Skip to main content
Endocrinology logoLink to Endocrinology
. 2011 Feb 1;152(3):946–956. doi: 10.1210/en.2010-0715

Estradiol Alters Fos-Immunoreactivity in the Hippocampus and Dorsal Striatum during Place and Response Learning in Middle-Aged But Not Young Adult Female Rats

Kristen E Pleil 1, Melissa J Glenn 1, Christina L Williams 1,
PMCID: PMC3040062  PMID: 21285311

Estradiol administration before place and response training revealed marked effects on hippocampal and striatal Fos-immunoreactivity in middle-aged but not young adult female rats.

Abstract

Evidence from lesion and inactivation studies suggests that the hippocampus (HPC) and dorsal striatum compete for control over navigation behavior, and there is some evidence in males that the structure with greater relative activation controls behavior. Estradiol has been shown to enhance HPC-dependent place learning and impair dorsal striatum-dependent response learning in female rats, possibly by increasing hippocampal activation and/or decreasing striatal activation. We used Fos-immunoreactivity (Fos-IR) to examine the activation of several subregions of the HPC and striatum in ovariectomized female rats with or without estradiol replacement 30 min after place or response learning. In 4-month-old rats, neither task nor estradiol increased Fos-IR above explore control levels in any subregion analyzed, even though estradiol impaired response learning. In 12-month-old rats, estradiol increased Fos-IR in the dentate gyrus, dorsal medial striatum, and dorsal lateral striatum in place task learners, while the absence of estradiol increased Fos-IR in these regions in response task learners. However, learning rate was not affected by estradiol in either task. We also included a group of long-term ovariectomized 12-month-old rats that displayed impaired place learning and altered Fos-IR in CA1 of the HPC. These results suggest that task-specific effects of estradiol on hippocampal and striatal activation emerge across age but that relative hippocampal and striatal activation are not related to learning rate during spatial navigation learning.


Lesions or inactivation of the dorsal hippocampus (HPC) impair place learning and in some cases enhance response learning, while lesions or inactivation of the dorsal striatum (DS) impair response learning but sometimes enhance place learning (14). These findings suggest that the HPC and DS may compete for behavioral control and the neural system with greater relative output guides spatial navigation (5). Estradiol has been shown to bias rats to use a place strategy on navigation tasks that can be solved using either strategy, and it can enhance explicit place learning and impair explicit response learning (e.g., Ref. 6). Evidence that these effects of estradiol occur locally within the HPC and DS (7, 8) suggests that estradiol may modulate navigation learning by altering the engagement of hippocampal and/or dorsal striatal ensembles at the onset or during navigation learning, however this hypothesis has never been assessed.

The immediate-early gene c-fos has been used to measure neural activation during other hormonally-modulated behaviors, including aggression, sex, and maternal behaviors (913), revealing that estradiol may promote behavioral responses by enhancing activation in relevant neural regions (13). c-fos has also been used to examine the relative activation of the HPC and DS during place and response learning in males (1416). However, while one study found that activation in the DS but not HPC distinguished response from place learners (14), two other studies found that activation in hippocampal but not dorsal striatal subregions was greater in rats that used a place strategy to solve a navigation task than those that used a response strategy (15, 16). Despite the varied results, these data suggest that differential activation within subregions of the HPC and DS occurs during place and response navigation.

While estradiol appears to be a strong modulator of navigation behavior in the young adult female, its effects during middle age, as cycling ovarian hormones decline, remain unclear (17, 18). While some studies have shown that estradiol improves place navigation in middle-aged females (1924), others have shown estradiol to be ineffective at this age (25, 26). The DS may also lose responsivity to estradiol as rats age (e.g., Ref. 27). In addition, long-term ovarian hormone deprivation has been shown to be detrimental to general cognition and hippocampal function in particular (28, 29; see also Ref. 30). Thus, the effects of estradiol and long-term hormone deprivation on the function of the HPC and DS in middle-aged females during spatial navigation need further examination.

The current study had several goals: 1) to determine whether the HPC and DS show different activation patterns when female rats are engaged in place vs. response learning, 2) to examine whether estradiol's effects on place and response learning are reflected in changes in hippocampal and striatal activation, and 3) to determine whether aging and long-term estradiol deprivation influence hippocampal and striatal activation during navigation learning.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

Subjects were 4- and 12-month-old female Sprague Dawley CD strain rats purchased from Charles Rivers Laboratories (Kingston, NY) at 3 months of age (n = 90). They were paired-housed in individually-ventilated cages and given ad libitum access to water and a standard diet (Rodent Diet 5001, PMI Nutrition International, Inc., Brentwood, MO). The temperature-controlled colony room was maintained on a 12:12 h light:dark cycle with lights on at 0700 daily, and all behavioral training took place during the lights-on phase of the day. Rats were handled daily for 10 d before ovariectomy and then an additional 7 d before behavioral testing. All procedures were approved by and conducted in accordance with the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Duke University.

Experimental design

The current study used rats in three hormonal conditions, as shown in Fig. 1: A) Rats ovariectomized (OVX) at 3 mo and behaviorally trained at 4 mo (n = 46); B) Rats OVX at 11 mo and behaviorally trained at 12 mo (n = 26); and C) Rats OVX at 3 mo and behaviorally trained at 12 mo (n = 18). Short-term OVX rats (groups A and B) received either two sc injections of 10 μg 17 β-estradiol (Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO) dissolved in 0.1 ml sesame oil (Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO) or the oil vehicle alone 48 and 24 h before behavioral training, as illustrated in Fig. 1, A and B. This hormone replacement regimen has been used previously to demonstrate estradiol's modulation of place and response learning (6) and has been shown to increase a number of mechanisms of hippocampal plasticity (31, 32). All long-term OVX rats received oil injections (Fig. 1C). Thirty minutes after place or response training or control trials, rats were perfused transcardially and their brains harvested for quantification of Fos-immunoreactivity (Fos-IR) in several hippocampal and dorsal striatal subregions, as delineated in Fig. 1. This sacrifice time point was chosen because Fos expression peaks 90–120 min after induction (24, 3335) and all rats completed training within 60–100 min of the start of training. This time point has previously been used to reveal differences in Fos-IR between male place and response task learners (14).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Experimental timeline of hormonal manipulations and behavioral training for rats trained at 4 months of age (A) and 12 months of age (B and C), and timeline of behavioral training, probe testing, and sacrifice (D) and illustration of samples taken from left hemisphere of DLS and DMS (E, left) and DG, hilus, CA1, and CA3 of the HPC (E, right).

