Abstract
Multiple regression voxel-based morphometry analyses were used to examine the relationship between regional gray matter volumes and neurocognitive performance in 10 patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment and 20 healthy age-matched controls. Cognitive functioning was assessed with seven standardized neuropsychological tests. Patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment exhibited impaired cognitive performance (on the Mini Mental State Examination, tests of verbal fluency, verbal and spatial learning and memory, and visual-motor abilities) and reduced gray matter volume in the right temporal pole. Across all participants, better performance on several neuropsychological tests was associated with higher regional gray matter volumes. Voxel-based morphometry provides an operator-unbiased means to investigate volumetric differences, which may be related to impaired neuropsychological functioning.
Keywords: aging, gray matter volume, mild cognitive impairment, neuropsychological assessment, temporal lobe, voxel-based morphometry
Introduction
Mild cognitive impairment, characterized by slight impairment in cognitive functioning but preserved ability to function in daily life, is a transitional state between normal aging and dementia [1]. Criteria for amnestic form of mild cognitive impairment emphasize impaired memory function for age and education, with preserved general cognitive function [1]. Patients with mild cognitive impairment are at increased risk of developing Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s disease-related neurodegeneration includes six histopathological stages; the disease process is initially confined to the entorhinal cortex, the transentorhinal cortex, and the anterodorsal nucleus of the thalamus and progresses to involve the hippocampal region [2].
Voxel-based morphometry is a neuroimaging tool that provides an operator-unbiased means to investigate volumetric changes [3]. Several studies applied voxel-based morphometry to examine brain tissue volume reduction in mild cognitive impairment [4,5]. Volume reduction detected by voxel-based morphometry techniques in mild cognitive impairment is most notable in the medial temporal lobe, the insula, and the thalamus [4,5]. Patients who converted to Alzheimer’s disease had lower baseline gray matter (GM) volumes of the hippocampus, parahippocampal cortex, and lingual and fusiform gyri [6] and volume reduction in the parietal and cingulate cortices [4]. Although previous studies have documented that volumetric changes can be detected in patients with mild cognitive impairment, and may be predictive of progression to Alzheimer’s disease, only a limited number of studies focused on the relationship between structural changes and functional impairment. A positive correlation between executive function and the left middle frontal gyrus volume has been observed in patients with mild cognitive impairment [7]. Another study found a negative correlation between scores of Trail Making Test, part A, and gray matter density in the right precuneus in progressive patients with mild cognitive impairment and a positive correlation between delayed wordlist recall and hippocampal gray matter density in patients with stable mild cognitive impairment [8].
The goal of our study was to compare gray matter volumes in a group of patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment and healthy controls, and evaluate the relationship between regional gray matter volumes and performance on neuropsychological tests. Voxel-based morphometry provided a novel approach to localize anatomical correlates of cognitive impairment that was indicated by impaired performance on neuropsychological tests covering a wide range of cognitive capacities.
Methods
Ten patients with a clinical diagnosis of amnestic mild cognitive impairment (mean age 73.5±5.5 years, five men, age range 65.3–80.3 years) and 20 healthy elderly volunteers (mean age 75.5±4.6 years, 10 men, age range 68.0–86.0 years) were examined. Patients were recruited from the Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center. Healthy volunteers were recruited by advertising in the city area. The neurological, psychiatric, and neuropsychological status of all patients and controls was evaluated using standardized instruments and methods. The psychiatric evaluation included administration of the Structured Clinical Interview for Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, Fourth Edition, Axis I Disorders (clinical version) [9] and the Clinical Dementia Rating scale [10]. All patients had Clinical Dementia Rating of 0.5 and Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) scores within normal limits for age and education [11] (Table 1). Diagnosis of amnestic mild cognitive impairment was based on Petersen’s criteria [1], which included a memory complaint, poor memory on testing, otherwise normal cognitive functioning, and no decline in functional ability. Final diagnoses were reached by clinical consensus. Exclusion criteria included abnormal findings on clinical neurological examination (other than mild cognitive impairment criteria for the patient group), head trauma with loss of consciousness, history of previous neurological illness, and general contraindications to an MRI examination. The study was approved by the Johns Hopkins Institutional Review Board and all participants gave written informed consent.
Table 1.
Group | Controls |
Mild cognitive impairment |
P value | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | ||
Number of participants | 20 | 10 | |||
Number of men/women | 10/10 | 5/5 | |||
Age (years) | 75.3 | 4.8 | 72.7 | 5.3 | NS |
Mini Mental State Examination | 28.9 | 1.2 | 26.3 | 2.9 | 0.003a |
Verbal fluency | |||||
Phonemic category | 30.0 | 7.7 | 27.0 | 8.8 | NS |
Semantic category | 47.4 | 10.6 | 35.8 | 7.8 | 0.007a |
Hopkins Verbal Learning | |||||
Test-Revised | |||||
Total Recall | 25.4 | 5.1 | 16.9 | 6.2 | 0.001a |
Delayed Recall | 9.1 | 1.8 | 3.3 | 3.8 | 0.002b |
Recognition Discrimination Index | 10.8 | 1.6 | 7.1 | 2.5 | 0.002b |
Hopkins Board | |||||
Trials to Learning Criterion | 4.4 | 2.6 | 8.8 | 1.7 | < 0.001a |
Delayed Recall of Items | 8.4 | 0.9 | 5.8 | 2.7 | 0.001b |
Delayed Recall of Locations | 8.6 | 0.9 | 5.4 | 3.4 | 0.017b |
Trail Making Test | |||||
Part A | 47.9 | 13.2 | 39.1 | 14.1 | NS |
Part B | 90.0 | 30.6 | 143.1 | 102.7 | NS |
Visual Motor Integration | 20.1 | 3.4 | 16.7 | 3.8 | 0.024a |
Brief Test of Attention | 6.8 | 2.6 | 5.8 | 2.9 | NS |
Data are presented as means and standard deviations (SD).
Age, phonemic and semantic categorical Verbal Fluency Test scores, and Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised Total Recall raw scores were all normally distributed variables based on the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test and the Shapiro-Wilk tests. The Trail Making Test (part A) and Brief Test of Attention scores were found normally distributed only by the Shapiro–Wilk test and Visual Motor Integration scores were normally distributed only by the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test.
NS, not significant.
Statistically significant values, two-tailed Student’s t-test.
Statistically significant values, Mann–Whitney U test.
The neuropsychological test battery consisted of seven tests including the MMSE [12]. Verbal Fluency was evaluated based on word list generation with the letters s and p used in a similar fashion as in the Controlled Oral Word Association Test [12]. Category fluency was evaluated with ‘animals’ and ‘supermarket items’ as semantic categories [12]. The Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised (HVLT-R) [13] was applied as a word-list learning task that evaluates the sum of total correct responses for initial three learning trials (total recall), the number of items recalled during a fourth trial after a delay (delayed recall), and the difference between the total number of true-positives and false-positives during the final trial of delayed yes/no-recognition (Recognition Discrimination Index). The Hopkins Board [14] was used to assess memory for objects and their locations on a 3 × 3 board. Scores were obtained for the number of trials to criterion, the number of correctly recalled items (delayed recall of items), and locations (delayed recall of locations). The Trail Making Test [12] was used to evaluate psychomotor and information processing speed, sequencing, and visual scanning. Time and number of errors made during tracing of a path along 25 numbers (part A) and accordingly numbers 1–13 with interspersed letters A–L (part B) were recorded. The Beery–Buktenica Developmental Test of visual-motor integration (VMI) [12], assessing constructional praxis, requires the participant to copy a series of line drawings of increasing difficulty. In the Brief Test of Attention [15], participants listened to 10 lists of letters and numbers, and were required to state the count of numbers or letters presented in each list. The numbers of correct responses formed the total score.
