Abstract
Objective
Debate about the nature of the somatoform disorders and their current diagnostic classification has been stimulated by the anticipation of new editions of the DSM and ICD diagnostic classifications. In the current paper we systematically review the literature on the neuroimaging of somatoform disorders and related conditions with the aim of addressing two specific questions: Is there evidence of altered neural function or structure that is specifically associated with somatoform disorders? What conclusions can we draw from these findings about the etiology of somatoform disorders?
Methods
Studies reporting neuroimaging findings in patients with a somatoform disorder, or a functional somatic syndrome (such as Fibromyalgia) were found using Pubmed, PsycINFO and EMBASE database searches. Reported structural and functional neuroimaging findings were then extracted to form a narrative review.
Results
A relatively mature literature on symptoms of pain, and less developed literatures on conversion and fatigue symptoms were identified. The available evidence indicates that, when compared to non-clinical groups, somatoform diagnoses are associated with increased activity of limbic regions in response to painful stimuli and a generalized decrease in grey matter density; however methodological considerations restrict the interpretation of these findings.
Conclusions
While the neuroimaging literature has provided evidence about the possible mechanisms underlying somatoform disorders this is not yet sufficient to provide a basis for classification. By adopting a wider variety of experimental designs and a more dynamic approach to diagnosis there is every reason to be hopeful that neuroimaging data will play a significant role in future taxonomies.
Keywords: neuroscience, somatoform disorders, etiology, brain imaging
Introduction
The somatoform disorders are defined in the ICD10 and DSMIV psychiatric classifications as syndromal conditions characterized by somatic symptoms which cannot be accounted for either by identified disease pathology or by another Axis I psychiatric disorder (1, 2) (Table one). Perhaps confusingly, a parallel and overlapping medical classification of functional somatic syndromes such as fibromyalgia and chronic fatigue syndrome (3) is also in use and largely describes the same patients. The etiology of these conditions is not well understood with the existing diagnostic classifications being based largely on clinical utility. Consequently a range of questions remain outstanding: are these conditions different from malingering? Are they distinct from depression and anxiety? Do they represent many distinct conditions or only one? This ignorance fuels vigorous debate about how best to conceptualize and classify these conditions in the new editions of the ICD and DSM psychiatric diagnostic classifications (4-7). Here we ask what neuroimaging studies might tell us about the nature of these conditions and whether they can inform future diagnostic classification.
Table 1.
Somatization disorder |
Undifferentiated somatoform disorder |
Conversion disorder |
Pain disorder |
Hypochondriasis |
Body dysmorphic disorder |
Somatoform disorder, not otherwise specified |
What might neuroimaging tell us?
Neuroimaging studies employ a variety of techniques (8) that can tell us about brain anatomy (“structural” techniques such as voxel based morphometery; VBM) or about brain activity (“functional” techniques such functional magnetic resonance imaging; fMRI). Before reviewing what imaging does tell us we might usefully ask what it could realistically tell us about conditions of unknown etiology, such as the somatoform disorders.
Perhaps the most clinically useful role for neuroimaging is to provide information about whether a disorder is present or not; that is to use neuroimaging as a test to make a diagnosis. Certainly structural imaging currently has a role as a ‘negative diagnostic marker’ by detecting the absence of gross neural pathology in the somatoform disorders. Might neuroimaging also have a role as a ‘positive diagnostic marker’? There are some a priori grounds for optimism here as neuroimaging provides precise, quantifiable, multi-dimensional (in three-dimensional space and time) observations that must surely have the potential to add to the clinical observations that currently guide diagnosis. One problem is that the very search for such imaging markers is predicated on the validity of the existing diagnoses; the absence of this, as has been suggested above for somatoform disorders, would seriously undermine the likelihood of discovering such a diagnostic marker. For example, if patients may obtain the diagnostic label of “conversion disorder” as a consequence of a range of different (neural) mechanisms, then simply comparing the neuroimaging data of patients with the diagnosis to those without, is unlikely to reveal any specific ‘neural signature’ for this diagnosis. A more subtle question asks whether neuroimaging can identify relevant component mental processes associated with the diagnosis, rather than the diagnoses itself. For example might it differentiate conversion disorder (a diagnosis in which symptoms are believed to arise subconsciously) from malingering (in which symptoms are regarded as consciously manufactured) by reliably detecting the component mental process of the intention to deceive? Whilst there has been increased interest in using neuroimaging in this sort of targeted, mind reading role (9), an unequivocal demonstration that it is possible remains elusive, even in a controlled, non-clinical context (e.g. 10, 11-14). Thus while neuroimaging may in principle be able to provide positive diagnostic information for conditions of unknown etiology, the chance of finding such markers depends critically on the validity of the diagnostic system in use. And whilst this dependence on diagnosis can be sidestepped to some extent by focusing on specific cognitive processes rather than diagnosis, the ability to reliably detect such processes remains to be proven.
A more fundamental approach to neuroimaging explores the etiological mechanisms underlying these conditions, rather than simply describing diagnostic markers. Ultimately, the attraction of such theoretically informed investigation is that it offers a means of transcending the core limitation of current psychiatric practice; etiological ignorance. But there are potential pitfalls in interpretation here also, particularly if neuroimaging data is considered in isolation (15). One is the over-interpretation of differences in neural activation or structure observed between clinical and control groups. While it is tempting to conclude that any such difference must reflect the fundamental abnormalities of brain function which cause the illness, the truth is that they can be interpreted in at least three different ways: as a cause of the illness, as an effect of the illness, or as a compensation for the illness (16). Consequently the way in which neuroimaging data is interpreted is heavily dependent on the model of illness being tested. An important corollary of this is that neuroimaging studies generally provide information about the involvement of a specific neural system in a disorder rather than giving a complete account of the neural abnormalities associated with the disorder. For example, while the demonstration of amygdala hyperactivity to negative information in depression (17, 18) is consistent with both the cognitive models of the illness (19, 20) and with our understanding of the role of the amygdala in cognition (21), it is very unlikely that a hyperactive amygdala is the only neural abnormality associated with depression.
In summary, neuroimaging may be used either to describe or explore psychiatric disorders. When the validity of the diagnoses is questionable, as has been suggested for the somatoform disorders, it seems likely that neuroimaging techniques may most efficiently address questions of taxonomy by exploring the etiology of the disorders.
Aims
In the current review we sought to answer two key questions:
Is there evidence of altered neural function or structure that is specifically associated with somatoform disorders?
What conclusions can we draw about the etiology of somatoform disorders from these findings?