Ovariectomies

To remove the source of circulating gonadal hormones, rats were anesthetized with a combination of 80 mg/kg ketamine plus 10 mg/kg xylazine and OVX via bilateral incisions through the abdomen. The ovary and ovarian fat on each side of the body were exposed, tied off, and surgically removed. The site of removal was cauterized and placed back into the abdominal cavity, and the muscle wall and skin were sutured. Antibiotic cream and buprenorphine (0.5 mg/kg, ip) were administered at the end of all surgeries and again 12 h later to prevent infection and pain, respectively. To confirm that rats were no longer cycling, vaginal samples were collected from each rat daily for 1 week before estradiol or oil injections. Cells were collected on a moistened cotton swab, rolled onto a glass slide, and examined at ×10 magnification to determine estrous cycle status based on the proportion of leukocytes, epithelial, cornified, and nucleated cells (36).

Apparatus and behavioral training

All behavioral training and probe testing took place within a single session in a black plastic pool with a diameter of 1.8 m filled with approximately 30 cm of water mixed with black water-based paint maintained at 24–25 C. The maze was located in a 6.5 m × 3.8 m rectangular room that was rich with cues including a curtain, cart with cages, metal counter, shelves, and posters with high-contrast patterns.

Rats in each hormonal condition were randomly assigned to receive training on either a place or response version of the water maze or to serve as explore controls. Cage mates were transported to the test room in individual holding cages on a cart that remained in a stable location throughout training. To habituate rats to the maze, each rat was placed in the pool and allowed to swim for 30 s. During training, rats were placed in the water at one of four locations (N, S, E, and W) at the beginning of each trial in a pseudo-random order. Each trial ended when the rat climbed onto a black escape platform with diameter 10.2 cm. If the rat failed to find the platform at the end of 60 s, the experimenter guided it to the platform where it remained for 10 s before being returned to its holding cage. Cage mates alternated trials in this fashion until each had reached the learning criterion (see below) or 40 trials were completed. Training trials were immediately followed by one probe trial in which the platform was removed and the rat was allowed to swim for 60 s. During all trials, the path of the animal was recorded in real time using HVS Image (Buckingham, WH, UK). Some rats were randomly assigned to serve as controls for motor activity and latent learning associated with exploration of the environment while swimming in the pool. Each explore control was yoked to a behaviorally-trained rat such that it swam for the same amount of time and the same number of trials.

Place task

Rats were required to find an escape platform below the surface of the water that was in the center of one quadrant of the maze throughout training by using extramaze cues (e.g., salient landmarks and room geometry). Platform locations were counterbalanced within and across experimental groups. Criterion for learning was achieved when the rat performed four consecutive trials from random start locations with latencies under 10 s. Then the rat was given one probe trial as described above.

Response task

Rats were required to find a visible escape platform when extramaze cues were hidden by a curtain drawn around the pool. The platform was always located in the quadrant either to the left or right of the random start position of the rat, so that it was always in the same relative position to the rat's start location. Thus, rats could navigate to the platform by using a visual strategy with the platform as a beacon and/or acquiring a response strategy (e.g., go to the left). To encourage rats to acquire a response strategy, criterion for learning was reached when the rat completed four consecutive trials in which the latencies were within 10 s of each other and the rat made a similar path or turn sequence to reach the platform. A probe trial with no platform was conducted on the following trial.

Perfusion, immunohistochemistry, and quantification of Fos-IR

Rats were anesthetized with an ip injection of 80 mg/kg ketamine and 10 mg/kg xylazine and then perfused transcardially with 0.9% saline followed by 4% paraformaldehyde. Brains were removed and postfixed in 4% paraformaldehyde for 4 d and then stored in sodium azide at 4 C until sectioned. Every fifth 50-μm section of HPC and DS was collected and used for immunohistochemistry, and subjects from all experimental groups were included in each immunohistochemistry run to ensure identical labeling conditions for all groups. Tissue was washed in 50 mm PBS, incubated in 1% hydrogen peroxide to remove endogenous peroxidase, and washed in PBS. Tissue was blocked in 0.1% BSA and 0.2% TX-100 and incubated in the primary antibody solution containing 1:5000 c-Fos polyclonal rabbit IgG (Sigma, St. Louis, MO) overnight. Tissue was washed in PBS and incubated in a secondary solution of 0.1% BSA and 1:500 concentration of biotinylated antirabbit made in goat. Tissue was washed in PBS, incubated in an avidin biotin complex (Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA) solution with 0.1% BSA, and washed in PBS. Tissue was incubated in a 3,30-diaminobenzidine solution (Sigma) until brown, indicating that Fos-IR had occurred. Sections were washed in PBS, mounted onto glass slides, dehydrated, and coverslipped with Permount mounting medium (Fisher Scientific, Fair Lawn, NJ).

The optical fractionator method was used to estimate the number of Fos-IR cells in subregions of the HPC and DS (StereoInvestigator, MicroBrightField, Colchester, VT). Cells were only counted as Fos-IR if they were in focus in the inner 20-μm block of tissue and the solid reaction product covered at least half of the nucleus. In the dorsal lateral striatum (DLS) and dorsal medial striatum (DMS), a 150 μm × 150 μm sample was counted within a 187.5 μm × 187.5 μm grid throughout a uniform-sized square sample outlined by the experimenter, illustrated in Fig. 1E (left). In CA1, CA3, dentate gyrus (DG), and hilus of the HPC, a 120 μm × 120 μm sample was counted within a 150 μm × 150 μm grid throughout the extent of each subregion within a contour drawn by the experimenter to include principle cell layers and adjacent cell layers, illustrated in Fig. 1E (right). Three left-hemisphere sections were analyzed for each subregion of each rat at an interval of five sections, between +1.2 mm anterior to bregma and −0.2 mm posterior to bregma for the DS and between −2.8 and −4.0 mm posterior to bregma for the HPC according to Paxinos & Watson's rat brain atlas (37). Estimated counts based on section thickness were divided by estimated volume to attain a density measure of Fos-IR for each brain subregion of each rat.

Statistical analysis

Similar but separate analyses were calculated for 4- and 12-month-old rats treated with oil and estradiol, as well as for short-term and long-term OVX 12-month-olds. To examine the effects of task and estradiol replacement on learning place and response tasks, the number of trials to reach behavioral criterion was examined using ANOVAs with estradiol status (oil, estradiol) and task (place, response, explore control) as independent variables. Because behavioral criteria were based on latencies, mean swim speed across the first 10 trials (before any rats reach criterion) was compared between oil- and estradiol-treated rats within each task and revealed no differences (P > 0.10), suggesting that differences in latency reported in the Results section were not attributable to differences in swim speed. t tests were also used to compare oil- and estradiol-treated rats in the number of platform crossings and percent time spent in each maze quadrant during the place task probe to determine the accuracy and precision of the platform location learned by rats trained on the place task. In the response task, rats that made a similar turn sequence on the probe trial as the last four training trials were categorized as having acquired a response strategy, while those that searched randomly for the platform during the probe were categorized as having used a visual strategy.

To examine effects of estradiol and task on hippocampal and striatal Fos-IR, we used ANOVAs with the number of Fos-IR cells per mm3 as the dependent variable and task (place, response, explore control) and estradiol treatment (oil, estradiol) as the independent variables for each subregion quantified (DG, hilus, CA1, CA3, DMS, and DLS). t tests were calculated to determine whether strategy used on the response probe affected Fos-IR, but only hippocampal analyses in 4-month-olds were significant, so they are the only ones reported in the results (all others, P > 0.15).

To compare the relative activation of striatal and hippocampal subregions between oil- and estradiol-treated rats during place and response learning, ratios between Fos-IR in different subregions of HPC and DS (DG:DMS, DG:DLS, CA1:DMS, CA1:DLS, and DMS:DLS) were compared. We also calculated correlations between subregions (DMS vs. DLS, DG vs. CA1, DG vs. DMS, DG vs. DLS, CA1 vs. DMS, and CA1 vs. DLS) to determine whether there was a linear relationship between Fos-IR in different subregions, and we examined the relationship between Fos-IR in each subregion and the number of trials to reach behavioral criterion to determine whether any effects in activation might be related to the amount of training or motor behavior rather than only task or estradiol replacement, but these correlations were not significant and so are not reported below (P > 0.05). Because explore controls used in the place and response tasks did not differ in any measure analyzed, they were combined for data analysis. All analyses described above were also used to determine whether duration of OVX in 12-month-old rats influenced these measures.

Results

Effects of estradiol treatment on learning and Fos-IR in 4-month-old rats

Learning

Estradiol replacement did not affect the number of training trials to reach behavioral criterion on the place task but estradiol-treated rats were impaired in the rate of response learning, revealed by a main effect of task [F (1, 14) = 6.29, P = 0.025] and an estradiol status × task interaction [F (1, 14) = 4.71, P = 0.048], but no main effect of estradiol status (P > 0.40; Fig. 2A). Direct comparisons confirmed that estradiol-treated rats required more trials to reach criterion than oil-treated rats in the response task [t (19) = 4.02, P = 0.001] but not the place task (P > 0.15). However, oil-treated rats learned the place and response tasks at the same rate (P > 0.55).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

The mean number of trials to reach behavioral criterion in the place and response tasks (A) and the number of Fos-IR cells per mm3 in the DG (B), DMS (C), and DLS (D) of 4-month-olds. *, P = 0.001.

Analyses of the 60-s place task probe trial revealed that both oil- and estradiol-treated rats learned the platform location with a high level of accuracy and precision (target quadrant: oil: 54 ± 3%, estradiol: 52 ± 5%; all other quadrants: oil: <18 ± 4%, estradiol: <18 ± 4%; P > 0.80). In the response task, analysis of the path used on the probe trial revealed that a majority of oil-treated rats (90%) and estradiol-treated rats (67%) used a response strategy to locate the target in the absence of a visible platform during the probe trial. That is, they traveled the same path on the probe trial as they did on the last four criterion trials, even though there was no visible platform. The remainder of the rats swam randomly throughout the maze on the probe trial, indicating that they had probably learned to find the platform by using it as a visual beacon, but they learned the response task at a similar rate (P > 0.10).

Fos-immunoreactivity

Hippocampus

Neither task nor estradiol replacement increased Fos-IR in any hippocampal subregions analyzed, as shown in Fig. 2B and Table 1 (P > 0.20), nor were there any interactions between the two. However, response task learners that used a response strategy on the probe trial had more Fos-IR in the DG and hilus than rats that used a visual strategy [DG: t (17) = 2.84, P = 0.011; hilus: t (17) = 2.45, P = 0.025]. In contrast, no differences in Fos-IR between these two groups were detected in CA1 or CA3 (P > 0.10).

Table 1.

The number of Fos-IR cells per mm3 ± sem in CA1, CA3, and hilus of 4-month-old rats

Place
Response
Control
Oil Estradiol Oil Estradiol Oil Estradiol
CA1 7087 ± 466 6729 ± 491 6733 ± 498 6885 ± 307 6071 ± 754 5774 ± 407
CA3 2610 ± 355 2614 ± 405 2409 ± 311 2456 ± 296 2469 ± 290 1693 ± 570
Hilus 7034 ± 1068 7407 ± 628 7030 ± 832 6583 ± 674 6782 ± 1679 5323 ± 1629

Dorsal striatum

Overall, task did not affect Fos-IR in the DS, but estradiol-treated rats had increased Fos-IR in the DMS [F (1, 40) = 4.68, P = 0.037] and DLS [F (1, 40) = 10.23, P = 0.003]. However, there were no interactions between task and estradiol status (P > 0.20), suggesting that estradiol effects on Fos-IR were slight.

Ratios and correlations between Fos-IR in hippocampal and striatal subregions

We found a main effect of estradiol status in several of the ratios between hippocampal and striatal subregions [DG:DLS (F [1, 40] = 8.32, P = 0.006), CA1:DLS (F [1, 40] = 9.51, P = 0.003), CA1:DMS (F [1, 40] = 4.17, P = 0.048], and DMS:DLS [F (1, 40) = 4.61, P = 0.038], but no other effects or interactions (P > 0.10; data not shown), suggesting that estradiol altered the relative engagement of hippocampal and striatal subregions during navigation learning. However, these differences were driven by the fact that estradiol-treated rats had increased striatal but not hippocampal activation. Fos-IR in the DMS and DLS were positively correlated for response task learners (r = 0.791, P < 0.0001), place task learners (r = 0.699, P = 0.003), and all 4-month-old rats combined (r = 0.760, P < 0.0001), suggesting direct communication between these two subregions. No other correlations were significant (P > 0.15).

Effects of estradiol treatment on learning and Fos-immunoreactivity in 12-month-old rats

Learning

Similar to 4-month-olds, estradiol replacement did not affect the number of training trials needed to reach behavioral criterion in the place task, but unlike 4-month-olds, estradiol did not impair response learning rate (P > 0.60). However, response task learners took significantly more trials than place task learners to reach criterion [F (1, 18) = 19.06, P = 0.0004; t (9) = 2.83, P = 0.020], as shown in Fig. 3A.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3.

The mean number of trials to reach behavioral criterion in the place and response tasks (A) and the number of Fos-IR cells per mm3 in the DG (B), DMS (C), and DLS (D) of short-term OVX 12-month-olds. +, Main effect of task with P < 0.001; *, P < 0.05 within task; #, significantly different from all other oil-treated groups with P < 0.05; $, P < 0.10.

Analyses of the percent time spent in each quadrant of the maze and the number of platform location crossings during the place task probe trial revealed that both oil- and estradiol-treated rats learned the platform location with a high level of accuracy and precision (target quadrant: oil: 49 ± 7%, estradiol: 47 ± 5%; all other quadrants: oil: <23 ± 10%, estradiol: <23 ± 8%; P > 0.30). On the response probe trial, while a majority of oil-treated rats (67%) used a response strategy to locate the target in the absence of a visible platform, only 17% of estradiol-treated rats used a response strategy, but they learned at a similar rate (P > 0.10), suggesting that estradiol impaired response learning to the point where rats were unable to acquire a response strategy.

Fos-immunoreactivity

Hippocampus

Neither task nor estradiol alone affected Fos-IR in any hippocampal subregion examined (P > 0.25), similar to 4-month-olds. However, task and estradiol interacted to modulate Fos-IR in the DG [F (2, 20) = 6.36, P = 0.007] but not in other hippocampal subregions (P > 0.15), as shown in Fig. 3B and Table 2 and illustrated in Fig. 4. Direct comparisons revealed that estradiol-treated rats had increased Fos-IR in the DG during the place task [t (8) = 2.34, P = 0.048] but did not display the increase in Fos-IR that oil-treated rats displayed in the response task [t (10) = 2.50, P = 0.032; Fig. 3B]. However, oil-treated groups did not differ from each other in Fos-IR (P > 0.10), suggesting that estradiol modulated DG Fos-IR in a task-specific manner.

Table 2.

The number of Fos-IR cells per mm3 ± sem in CA1, CA3, and hilus of 12-month-old rats

Place
Response
Control
Oil Estradiol Oil Estradiol Oil Estradiol
CA1 4505 ± 670 5022 ± 595 4287 ± 152 4195 ± 629 4564 ± 219 4093 ± 408
CA3 1723 ± 187 1906 ± 302 1515 ± 239 1576 ± 156 2581 ± 644 1548 ± 345
Hilus 5155 ± 866 5294 ± 522 4931 ± 438 3610 ± 690 3853 ± 585 3884 ± 935
Fig. 4.

Fig. 4.

Examples of Fos-IR in the DMS of short-term OVX 12-month-old rats (top six panels) administered oil (left column) or estradiol (right column) that learned place (first row) or response (second row) tasks or served as explore controls (third row). Bottom two panels are representative photomicrographs from the DG.

Dorsal striatum

Estradiol replacement alone did not affect Fos-IR in either subregion of the DS (P > 0.85). However, task affected Fos-IR in the DMS [F (2, 20) = 4.15, P = 0.031], with response task learners having greater Fos-IR than explore controls [t (6) = 3.55, P = 0.012] and place task learners [t (9) = 2.38, P = 0.041], as illustrated in Figs. 3C and 4. However, task did not affect Fos-IR in the DLS (P > 0.20). Similar to effects on Fos-IR in the DG, estradiol status and task interacted to modulate Fos-IR in the DMS [F (2, 20) = 9.26, P = 0.001] and DLS [F (2, 20) = 5.23, P = 0.015], as shown in Fig. 3, C and D. Estradiol-treated rats had greater striatal Fos-IR than oil-treated rats in the place task [DMS: t (8) = 2.85, P = 0.021; DLS: t (8) = 2.43, P = 0.041] but did not display the increase in Fos-IR observed in oil-treated rats in the response task [DMS: t (10) = 2.95, P = 0.015; DLS: t (10) = 2.04, P = 0.069; Fig. 3, C and D]. Thus, the patterns of Fos-IR in the DMS and DLS were similar to the pattern observed in the DG but not other hippocampal subregions.

Ratios and correlations between Fos-IR in hippocampal and striatal subregions

Neither task nor estradiol replacement affected the ratios of Fos-IR between any hippocampal and striatal subregions (P > 0.15). However, Fos-IR in the DG, DMS, and DLS were positively correlated with one another for all rats, as shown in Table 3, but CA1 Fos-IR was not significantly correlated with Fos-IR in any of these subregions (P > 0.20). When place and response task learners were analyzed separately, similar correlations were significant for place task learners (DMS vs. DLS, DG vs. DMS, DG vs. DLS; all others, P > 0.10), as shown in Table 3. There were fewer significant correlations for response task learners (DMS vs. DLS, DG vs. DMS; all others, P > 0.10), but similar patterns of and correlations between Fos-IR across tasks suggest that activation of these three subregions are related during navigation, regardless of the strategy used.

Table 3.

Correlations between hippocampal and striatal Fos-IR in 12-month-old short-term OVX rats

All Place Response
DG vs. CA1 0.280 0.167 0.298
DMS vs. DLS 0.740**** 0.844** 0.660*
DG vs. DMS 0.690*** 0.789** 0.617*
DG vs. DLS 0.583** 0.649* 0.470
CA1 vs. DMS 0.105 0.120 0.030
CA1 vs. DLS 0.255 0.299 0.135
*

, P < 0.05;

**

, P < 0.01;

***

, P < 0.001;

****

, P < 0.0001.

Effects of long-term ovariectomy on learning and Fos-IR in 12-month-old rats

Learning

An ANOVA for the number of training trials to reach behavioral criterion revealed a main effect of task [F (1, 19) = 8.33, P = 0.009] and a main effect of duration of OVX [F (1, 19) = 5.93, P = 0.025] but no interaction (P > 0.45). However, direct comparisons showed that compared with short-term OVX rats, long-term OVX rats (9 mo) were impaired in learning rate on the place task [t (8) = 2.33, P = 0.048] but not the response task (P > 0.25), as shown in Fig. 5A.

Fig. 5.

Fig. 5.

The mean number of trials to reach behavioral criterion in the place and response tasks (A) and Fos-IR in CA1 (B) of short-term and long-term OVX 12-month-olds. *, P < 0.05.

For the place task, analysis of the percent time spent in each quadrant of the maze and the number of platform location crossings during probe trial revealed that both short-term and long-term OVX rats learned the platform location with a high level of accuracy and precision (long-term OVX target quadrant: 43 ± 7%; all other quadrants: <24 ± 9%; P > 0.55). Path analysis from the response probe trial revealed that a majority of long-term OVX rats (67%) and short-term OVX rats (67%) used a response strategy to locate the target in the absence of a visible platform. These results suggest that long-term OVX impaired place learning rate but not the degree to which rats learned the platform location, and it did not affect any aspect of response learning.

Fos-immunoreactivity

Hippocampus

Long-term OVX explore controls had significantly less Fos-IR than short-term OVX rats in CA1 (P = 0.05) but not other hippocampal subregions (P > 0.45), as shown in Fig. 5B. Long-term OVX response task learners had less DG Fos-IR than short-term OVX response task learners [F (2, 25) = 3.43, P = 0.048; t (10) = 3.07, P = 0.012], but short-term OVX and long-term OVX rats had similar Fos-IR in the place task and in the control condition (P > 0.70). In CA1, there was a main effect of duration of OVX [F (1, 25) = 6.69, P = 0.016] and a trend of task (P = 0.075), but no interaction (P > 0.15; Fig. 5B). While response and control long-term OVX rats had less Fos-IR than short-term OVX rats [t (10) = 3.70, P = 0.004; t (6) = 2.39, P = 0.05, respectively], place task learners did not (P > 0.95), as shown in Fig. 1. These results suggest that long-term OVX rats that learned the place task increased CA1 activation up to short-term OVX levels to learn the place task. All other ANOVAs revealed no effects or interactions (P > 0.10).

Dorsal striatum

There were no effects of task or duration of OVX on DMS Fos-IR (P > 0.05). In the DLS, there was a main effect of task [F (2, 25) = 4.20, P = 0.027] and OVX duration [F (1, 25) = 5.54, P = 0.027] but no interaction (P > 0.80). However, no direct comparisons were significant (P > 0.05).

Ratios and correlations between Fos-IR in hippocampal and striatal subregions

Neither task nor long-term OVX affected the relative amount of activation in the hippocampal and striatal subregions examined. While several ANOVAs revealed main effects of either task or duration of OVX (DG:DLS, DMS:DLS, CA1:DLS), there were no significant interactions in any analyses (P > 0.05), and no direct comparisons revealed significant differences (P > 0.15). Similar to other groups, Fos-IR in the DG, DMS, and DLS were correlated with one another. In addition, Fos-IR in the DG and CA1 were also positively correlated (data not shown). However, CA1 was not correlated with either the DMS or DLS (P > 0.25). When place and response task learners were analyzed separately, similar correlations were significant or nearly significant for response task learners (DMS vs. DLS, DG vs. DMS, DG vs. DLS, DG vs. CA1; all others, P > 0.10). There were few significant correlations for place task learners (DG vs. CA1, DG vs. DMS; all others, P > 0.25). These results suggest that as in other groups, activation in the DG, DMS, and DLS were related, but additionally, CA1 activation corresponded with these subregions.

Discussion

The current results suggest a complex interaction between hippocampal and striatal subregions activated during place and response learning that is influenced by aging and is highly modulated by estradiol. However, our data do not support the hypothesis that the HPC and DS compete for control over navigation behavior via selective activation in females as has been suggested for males (5), nor that activation in the HPC and DS are directly related to ease of place or response learning (38). We found that estradiol-treated rats had greater Fos-IR in the DG, DMS, and DLS than oil-treated OVX rats in a place task, but estradiol appeared to prevent the increase of Fos-IR in these subregions that was observed in oil-treated rats that learned a response task at 12 but not 4 months of age. Estradiol's effects on Fos-IR required rats to be engaged in navigation task, because estradiol did not alter activation in any hippocampal or striatal subregions in rats that simply explored the pool. In contrast to some previous published findings (e.g., Refs. 6, 19, 22), estradiol replacement did not improve place learning at either age, however mild stress from exposure to the water may have promoted rapid learning and masked an effect of estradiol. Consistent with previous work (e.g., 6), estradiol impaired response learning in young adult females. Further, we showed for the first time that aging impairs response learning in females and that estradiol exacerbates this impairment in middle age. These results suggest that under some conditions, estradiol modulates both learning and neural activation in females during spatial navigation, but these are not directly related.

Learning rate is not related to Fos-IR above a threshold

A majority of the current results do not support the contention that relative hippocampal and striatal activation control navigation strategy use. For example, 4-month-old estradiol-treated rats were impaired in response learning rate compared with oil-treated rats but did not have altered Fos-IR in the DS. Moreover, there were no significant correlations between Fos-IR in any subregions analyzed and learning rate on either task for any 4-month-old groups, suggesting that striatal activation is not related to ease of learning a response task. In the HPC, estradiol administration increased DG Fos-IR in 12-month-old place task learners but did not alter learning. While these results suggest that learning rate is not related to hippocampal activation, there was some evidence that successful place learning required CA1 activation to be at threshold. Long-term OVX rats had less CA1 Fos-IR than short-term OVX rats in all groups but place task learners, suggesting that long-term OVX rats had to increase CA1 activation to reach the threshold of activation required to learn the place task.

Estradiol affects learning and Fos-IR differently in 4- and 12-month-olds

Estradiol has been shown to increase baseline levels of neural activation in the HPC and DS (e.g., Refs. 39, 40), but in this study estradiol did not increase neural activation in explore controls that simply swam in the maze. In behaviorally-trained rats, estradiol replacement and task interacted to modulate activation in the DG, DLS, and DMS beyond explore control levels in 12-month but not 4-month-olds. One possible reason for this age-related difference is that the level of neural activation that occurred during swimming/exploring alone may have already been sufficient to support learning in 4-month-olds but not in 12-month-olds. Thus, estradiol modulation of neural activation during training may have functioned to enhance exploration-induced activation to ensure quick learning in 12-month-olds that may have found the task more difficult than 4-month-olds. This proposed role of estradiol is similar to its known function in mediating lordosis in the female rodent, in which strong tactile stimulation can elicit lordosis, but estradiol (and progesterone) sensitizes the neural circuit so that gentle flank stimulation is sufficient to elicit lordosis (41). Alternatively, while our sacrifice time point was chosen to capture peak Fos-IR induced by the learning phase of the task, we may have actually observed the neural correlate for later memory. Estradiol administration aids in the consolidation of spatial memory in an age-dependent manner (4245), so it is possible that the Fos-IR pattern we observed would have been more reflective of later success for memory of the platform location.

Functions of and coordination between hippocampal and striatal subregions during navigation learning

Together, our results suggest somewhat different functions of hippocampal and striatal subregions during navigation in females than what has previously been reported in males. While we found that DLS activation is higher in response task learners than other groups (like 14), we found a more robust effect in the DMS than the DLS, possibly because our training required subjects to form a representation of the task that included the outcome (46, 47), and we attempted to capture peak Fos-IR during the learning phase of the task (as in 14). However, this pattern of activation in the DS was also observed in the DG, suggesting that Fos-IR measured here may reflect the similar sensory inputs received by the DG and DS from the entorhinal cortex (4850) rather than similar functions of all three subregions. Therefore, these results do not suggest that the HPC and DS compete for control during navigation learning via selective activation (15, 16). These differences highlight the importance of task and sacrifice time point on activation of the HPC and DS, and they suggest that there may be marked sex differences in the roles of hippocampal and striatal subregions during navigation learning, with females highly tuned to the effects of estradiol for these functions.

Measuring activation vs. plasticity

Results from the current and previous studies indicate that both the HPC and DS are engaged during navigation tasks, but our results suggest that there may be a different relationship between engaged brain regions, estradiol, and behavioral control during spatial navigation than simple neural activation. Evidence that estradiol increases basal forebrain cholinergic input to the HPC (e.g., 51) and that cholinergic transmission in the HPC and DS is correlated with strategy use (e.g., 52, 53) suggests that estradiol may influence the relative engagement of the HPC and DS via differential cholinergic activity in these regions. Alternatively, control over navigation behavior may better indexed by measures of plasticity. For example, the same estradiol administration paradigm used in this and other studies (e.g., 6) has also been shown to increase a number of NMDA receptor-dependent mechanisms of plasticity in the HPC (31, 32, 5458) that have been correlated with performance on hippocampal-dependent tasks (59, 60). Further, estradiol decreases striatal NMDA receptor binding (61) and enhances the impairment in response learning caused by blocking dopamine receptors (7), which are required for several forms of striatal plasticity (62, 63). Thus, estradiol's modulation of hippocampal and striatal plasticity may be a potential mechanism by which it influences navigation strategy use.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grant AG009525 (to C.L.W.).

Disclosure Summary: The authors have nothing to declare.

Footnotes

Abbreviations:
DG
Dentate gyrus
DLS
dorsal lateral striatum
DMS
dorsal medial striatum
DS
dorsal striatum
Fos-IR
Fos-immunoreactivity
HPC
hippocampus
OVX
ovariectomized.

References

  • 1. Olton DS, Papas BC. 1979. Spatial memory and hippocampal function. Neuropsychologia 17:669–682 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Chang Q, Gold PE. 2003. Intra-hippocampal lidocaine injections impair acquisition of a place task and facilitate acquisition of a response task in rats. Behav Brain Res 144:19–24 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Mitchell JA, Hall G. 1988. Caudate-putamen lesions in the rat may impair or potentiate maze learning depending upon availability of stimulus cues and relevance of response cues. Q J Exp Psychol B 40:243–258 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Devan BD, McDonald RJ, White NM. 1999. Effects of medial and lateral caudate-putamen lesions on place- and cue-guided behaviors in the water maze: relation to thigmotaxis. Behav Brain Res 100:5–14 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. White NM, McDonald RJ. 2002. Multiple parallel memory systems in the brain of the rat. Neurobiol Learn Mem 77:125–184 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Korol DL, Kolo LL. 2002. Estrogen-induced changes in place and response learning in young adult female rats. Behav Neurosci 116:411–420 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Zurkovsky L, Brown SL, Boyd SE, Fell JA, Korol DL. 2007. Estrogen modulates learning in female rats by acting directly at distinct memory systems. Neuroscience 144:26–37 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Zurkovsky L, Brown SL, Korol DL. 2006. Estrogen modulates place learning through estrogen receptors in the hippocampus. Neurobiol Learn Mem 86:336–343 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Joppa MA, Meisel RL, Garber MA. 1995. -Fos expression in female hamster brain following sexual and aggressive behaviors. Neuroscience 68:783–792 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Pfaus JG, Heeb MM. 1997. Implications of immediate-early gene induction in the brain following sexual stimulation of female and male rodents. Brain Res Bull 44:397–407 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Dudley CA, Rajendren G, Moss RL. 1992. Induction of FOS immunoreactivity in central accessory olfactory structures of the female rat following exposure to conspecific males. Mol Cell Neurosci 3:360–369 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Bennett AL, Greco B, Blasberg ME, Blaustein JD. 2002. Response to male odours in progestin receptor- and oestrogen receptor-containing cells in female rat brain. J Neuroendocrinol 14:442–449 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Sheehan T, Numan M. 2002. Estrogen, progesterone, and pregnancy termination alter neural activity in brain regions that control maternal behavior in rats. Neuroendocrinology 75:12–23 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Gill KM, Bernstein IL, Mizumori SJ. 2007. Immediate early gene activation in hippocampus and dorsal striatum: effects of explicit place and response training. Neurobiol Learn Mem 87:583–596 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Teather LA, Packard MG, Smith DE, Ellis-Behnke RG, Bazan NG. 2005. Differential induction of c-Jun and Fos-like proteins in rat hippocampus and dorsal striatum after training in two water maze tasks. Neurobiol Learn Mem 84:75–84 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Colombo PJ, Brightwell JJ, Countryman RA. 2003. Cognitive strategy-specific increases in phosphorylated cAMP response element-binding protein and c-Fos in the hippocampus and dorsal striatum. J Neurosci 23:3547–3554 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Hogervorst E, Williams J, Budge M, Riedel W, Jolles J. 2000. The nature of the effect of female gonadal hormone replacement therapy on cognitive function in post-menopausal women: a meta-analysis. Neuroscience 101:485–512 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Markowska AL. 1999. Sex dimorphisms in the rate of age-related decline in spatial memory: relevance to alterations in the estrous cycle. J Neurosci 19:8122–8133 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Markham JA, Pych JC, Juraska JM. 2002. Ovarian hormone replacement to aged ovariectomized female rats benefits acquisition of the morris water maze. Horm Behav 42:284–293 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Talboom JS, Williams BJ, Baxley ER, West SG, Bimonte-Nelson HA. 2008. Higher levels of estradiol replacement correlate with better spatial memory in surgically menopausal young and middle-aged rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem 90:155–163 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Bimonte-Nelson HA, Francis KR, Umphlet CD, Granholm AC. 2006. Progesterone reverses the spatial memory enhancements initiated by tonic and cyclic oestrogen therapy in middle-aged ovariectomized female rats. Eur J Neurosci 24:229–242 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Daniel JM, Hulst JL, Berbling JL. 2006. Estradiol replacement enhances working memory in middle-aged rats when initiated immediately after ovariectomy but not after a long-term period of ovarian hormone deprivation. Endocrinology 147:607–614 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Rodgers SP, Bohacek J, Daniel JM. 2010. Transient estradiol exposure during middle age in ovariectomized rats exerts lasting effects on cognitive function and the hippocampus. Endocrinology 151:1194–1203 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Morgan JI, Cohen DR, Hempstead JL, Curran T. 1987. Mapping patterns of c-fos expression in the central nervous system after seizure. Science 237:192–197 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Feng Z, Cheng Y, Zhang JT. 2004. Long-term effects of melatonin or 17 beta-estradiol on improving spatial memory performance in cognitively impaired, ovariectomized adult rats. J Pineal Res 37:198–206 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Ziegler DR, Gallagher M. 2005. Spatial memory in middle-aged female rats: assessment of estrogen replacement after ovariectomy. Brain Res 1052:163–173 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Roy EJ, Sheinkop S, Wilson MA. 1982. Age alters dopaminergic responses to estradiol. Eur J Pharmacol 82:73–75 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Markowska AL, Savonenko AV. 2002. Effectiveness of estrogen replacement in restoration of cognitive function after long-term estrogen withdrawal in aging rats. J Neurosci 22:10985–10995 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Frick KM, Burlingame LA, Arters JA, Berger-Sweeney J. 2000. Reference memory, anxiety and estrous cyclicity in C57BL/6NIA mice are affected by age and sex. Neuroscience 95:293–307 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Bimonte-Nelson HA, Singleton RS, Hunter CL, Price KL, Moore AB, Granholm AC. 2003. Ovarian hormones and cognition in the aged female rat: I. Long-term, but not short-term, ovariectomy enhances spatial performance. Behav Neurosci 117:1395–1406 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Gould E, Woolley CS, Frankfurt M, McEwen BS. 1990. Gonadal steroids regulate dendritic spine density in hippocampal pyramidal cells in adulthood. J Neurosci 10:1286–1291 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Woolley CS, McEwen BS. 1993. Roles of estradiol and progesterone in regulation of hippocampal dendritic spine density during the estrous cycle in the rat. J Comp Neurol 336:293–306 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Hoffman GE, Lyo D. 2002. Anatomical markers of activity in neuroendocrine systems: are we all ‘fos-ed out?’ J Neuroendocrinol 14:259–268 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Hoffman GE, Smith MS, Verbalis JG. 1993. c-Fos and related immediate early gene products as markers of activity in neuroendocrine systems. Front Neuroendocrinol 14:173–213 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Sagar SM, Sharp FR, Curran T. 1988. Expression of c-fos protein in brain: metabolic mapping at the cellular level. Science 240:1328–1331 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Matthews MK, Jr, Kenyon R. 1984. Four- versus five-day estrous cycles in rats: vaginal cycling and pregnancy. Physiol Behav 33:65–67 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Paxinos G, Watson C. 1998. The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates. New York: Academic Press; [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Packard MG, McGaugh JL. 1996. Inactivation of hippocampus or caudate nucleus with lidocaine differentially affects expression of place and response learning. Neurobiol Learn Mem 65:65–72 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Dominguez-Salazar E, Shetty S, Rissman EF. 2006. Rapid neural Fos responses to oestradiol in oestrogen receptor alphabeta double knockout mice. J Neuroendocrinol 18:195–202 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Rudick CN, Woolley CS. 2000. Estradiol induces a phasic Fos response in the hippocampal CA1 and CA3 regions of adult female rats. Hippocampus 10:274–283 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Pfaff D, Montgomery M, Lewis C. 1977. Somatosensory determinants of lordosis in female rats: behavioral definition of the estrogen effect. J Comp Physiol Psychol 91:134–145 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Gresack JE, Kerr KM, Frick KM. 2007. Life-long environmental enrichment differentially affects the mnemonic response to estrogen in young, middle-aged, and aged female mice. Neurobiol Learn Mem 88:393–408 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Packard MG, Teather LA. 1997. Intra-hippocampal estradiol infusion enhances memory in ovariectomized rats. Neuroreport 8:3009–3013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Packard MG, Teather LA. 1997. Posttraining estradiol injections enhance memory in ovariectomized rats: cholinergic blockade and synergism. Neurobiol Learn Mem 68:172–188 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Gresack JE, Frick KM. 2006. Post-training estrogen enhances spatial and object memory consolidation in female mice. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 84:112–119 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Yin HH, Knowlton BJ. 2006. The role of the basal ganglia in habit formation. Nat Rev Neurosci 7:464–476 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Yin HH, Knowlton BJ, Balleine BW. 2004. Lesions of dorsolateral striatum preserve outcome expectancy but disrupt habit formation in instrumental learning. Eur J Neurosci 19:181–189 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Finch DM. 1996. Neurophysiology of converging synaptic inputs from the rat prefrontal cortex, amygdala, midline thalamus, and hippocampal formation onto single neurons of the caudate/putamen and nucleus accumbens. Hippocampus 6:495–512 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Finch DM, Gigg J, Tan AM, Kosoyan OP. 1995. Neurophysiology and neuropharmacology of projections from entorhinal cortex to striatum in the rat. Brain Res 670:233–247 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Dolorfo CL, Amaral DG. 1998. Entorhinal cortex of the rat: topographic organization of the cells of origin of the perforant path projection to the dentate gyrus. J Comp Neurol 398:25–48 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Hammond R, Nelson D, Gibbs RB. 7 August 2010. GPR30 co-localizes with cholinergic neurons in the basal forebrain and enhances potassium-stimulated acetylcholine release in the hippocampus. Psychoneuroendocrinology 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.07.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Pych JC, Chang Q, Colon-Rivera C, Haag R, Gold PE. 2005. Acetylcholine release in the hippocampus and striatum during place and response training. Learn Mem 12:564–572 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Chang Q, Gold PE. 2003. Switching memory systems during learning: changes in patterns of brain acetylcholine release in the hippocampus and striatum in rats. J Neurosci 23:3001–3005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Gazzaley AH, Weiland NG, McEwen BS, Morrison JH. 1996. Differential regulation of NMDAR1 mRNA and protein by estradiol in the rat hippocampus. J Neurosci 16:6830–6838 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Romeo RD, McCarthy JB, Wang A, Milner TA, McEwen BS. 2005. Sex differences in hippocampal estradiol-induced N-methyl-D-aspartic acid binding and ultrastructural localization of estrogen receptor-alpha. Neuroendocrinology 81:391–399 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Weiland NG. 1992. Estradiol selectively regulates agonist binding sites on the N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor complex in the CA1 region of the hippocampus. Endocrinology 131:662–668 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Woolley CS, Weiland NG, McEwen BS, Schwartzkroin PA. 1997. Estradiol increases the sensitivity of hippocampal CA1 pyramidal cells to NMDA receptor-mediated synaptic input: correlation with dendritic spine density. J Neurosci 17:1848–1859 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Murphy DD, Segal M. 1996. Regulation of dendritic spine density in cultured rat hippocampal neurons by steroid hormones. J Neurosci 16:4059–4068 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Daniel JM, Dohanich GP. 2001. Acetylcholine mediates the estrogen-induced increase in NMDA receptor binding in CA1 of the hippocampus and the associated improvement in working memory. J Neurosci 21:6949–6956 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Leuner B, Falduto J, Shors TJ. 2003. Associative memory formation increases the observation of dendritic spines in the hippocampus. J Neurosci 23:659–665 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Cyr M, Ghribi O, Di Paolo T. 2000. Regional and selective effects of oestradiol and progesterone on NMDA and AMPA receptors in the rat brain. J Neuroendocrinol 12:445–452 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Lovinger DM. 2010. Neurotransmitter roles in synaptic modulation, plasticity and learning in the dorsal striatum. Neuropharmacology 58:951–961 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Yin HH, Mulcare SP, Hilario MR, Clouse E, Holloway T, Davis MI, Hansson AC, Lovinger DM, Costa RM. 2009. Dynamic reorganization of striatal circuits during the acquisition and consolidation of a skill. Nat Neurosci 12:333–341 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Endocrinology are provided here courtesy of The Endocrine Society

RESOURCES