MRI was performed on a GE 1.5T MR scanner (General Electric Medical Systems, Milwaukee, Wisconsin, USA), using the standard quadrature birdcage head coil. The anatomical images were acquired using a high-resolution coronal three-dimensional T1-weighted spoiled-gradient-recalled echo in the steady-state sequence with 1.5mm slice thickness, 124 slices, 11.1 ms repetition time, 2.1 ms echo time, 20° flip angle, 240 × 240mm2 field-of-view, and a matrix of 256 × 192 zero filled to 256 × 256 for a resulting nominal voxel size of 0.94 × 0.94 × 1.5mm3. The scan time was 4 min 30 s.
Voxel-based morphometry analyses were conducted using an optimized protocol [16] in SPM2 (Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, University College London, London, UK). MR images were segmented into gray matter, white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid. This involved brain extraction and nonlinear spatial normalization to segmented gray matter and white matter standard template images in standard Talairach space [17]. Normalization was conducted using 10 × 10 × 7 discrete cosine transform basis functions. To further improve the normalization and brain extraction, a second segmentation was performed. The resulting image was modulated with the Jacobian determinant of the deformation field used in the normalization step, to reintroduce information about the volume change into the image. The resulting gray matter segments were smoothed with a three-dimensional Gaussian 8mm full-width at half-maximum kernel [3].
Comparison of neuropsychological test scores between the control and amnestic mild cognitive impairment groups was performed using parametric (Student’s t-test) and nonparametric (Mann–Whitney U test) tests, depending on the results of the Shapiro–Wilk and Kolmogorov–Smirnov normality tests.
To evaluate the effect of age on a specific neuropsychological test performance, random (mixed) effects general linear model (GLM) analyses with group (amnestic mild cognitive impairment, controls) as fixed factors and age as covariate were constructed, controlling for sex. Dependent variables were the scores of the normally distributed phonemic and semantic categorical Verbal Fluency tests, HVLT-R Total Recall raw scores, VMI raw scores, Trail Making Test (part A) scores, and Brief Test of Attention scores. HVLT-R delayed recall and recognition discrimination and Trail Making Test (part B) scores were not included in the GLM analyses because normality was not confirmed. The homogeneity of variance for all variables was assessed with the Box’s M-test, and equal error variances with the Levene’s test. Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS software (SPSS Inc., Chicago, Illinois, USA).
In the voxel-based morphometry analysis, GLM was used to obtain voxel-wise fit coefficients of the gray matter volume parametric maps. The main voxel-based morphometry analysis was performed using multiple linear regression, with age and group as independent variables, controlling for sex. Age values were converted to Z-scores before they were used in the analyses. Voxel-wise β-coefficients were calculated for gray matter volume differences attributable to age and group. All neuropsychological test scores were modeled in separate voxel-based morphometry analyses using GLM, with age and sex as independent variables. To correct for large number of comparisons, a cluster-based approach was used (i.e. the correction was based on spatial extent and not on the peak height of a cluster) [18]. In all analyses, including group comparisons and regression with neuropsychological test scores, a corrected statistical significance threshold was set to P value less than 0.05.
Results
Demographic information and neuropsychological test scores are reported in Table 1. Statistical analyses of neuropsychological test scores showed significantly lower performance in the amnestic mild cognitive impairment group on five out of seven administered tests: MMSE, Verbal Fluency Test (semantic category only), HLVT-R (Total Recall, Delayed Recall, Recognition Discrimination Index), Hopkins Board (Trials to Learning Criterion, Delayed Recall of Items, Delayed Recall of Locations), and VMI (all P ≤0.024).
Voxel-based morphometry analyses revealed a main effect of group. Although several clusters were identified that indicate lower gray matter volumes in the amnestic mild cognitive impairment group compared with controls, only one cluster in the right temporal pole (Brodmann area 38; cluster size=359 voxels, P<0.016, Talairach Coordinates x=25, y=11, z=− 37) was significant (See Fig. 1a of Supplemental digital content 1 http://links.lww.com/WNR/A32).
There was also a strong negative main effect of age on regional gray matter volumes, detected in the pooled data from both groups. Five significant clusters were detected (Table 2), in the right hippocampus/parahippocampal gyrus (See Fig. 1b of Supplemental digital content 1 http://links.lww.com/WNR/A32), left lingual gyrus, and left middle frontal gyrus (P<0.05). There was no significant main effect of sex.
Table 2.
Cluster size | Cluster P | Peak T | Talairach coordinates |
Region | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
X | Y | Z | ||||
778 | 0.001 | 4.47 | 28 | − 14 | − 18 | Right hippocampus/parahippocampal gyrus |
3.81 | 23 | −7 | − 15 | Right parahippocampal gyrus | ||
368 | 0.015 | 4.87 | − 15 | − 82 | − 14 | Left lingual gyrus |
272 | 0.032 | 4.27 | − 44 | 22 | 31 | Left middle frontal gyrus |
4.26 | − 50 | 12 | 31 | Left middle frontal gyrus |
Data from all participants were used in the voxel-based morphometry analysis.
Correlations between neuropsychological test scores and regional gray matter volumes are summarized in Table 3. Better scores on the Trail Making Test (part B) were associated with a higher gray matter volume around the central sulcus and better scores on VMI were associated with a higher gray matter volume in the right superior temporal gyrus and the right middle frontal gyrus (See Fig. 1c of Supplemental digital content 1 http://links.lww.com/WNR/A32). Higher scores on the MMSE were associated with a higher gray matter volume in the right middle occipital gyrus. For the Hopkins Board, a significant relationship between the number of trials to learning criterion and gray matter volume in the right hippocampus (See Fig. 1d Supplemental digital content 1 http://links.lww.com/WNR/A32) and the left temporal and frontal lobe was detected.
Table 3.
Test | Cluster size | Cluster P | Peak T | Talairach coordinates |
Region | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
X | Y | Z | |||||
MMSE | 283 | 0.029 | 5.1 | 35 | − 82 | 7 | Right middle occipital gyrus |
HB-TTC | 268 | 0.033 | 4.13 | 24 | − 6 | − 13 | Right parahippocampal gyrus |
268 | 0.033 | 3.71 | 29 | − 14 | − 18 | Right parahippocampal gyrus | |
250 | 0.039 | 4.41 | − 50 | − 30 | − 20 | Left fusiform gyrus | |
TMT-B | 3183 | 0.001 | 8.15 | 50 | − 16 | 57 | Right postcentral gyrus |
3183 | 0.001 | 4.09 | 35 | − 12 | 63 | Right precentral gyrus | |
369 | 0.033 | 5.22 | 37 | 12 | 57 | Right middle frontal gyrus | |
322 | 0.044 | 3.98 | 46 | 48 | 18 | Right middle frontal gyrus | |
VMI | 483 | 0.017 | 4.36 | 50 | 2 | 1 | Right superior temporal gyrus |
365 | 0.034 | 4.6 | 45 | 31 | 19 | Right middle frontal gyrus |
Data from all participants were used in the voxel-based morphometry analyses.
HB-TTC, Hopkins Board-Trials to Learning Criterion; MMSE, Mini Mental State examInation; TMT-B, Trail-Making Test-part B; VMI, Beery–Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual Motor Integration.
Discussion
In this prospective cross-sectional pilot study, voxel-based morphometry was used to assess gray matter volume differences between patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment and age-matched controls, and to examine the relationship between regional gray matter volumes and neuropsychological performance. In patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment, lower gray matter volume was detected in the right temporal pole. Analyses of data from all participants revealed age-related decreases in gray matter volumes in several regions, most notably in the right hippocampus and the parahippocampal gyri. Better performance on MMSE, Hopkins Board, Trail Making Test (part B), and VMI was associated with higher regional gray matter volumes.
In previous studies, volume reductions detectable with voxel-based morphometry in mild cognitive impairment were observed in the hippocampal and inferolateral temporal regions [4], superior temporal gyri, uncus, posterior cingulate, and adjacent precuneus [19]. Areas of involvement also include the thalamus, the insula [4], and frontal and parietal regions [7,8,20]. In patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment, inferior and medial temporal lobes [21], frontal and parietal gray matter are affected [20]. Our results confirm earlier findings of gray matter volume reduction in the medial temporal lobe [4], which has a known role in learning and memory and deteriorates early in the course of Alzheimer’s disease [22]. The extent of volume reduction in patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment in our study was less widespread than in a recent study including patients with progressive disease [21]. It is therefore likely that our patients were mostly stable at the time of evaluation and that perhaps widespread structural deficits have not yet manifested. The difference found in the right temporal lobe, and not in the left medial temporal lobe, suggests that there may be laterality to volume loss. Although right laterality in tissue atrophy has been reported [23], the findings are not consistent across studies [24].
Association between neuropsychological test performance and gray matter volume reduction in the temporal neocortex and other cortical areas is also consistent with literature data [25]. In our study, multivariate volumetric analysis, including scores of 12 neuropsychological measures, provided an assessment of the correlations with the observed volumetric changes. The association between the limbic and temporal lobe gray matter volumes and the performances on a test of spatial location learning (Hopkins Board; number of trials to learning criterion) and on a test assessing visual-motor abilities (the Beery VMI) indicates that these tests may be sensitive to isolate early changes underlying the disease process involving gray matter volume reduction. The relationship between impaired performance on the spatial location learning test and gray matter volume in the right parahippocampal gyrus complements a previous report of volume reduction in the right hippocampus-amygdala region in mild cognitive impairment [5]. In our study, a significant relationship between MMSE scores and gray matter volumes in the right middle occipital gyrus was also detected. Although the occipital region has not been documented to be involved in early disease progression of mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer’s disease, a recent pilot FDG PET study in individuals with probable Alzheimer’s disease reported an association between the performance on the MMSE drawing task and glucose uptake in several brain regions, including the occipital lobe [26].
Although the patient and control groups differed in neuropsychological performance, volumes of brain regions correlating with the neuropsychological tests were not identical as the areas showing volume group differences. It may be possible that the group comparison was not as sensitive as the correlation analyses (which were performed using data from all participants). Another possible explanation includes modulation of neuropsychological performance involving the medial temporal lobe in patients by changes in other cortical regions (either in connectivity or activity).
Conclusion
A novel technique of assessment of the relationship between neuropsychological test scores and regional gray matter volumes was applied in the elderly, including patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Despite limitations because of a relatively small number of patients, the presented results are consistent with previously published voxel-based morphometry studies and provide essential information on the association between gray matter volumes and cognitive performance.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Jessica Lilyestrom for administering the neuropsychological tests. This research was supported by the following grants from the National Institutes of Health: RO3AG17364, P50 AG05146 (Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center). The work was carried out at the Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland, USA.
Footnotes
Supplemental digital content is available for this article. Direct URL citations appear in the printed text and are provided in the HTML and PDF versions of this article on the journal’s Website (www.neuroreport.com).
References
- 1.Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol. 1999;56:303–308. doi: 10.1001/archneur.56.3.303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Braak H, Braak E. Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol (Berl) 1991;82:239–259. doi: 10.1007/BF00308809. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Ashburner J, Friston KJ. Voxel-based morphometry-the methods. Neuroimage. 2000;11:805–821. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0582. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Karas GB, Scheltens P, Rombouts SA, Visser PJ, van Schijndel RA, Fox NC, et al. Global and local gray matter loss in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroimage. 2004;23:708–716. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Pennanen C, Testa C, Laakso MP, Hallikainen M, Helkala EL, Hanninen T, et al. A voxel based morphometry study on mild cognitive impairment. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2005;76:11–14. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2004.035600. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Chetelat G, Landeau B, Eustache F, Mezenge F, Viader F, de la Sayette V, et al. Using voxel-based morphometry to map the structural changes associated with rapid conversion in MCI: a longitudinal MRI study. Neuroimage. 2005;27:934–946. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Duarte A, Hayasaka S, Du A, Schuff N, Jahng GH, Kramer J, et al. Volumetric correlates of memory and executive function in normal elderly, mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurosci Lett. 2006;406:60–65. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.07.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Hamalainen A, Tervo S, Grau-Olivares M, Niskanen E, Pennanen C, Huuskonen J, et al. Voxel-based morphometry to detect brain atrophy in progressive mild cognitive impairment. Neuroimage. 2007;37:1122–1131. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.06.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders-Clinical Version (SCID-CV) Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Hughes CP, Berg L, Danziger WL, Coben LA, Martin RL. A new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. Br J Psychiatry. 1982;140:566–572. doi: 10.1192/bjp.140.6.566. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Crum RM, Anthony JC, Bassett SS, Folstein MF. Population-based norms for the Mini-Mental State Examination by age and educational level. JAMA. 1993;269:2386–2391. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW. Neuropsychological assessment. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Brandt J, Benedict RHB. Hopkins verbal learning test-revised, professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Brandt J, Shpritz B, Munro CA, Marsh L, Rosenblatt A. Differential impairment of spatial location memory in Huntington’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2005;76:1516–1519. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2004.059253. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Schretlen D. Brief test of attention professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Good CD, Johnsrude IS, Ashburner J, Henson RN, Friston KJ, Frackowiak RS. A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. Neuroimage. 2001;14:21–36. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Talairach J, Tournoux P. Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain. New York: Thieme Medical Publisher Inc; 1988. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Friston KJ, Worsley KJ, Frackowiak RSJ, Mazziotta JC, Evans AC. Assessing the significance of focal activations using their spatial extent. Hum Brain Mapp. 1994;1:210–220. doi: 10.1002/hbm.460010306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Fan Y, Batmanghelich N, Clark CM, Davatzikos C. Spatial patterns of brain atrophy in MCI patients, identified via high-dimensional pattern classification, predict subsequent cognitive decline. Neuroimage. 2008;39:1731–1743. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Barbeau EJ, Ranjeva JP, Didic M, Confort-Gouny S, Felician O, Soulier E, et al. Profile of memory impairment and gray matter loss in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46:1009–1019. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.11.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Whitwell JL, Shiung MM, Przybelski SA, Weigand SD, Knopman DS, Boeve BF, et al. MRI patterns of atrophy associated with progression to AD in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neurology. 2008;70:512–520. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000280575.77437.a2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Gomez-Isla T, Price JL, McKeel DW, Jr, Morris JC, Growdon JH, Hyman BT. Profound loss of layer II entorhinal cortex neurons occurs in very mild Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurosci. 1996;16:4491–4500. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-14-04491.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Csernansky JG, Wang L, Joshi S, Miller JP, Gado M, Kido D, et al. Early DAT is distinguished from aging by high-dimensional mapping of the hippocampus. Dementia of the Alzheimer type. Neurology. 2000;55:1636–1643. doi: 10.1212/wnl.55.11.1636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Chetelat G, Baron JC. Early diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: contribution of structural neuroimaging. Neuroimage. 2003;18:525–541. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(02)00026-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Chetelat G, Desgranges B, De La Sayette V, Viader F, Eustache F, Baron JC. Mapping gray matter loss with voxel-based morphometry in mild cognitive impairment. Neuroreport. 2002;13:1939–1943. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200210280-00022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Mishina M, Ishii K, Kitamura S, Kobayashi S, Ishiwata K, Katayama Y. P2-337 Drawing task in ADAS-cog and MMSE: an FDG PET study. Alzheimer’s Dementia. 2006;2:S340. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.