With these aims in mind we structure the review around the type of symptom (and thus neural system) investigated, rather than around the diagnosis of the patients studied. This strategy allows us to ask whether different diagnoses are associated with abnormalities in similar neural systems. We also include relevant background work from studies of non-clinical populations when they illustrate relevant cognitive neuroanatomical models. We conclude by summarizing the chfallenges facing the use of neuroimaging techniques in the somatoform disorders and by suggesting strategies by which these challenges may be met.
METHODS
Study Selection
Medline, Embase and Psychinfo databases were searched for relevant articles published between 1960 and 2009. The full search strategy is described in Supplemental Digital Content 1. See Supplemental Digital Content 2 for a flow chart describing the number of records found in the search and final number identified after screening. The great majority of the identified publications reported explorations of the neural circuitry involved in one of three symptoms clusters: pain symptoms, loss of motor or sensory function, and fatigue symptoms. We therefore considered each symptom cluster separately. For each we provide a descriptive review of the literature and an analysis of its etiological implications.
Limitations
At the outset, a number of limitations to the current review should be acknowledged. First, although we have systematically searched the literature, the boundaries of the topic are not clear because competing taxonomies are in use (3). As a result, our review incorporates data from patients diagnosed with somatoform diagnoses and also those with only symptoms and those with diagnoses of functional somatic syndromes. Indeed, the use of symptom provocation designs in the functional neuroimaging literature lends itself to the recruitment of patients with a specific symptom rather than a syndromal diagnosis and, as a result, the majority of studies identified in the current review have recruited patients with specific symptoms rather than those with DSM-IV diagnoses. Second we have limited the review to published reports and have not sought to identify unpublished work. Third, as the nature of the data permits only narrative summary, the process of data abstraction from the papers was not done blind to the questions being addressed making bias possible.
RESULTS
Disorders in which pain or discomfort are the primary symptom
Patients with pain attract a range of diagnoses including pain disorder, fibromyalgia (FM), somatization disorder and irritable bowel syndrome (IBS). The relatively high prevalence of these disorders in the general population, the extensive literature on the processing of pain in non-clinical populations and the ease of delivering a controlled painful stimulus during testing have led to a relatively large and sophisticated literature on this topic. The dominant model in pain perception describes a “neuromatrix” (22, 23) which is activated in response to painful stimuli. This matrix includes sensory areas such as somatosensory cortex, limbic regions such as the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and insula which are thought to process the emotional aspects of the percept and frontal regions such as the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) thought to subserve attentional control (24). It has been proposed that the emotional-limbic and attentional-prefrontal systems interact with the sensory-discriminant system and that this interaction can produce a sensitization (or desensitization) to painful stimuli (24). While it is important to be aware that “pain” may incorporate a number of different experiences (e.g. visceral or somatic pain) and that the overlap between the neural structures involved in these experiences is not absolute, there is evidence that a limited set of higher order areas perform similar modulatory functions across pain types (24, 25). Evidence for the higher order modulation of pain perception has been found in studies where neural activation of the ACC, somatosensory and frontal cortices to painful stimuli correlate more strongly with the subjective experience of pain than with the objective intensity of the stimulus whereas activation of ascending sensory regions (e.g. the thalamus) reflects the objective characteristics of the painful stimuli (26). Furthermore, psychological responses to pain such as an increased tendency to catastrophize, which has been proposed as a diagnostic marker of somatoform conditions (27), are associated with increased ACC activity during painful stimulation (28). Lastly, hypnotically suggested pain appears to produce a pattern of brain activation similar to that associated with an actual painful stimulus, indicating that central mechanisms may be sufficient to produce the experience of pain even in the absence of external stimulation (29, 30). In summary, work in non-clinical samples suggests that painful symptoms in the absence of disease may, at least in part, result from aberrant higher order modulation of somatic percepts (31).
Is there any evidence for abnormalities in the activity of these regions in relevant clinical populations? A number of studies, listed in Table 2, have directly compared clinical and control groups during administration of unpleasant stimuli (for recent reviews see also 25, 32).
Table 2.
Study | Clinical Diagnosis |
N per group |
Imaging Modality |
Type of Pain | Areas of increased activation in clinical group |
Areas of decreased activation in clinical group |
Notes |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(105) | Atypical Facial Pain |
6 | PET | Heat | ACC (BA 24) | Left Frontopolar Cortex (BA 10) |
No information on the objective intensity of the painful stimuli given in text |
(106) | Fibromyalgia | 10 | SPECT | Pressure (not in scanner) |
Nil | Thalamus and Caudate |
Pain thresholds assessed outside scanner. No pain administered during scan. |
(107) | IBS | 6 | PET | Rectal Distension |
Left Frontopolar cortex (BA10) | ACC (BA 24, 32) | Between group comparisons performed qualitatively. |
(108) | IBS | 16 | fMRI | Rectal Distension |
ACC (BA 24) | Nil | |
(109) | IBS | 12 | PET | Rectal Distension |
ACC (BA 24) | Orbitofrontal cortex (BA 11); Frontal Pole (BA 10); Putamen; Thalamus |
|
(31) | Fibromyalgia | 16 | fMRI | Pressure | Sensory Cortex (BA 2); Insula; Superior Temporal Gyrus (BA 22); ACC (BA 24) ; Precuneus (BA 31); Cerebellum |
Left Frontopolar Cortex (BA 10) |
|
(110) | Chronic Lower Back Pain |
16 | PET | Heat | Nil | Nil | |
(111) | IBS + FM | 10 | PET | Pressure (somatic) and Rectal Distension |
Mid ACC (BA 24/32) | Nil | Comparison performed between patients with IBS and patients with both IBS and FM. Activity in the ACC was greatest for the IBS group during rectal distension and for the IBS+FM group during somatic pressure. |
(112) | IBS | 9 | fMRI | Rectal Distension and Heat |
ACC (BA 24); insula; PFC (BA 9) |
Pain induced using both rectal distension and cutaneaous heat. |
|
(113) | Chronic Lower Back Pain and Fibromyalgia |
11/16 | fMRI | Pressure | Bilateral Somatosensory Cortex | Nil | Between group comparisons performed qualitatively. |
(114) | IBS | 9 | fMRI | Rectal Distension |
ACC and posterior Cingulate/Precuneus, Bilateral Striatum and Bilateral dlPFC |
N/A | Areas of activation inferred from graphical presentation of quantitative between group analysis (no table of activation provided in text). NB majority of paper describes a qualitative analysis. |
(115) | IBS | 8 | fMRI | Rectal Distension |
ACC (BA 32), PFC (BA 10) | ACC (BA 24 / 32) PFC (BA 10) |
Complex study with small sample size. Three intensities of stimulation were used (subliminal, liminal and supraliminal). Activity in IBS group greater in ACC and PFC during liminal stimulation. ACC activity was greater in the control group during both sub and supraliminal stimulation. |
(116) | IBS | 7 | PET | Rectal Distension |
ACC (32/25), Left Amygdala, PFC (BA 9/32) |
Dorsal Pons/PAG | In this comparison patients with IBS were compared to patients with quiescent ulcerative colitis. |
(117) | IBS | 5 | PET | Rectal Distension |
ACC (BA 32), Parahippocampal gyrus |
Nil | Study scanned patients before and after cognitive therapy. The reported regions were those in which activity was reduced by treatment. Extremely small groups used. |
(118) | Somatoform Pain Disorder |
17 | fMRI | Pin Prick | Insula; Dorsomedial PFC (BA 8); Superior Temporal Gyrus (BA 21); Hippocampus; Putamen; Thalamus |
Motor Cortex (BA 4) |
|
(119) | Multiple Chemical Sensitivity |
12 | PET | Odorant | ACC (BA 24); Precuneus/Cuneus (BA 7,18); Right Right Parahippocampal Gyrus (BA 19) |
Amygdala | Noxious stimuli was odorant rather than pain |
(120) | Somatoform Pain Disorder |
12 | fMRI | Heat | Amygdala; Insula; Parahippocampal Gyrus; Primary Sensory Cortex |
Orbitofrontal Cortex (BA 11) |
|
(121) | IBS | 15 | fMRI | Rectal Distension |
ACC (BA 24/32); Insula, Brainstem |
Nil | NB Anticipation of cue was associated with decreased deactiviation of amygdala and insula in patients |
(122) | Fibromyalgia | 18 | fMRI | Incision | ACC (BA 24/32); PFC (BA 6/8); Thalamus |
Thalamus | Very liberal threshold (P<0.01 uncorrected) used in imaging analysis. An incision was used to induce acute pain. |
(123) | Fibromyalgia | 16 | fMRI | Pressure | ACC (BA 32); Thalamus |
Average pressure for control group was approximately twice that for patient group. No description of method for multiple comparison correction given |
|
(124) | Fibromyalgia | 9 | fMRI | Pressure | ACC (BA 24); Sensory Cortex (BA 4); Insula; Frontal Operculum (BA 44); Basal Ganglia |
Novel analysis in which the time course of the neural response is first inferred from the data |
NB The study by Bernstein and colleagues (125) which compared patients with IBS and control subjects has been omitted from the table as it employed a particularly unusual statistical approach rendering it difficult to compare with the other papers listed.
IBS=Irritable Bowel Syndrome
As can be seen, an overall pattern of increased activation in response to unpleasant stimuli is apparent in the clinical groups, with the ACC and insula cortices being the most commonly identified regions. This observation is supported by the electrophysiological literature in which painful somatic symptoms have been associated with increased sensory evoked potentials in response to aversive stimuli (33), with the sources of these responses being estimated to lie in the anterior cingulate, insula and sensory cortices (34). The increased activity in these limbic areas is consistent with a cognitive model of somatoform disorders (35) which predicts a heightened salience of noxious percepts in these patients. The neuroimaging data therefore provides some support for the contention that medically unexplained pain may reflect abnormalities in the higher order modulation of perception.
Recent studies of patients with somatoform pain disorders have ventured beyond simply studying the response to painful stimuli. Two studies have used suggestion (both with and without hypnosis) to modify the pain experience of patients with fibromyalgia. Both found that activity in a range of pain responsive regions (including the ACC) was influenced by the intervention (36, 37). Similar results have been reported for patients with IBS administered placebo (38), and patients who have repeated administration of the noxious stimuli a number of times over the course of a year (39). By using experimental interventions to alter both neural activity and subjective experience these studies provide corroborative evidence that activity in the identified regions may be causally related to the painful symptoms of patients rather than being merely epiphenomena.
Interestingly depressed patients, who often report somatic complaints, do not seem to show increased activity in these regions during the administration of painful stimuli (40), although they do show increased activity during anticipation of the painful stimuli (41) suggesting that somatoform pain disorders may be differentiated from depression.
A number of studies have investigated the brain neurochemistry of patients with somatoform pain disorders using a range of PET ligands. Evidence from these favors both dopaminergic (42-45) and opioid (46) abnormalities suggesting a possible neurochemical underpinning for the altered functional responses to pain described above.
Other studies (47-59) have used EEG techniques, mainly during sleep, to investigate the electrophysiological abnormalities associated with painful symptoms, particularly in patients with Fibromyalgia. While these studies suggest that abnormalities of sleep architecture are commonly associated with the disorders, it has proven difficult to demonstrate replicable and specific findings.
Lastly, there has been increasing interest in possible structural abnormalities of the brain associated with somatoform pain disorders. It has been suggested that chronic pain syndromes (whether somatoform or not) are associated with decreased grey matter (60), an hypothesis supported by studies of both fibromyalgia (61-64, although see also 65) and IBS (66). The relevance of these findings is supported by the reporting of an association between impaired cognition and decreased grey matter in patients with fibromyalgia (67).
In summary, compared to healthy controls, patients with somatoform pain symptoms show abnormalities of both brain structure and function. The best characterized of these is increased activity in a number of regions of the pain neuromatrix in response to noxious stimuli. These regions include those believed to be involved in the emotional appraisal of stimuli (ACC, insula).
Disorders in which loss of motor or sensory function is the primary symptom
Loss or excess of motor or sensory function without organic disease may be diagnosed as conversion disorder, a diagnosis currently classified under somatoform disorders in DSMIV. Conversion disorder should be an ideal candidate to investigate using functional imaging as there is an extensive literature on the neural systems involved in motor control and sensation, there are robust electrophysiological techniques which can be used to demonstrate that peripheral aspects of both systems are intact and the relevant neural systems can be readily probed by asking patients to attempt to move a paralyzed limb, or by stimulation of the anesthetic sense. Unfortunately it has been difficult for investigators to recruit sufficient numbers of patients to mount large studies. Consequently the neuroimaging literature on conversion disorder is largely limited to single case studies and small case series. The largest cohort of patients included in a published study so far is twelve. Despite the limited empirical evidence, there is a rich history of theorizing about the etiology of conversion disorders (68, 69). Current models reflect electrophysiological findings that the primary motor (70) and sensory cortices (71, 72) as well as peripheral nervous system function appears to be intact in patients with conversion disorder. By exclusion, therefore, any pathology is presumed to reside in higher order neural systems.
The evidence from functional imaging studies of motor conversion symptoms is summarized in Table 3. All but one of the studies reviewed had insufficient numbers to justify statistical comparison between groups; therefore the differences reported in these studies are from qualitative analysis of activation patterns only. It is difficult to draw any firm conclusions from such analyses. A simple review of the effects reported in each study (see Table 3) does not reveal any strikingly consistent finding. Data from structural imaging studies is even more sparse with only a single study being identified (73). In summary, the available data on motor conversion disorder is interesting but limited.
Table 3.
Study | Comparison | Control Group | N | Imaging Modality |
Areas of increased activation in clinical group |
Areas of decreased activation in clinical group |
Notes |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(126) | Attempted movement vs. rest |
None | 1 | PET | Increased activity in ACC (BA 24/32) and OFC (BA 10) |
No statistical analyses performed. Authors note an absence of motor cortex activity |
|
(127) | Resting | None | 5 | SPECT | Left Temporal and Parietal regions |
No statistical analyses performed |
|
(128) | Attempted movement vs. rest |
Healthy controls who feigned weakness |
2 | PET | Left dlPFC (BA 46) |
Qualitative between group comparison |
|
(129) | Movement vs. rest and observation of moving hand vs. observation of resting hand |
Healthy controls who moved limbs normally |
4 | fMRI | Motor cortex during movement observation (BA 4) |
No differences observed during movement itself. Qualitative comparison between groups |
|
(130) | Attempted movement vs. rest |
Healthy controls who feigned weakness |
4 | fMRI | Frontal (BA 47), striatal and lingual cortex (BA 18) |
Motor (BA 4) and OFC (BA 10/46) |
Qualitative comparison between groups |
(131, 132) |
Mental rotation of affected vs. unaffected hand |
Within subject analysis— unaffected side acts as control |
8 | fMRI | Medial frontal (BA 9/11) and superior temporal cortex (BA 22) |
Non-parametric statistical analysis performed within subjects |
|
(133) | Go NoGo Task | Healthy controls who feigned weakness |
1 | fMRI | Ventromedial frontal (BA32/10) and left orbital frontal (BA 47) |
Qualitative comparison between groups. Effects reported during movement preparation |
The results of the studies which have investigated sensory conversion symptoms are summarized in Table 4. Some of these have had sample sizes adequate to permit between group statistical analyses. It is difficult to discern any emerging pattern in the data other than that activation of the relevant sensory cortex is decreased. Given the electrophysiological evidence indicating that sensory evoked potentials are normal in these populations, this finding suggests a modulatory effect on sensory processing occurring after the initial sensory cortical response.
Table 4.
Study | Comparison | N | Imaging Modality |
Areas of increased activation in clinical group |
Areas of decreased activation in clinical group |
Notes |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(134) | Median nerve stimulation vs. rest |
1 | SPECT | Contralateral Parietal Lobe |
Perfusion abnormality resolved with recovery |
|
(135) | Vibratory tactile stimulation vs. rest |
7 | SPECT | Contralateral basal ganglia and thalamus |
In a subset of 4 patients who recovered the hypoperfusion was found to have resolved and baseline perfusion of the caudate nucleus was found to predict recovery |
|
(136) | Painful and non-painful tactile stimulation of anaesthetic vs. normal limb |
4 | fMRI | Anterior ACC | PFC, posterior ACC and sensory cortex |
Across the group conditions were compared with a qualitative analysis |
(137)a | Monocular visual stimulation vs. rest. |
5 | fMRI | Left inferior frontal cortex (BA 47), insula and uncus (BA 34) and bilateral striatum |
Visual cortex (BA 17/18) and ACC (BA 25) |
A non-clinical control group was also tested allowing a between group analysis. |
(138) | Vibratory stimulation of anaesthetic limb vs. bilateral limb stimulation |
3 | fMRI | Contralateral sensory cortex |
Contralateral (to the symptoms) sensory cortex was activated during bilateral stimulation. |
All patient groups reported loss of somatic sensation except in which the presenting complaint was unexplained visual loss.
Unfortunately, perhaps the strongest conclusion that can be drawn from the neuroimaging studies of motor and sensory conversion disorder is that the samples studied have been so small that it remains unclear which findings represent signal and which noise.
Disorders in which fatigue is the primary symptom
Fatigue and its syndrome, chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS) presents a different challenge from conversion disorder for those designing neuroimaging studies. Simply put, there is little a priori evidence as to which areas of the brain are most likely to be involved in the sensation of fatigue. Thus while it has proved possible to recruit and study reasonably sized samples of patients, it is less clear which tasks should be completed during imaging and which areas of the brain should be focused on.
Early imaging studies of CFS investigated whether the diagnosis was associated with gross structural abnormalities of the brain. These produced mixed results with some studies suggesting no specific abnormalities (74, 75), while others suggested abnormalities in subgroups such as those without psychiatric comorbidity (76). More recently the analysis of structural scans has evolved such that statistical inference can be performed on brain structure using a voxelwise approach. One such approach, voxel based morphometery has produced interesting findings in patients with CFS. An initial report of decreased gray matter volume in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of patients with CFS (77) was complemented by a report from a different group which reported a global reduction in gray matter volume which correlated with functional status (78). Importantly the authors were then able to demonstrate a small but significant increase in gray matter volume in patients following treatment with cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) (79).
Early functional neuroimaging studies of resting brain activity in patients with CFS suggested both generalized decreases in cerebral perfusion and localized deficits in the brain stem (80, 81) and possibly ACC (82). There have, however, been no recent replications of this early data. Other functional imaging studies have used a symptom provocation approach. One popular method has been to use a continuous cognitive task (such as adding together sequential digits which are presented every few seconds) which is argued to be “fatiguing”. Studies using such methodology have reported increased activation in the cingulate (83), supplemental motor areas, superior parietal cortex (84), medial prefrontal cortex (85), inferior frontal crotex, superior temporal cortex, hippocampus, cerebellum (86), precuneus, lingual gyrus and cerebellum, with decreased activation in dorsolateral PFC (87) and caudate (88). There has been little replication of findings between studies. Finally a single study investigated the authors’ hypothesis that CFS was associated with an abnormality of the serotonergic system. Using PET with a specific radioligand for the 5-HT 1a receptor a generalized decrease in binding in patients was demonstrated, particularly in the hippocampus (89).
The electrophysiological investigation of CFS has provided similar results to those found with pain; with relatively nonspecific abnormalities of sleep architecture being reported (90-93).
In summary, a number of different structural and functional approaches have been used to image patients with chronic fatigue. Interpretation of the data they have produced is hampered by the lack of strong a priori predictions about which areas of the brain are likely to be implicated in the production of fatigue. None of the results have been reliably replicated although preliminary evidence does suggest a number of interesting lines of enquiry, with the most compelling data currently available being for a partially reversible decrease of gray matter volume in patients.
Other disorders
Relatively few studies of patients with a diagnosis of “somatization disorder” have been published. These report resting activation (94-96) and structural differences (97) in patients with a range of somatic symptoms, with pain appearing to be the most common. The published studies involve small samples (maximum n=11, with similar cohorts of patients repeatedly analyzed in different studies) so we cannot draw clear conclusion about the patterns of activation associated with this diagnosis.
Body dysmorphic disorder (BDD) is included (controversially) in the somatoform disorders construct. Two structural studies provided no consistent findings when comparing patients with controls (98, 99). A second, fMRI, study demonstrated increased activation in left sided frontal and temporal regions in BDD patients versus controls when a face matching task was completed (100). The neuroimaging literature on BDD must be regarded as preliminary.
COMMENT
In this review we aimed to address two main questions:
(1) Is there evidence of altered neural function or structure specifically associated with somatoform disorders?
A large number of different functional and structural neuroimaging abnormalities have been reported in patients with somatoform symptoms compared to non-clinical controls. In keeping with the neuroimaging literature for other psychiatric disorders, very few of these findings have been reliably replicated, the most convincing exceptions to this rule being an increased activity of limbic structures (insula and ACC) in response to aversive stimuli in patients with pain and decreased gray matter in patients with both pain and fatigue. The great majority of studies published to date have compared patients with a somatoform diagnosis to non-clinical controls. There is therefore little direct evidence to suggest that any of the neuroimaging abnormalities found is specific to the somatoform diagnoses, as opposed to other psychiatric diagnoses such as depression which may present with similar symptoms.
(2) Can we draw any conclusions about the etiology of somatoform disorders from these findings?
For neuroimaging data to provide a meaningful account of pathology; it must be interpreted relative to a model of the condition. In this light the increased ACC and insula activity reported in response to a variety of painful stimuli across a range of functional diagnoses is consistent both with the presumed role of these areas in mediating the emotional response to painful stimuli (24) and with cognitive accounts of functional symptoms which stress the role of increased personal salience of these percepts (35). Further, the finding that common brain regions show increased activity across various diagnoses provides some support for the proposition that similar neural processes may underlie a variety of functional diagnoses (3). However, the evidence that this neuroimaging abnormality reflects a causal illness processes is weak; as we have discussed above, a number of alternative interpretations may account for the association such as the neuroimaging findings representing a consequence or compensation for the illness. It is therefore encouraging that recent studies have used alternative designs such as experimental interventions to alter both cognitive and brain function in patient groups (e.g. hypnosis as used in 36, 37). The demonstration that symptoms covary with brain activity provides better evidence of causality than the simple case-control approach which makes up the majority of the published literature.
Interpretation of the structural change of loss of gray matter associated with symptoms of fatigue and pain is more challenging as it appears that this loss occurs over much of the cortex, rather than being localized to a particular neural system. A possible explanation comes from the animal literature in which environmental enrichment is found to increase neuronal plasticity (101). If correct, this would suggest that loss of gray matter reflects the limiting effects of the illness and is therefore best considered a consequence, rather than a cause of the illness.
In summary, interpretation of the etiological significance, even of the most robust neuroimaging findings described in the current review, requires caution as the methodologies commonly used to date provide only indirect evidence of causality. Encouragingly, experimental approaches which may more directly assess questions of causality are increasingly being employed.
How might neuroimaging more effectively contribute to our understanding and management of somatoform disorders?
Despite the reservations expressed above and the lack of any clinically useful insights so far, we remain optimistic that neuroimaging may prove useful in developing our understanding of the nature of the somatoform disorders. In order to do so, we believe that it must form part of a concerted approach in which imaging neuroscience has greater interaction with other disciplines. The attempt to isolate a regional abnormality that ‘causes’ the somatoform disorders is likely to prove fruitless, notably because, as we have pointed out, such regional abnormalities are inherently ambiguous: it cannot be known whether they signify a cause of, a consequence of or a compensation for the disorder. However, this is not to say that they won’t offer clues about the underlying pathological processes, clues that may be followed up productively using an iterative approach in which other brain and behavioral observations inform and are informed by functional and structural neuroimaging. A second cause for optimism about the role of neuroimaging in the somatoform disorders comes from other areas of the neuroimaging literature which have overcome many of the difficulties with interpretation we have documented in this review. We therefore conclude by summarizing what we see as the broad challenges facing neuroimaging in somatoform disorders and the ways in which these challenges may be met. We do so, where possible, with reference to the successes of neuroimaging in other psychiatric disorders.
1. Etiology
As we have noted, neuroimaging currently contributes most to the diagnosis of somatoform disorders by excluding other causes of the symptoms. It is to be hoped that the identification of particular disturbances of processing in the brain may offer the clinician a sign to look for, rather than simply one to exclude. But this development will only be possible against the background of useful cognitive models of the disorder. That is, the imaging measure must be testing a hypothesis about a specific process or set of processes whose presence or absence could be invoked to explain the symptoms, an approach which has already achieved some success in cognitive neuroscience (e.g.,102). Encouragingly, there has been an increasing trend for neuroimaging studies of somatoform disorders to utilize experimental interventions in order to assess the role of specific cognitive processes (34, 35). We suggest that such studies are likely to be of increasing importance in elucidating the etiology of the disorders.
2. Diagnosis
Given the widespread acknowledgement that the existing diagnostic systems are unsatisfactory it follows that, especially in the context of traditional case-control methodology, any groupwise difference imaging observation will only reify the diagnostic system that led to grouping in the first place. Clearly, there is a need to adopt more dynamic methods that take into account individual variability and seek to identify both neural commonalities across apparently disparate conditions and neural distinctions between apparently similar conditions. Such an approach has been deployed in depression where it has been suggested that specific symptoms with a known neural architecture, such as anhedonia, offer improved phenotypic definition when compared to clinical diagnosis (103).
3. Prediction
Better diagnostic systems model the real world more accurately than their competitors. This is reflected in the ability of the diagnosis to provide information which predicts prognosis, both with and without treatment. Encouragingly, functional neuroimaging has already been used to predict responsiveness to treatment in a group of depressed patients (104) indicating that, in principle, this is an achievable goal, although it should be noted that even here the neuroimaging outcomes do not differ sufficiently between depressed and control groups to allow the prediction of a given individual’s prognosis. If neural observations are to have validity in the setting of somatoform disorders they must be held to the same requirement, that is, they must predict, at an individual level, the prognosis of the condition as well as informing the choice of treatment.
Conclusion
Neuroimaging studies have begun to delineate the neural processes implicated in the somatoform disorders. By adopting a wider variety of experimental designs and a more dynamic approach to diagnosis there is reason to be hopeful that neuroimaging data will play a significant role in shaping future taxonomies.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
Financial Disclosure: The authors report no financial conflict of interest.
Funding Support: Dr Browning is supported by a Clinical Training Fellowship from the Wellcome Trust (WT081672MA). Prof Fletcher is supported by a grant from the Bernard Wolfe Health Neuroscience fund. Professor Sharpe is supported by the University of Edinburgh
Funding: The preparation of this review was supported by grants to the authors from the Wellcome Trust (MB), the Bernard Wolfe Health Neuroscience fund (PCF) and the University of Edinburgh (MS).
Abbreviations
- 5-HT
5 hydroxytryptamine (serotonin)
- ACC
anterior cingulate cortex
- CBT
cognitive behavioral therapy
- CFS
chronic fatigue syndrome
- dlPFC
dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
- DSM
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
- EEG
electroencephalography
- FM
fibromyalgia
- fMRI
functional magnetic resonance imaging
- IBS
irritable bowel syndrome
- ICD
International Classification of Diseases
- PET
positron emission tomography
- VBM
voxel based morphometery
Footnotes
List of Supplemental Digital Content Files:
Supplemental Digital Content 1: PDF.
Supplemental Digital Content 2: PDF.
This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- 1.WHO . The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders: clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. World Health Organization; Geneva: 1992. [Google Scholar]
- 2.APA . Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed. American Psychiatric Association; Washington, DC: 1994. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Wessely S, Nimnuan C, Sharpe M. Functional somatic syndromes: one or many? Lancet. 1999;354:936–9. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(98)08320-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Mayou R, Kirmayer LJ, Simon G, Kroenke K, Sharpe M. Somatoform disorders: time for a new approach in DSM-V. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162:847–55. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.5.847. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Brown RJ, Cardeña E, Nijenhuis E, Sar V, van der Hart O. Should conversion disorder be reclassified as a dissociative disorder in DSM V? Psychosomatics. 2007;48:369–78. doi: 10.1176/appi.psy.48.5.369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Fava GA, Wise TN. Issues for DSM-V: psychological factors affecting either identified or feared medical conditions: a solution for somatoform disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164:1002–3. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.7.1002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Rief W, Isaac M. Are somatoform disorders ‘mental disorders’? A contribution to the current debate. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2007;20:143–6. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e3280346999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Glabus MF. Neuroimaging Part A: Pt A (internation Review of Neurobiology) Academic Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Spence SA, Hunter MD, Farrow TF, Green RD, Leung DH, Hughes CJ, Ganesan V. A cognitive neurobiological account of deception: evidence from functional neuroimaging. Philosohpical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B. 2004;359:1755–62. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2004.1555. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Haynes JD, Sakai K, Rees G, Gilbert S, Frith C, Passingham RE. Reading hidden intentions in the human brain. Curr Biol. 2007;17:323–8. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.11.072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Sip KE, Roepstorff A, McGregor W, Frith CD. Detecting deception: the scope and limits. Trends Cogn Sci. 2008;12:48–53. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Greely HT, Illes J. Neuroscience-based lie detection: the urgent need for regulation. Am J Law Med. 2007;33:377–431. doi: 10.1177/009885880703300211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Davatzikos C, Ruparel K, Fan Y, Shen DG, Acharyya M, Loughead JW, Gur RC, Langleben DD. Classifying spatial patterns of brain activity with machine learning methods: application to lie detection. Neuroimage. 2005;28:663–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Poldrack RA. The role of fMRI in cognitive neuroscience: where do we stand? Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2008;18:223–7. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2008.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Fletcher PC. Functional neuroimaging of psychiatric disorders: exploring hidden behaviour. Psychol Med. 2004;34:577–81. doi: 10.1017/S0033291704002430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Lewis DA. Distributed disturbances in brain structure and function in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157:1–2. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Siegle GJ, Steinhauer SR, Thase ME, Stenger VA, Carter CS. Can’t shake that feeling: Event-related fMRI assessment of sustained amygdala activity in response to emotional information in depressed individuals. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;51:693–707. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01314-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Sheline YI, Barch DM, Donnelly JM, Ollinger JM, Snyder AZ, Mintun MA. Increased amygdala response to masked emotional faces in depressed subjects resolves with antidepressant treatment: An fMRI study. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;50:651–8. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01263-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Mathews A, MacLeod C. Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2005;1 doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.143916. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Williams JMG, Watts FN, MacLeod CM, Mathews A. Cognitive Psychology and Emotional Disorders. John Wiley & Sons; New York: 1997. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Phelps EA, Le Doux JE. Contributions of the Amygdala to Emotion Processing: From Animal Models to Human Behavior. Neuron. 2005;48:175–87. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.09.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Melzack R, Wall PD. Pain mechanisms: a new theory. Science. 1965;150 doi: 10.1126/science.150.3699.971. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Tracey I, Becerra L, Chang I, Breiter H, Jenkins L, Borsook D, González RG. Noxious hot and cold stimulation produce common patterns of brain activation in humans: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neurosci Lett. 2000;288:159–62. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(00)01224-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Wiech K, Ploner M, Tracey I. Neurocognitive aspects of pain perception. Trends in Cognitive Science. 2008;12:306–13. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Mayer EA, Naliboff BD, Craig AD. Neuroimaging of the brain–gut axis: from basic understanding to treatment of functional GI disorders. Gastroenterology. 2006;131:1925–42. doi: 10.1053/j.gastro.2006.10.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Coghill RC, McHaffie JG, Yen YF. Neural correlates of interindividual differences in the subjective experience of pain. Proceedings National Academy of Science. 2003;100:8538–42. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1430684100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.DSM Proposed revision of the somatoform disorders category for DSM-V. 2010 [Google Scholar]
- 28.Gracely RH, Geisser ME, Giesecke T, Grant MA, Petzke F, Williams DA. Pain catastrophizing and neural responses to pain among persons with fibromyalgia. Brain. 2004;127 doi: 10.1093/brain/awh098. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Derbyshire SW, Whalley MG, Stenger VA, Oakley DA. Cerebral activation during hypnotically induced and imagined pain. Neuroimage. 2004;23:392–401. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.04.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Raij TT, Numminem J, Narvanen S, Hiltunen J, Hari R. Brain correlates of subjective reality of physically and psychologically induced pain. Proceedings National Academy of Science. 2005;102:2147–51. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0409542102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Gracely RH, Petzke F, Wolf JM, Clauw DJ. Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence of augmented pain processing in fibromyaglia. Arthritis and Rheumatisim. 2002;46:1333–43. doi: 10.1002/art.10225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Wood PB. Neuroimaging in functional somatic syndromes. Int Rev Neurobiol. 2005;67:119–63. doi: 10.1016/S0074-7742(05)67005-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Diers M, Koeppe C, Yilmaz P, Thieme K, Markela-Lerenc J, Schiltenwolf M, Van Ackern K, Flor H. Pain ratings and somatosensory evoked responses to repetitive intramuscular and intracutaneous stimulation in fibromyalgia syndrome. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology. 2008;25:153–60. doi: 10.1097/WNP.0b013e31817759c5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Drewes AM, Rossel P, Le Pera D, Arendt-Nielsen L, Valeriani M. Cortical neuroplastic changes to painful colon stimulation in patients with irritable bowel syndrome. Neuroscience Letters. 2005;375:157–61. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.11.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Brown RJ. Psychological mechanisms of medically unexplained symptoms: an integrative conceptual model. Psychol Bull. 2004;130:793–812. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.5.793. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Wik G, Fischer H, Bragée B, Finer B, Fredrikson M. Functional anatomy of hypnotic analgesia: a PET study of patients with fibromyalgia. Eur J Pain. 1999;3:7–12. doi: 10.1053/eujp.1998.0093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Derbyshire SW, Whalley MG, Oakley DA. Fibromyalgia pain and its modulation by hypnotic and non-hypnotic suggestion: An fMRI analysis. Eur J Pain. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2008.06.010. doi:10.1016/j.ejpain.2008.06.010 2008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Price DD, Craggs J, Nicholas VG, Perlstein WM, Robinson ME. Placebo analgesia is accompanied by large reductions in pain-related brain activity in irritable bowel syndrome patients. Pain. 2007;127:63–72. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2006.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Naliboff BD, Berman S, Suyenobu B, Labus JS, Chang L, Stains J, Mandelkern MA, Mayer EA. Longitudinal Change in Perceptual and Brain Activation Response to Visceral Stimuli in Irritable Bowel Syndrome Patients. Gastroenterology. 2006;131:352–65. doi: 10.1053/j.gastro.2006.05.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Bar KJ, Wagner G, Koschke M, Boettger S, Boettger MK, Schlosser R, Sauer H. Increased prefrontal activation during pain perception in major depression. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62:1281–7. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.02.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Strigo IA, Simmons AN, Matthews SC, Craig AD, Paulus MP. Association of major depressive disorder with altered functional brain response during anticipation and processing of heat pain. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65:1275–84. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.11.1275. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Hagelberg N, Forssell H, Aalto S, Rinne JO, Scheinin H, Taiminen T, Nagren K, Eskola O, Jaaskelainen SK. Altered dopamine D2 receptor binding in atypical facial pain. Pain. 2003;106:43–8. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(03)00275-6. References. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Wood PB, Schweinhardt P, Jaeger E, Dagher A, Hakyemez H, Rabiner EA, Bushnell MC, Chizh BA. Fibromyalgia patients show an abnormal dopamine response to pain. Eur J Neurosci. 2007;25:3576–82. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05623.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Wood PB, Patterson JC, II, Sunderland JJ, Tainter KH, Glabus MF, Lilien DL. Reduced presynaptic dopamine activity in fibromyalgia syndrome demonstrated with positron emission tomography: a pilot study. J Pain. 2007;8:51–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2006.05.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Wood PB, Glabus MF, Simpson R, Patterson IJC. Changes in Gray Matter Density in Fibromyalgia: Correlation With Dopamine Metabolism. Journal of Pain. 2009;10:609–18. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2008.12.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Harris RE, Clauw DJ, Scott DJ, McLean SA, Gracely RH, Zubieta JK. Decreased central mu-opioid receptor availability in fibromyalgia. J Neurosci. 2007;27:10000–6. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2849-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Saletu B, Prause W, Anderer P, Mandl M, Aigner M, Mikova O, Saletu-Zyhlarz GM. Insomnia in somatoform pain disorder: sleep laboratory studies on differences to controls and acute effects of trazodone, evaluated by the Somnolyzer 24 × 7 and the Siesta database. Neuropsychobiology. 2005;51:148–63. doi: 10.1159/000085207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Chervin RD, Teodorescu M, Kushwaha R, Deline AM, Brucksch CB, Ribbens-Grimm C, Ruzicka DL, Stein PK, Clauw DJ, Crofford LJ. Objective measures of disordered sleep in fibromyalgia. Journal of Rheumatology. 2009;36:2009–16. doi: 10.3899/jrheum.090051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Anch AM, Lue FA, MacLean AW, Moldofsky H. Sleep physiology and psychological aspects of the fibrositis (fibromyalgia) syndrome. Can J Psychol. 1991;45:179–84. doi: 10.1037/h0084280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Doherty M, Smith J. Elusive ‘alpha-delta’ sleep in fibromyalgia and osteoarthritis. Annals of the Rheumatic Diseases. 1993;52:245. doi: 10.1136/ard.52.3.245-b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Drewes AM, Nielsen KD, Taagholt SJ, Bjerregard K, Svendsen L, Gade J. Sleep intensity in fibromyalgia: focus on the microstructure of the sleep process. Br J Rheumatol. 1995;34:629–35. doi: 10.1093/rheumatology/34.7.629. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Horne JA, Shackell BS. Alpha-like EEG activity in non-REM sleep and the fibromyalgia (fibrositis) syndrome. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 1991;79:271–6. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(91)90122-k. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Landis CA, Lentz MJ, Rothermel J, Buchwald D, Shaver JLF. Decreased sleep spindles and spindle activity in midlife women with fibromyalgia and pain. Sleep. 2004;27:741–50. doi: 10.1093/sleep/27.4.741. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Manu P, Lane TJ, Matthews DA, Castriotta RJ, Watson RK, Abeles M. Alpha-delta sleep in patients with a chief complaint of chronic fatigue. Southern Medical Journal. 1994;87:465–70. doi: 10.1097/00007611-199404000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Moldofsky H, Scarisbrick P, England R, Smythe H. Musculosketal symptoms and non-REM sleep disturbance in patients with “fibrositis syndrome” and healthy subjects. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1975;37:341–51. doi: 10.1097/00006842-197507000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Roizenblatt S, Moldofsky H, Benedito-Silva AA, Tufik S. Alpha sleep characteristics in fibromyalgia. Arthritis & Rheumatism. 2001;44:222–30. doi: 10.1002/1529-0131(200101)44:1<222::AID-ANR29>3.0.CO;2-K. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Saskin P, Moldofsky H, Lue FA. Sleep and posttraumatic rheumatic pain modulation disorder (fibrositis syndrome) Psychosomatic Medicine. 1986;48:319–23. doi: 10.1097/00006842-198605000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Nomura T, Fukudo S, Matsuoka H, Hongo M. Abnormal electroencephalogram in irritable bowel syndrome. Scandinavian Journal of Gastroenterology. 1999;34:478–84. doi: 10.1080/003655299750026209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Perlis ML, Giles DE, Bootzin RR, Dikman ZV, Fleming GM, Drummond SP, Rose MW. Alpha sleep and information processing, perception of sleep, pain, and arousability in fibromyalgia. International Journal of Neuroscience. 1997;89:265–80. doi: 10.3109/00207459708988479. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.May A. Chronic pain may change the structure of the brain. Pain. 2008;137:7–15. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2008.02.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Lutz J, Jager L, De QD, Krauseneck T, Padberg F, Wichnalek M, Beyer A, Stahl R, Zirngibl B, Morhard D, Reiser M, Schelling G. White and gray matter abnormalities in the brain of patients with fibromyalgia: A diffusion-tensor and volumetric imaging study. Arthritis Rheum. 2008;58:3960–9. doi: 10.1002/art.24070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Valet M, Gundel H, Sprenger T, Sorg C, Muhlau M, Zimmer C, Henningsen P, Tolle TR. Patients with pain disorder show gray-matter loss in pain-processing structures: A voxel-based morphometric study. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2009;71:49–56. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31818d1e02. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Kuchinad A, Schweinhardt P, Seminowicz DA, Wood PB, Chizh BA, Bushnell MC. Accelerated brain gray matter loss in fibromyalgia patients: Premature aging of the brain? J Neurosci. 2007;27:4004–7. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0098-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Schmidt-Wilcke T, Luerding R, Weigand T, Jurgens T, Schuierer G, Leinisch E, Bogdahn U. Striatal grey matter increase in patients suffering from fibromyalgia - A voxel-based morphometry study. Pain. 2007;132:S109–S16. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2007.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Hsu MC, Harris RE, Sundgren PC, Welsh RC, Fernandes CR, Clauw DJ, Williams DA. No consistent difference in gray matter volume between individuals with fibromyalgia and age-matched healthy subjects when controlling for affective disorder. Pain. 2009;143:262–7. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2009.03.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Davis KD, Pope G, Chen J, Kwan CL, Crawley AP, Diamant NE. Cortical thinning in IBS: Implications for homeostatic, attention, and pain processing. Neurology. 2008 Jan;70(2):154. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000295509.30630.10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Luerding R, Weigand T, Bogdahn U, Schmidt-Wilcke T. Working memory performance is correlated with local brain morphology in the medial frontal and anterior cingulate cortex in fibromyalgia patients: structural correlates of pain-cognition interaction. Brain. 2008;131:12–31. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Vuilleumier P. Hysterical conversion and brain function. Prog Brain Res. 2005;150:309–29. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(05)50023-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Halligan PW, Bass C, Marshall JC. Contemporary Approaches to the Study of Hysteria: Clinical and Theoretical Perspectives. Oxford Univeristy Press; Oxford: 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 70.Meyer BU, Britton TC, Benecke R, Bischoff C, Machetanz J, Conrad B. Motor responses evoked by magnetic brain stimulation in psychogenic limb weakness: diagnostic value and limitations. J Neurol. 1992;239:251–5. doi: 10.1007/BF00810346. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Hoechstetter K, Meinck HM, Henningsen P, Scherg M, Rupp A. Psychogenic sensory loss: magnetic source imaging reveals normal tactile evoked activity of the human primary and secondary somatosensory cortex. Neurosci Lett. 2002;323:137–40. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(02)00130-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Fukuda M, Hata A, Niwa S, Hiramatsu K, Yokokoji M, Hayashida S, Itoh K, Nakagome K, Iwanami A. Event-related potential correlates of functional hearing loss: reduced P3 amplitude with preserved N1 and N2 components in a unilateral case. Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. 1996;50:85–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.1996.tb01668.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Atmaca M, Aydin A, Tezcan E, Poyraz AK, Kara B. Volumetric investigation of brain regions in patients with conversion disorder. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2006;30:708–13. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2006.01.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Greco A, Tannock C, Brostoff J, Costa DC. Brain MR in chronic fatigue syndrome. American Journal of Neuroradiology. 1997;18:1265–9. 1997. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Cope H, Pernet A, Kendall B, David A. Cognitive functioning and magnetic resonance imaging in chronic fatigue. Br J Psychiatry. 1995;167:86–94. doi: 10.1192/bjp.167.1.86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Lange G, DeLuca J, Maldjian JA, Lee H, Tiersky LA, Natelson BH. Brain MRI abnormalities exist in a subset of patients with chronic fatigue syndrome. Journal of Neurological Science. 1999;171:3–7. doi: 10.1016/s0022-510x(99)00243-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Okada T, Tanaka M, Kuratsune H, Watanabe Y, Sadato N. Mechanisms underlying fatigue: a voxel-based morphometric study of chronic fatigue syndrome. Biomed Central Neurology. 2004;4:14. doi: 10.1186/1471-2377-4-14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.de Lange FP, Kalkman JS, Bleijenberg G, Hagoort P, van der Meer JW, Toni I. Gray matter volume reduction in the chronic fatigue syndrome. Neuroimage. 2005;26:777–81. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.de Lange FP, Koers A, Kalkman JS, Bleijenberg G, Hagoort P, van der Meer JW, Toni I. Increase in prefrontal cortical volume following cognitive behavioural therapy in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome. Brain. 2008;131:2172–80. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Costa DC, Tannock C, Brostoff J. Brainstem perfusion is impaired in chronic fatigue syndrome. Quarterly Journal of Medicine. 1995;88:767–73. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Tirelli U, Chierichetti F, Tavio M, Simonelli C, Bianchin G, Zanco P, Ferlin G. Brain positron emission tomography (PET) in chronic fatigue syndrome: preliminary data. Am J Med. 1998;105:54S–8S. doi: 10.1016/s0002-9343(98)00179-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Siessmeier T, Nix WA, Hardt J, Schreckenberger M, Egle UT, Bartenstein P. Observer independent analysis of cerebral glucose metabolism in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2003;74:922–8. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.74.7.922. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Schmaling KB, Lewis DH, Fiedelak JI, Mahurin R, Buchwald DS. Single-photon emission computerized tomography and neurocognitive function in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome. Psychosom Med. 2003;65 doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000038942.33335.9b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Lange G, Steffener J, Cook DB, Bly BM, Christodoulou C, Liu WC, Deluca J, Natelson BH. