s, in spite of three previous decades of social change toward more permissive attitudes about sex and more egalitarian attitudes toward men’s and women’s roles (Thornton and Young-DeMarco 2001; Veroff et al. 1981). It appears that these gender double standards in parents’ preferences for their children’s behavior have not been eradicated by these other changes toward more permissive and egalitarian attitudes. Waite and Goldscheider (1991) argue that marriage and childbearing in the United States will either continue to decline or be fueled by a second gender role revolution – continued social change toward egalitarian gender role attitudes that include more widespread endorsement of men’s involvement in family life. Persistent gender double standards about sex and courtship are not the same as double standards about men’s roles in families. However, the persistence of parenting gender double standards likely contributes to the persistence of separate and distinct gender roles. As long as substantial double standards remain a part of parental socialization of children, completely egalitarian gender role attitudes seem unlikely, and the gender gaps in family behaviors are likely to remain.
Further research on gender double standards may hold one of the keys to understanding the origins of gender differences in other domains of social behavior as well. For example, here we document parents’ tendencies to prefer older ages of marriage for their sons than for their daughters. These double standards parallel some of the observed behavioral differences in the general population – women enter marital relationships earlier than men (Casper and Bianchi 2002; Thornton et al. 2007; Waite and Goldscheider 1991). Additional research will be needed to illustrate the point empirically, but it seems quite likely that these gender double standards in parenting values shape, at least in part, the important gender differences in family formation behavior. If true in this domain, then parents’ gender double standards in other domains of social life may also contribute to gender differences in behavior. Given the strong double standards documented in the parenting attitudes examined here, the intergenerational origins of the gender double standard may prove particularly fruitful. This line of inquiry is a high priority to investigate the factors shaping parenting double standards that may ultimately produce substantial gender differences in social behaviors.
Footnotes
This research was supported by grant R37 HD039425 from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. A previous version of this paper was presented at the 2003 annual meetings of the National Council on Family Relations. We thank Arland Thornton and Jennifer Barber for their helpful comments. Any errors or omissions are the responsibility of the authors.
Note an important feature of the experimental design is that respondents were not permitted to return to earlier questions to alter their responses.
Our final measures of attitudes toward appropriate ages for sex and dating are based on follow up questions to respondent’s initial responses, so we investigated the possibility their initial responses might have yielded different results. First, we found that the final measures are highly correlated with initial responses – Pearson’s r of .89 in the case of sex and .81 in the case of dating. Second, we found that our multivariate models yielded exactly the same substantive results when using respondent’s initial answers as the dependent variable.
Specifically, we use the SAS MIXED procedure to statistically account for the correlation between son and daughter attitudes provided by the same respondent.
Note we also conducted a variety of other tests to investigate the sensitivity of our results to alternative formulations of the model. Most important, for those responses that are ordinal with a relatively small number of response alternatives, we re-estimated the models using ordered logistic regression. To accomplish this we relax the multilevel clustering assumption, which itself yields virtually no differences in estimated results. Then we re-estimated our models of the dating, sex, cohabitation, work before marriage, no marriage, and no children outcomes using ordered logistic regression. This technique changes the metric of coefficients, but it yields the same direction of effects and the same conclusions about statistical significance as our models estimated using OLS regression.
In analyses not shown in the tables, we calculated standardized regression coefficients to compare magnitudes of effects across models with dependent variables in different metrics. Similar to our analysis of standard deviation scores discussed with Table 4, these analyses show effects of gender on attitudes about marriage and childbearing are much smaller that effects of gender on courtship and marriage timing.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Alwin Duane F, Cohen Ronald L, Newcomb Theodore M. Political Attitudes Over the Life Span: The Bennington Women after Fifty Years. Madison, WI: The University of Wisconsin Press; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Antill John K. Parents’ Beliefs and Values About Sex Roles, Sex Differences, and Sexuality. In: Shaver P, Hendrick C, editors. Sex and Gender: Review of Personality and Social Psychology. Vol. 7. 1987. pp. 294–328. [Google Scholar]
- Aquilino William S. Children, Unwed Motherhood, and Child Care: The Life Course of Children Born to Unmarried Mothers: Childhood Living Arrangements and Young Adult Outcomes. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1996;58(2):293–310. [Google Scholar]
- Aquilino William S, Lo Sciuto Leonard A. Effects of Interview Mode on Self Reported Drug Use. Public Opinion Quarterly. 1990;54:362–395. [Google Scholar]
- Axinn William G, Thornton Arland. The Relationship Between Cohabitation and Divorce: Selectivity or Causal Influence? Demography. 1992;29(3):357–374. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Axinn William G, Thornton Arland. Mothers, Children, and Cohabitation: The Intergenerational Effects of Attitudes and Behavior. American Sociological Review. 1993;58(2):233–246. [Google Scholar]
- Barber Jennifer S. Intergenerational Influences on the Entry into Parenthood: Mothers’ Preferences for Family and Nonfamily Behavior. Social Forces. 2000;79:319–348. [Google Scholar]
- Barber Jennifer S. Ideational Influences on the Transition to Parenthood: Attitudes Toward Childbearing and Competing Alternatives. Social Psychology Quarterly. 2001a;64(2):101–127. [Google Scholar]
- Barber Jennifer S. The Intergenerational Transmission of Age at First Birth Among Married and Unmarried Men and Women. Social Science Research. 2001b;30:219–247. [Google Scholar]
- Barber Jennifer S, Axinn William G. New Ideas and Fertility Limitation: The Role of Mass Media. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 2004;66:1180–1200. [Google Scholar]
- Belli Robert F, Traugott Michael W, Young Margaret, McGonagle Katherine A. Reducing Vote Overreporting in Surveys: Social Desirability, Memory Failure, and Source Monitoring. Public Opinion Quarterly. 1999;63:90–108. [Google Scholar]
- Bishop George S, Tuchfarber Alfred J, Oldendick Robert W. Opinions on Fictitious Issues: The Pressure to Answer Survey Questions. Public Opinion Quarterly. 1986;50:240–250. [Google Scholar]
- Blake Judith. Is Zero Preferred? American Attitudes Toward Childlessness in the 1970s. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1979;41:245–257. [Google Scholar]
- Brewster Karin L, Padavic Irene. Changes in Gender Ideology, 1977–1996: The Contribution of Intracohort Change and Population Turnover. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 2000;62:477–487. [Google Scholar]
- Brooks Clem, Bolzendahl Catherine. The Transformation of US Gender Role Attitudes; Cohort Replacement, Social-Structural Change, and Ideological Learning. Social Science Research. 2004;33:106–133. [Google Scholar]
- Bumpass Larry L. What’s Happening to the Family? Interactions Between Demographic and Institutional Change. Demography. 1990;27:483–498. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Casper Lynne M, Bianchi Suzanne M. Continuity and Change in the American Family. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Cassidy Margaret L, Warren Bruce O. Family Employment Status and Gender Role Attitudes: A Comparison of Women and Men College Graduates. Gender and Society. 1996;10:312–329. [Google Scholar]
- Cherlin Andrew, Walters Pamela Barnhouse. Trends in United States Men’s and Women’s Sex-Role Attitudes: 1972–1978. American Sociological Review. 1981;46:453–460. [Google Scholar]
- Clarkberg Marin, Stolzenberg Ross M, Waite Linda J. Attitudes, Values, and Entrance into Cohabitational Versus Marital Unions. Social Forces. 1995;74:609–634. [Google Scholar]
- Conrade Glenys, Ho Robert. Differential Parenting Styles for Fathers and Mothers: Differential Treatment for Sons and Daughters. Australian Journal of Psychology. 2001;53:29–35. [Google Scholar]
- Crawford Mary, Popp Danielle. Sexual Double Standards: A Review and Methodological Critique of Two Decades of Research. The Journal of Sex Research. 2003;40:13–36. doi: 10.1080/00224490309552163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crowley Kevin, Callanan Maureen A, Tenenbaum Harriet R, Allen Elizabeth. Parents Explain More Often to Boys Than to Girls During Shared Scientific Thinking. Psychological Science. 2001;12:258–261. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crowne Douglas P, Marlowe David. The Approval Motive; Studies in Evaluative Dependence. New York: Wiley; 1964. [Google Scholar]
- Cunningham Mick, Beutel Ann M, Barber Jennifer S, Thornton Arland. Reciprocal Relationships Between Gender-Role Attitudes and Life Course Experiences in Young Adulthood. Social Science Research 2005 [Google Scholar]
- DeLameter John, MacCorquodale Patricia. Premarital Sexuality. Madison: University of Wisconsin Press; 1979. [Google Scholar]
- Eichler Margrit. The Double Standard: A Feminist Critique of Feminist Social Science. New York: St. Martin’s Press; 1980. [Google Scholar]
- Fagot Beverly I. Sex Differences in Toddlers’ Behavior and Parental Reaction. Developmental Psychology. 1974;10:554–558. [Google Scholar]
- Fan Pi-Ling, Marini Margaret Mooney. Influences on Gender-Role Attitudes During the Transition to Adulthood. Social Science Research. 2000;29:258–283. [Google Scholar]
- Ferree Myra Marx. Beyond Separate Spheres: Feminism and Family Research. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1990;52:866–884. [Google Scholar]
- Finkel Steven E, Guterbock Thomas M, Borg Marian J. Race-of-Interviewer Effects in a Pre-election Poll: Virginia 1989. Public Opinion Quarterly. 1991;55:313–330. [Google Scholar]
- Galper Ruth Ellen, Luck Dana. Gender, Evaluation, and Causal Attribution: The Double Standard is Alive and Well. Sex Roles. 1980;6:273–283. [Google Scholar]
- Gentry Margaret. The sexual double standard. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 1998;22:505–511. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.1995.tb00093.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geronimus Arline T, Korenman Sanders. The Socioeconomic Consequences of Teen Childbearing Reconsidered. Quarterly Journal of Economics. 1992;107(4):1187–1214. [Google Scholar]
- Gervai Judit, Turner Patricia J, Hinde Robert A. Gender-related Behavior, Attitudes, and Personality in Parents of Young Children in England and Hungary. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 1995;18:105–126. [Google Scholar]
- Grieshaber Susan. Constructing the Gendered Infant. In: Yelland Nicola., editor. Gender in Early Childhood. New York, NY: Routledge; 1998. pp. 15–35. [Google Scholar]
- Harrison Danny E, Bennett Walter H, Globetti Gerald. Attitudes of Rural Youth Toward Premarital Sexual Permissiveness. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1969;31:783–787. [Google Scholar]
- Hendrick Susan S, Hendrick Clyde. Love and Sex Attitudes and Religious Beliefs. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 1987;5:391–398. [Google Scholar]
- Hoffman Lois Wladis. Changes in Family Roles, Socialization, and Sex Differences. American Psychologist. 1977;32:644–657. [Google Scholar]
- Hoffman Saul. Teenage Childbearing Is Not So Bad After All…Or Is It? A Review of the New Literature. Family Planning Perspectives. 1998;30(5):236–239. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hong Sung-Mook. Gender, Religion, and Sexual Permissiveness: Some Recent Australian Data. The Journal of Psychology. 1983;115:17–22. [Google Scholar]
- Istvan Joseph, Griffitt William. Effects of Sexual Experience on Dating Desirability and Marriage Desirability: An Experimental Study. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1980;42:377–385. [Google Scholar]
- Jacoby Arthur P, Williams John D. Effects of Premarital Sexual Standards and Behavior on Dating and Marriage Desirability. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1985;47:1059–65. [Google Scholar]
- Kiecolt K Jill, Acock Alan C. The Long-Term Effects of Family Structure on Gender-Role Attitudes. Journal of Marriage and Family. 1988;50:709–717. [Google Scholar]
- King Karl, Balswick Jack O, Robinson Ira E. The Continuing Premarital Sexual Revolution Among College Females. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1977;39:455–459. [Google Scholar]
- Kinnaird Keri L, Gerrard Meg. Premarital Sexual Behavior and Attitudes Toward Marriage and Divorce among Young Women as a Function of Their Mothers’ Marital Status. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1986;48:757–765. [Google Scholar]
- Klepinger Daniel H, Lundberg Shelly, Plotnick Robert D. Adolescent Fertility and the Educational Attainment of Young Women. Family Planning Perspectives. 1995;27(1):23–28. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krysan Maria. Privacy and the Expression of White Racial Attitudes: A Comparison Across Three Contexts. Public Opinion Quarterly. 1998;62:506–544. [Google Scholar]
- Krysan Maria, Schuman Howard, Jo Scott Lesli, Beatty Paul. Response Rates and Response Content in Mail Versus Face-to-Face Surveys. Public Opinion Quarterly. 1994;58:381–399. [Google Scholar]
- Laner Mary Riege, Housker Steve L. Sexual Permissiveness in Younger and Older Adults. Journal of Family Issues. 1980;1:103–124. doi: 10.1177/0192513x8000100106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laumann Edward O, Gagnon John H, Michael Robert T, Michaels Stuart. The Social Organization of Sexuality: Sexual Practices in the United States. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Lesthaeghe Ron. A Century of Demographic and Cultural Change in Western Europe: An Exploration of Underlying Dimensions. Population and Development Review. 1983;9(3):411–436. [Google Scholar]
- Lesthaeghe Ron. Monograph. Vol. 38. The Hague: Netherlands Interdisciplinary Demographic Institute; 2002. Meaning and Choice: Value Orientations and Life Course Decisions. [Google Scholar]
- Lesthaeghe Ron, Surkyn John. Cultural Dynamics and Economic Theories of Fertility Change. Population and Development Review. 1988;14(1):1–45. [Google Scholar]
- MacCorquodale Patricia, DeLamater John. Self-Image and Premarital Sexuality. Journal of Marriage and Family. 1979;41:327–339. [Google Scholar]
- Maranell Gary M, Dodder Richard A, Mitchell David F. Social Class and Premarital Sexual Permissiveness: A Subsequent Test. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1970;32:85–88. [Google Scholar]
- Marini Margaret Mooney. The Transition to Adulthood: Sex Differences in Educational Attainment and Age at Marriage. American Sociological Review. 1978;43:483–507. [Google Scholar]
- Mark Melvin M, Miller Mark L. The Effects of Sexual Permissiveness, Target Gender, Subject Gender, and Attitudes Toward Women on Social Perception: In Search of the Double Standard. Sex Roles. 1986;15:311–322. [Google Scholar]
- Martin Karin A. Becoming a Gendered Body: Practices of Preschools. American Sociological Review. 1998;63:494–511. [Google Scholar]
- Mason Karen Oppenheim, Lu Yu-Hsia. Attitudes Toward Women’s Familial Roles: Changes in the United States, 1977–1985. Gender and Society. 1988;2:39–57. [Google Scholar]
- McGovern Mary Ann. Sensitivity and Reciprocity in the Play of Adolescent Mothers and Young Fathers with Their Infants. Family Relations. 1990;39:427–431. [Google Scholar]
- Michael Robert T, Tuma Nancy B. Entry into Marriage and Parenthood by Young Men and Women: The Influence of Family Background. Demography. 1985;22:515–544. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Milhausen Robin R, Herold Edward S. Does the Sexual Double Standard Still Exist? Perceptions of University Women. Journal of Sex Research. 1999;36:361–368. [Google Scholar]
- Modell John. Into One’s Own: From Youth to Adulthood in the United States 1920–1975. Berkeley: University of California Press; 1989. [Google Scholar]
- Mondschein Emily R, Adolph Karen E, Tamis-LeMonda Catherine S. Gender Bias in Mothers’ Expectations about Infant Crawling. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2000;77:304–316. doi: 10.1006/jecp.2000.2597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morgan S Philip, Waite Linda J. Parenthood and the Attitudes of Young Adults. American Sociological Review. 1987;52:541–547. [Google Scholar]
- Moore Kristin A, Stief Thomas M. Changes in Marriage and Fertility Behavior: Behavior Versus Attitudes of Young Adults. Youth and Society. 1991;22:362–386. doi: 10.1177/0044118x91022003004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nolin Mary Jo, Petersen Karen Kay. Gender Differences in Parent-Child Communication about Sexuality. Journal of Adolescent Research. 1992;7:59–79. [Google Scholar]
- Oliver Mary Beth, Hyde Janet Shibley. Gender Differences in Sexuality: A Meta Analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 1993;114:29–51. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oliver Mary Beth, Sedikides Constantine. Effects of Sexual Permissiveness on Desirability of Partner as a Function of Low and High Commitment to Relationship. Social Psychology Quarterly. 1992;55:321–333. [Google Scholar]
- O’Sullivan Lucia F. Less is More: The Effects of Sexual Experience on Judgments of Men’s and Women’s Personality Characteristics and Relationship Desirability. Sex Roles. 1995;33:159–181. [Google Scholar]
- Peplau Letitia A, Rubin Zick, Hill Charles T. Sexual Intimacy in Dating Relationships. Journal of Social Issues. 1977;33(2):86–109. [Google Scholar]
- Phillips Derek L, Clancy Kevin J. Some Effects of ‘Social Desirability’ in Survey Studies. American Journal of Sociology. 1972;77:921–940. [Google Scholar]
- Plotnick Robert D. The Effects of Attitudes on Teenage Premarital Pregnancy and Its Resolution. American Sociological Review. 1992;57:800–811. [Google Scholar]
- Polit Denise F. Stereotypes Relating to Family-Size Status. Journal of Marriage and Family. 1978;40:105–114. [Google Scholar]
- Pomerleau Andrée, Bolduc Daniel, Malcuit Gérard, Cossette Louise. Pink or Blue: Environmental Gender Stereotypes in the First Two Years of Life. Sex Roles. 1990;22:359–367. [Google Scholar]
- Press Julie E, Townsley Eleanor. Wives’ and Husbands’ Housework Reporting: Gender, Class, and Social Desirability. Gender and Society. 1998;12(2):188–218. [Google Scholar]
- Presser Stanley. Can Changes in Context Reduce Vote Overreporting in Surveys? Public Opinion Quarterly. 1990;54:586–593. [Google Scholar]
- Price-Bonham Sharon, Skeen Patsy. Black and White Fathers’ Attitudes Toward Children’s Sex Roles. Psychological Reports. 1982;50:1187–1190. [Google Scholar]
- Raudenbush Stephen W, Bryk Anthony S. Hierarchical Linear Models: Applications and Data Analysis Methods. 2. Sage Publications; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Reiss Ira L. Premarital Sexual Standards in America. New York: The Free Press; 1960. [Google Scholar]
- Reiss Ira L. The Scaling of Premarital Sexual Permissiveness. Journal of Family and Marriage. 1964;26:188–198. [Google Scholar]
- Reiss Ira L. The Social Context of Premarital Sexual Permissiveness. New York: Holt, Reinhardt and Winston; 1967. [Google Scholar]
- Ricks Shirley S. Father-Infant Interactions: A Review of Empirical Research. Family Relations. 1985;34:505–511. [Google Scholar]
- Rindfuss Ronald R, Philip Morgan S, Gray Swicegood C. First Births in America: Changes in the Timing of Parenthood. Berkeley: University of California Press; 1988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roche John P. Premarital Sex: Attitudes and Behavior by Dating Stage. Adolescence. 1986;21:107–121. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rossi Alice S. Gender and Parenthood. American Sociological Review. 1984;49:1–19. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rossiter John R, Robertson Thomas R. Children’s Television Viewing: An Examination of Parent-Child Consensus. Sociometry. 1975;38:308–326. [Google Scholar]
- Rothbart Mary K, Maccoby Eleanor E. Parents’ Differential Reactions to Sons and Daughters. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1966;4:237–243. doi: 10.1037/h0023698. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schalet Amy T. Raging Hormones, Regulated Love: Adolescent Sexuality and the Constitution of the Modern Individual in the United States and the Netherlands. Body and Society. 2000;6:75–105. [Google Scholar]
- Sears Elizabeth. The Ages of Man: Medieval Interpretations of the Life Cycle. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 1986. [Google Scholar]
- Seavey Carol, Katz Phyllis A, Zalk Sue R. Baby X: The Effect of Gender Labels on Adult Responses to Infants. Sex Roles. 1975;1:103–109. [Google Scholar]
- Sheeran Paschal, Abrams Dominic, Abraham Charles, Spears Russell. Religiosity and Adolescents’ Premarital Sexual Attitudes and Behavior: An Empirical Study of Conceptual Issues. European Journal of Social Psychology. 1993;23:39–52. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2420230104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheeran Paschal, Spears Russell, Abraham Charles S, Abrams Dominic. Religiosity, Gender and the Double Standard. The Journal of Psychology. 1996;130:23–33. doi: 10.1080/00223980.1996.9914985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sidorowicz Laura S, Sparks Lunney G. Baby X Revisited. Sex Roles. 1980;6:67–73. [Google Scholar]
- Sigle-Rushton Wendy, McLanahan Sara. The Living Arrangements of New Unmarried Mothers. Demography. 2002;39(3):415–433. doi: 10.1353/dem.2002.0032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singh BK. Trends in Attitudes Toward Premarital Sexual Relations. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1980;42:387–393. [Google Scholar]
- Smock Pamela J. Cohabitation in the United States: An Appraisal of Research Themes, Findings, and Implications. Annual Review of Sociology. 2000;16:703–26. [Google Scholar]
- Sprecher Susan. Premarital Sexual Standards for Different Categories of Individuals. Journal of Sex Research. 1989;26:232–248. [Google Scholar]
- Sprecher Susan, Hatfield Elaine. Premarital sexual standards among U.S. College Students: Comparison with Russian and Japanese students. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 1996;25:261. doi: 10.1007/BF02438165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sprecher Susan, McKinney Kathleen, Orbuch Terri L. Has the Double Standard Disappeared?: An Experimental Test. Social Psychology Quarterly. 1987;50:24–31. [Google Scholar]
- Sprecher Susan, McKinney Kathleen, Walsh Robert, Anderson Carrie. A Revision of the Reiss Premarital Sexual Permissiveness Scale. Journal of Marriage and Family. 1988;50:821–828. [Google Scholar]
- Stephan Cookie White, Corder Judy. The Effects of Dual-career Families on Adolescents’ Sex-Role Attitudes, Work and Family Plans, and Choices of Important Others. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1985;47:921–929. [Google Scholar]
- Theriault Stephen W, Holmberg Diane. The New Old-Fashioned Girl: Effects of Gender and Social Desirability on Reported Gender-Role Ideology. Sex Roles. 1998;39(1/2):97–112. [Google Scholar]
- Thornton Arland. Changing Attitudes toward Family Issues in the United States. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1989;51:873–893. [Google Scholar]
- Thornton Arland, Axinn William G. A Review of the Advantages and Limitations of the Intergenerational Panel Study of Parents and Children. Institute for Social Research, The University of Michigan; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Thornton Arland, Young-DeMarco Linda. Four Decades of Trends in Attitudes Toward Family Issues in the United States: The 1960s through the 1990s. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 2001;63(4):1009–1037. [Google Scholar]
- Thornton Arland, Alwin Duane F, Camburn Donald. Causes and Consequences of Sex-Role Attitudes and Attitude Change. American Sociological Review. 1983;48:211–227. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thornton Arland, Axinn William G, Xie Yu. Marriage and Cohabitation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Tolman Deborah L. Adolescent Girls, Women and Sexuality: Discerning Dilemmas of Desire. In: Gilligan C, Rogers A, Tolman D, editors. Women girls and psychotherapy: Reframing Resistance. New York: Haworth; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Tolman Deborah L. Doing Desire: Adolescent Girls’ Struggles for/with Sexuality. Gender and Society. 1994;8:324–342. [Google Scholar]
- Treboux Dominique, Busch-Rossnagel Nancy A. Social Network Influences on Adolescent Sexual Attitudes and Behaviors. Journal of Adolescent Research. 1990;5:175–189. [Google Scholar]
- Trent Katherine, South Scott J. Sociodemographic Status, Parental Background, Childhood Family Structure, and Attitudes Toward Family Formation. Journal of Marriage and Family. 1992;54:427–439. [Google Scholar]
- Veroff Joseph, Douvan Elizabeth, Kulka Richard A. The Inner American: A Self-Portrait from 1957 to 1976. New York: Basic Books; 1981. [Google Scholar]
- Waite Linda J, Goldscheider Frances Korbin. New Families, No Families?: The Transformation of the American Home Prepared. Berkeley: University of California Press; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Wood Eileen, Senn Charlene Y, Desmarais Serge, Park Laura, Verberg Norine. Sources of Information About Dating and Their Perceived Influence on Adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Research. 2002;17:401–417. [Google Scholar]