Abstract
Aims
Specific polymorphisms of the apolipoprotein E (APOE) and angiotensin-converting enzyme (ACE) genes appear to increase risk for Alzheimer’s disease and cognitive dysfunction in the general population, yet little research has examined whether genetic factors influence risk of cognitive dysfunction in patients with Type 1 diabetes. The long-term follow-up of the Diabetes Control and Complications Trial/Epidemiology of Diabetes Interventions and Complications (DCCT/EDIC) population provides an opportunity to examine if specific genetic variations in APOE and ACE alter risk for cognitive decline.
Methods
Neurocognitive function in Type 1 diabetic subjects from the DCCT/EDIC study was assessed at DCCT entry and re-assessed approximately 18 years later, using a comprehensive cognitive test battery. Glycated haemoglobin (HbA1c) and the frequency of severe hypoglycaemic events leading to coma or seizures were measured over the 18-year follow-up. We determined whether the APO εs4 and ACE intron 16 indel genotypes were associated with baseline cognitive function and with change over time, and whether they conferred added risk in those subjects experiencing severe hypoglycaemic events or greater glycaemic exposure.
Results
None of the APOE or ACE polymorphisms were associated with either baseline cognitive performance or change in cognition over the 18-year follow-up. Moreover, none of the genotype variations altered the risk of cognitive dysfunction in those subjects with severe hypoglycaemic episodes or high HbA1c.
Conclusions
In this sample of young and middle-aged adults with Type 1 diabetes, APOε4 and ACED alleles do not appear to increase risk of cognitive dysfunction.
Keywords: angiotensin-converting enzyme, apolipoprotein E, cognition, genetics, Type 1 diabetes
Introduction
Type 1 diabetes mellitus (Type 1 DM) leads to peripheral and autonomic neuropathy and micro- and macrovascular biomedical complications [1,2]. Increasing evidence also suggests that the central nervous system (CNS) may be affected adversely by diabetes and its treatment [3,4], but the underlying pathophysiological mechanisms remain poorly understood. Specific polymorphisms of the apolipoprotein E (APOE) and angiotensin-converting enzyme (ACE) genes have been reported to increase the risk for Alzheimer’s disease as well as the risk for cognitive dysfunction in the general population and in individuals with Type 2 diabetes [5–10], but few studies have explicitly examined whether genetic factors influence risk of cognitive dysfunction in patients with Type 1 DM [11].
The APOE gene is involved in lipid metabolism and plays a role in remodelling neurons and glial cells in response to injury. Because lipids do not pass the blood–brain barrier, genetic variations that influence lipid metabolism could affect the ability of neurons to repair themselves after a wide range of assaults including those from glycaemic dysregulation. Indeed, the APO ε4 allele appears to increase risk of cognitive dysfunction after exposure to vascular disease and brain damage as a result of mild to moderate traumatic head injury [12]. In general, individuals homozygous for the ε4 allele have greater risk than those with APOε2 and ε3 alleles. Even ε4 heterozygotes show added risk for cognitive dysfunction in some studies [5]. In children, associations between polymorphisms in the APOE genotypes and cognitive function have not been found [13], but differences in brain structure associated with the ε4 allele have been identified [14]. Moreover, cognitively intact middle-aged adults with APO ε4 appear to show decreased density in frontal and temporal regions of the brain [15].
The only study of patients with Type 1DM has suggested that the presence of the APO ε4 allele was associated with worse cognitive performance, but this was limited to women; no genetic influence on cognition was found among men in the study [11]. Although it is plausible that the presence of an APO ε4 allele could exacerbate the neurocognitive consequences of glycaemic insults (severe hypoglycaemia and/or chronic hyperglycaemia), no interaction between genotype and previous exposure to severe hypoglycaemia was found in that study and interactions with persistent elevations in glycated haemoglobin (HbA1c) were not tested. However, this was a small study with 96 individuals, so the potential of such known risk factors to amplify the effects of the APO ε4 allele would probably be missed because of inadequate power.
A smaller and less consistent body of research has also suggested that the ACE D allele confers risk of cognitive dysfunction and Alzheimer’s disease in older adults [16–19]. While the majority of these studies suggest that the presence of the D allele increases risk, a smaller group of studies also suggest the presence of the I allele increases risk [20]. The apparent risk may relate to the effects of the ACE gene on cardiovascular status and effects on the rennin–angiotensin system. The presence of the D allele increases ACE activity, which is associated with increased prevalence of hypertension and cardiovascular disease. ACE activity also appears to directly affect the formation of amyloid and so may directly influence the progression of Alzheimer’s disease [20]. It is not clear if ACE polymorphisms influence cognitive performance in younger adults or in those with diabetes. The vascular effects of ACE activity would be highly relevant to the risk of CNS disease in Type 1 DM, but no studies have examined this population.
We examined the association of the APOE and ACE polymorphisms on baseline cognitive performance and change in cognition in Type 1 DM patients followed in the Diabetes Control and Complications Trial (DCCT) and Epidemiology of Diabetes Interventions and Complications (EDIC) study over an average of 18 years. We addressed four primary research questions:
Is the APO ε4 or ACE D allele associated with level of cognitive functioning at DCCT baseline?
Is the APO ε4 or ACE D allele associated with change in cognitive functioning over the 18-year follow-up?
Does the APOε4 or ACED allele modify the effects of either severe hypoglycaemic events or elevated HbA1c on change in cognitive functioning?
Is there an interaction between either the APOε4 or ACED allele with gender or age such that women or older subjects with either polymorphism are at greater risk of cognitive decline than those without the polymorphism?
Secondarily, we carried out exploratory analyses to assess whether nine additional single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in ACE and nine additional SNPs in the APOE region, selected to tag common variation in these genes, were associated with cognitive performance [21,22].
Patients and methods
Study sample
Between 1983 and 1989, 1441 subjects with Type 1 DM, 13 to 39 years of age, were enrolled in the DCCT and completed a comprehensive cognitive assessment battery. Approximately half of the sample (n = 711) was randomly assigned to intensive therapy; the remainder (n = 730) were assigned to conventional therapy with 1–2 daily insulin injections. Details of the diabetes management regimen can be found elsewhere [1]. At the end of the DCCT in 1993, intensive therapy was recommended for all subjects and, in 1994, 1375 (96%) of the 1428 surviving members volunteered to participate in the EDIC observational follow-up study [23]. In 2004, 1144 (85% of surviving, eligible participants) completed the cognitive test battery again. For these analyses, the sample was limited to the self-identified white subjects who were genotyped (n = 1093).
Cognitive test protocol
Cognitive testing, as originally described for the DCCT [24], was conducted at each site by personnel who were trained and certified by the DCCT/EDIC Central Neuropsychological Coding Unit. The test protocol, which required 4–5 h to complete, included the following widely used, well-validated tests that were administered during the DCCT. Six subtests (Similarities, Comprehension, Digit Span, Digit Symbol, Block Design and Object Assembly) from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS), four subtests (Category Test, Tactual Performance Test, Trail Making Test and Finger Tapping Test) from the Halstead–Reitan Neuropsychological Battery, the Logical Memory and Visual Reproductions subtest from the Wechsler Memory Scale, the Digit Vigilance Test, the Grooved Pegboard Test, the Verbal Fluency (FAS) Test, the Four-Word Short Term Memory Test, the Symbol-Digit Learning Test and the Embedded Fingers Test. Tests were administered in a fixed order. Capillary blood glucose levels were routinely monitored immediately before the testing and at its midpoint to rule out hypoglycaemia during testing. If a subject was found to have a blood glucose level ≤ 3.9 mmol/l, testing was stopped, the patient given a snack and, after waiting at least 15 min, testing was resumed when the blood glucose was ≥ 5.0 mmol/l. Tests were scored by technicians at the Central Neuropsychological Coding Unit who were masked to treatment assignment and other biomedical variables.
Cognitive domains
During the DCCT, 24 test variables had been chosen a priori to be of particular diagnostic value when applied to patients with Type 1 DM. For each of these 24 test variables, a standardized (Z) score was calculated, with the mean and standard deviation from the baseline assessment of the DCCT cohort used as reference [24]. To reduce the number of comparisons, the 24 standardized scores were grouped into one of eight cognitive domains consistent with standard neuropsychological assessment strategies [25]. For each domain, the simple average of the standardized scores was used to represent the change from baseline.
Genetic analyses
Genotyping of the APOE haplotype, rs7412 (ε2/ε3) and rs429358 (ε3/ε4) and ACE intron 16 indel (rs4340) was performed as previously described [21,26]. Genotyping and selection of nine additional SNPs in ACE allowed us to capture the common variation in that gene occurring with minor allele frequency > 5% and nine additional SNPs that extended outside the APOE locus. This was based on linkage disequilibrium criteria (r2 > 0.8), which included the neighbouring genes APOC4 and APOC2 (see also Supporting Information, Table S1), and which are described elsewhere [22]. Quality control measures for the APOE and ACE markers used in the main analysis have been previously reported [21,26] as well as the quality control data for the additional 18 SNPs as described in the Supporting Information (Table S1) [27].
Biomedical evaluations
During the EDIC study, subjects completed an annual history, physical examination, electrocardiogram and laboratory testing, including serum creatinine and HbA1c, using the same methods as during the DCCT [1]. Participants reported the presence of sensory symptoms of peripheral neuropathy as part of a neuropathy screening [23]. During the DCCT, severe hypoglycaemia over the previous 3 months was ascertained at quarterly interviews and through self-report between these visits. During the EDIC study, the severe hypoglycaemic events that occurred in the 3 months prior to the annual visit were documented on the annual history form. For this analysis, severe hypoglycaemic events are limited to those leading to coma and/or seizure [28]. There were 1282 episodes of coma or seizure reported over the 18-year follow-up (854 events in 247 intensive treatment group subjects and 428 events in 182 conventional treatment group subjects).
Statistical analyses
Clinical characteristics were compared using the Kruskal–Wallis test to assess the differences between genotype groups for ordinal and numeric variables. The contingency chi-square test was used for categorical variables [29].
Various transformations were used to normalize the eight cognitive domains as follows: domain 2 (1.5^raw value), domain 3 (windsorize tails to 1st and 99th percentiles), domain 4 (windsorize to 1st percentile), domain 5 [log (raw score—minimum value), then windsorize to 1st percentile), domain 6 (windsorize to 2nd percentile), domain 7 (exponential^raw value, then windsorize to 99th percentile). Domains 1 and 8 remained untransformed.
The Kruskal Wallis test was used to assess the univariate relationship between ACE/APOE genotypes and each of the eight cognitive domains. For ACE we examined the three genotypes (I/I,D/D, I/D). For APOE we considered the presence of the ε4 and ε2 alleles separately and also modelled all six genotype groups simultaneously.
Separate analysis of covariance models were used to assess the effects of having at least one copy of the ACE Dor APO ε4 allele on the standardized quantitative score for each of the eight cognitive domain scores at DCCT baseline (see also Supporting Information, Table S2). Each model was adjusted for baseline age, gender, years of education and cohort assignment. We also examined 18 additional ACE/APOE markers that have been used in other genetic analyses of the DCCT/EDIC study outcomes [22].
Cognitive domain change scores were modelled using factorial analysis of variance models to identify any interactions with (i) treatment assignment, (ii) severe hypoglycaemia (0 vs. 1+ events), (iii) HbA1c (above or below the median of 7.6%), (iv) the combined effect of hypoglycaemia and HbA1c, (v) gender or (vi) age (above or below median age 46 years).
To address the potential for a Type 2 error (misinterpreting a null finding), we estimated our power to detect an effect using available data from the one other study of genetic effects on cognition in Type 1 DM [11]. With the available sample size, we have 80% power at alpha = 0.01 to detect a locus that accounts for 1.2% of the trait variance in a quantitative trait at an alpha of 0.01 for an additive genetic model and a minor allele frequency of 0.3 (the ε4 allele frequency). This is comparable with that of Ferguson et al. [11] where the estimated eta2 value (which we presume is equivalent to r2) was 26% for APOE on performance IQ and 25% on object assembly in women. The power of our study is also very consistent with other studies of non-diabetic samples examining the effects of APOE on cognitive function. Results were considered significant at the P = 0.01 level.
Results
Table 1 presents the clinical characteristics of participants at entry to the DCCT by genetic variations in both the APOE and ACE genotypes. The only statistically significant between-group difference at DCCT baseline was low-density lipoprotein (LDL) cholesterol across all six APOE genotypes as well as between the two ε4 groupings (3.0 ± 0.81 mmol/l for participants with ε4 vs. 2.7 ± 0.70 mmol/l for participants with no ε4; P < 0.0001). Because of the small number of individuals homozygous for the APO ε4 allele, we analysed the APOE data by comparing all variations and then comparing those subjects with and without the ε4 allele.
Table 1.
Characteristics of participants at entry into the DCCT by genetic variations in APOE and ACE
Variable | ACE | APOE | APOE | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Genotypes | n | II | ID | DD | ε2/ε2 | ε2/ε3 | ε2/ε4 | ε3/ε3 | ε3/ε4 | ε4/ε4 | ε4 | No ε4 |
Total (n) (%) | 1093 | 252 (23.1) | 539 (49.3) | 302 (27.6) | 5 (0.5) | 137 (12.5) | 23 (2.1) | 661 (60.5) | 247 (22.6) | 20 (1.8) | 290 (26.5) | 803 (73.5) |
Sex | ||||||||||||
Male | 578 | 132 | 292 | 154 | 2 | 72 | 14 | 347 | 130 | 13 | 157 | 421 |
Female | 515 | 120 | 247 | 148 | 3 | 65 | 9 | 314 | 117 | 7 | 133 | 382 |
Treatment | ||||||||||||
Intensive | 564 | 138 | 281 | 145 | 3 | 73 | 12 | 329 | 136 | 11 | 159 | 405 |
Conventional | 529 | 114 | 258 | 157 | 2 | 64 | 11 | 332 | 111 | 9 | 131 | 398 |
Cohort | ||||||||||||
Primary | 531 | 115 | 271 | 145 | 2 | 68 | 9 | 322 | 120 | 10 | 139 | 392 |
Secondary | 562 | 137 | 268 | 157 | 3 | 69 | 14 | 339 | 127 | 10 | 151 | 411 |
Education (years) | 1093 | 13.7 ± 2.6 | 14.2 ± 2.5 | 14.1 ± 2.6 | 12.4 ± 2.6 | 14.3 ± 2.5 | 13.4 ± 2.6 | 14.1 ± 2.6 | 14.1 ± 2.5 | 14.3 ± 3.6 | 14.0 ± 2.6 | 14.1 ± 2.6 |
Age (years) | 1093 | 26.8 ± 7.2 | 27.1 ± 6.9 | 27.0 ± 6.6 | 26.4 ± 8.1 | 27.1 ± 6.7 | 28.8 ± 7.1 | 27.0 ± 6.9 | 26.9 ± 6.7 | 26.3 ± 7.8 | 27.0 ± 6.8 | 27.0 ± 6.9 |
Duration (years) | 1093 | 6.2 ± 4.2 | 5.8 ± 4.2 | 6.0 ± 4.2 | 5.4 ± 3.4 | 6.1 ± 4.4 | 7.5 ± 4.8 | 5.9 ± 4.2 | 5.7 ± 4.1 | 6.0 ± 4.4 | 5.9 ± 4.2 | 5.9 ± 4.2 |
HbA1c (%) | 1093 | 9.0 ± 1.6 | 8.9 ± 1.6 | 8.9 ± 1.6 | 8.4 ± 1.0 | 9.1 ± 1.5 | 9.5 ± 1.6 | 8.9 ± 1.6 | 9.0 ± 1.6 | 9.0 ± 1.5 | 9.0 ± 1.6 | 9.0 ± 1.6 |
LDL cholesterol (mmol/l) |
1093 | 2.8 ± 0.8 | 2.8 ± 0.7 | 2.8 ± 0.8 | 1.8 ± 0.9 | 2.5 ± 0.6 | 2.7 ± 0.7 | 2.8 ± 0.7 | 3.0 ± 0.8 | 3.2 ± 1.1† | 3.0 ± 0.8 | 2.7 ± 0.7† |
Domains | 1093 | |||||||||||
Problem solving | −0.03 ± 0.04 | 0.00 ± 0.03 | 0.01 ± 0.03 | 0.46 ± 0.14 | −0.05 ± 0.05 | 0.32 ± 0.12 | −0.02 ± 0.02 | 0.04 ± 0.04 | −0.08 ± 0.12 | 0.06 ± 0.05 | −0.02 ± 0.02 | |
Learning | 0.05 ± 0.05 | −0.04 ± 0.04 | 0.05 ± 0.04 | −0.29 ± 0.30 | 0.06 ± 0.06 | −0.15 ± 0.15 | 0.00 ± 0.03 | −0.01 ± 0.05 | 0.18 ± 0.14 | −0.01 ± 0.05 | 0.01 ± 0.03 | |
Immediate memory | 0.07 ± 0.04 | −0.00 ± 0.03 | −0.01 ± 0.04 | 0.13 ± 0.39 | 0.06 ± 0.05 | −0.17 ± 0.15 | −0.01 ± 0.02 | 0.05 ± 0.04 | 0.06 ± 0.16 | 0.03 ± 0.04 | 0.01 ± 0.02 | |
Delayed recall | −0.02 ± 0.04 | −0.01 ± 0.03 | 0.05 ± 0.04 | −0.34 ± 0.13 | 0.02 ± 0.05 | −0.07 ± 0.15 | −0.01 ± 0.02 | 0.07 ± 0.04 | −0.22 ± 0.15 | 0.04 ± 0.04 | −0.01 ± 0.02 | |
Spatial information | −0.06 ± 0.04 | 0.04 ± 0.03 | −0.04 ± 0.04 | 0.40 ± 0.27 | −0.12 ± 0.06 | 0.25 ± 0.15 | −0.02 ± 0.03 | 0.05 ± 0.05 | −0.03 ± 0.16 | 0.06 ± 0.04 | −0.03 ± 0.02 | |
Attention | −0.00 ± 0.04 | 0.02 ± 0.03 | −0.00 ± 0.04 | −0.08 ± 0.18 | −0.01 ± 0.05 | −0.20 ± 0.11 | 0.02 ± 0.02 | 0.01 ± 0.04 | −0.03 ± 0.21 | −0.01 ± 0.04 | 0.02 ± 0.02 | |
Psychomotor efficiency | −0.03 ± 0.03 | 0.00 ± 0.02 | 0.02 ± 0.03 | 0.35 ± 0.10 | 0.01 ± 0.04 | 0.09 ± 0.08 | −0.01 ± 0.02 | 0.00 ± 0.03 | 0.03 ± 0.08 | 0.01 ± 0.02 | −0.00 ± 0.02 | |
Motor speed | 0.01 ± 0.05 | 0.03 ± 0.04 | −0.01 ± 0.05 | −0.39 ± 0.37 | 0.06 ± 0.08 | 0.10 ± 0.23 | 0.00 ± 0.04 | 0.02 ± 0.05 | −0.04 ± 0.22 | 0.02 ± 0.05 | 0.01 ± 0.03 |
Data are mean ± sd or n.
HbA1c value derived from assessment to determine study eligibility.
P < 0.01 by the χ2-test or Fisher’s exact test for categorical traits or the Kruskal–Wallis test for ordinal and numeric traits.
ACE, angotensin-converting enzyme; APOE, apolipoprotein E; DCCT, Diabetes Control and Complications Trial; HbA1c, glycated haemoglobin; LDL, low-density lipoprotein; sd, standard deviation.
There were no significant main effects of either ACE or APOE variation on baseline cognitive performance after adjusting for baseline age, gender, years of education and cohort assignment.
By EDIC study year 12, the age of participants ranged from 29 to 62 years (mean ± SD 45.7 ± 6.8). Table 2 and Table 3 show the main and interaction effects of ACE D and APO ε4 on change in cognition over time. Consistent with our earlier presentation of the cognitive findings [28], there is a statistically significant main effect for HbA1c when modelled alone as well as in combination with severe hypoglycaemia. Higher levels of HbA1c are associated with lower levels of cognitive functioning in relation to psychomotor efficiency and motor speed.
Table 2.
Main and interaction effects of ACE D on change in cognitive functioning, analysed by level of severe hypoglycaemia, HbA1c and severe hypoglycaemia by HbA1c
Domain | Problem solving |
Learning | Immediate memory |
Delayed recall |
Spatial information |
Attention | Psychomotor efficiency |
Motor speed |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model 1 | ||||||||
ACE D (yes vs. no) | 0.14 (0.70) | 1.21 (0.27) | 0.31 (0.58) | 0.21 (0.65) | 0.00 (0.99) | 0.57 (0.45) | 3.60 (0.06) | 0.10 (0.75) |
Severe hypo (0 vs. 1+) | 0.44 (0.51) | 1.38 (0.24) | 1.28 (0.26) | 0.05 (0.82) | 2.18 (0.14) | 0.18 (0.67) | 2.98 (0.08) | 2.34 (0.13) |
Severe hypo × ACE D | 0.00 (0.99) | 0.08 (0.78) | 0.33 (0.57) | 0.51 (0.47) | 0.41 (0.52) | 0.98 (0.32) | 0.07 (0.79) | 0.07 (0.79) |
Model 2 | ||||||||
ACE D (yes vs. no) | 0.57 (0.45) | 1.20 (0.27) | 0.35 (0.55) | 0.05 (0.82) | 0.13 (0.72) | 0.28 (0.60) | 2.06 (0.15) | 0.16 (0.69) |
HbA1c (low vs. high)† | 0.57 (0.45) | 0.64 (0.42) | 3.52 (0.06) | 1.23 (0.27) | 3.66 (0.06) | 6.33 (0.01) | 21.96 (< 0.01) | 18.53 (< 0.01) |
HbA1c × ACE D | 2.50 (0.11) | 0.76 (0.38) | 0.49 (0.48) | 1.19 (0.28) | 2.53 (0.11) | 1.04 (0.31) | 1.50 (0.22) | 1.37 (0.24) |
Model 3* | ||||||||
ACE D (yes vs. no) | 0.48 (0.49) | 1.44 (0.23) | 0.62 (0.43) | 0.01 (0.93) | 0.08 (0.77) | 0.12 (0.73) | 1.89 (0.17) | 0.23 (0.63) |
Severe hypo (0 vs. 1+) | 0.07 (0.79) | 1.33 (0.25) | 0.83 (0.36) | 0.00 (0.96) | 3.08 (0.08) | 0.05 (0.83) | 2.38 (0.12) | 1.77 (0.18) |
HbA1c (low vs. high)† | 0.43 (0.51) | 0.79 (0.37) | 4.16 (0.04) | 1.45 (0.23) | 2.81 (0.09) | 6.18 (0.01) | 22.11 (< 0.01) | 20.45 (< 0.01) |
Severe hypo × HbA1c × ACE D |
5.37 (0.02) | 0.73 (0.39) | 0.03 (0.85) | 0.74 (0.39) | 2.67 (0.10) | 0.62 (0.43) | 0.00 (0.99) | 0.16 (0.69) |
Data are F-statistics (P-values) for main effects and interaction effects.
Because there were no significant two-way interactions, they were not included in the model.
Higher HbA1c was associated with a greater cognitive decline in psychomotor efficiency and motor speed.
ACE, angiotensin-converting enzyme; HbA1c, glycated haemoglobin
Table 3.
Main and interaction effects of APO ε4 on change in cognitive functioning, analysed by level of severe hypoglycaemia, HbA1c and severe hypoglycaemia by HbA1c
Domain | Problem solving |
Learning | Immediate memory |
Delayed recall |
Spatial information |
Attention | Psychomotor efficiency |
Motor speed |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model 1 | ||||||||
APO ε4 | 0.40 (0.53) | 0.34 (0.56) | 0.13 (0.71) | 0.01 (0.92) | 1.29 (0.26) | 0.38 (0.54) | 0.00 (0.99) | 1.04 (0.31) |
Severe hypo (0 vs. 1+) | 0.30 (0.59) | 1.01 (0.32) | 2.31 (0.13) | 0.31 (0.58) | 1.24 (0.26) | 1.31 (0.25) | 3.35 (0.07) | 2.15 (0.14) |
Severe hypo × APO ε4 | 0.18 (0.67) | 0.71 (0.40) | 0.01 (0.92) | 0.73 (0.39) | 0.00 (0.97) | 4.58 (0.03) | 0.06 (0.80) | 0.01 (0.94) |
Model 2 | ||||||||
APO ε4 | 0.21 (0.65) | 0.10 (0.75) | 0.27 (0.61) | 0.01 (0.90) | 1.62 (0.20) | 0.87 (0.35) | 0.02 (0.88) | 1.12 (0.29) |
HbA1c (low vs. high)† | 0.05 (0.82) | 0.03 (0.86) | 1.44 (0.23) | 1.58 (0.21) | 0.39 (0.53) | 5.70 (0.02) | 19.93 (< 0.01) | 29.90 (< 0.01) |
HbA1c × APO ε4 | 0.01 (0.91) | 0.87 (0.35) | 0.58 (0.45) | 2.18 (0.14) | 0.97 (0.33) | 0.29 (0.59) | 0.02 (0.89) | 0.09 (0.77) |
Model 3* | ||||||||
APO ε4 | 0.30 (0.59) | 0.29 (0.59) | 0.19 (0.66) | 0.00 (0.98) | 1.40 (0.24) | 0.38 (0.54) | 0.05 (0.83) | 1.37 (0.24) |
Severe hypo (0 vs. 1+) | 0.34 (0.56) | 0.75 (0.39) | 2.31 (0.13) | 0.42 (0.52) | 1.55 (0.21) | 1.91 (0.17) | 2.84 (0.09) | 2.30 (0.13) |
HbA1c (low vs. high)† | 0.03 (0.86) | 0.00 (0.99) | 1.86 (0.17) | 1.88 (0.17) | 0.25 (0.62) | 5.65 (0.02) | 20.45 (< 0.01) | 31.58 (< 0.01) |
Severe Hypo × HbA1c × APO ε4 |
0.36 (0.55) | 0.14 (0.71) | 0.14 (0.71) | 1.67 (0.20) | 0.88 (0.35) | 3.58 (0.06) | 0.48 (0.49) | 1.27 (0.26) |
Data are F-statistics (P-values) for main effects and interaction effects.
Because there were no significant two-way interactions, they were not included in the model.
Higher HbA1c was associated with a greater cognitive decline in psychomotor efficiency and motor speed.
APO, apolipoprotein; HbA1c, glycated haemoglobin
There were no significant main effects for either APO ε4 or ACE D and no two-way interactions between genotype and either metabolic variable. There were also no statistically significant interactions between genotype, gender, age or treatment group (data not shown). There were also no differences in the impact of the ACE D gene on cognition between those who were on ACE inhibitors (n = 431 at year 12) and those who were not. Those taking ACE inhibitors had poorer cognitive function on one domain (domain 7) compared with those not taking an ACE inhibitor (P < 0.0001).
Separate analyses examined the effects of 18 additional ACE/APOE markers that have been used in other genetic analyses of the DCCT/EDIC study outcomes Only two of the 144 associations examined were significant: ACE rs4305 and delayed recall (P = 0.0089) and ACE rs4354 and problem solving (P < 0.0089). For ACE rs4305, genotypes A/B and B/B performed worse than A/A (mean cognitive scores: −0.23, −0.26, −0.08, respectively). In the second case, genotype A/B performed better than B/B (mean cognitive scores: 0.24, 0.05, respectively). However, when these P-values were conservatively adjusted with a Bonferroni correction, they were no longer significant.
Discussion
The primary goal of this study was to examine the extent to which the cognitive dysfunction frequently noted in adults with Type 1 DM [30] is linked to the presence of two genetic factors (APO ε4 and ACE D alleles) that have previously been found to increase the risk of cognitive dysfunction in older healthy adults [9,16]. Contrary to our expectation, our study of young and middle-aged diabetic adults found no evidence of a relationship between cognition and the two genetic factors. This was true both for our analysis of diabetic subjects at entry into the DCCT, when they had a relatively brief duration of diabetes and minimal microvascular complications, as well as in analyses of the same subjects retested approximately 18 years later, at a time when a large proportion had some microvascular complications as well as exposure to severe hypoglycaemic events.
Because some research suggests that the ε4 allele may confer added risk in response to other processes that can lead to brain injury [12,31,32], we also tested the hypothesis that genetic factors could modify the effects of common glycaemic exposures (recurrent, severe hypoglycaemic events or chronically elevated HbA1c) and thus influence cognitive performance in those important subgroups. Again, we found no evidence of such an effect. Recording of severe hypoglycaemic events varied in frequency from quarterly during DCCT to once each year (history limited to the prior 3 months) in the EDIC study. While this yielded substantial prospectively gathered information, we may still have underestimated the effects of hypoglycaemia on cognition, especially during the EDIC study when subjects were seen less frequently.
These null results deserve some comment. The evidence for genetic effects on cognitive function and/or Alzheimer’s disease in the non-diabetic population is more robust for APO ε4 than that for the ACE D allele and this is particularly true when examining older individuals, especially those with or at high risk for Alzheimer’s disease [9,17]. Only a few studies have included groups of subjects who are less than 60 years of age and the evidence for the effects of each genotype is less persuasive in such younger populations [8]. The relative youthfulness of our sample (mean sample age at follow-up was 47 years) may explain our null results, as well as the fact that, across studies, even among older adults, the magnitude of these effects tends to be quite small [9]. It is possible that such effects would be found in older Type 1 DM adults and only then would they magnify the effects of glycaemic exposure. Of interest, the relationship of APO ε4 to LDL cholesterol level and higher HbA1c to poorer cognitive function is consistent with the literature and this helps support the validity of the primary findings.
Our results are inconsistent with the single published study examining the relationship between APOE polymorphisms and cognition in patients with Type 1 DM [11]. In that cross-sectional study, genetic-cognition effects were limited to women with the ε4 allele who had an added risk of cognitive dysfunction compared with women without the allele or to diabetic men with or without the allele. With a far larger sample size (1093 subjects; 47% female; 290 with at least one ε4 allele, as compared with 96 subjects; 21 with at least one ε4 allele [11]), we have far more statistical power to identify these genetic/cognition/gender relationships and, given our sample size and our extended follow-up into the middle adult years, we still find no specific risk of cognitive dysfunction for women with at least one ε4 allele.
One important limitation of our study is that there were relatively few people in this study who were homozygous for APOE ε4. Such individuals are at greatest risk and so an effect could have been missed which would have been uncovered with a larger pool of high-risk individuals. However, increased risk for Alzheimer’s disease has clearly been shown in ε4 heterozygotes and 26.5% of DCCT/EDIC study white probands carry at least one ε4 allele (Table 1). Moreover, the DCCT/EDIC study is relatively large compared with most available clinical populations and the clinical assessments are very detailed, thereby making it less likely that effects have gone undetected in the age range and follow-up period studied.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
Funding for this study was provided by Grant R01 DK062218-02, R01 DK077510, the National Institutes of Health subcontract N01-6-2204, and contracts with the Division of Diabetes, Endocrinology and Metabolic Diseases of the National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases; National Eye Institute; National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke; The General Clinical Research Centers Program; National Center for Research Resources; The Herbert Graetz Psychosocial Research Fund; and by Genetech through a cooperative research and development agreement with the National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases. Contributions of free or discounted supplies and/or equipment included Lifescan, Roche, Aventis, Eli Lilly, Omnipod, Can-Am, B–D, Animas, Medtronic, Medtronic Minimed, Bayer (donation one time in 2008) and Omron. The content of this article is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases or the National Institutes of Health. A complete list of the individuals and institutions participating in the DCCT/EDIC Research Group appears in: Jacobson AM, Ryan CM, Cleary P, Waberski B, Burwood A, Weinger K et al. Long-term effects of diabetes and its treatment on cognitive function.
Abbreviations
- ACE
angiotensin-converting enzyme
- APOE
apolipoprotein E
- CNS
central nervous system
- DCCT
Diabetes Control and Complications Trial
- EDIC
Epidemiology of Diabetes Interventions and Complications
- HbA1c
glycated haemoglobin
- LDL
low-density lipoprotein
- SNP
single-nucleotide polymorphism
- Type1 DM
Type 1 diabetes mellitus
Footnotes
Trial registration: ClinicalTrials.gov number NCT00360893.
Competing interests
ADP holds a Canada Research Chair in Genetics of Complex Diseases and receives support from Genome Canada and the Premier Research Excellence Award. None of the remaining eleven authors have any relevant conflicts of interest to disclose.
Supporting Information
Additional Supporting Information may be found in the online version of this article:
Please note: Wiley-Blackwell are not responsible for the content or functionality of any supporting materials supplied by the authors. Any queries (other than missing material) should be directed to the corresponding author for the article.
References
- 1.Diabetes Control and Complications Trial Research Group. The effect of intensive treatment on the development and progression of long-term complications in insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus. N Engl J Med. 1993;329:977–986. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199309303291401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Diabetes Control and Complications Trial/Epidemiology of Diabetes Interventions and Complications (DCCT/EDIC) Study Research Group. Intensive diabetes treatment and cardiovascular disease in patients with Type 1 diabetes. N Engl J Med. 2005;353:2643–2653. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa052187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Musen G, Lyoo IK, Sparks CR, Weinger K, Hwang J, Ryan CM, et al. Effects of Type 1 diabetes on gray matter density as measured by voxel-based morphometry. Diabetes. 2006;55:326–333. doi: 10.2337/diabetes.55.02.06.db05-0520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Ryan CM. Diabetes and brain damage: more (or less) than meets the eye? Diabetologia. 2006;49:2229–2233. doi: 10.1007/s00125-006-0392-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Savitz J, Solms M, Ramesar R. Apolipoprotein E variants and cognition in healthy individuals: a critical opinion. Brain Res Rev. 2006;51:125–135. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2005.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Flory JD, Manuck SB, Ferrell RE, Ryan CM, Muldoon MF. Memory performance and the apolipoprotein E polymorphism in a community sample of middle-aged adults. Am J Med Genet (Neuropsychiatr Genet) 2000;96:707–711. doi: 10.1002/1096-8628(20001204)96:6<707::aid-ajmg1>3.0.co;2-v. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Parasuraman R, Greenwood PM, Sunderland T. The apolipoprotein E gene, attention, and brain function. Neuropsychology. 2002;16:254–274. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.16.2.254. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Baxter LC, Caselli RJ, Johnson SC, Reiman E, Osborn D. Appolipoprotein E ε4 affects new learning in cognitively normal individuals at risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2003;24:947–952. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(03)00006-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Small BJ, Rosnick CB, Fratiglioni L, Bäckman L. Apolipoprotein E and cognitive performance: a meta-analysis. Psychol Aging. 2004;19:592–600. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.19.4.592. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Peila R, Rodriguez BL, Launer LJ. Type 2 diabetes, APOE gene and the risk for dementia and related pathologies: The Honolulu-Asia Study. Diabetes. 2002;51:1256–1262. doi: 10.2337/diabetes.51.4.1256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Ferguson SC, Deary IJ, Evans JC, Ellard S, Hattersley AT, Frier BM. Apolipoprotein E influences aspects of intellectual ability in Type 1 diabetes. Diabetes. 2003;52:145–148. doi: 10.2337/diabetes.52.1.145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Jordan BD. Genetic influences on outcome following traumatic brian injury. Neurochem Res. 2007;32:905–915. doi: 10.1007/s11064-006-9251-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Turic D, Fisher PJ, Plomin R, Owen MJ. No association betweeen apolipoprotein E polymorhphisms and general cognitive ability in children. Neurosci Lett. 2001;299:97–100. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(00)01789-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Shaw P, Lerch JP, Pruessner JC, Taylor KN, Rose AB, Greenstein D, et al. Cortical morphology in children and adolescents with different apolipoprotein E gene polymorphisms: an observational study. Lancet Neurol. 2007;6:494–500. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(07)70106-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Wishart HA, Saykin AJ, McAllister TW, Rabin LA, McDonald BC, FlashmanL A, et al. Regional brain atrophy in cognitively intact adults with a single APOE e4 allele. Neurology. 2006;67:1221–1224. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000238079.00472.3a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Bartrés-Faz D, Junqué C, Clemente IC, López-Alomar A, Valveny N, López-Guillén A, et al. Angiotensin I converting enzyme polymorphism in humans with age-associated memory impairment: relationship with cognitive performance. Neurosci Lett. 2000;290:177–180. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(00)01349-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Richard F, Fromentin-David I, Ricolfi F, Ducimetière P, Di Menza C, Amouyel P, et al. The angiotensin I converting enzyme gene as a susceptibility factor for dementia. Neurology. 2001;56:1593–1595. doi: 10.1212/wnl.56.11.1593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Visscher PM, Tynan M, Whiteman MC, Pattie A, White I, Hayward C, et al. Lack of association between polymorphisms in angiotensin-converting-enzyme and methylenetretrahydrofolate reductase genes and normal cognitive aging in humans. Neurosci Lett. 2003;347:175–178. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(03)00691-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Stewart R, Powell J, Prince M, Mann A. ACE genotype and cognitive decline in an African-Carribean population. Neurobiol Aging. 2004;25:1369–1375. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2004.02.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Savaskan E. The role of the brain rennin-angiotensin system in neurodegenerative disorders. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2005;2:29–35. doi: 10.2174/1567205052772740. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Murdoch SJ, Boright AP, Paterson AD, Zinman B, Steffes M, Cleary P, et al. LDL composition in E2/2 subjects and LDL distribution by Apo E genotype in Type 1 diabetes. Atherosclerosis. 2007;192:138–147. doi: 10.1016/j.atherosclerosis.2006.04.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Al-Kateb H, Boright AP, Mirea L, Xie X, Sutradhar R, Mowjoodi A, et al. Multiple superoxide disumutase 1/splicing factor serine alanine 15 variants are associated with the development and progression of diabetic nephropathy: The Diabetes Control and Complications Trial/Epidemiology of Diabetes Interventions and Complications Genetics Study. Diabetes. 2008;57:218–228. doi: 10.2337/db07-1059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Epidemiology of Diabetes Interventions and Complications (EDIC) Design, implementation, and preliminary results of a long-term follow-up of the Diabetes Control and Complications Trial cohort. Diabetes Care. 1999;22:99–111. doi: 10.2337/diacare.22.1.99. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Diabetes Control and Complications Trial Research Group. Effects of intensive diabetes therapy on neuropsychological function in adults in the Diabetes Control and Complications Trial. Ann Intern Med. 1996;124:379–388. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-124-4-199602150-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Lezak MD. Neuropsychological Assessment. 3rd edn. New York: Oxford University Press; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Boright AP, Paterson AD, Mirea L, Bull SB, Mowjoodi A, Scherer SW, et al. Genetic variation at the ACE gene is associated with persistent microalbuminuria and severe nephropathy in Type 1 diabetes: the DCCT/EDIC Genetics Study. Diabetes. 2005;54:1238–1244. doi: 10.2337/diabetes.54.4.1238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Amercian Diabetes Association. OLA Supplementary Dataset 3. [Last accessed 10 December 2009];Descriptive information of 1441 autosomal SNPs genotyped using illumina technology. 2007 Available at http://diabetes.diabetesjournals.org/content/57/1/218/suppl/DC1. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Jacobson A, Ryan C, Cleary P, Waberski B, Burwood A, Weinger K, et al. Long-term effect of diabetes and its treatment on cognitive function. N Engl J Med. 2007;356:1842–1852. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa066397. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Snedecor G, Cochran WG. Statistical methods—8th edition. Ames, IA: Iowa State University Press; 1989. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Brands AMA, Biessels G-J, De Haan EHF, Kappelle LJ, Kessels RPC. The effects of Type 1 diabetes on cognitive performance: a meta-analysis. Diabetes Care. 2005;28:726–735. doi: 10.2337/diacare.28.3.726. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Peila R, White LR, Petrovich H, Masaki K, Ross GW, Havlik RJ, et al. Joint effect of APOE gene and midlife systolic blood pressure on late-life cognitive impairment: The Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. Stroke. 2001;32:2882–2889. doi: 10.1161/hs1201.100392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Louko A-M, Vilkki J, Niskakangas T. APOE genotype and cognition after subarachnoid haemorrhage: a longitudinal study. Acta Neurol Scand. 2006;114:315–319. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0404.2006.00676.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.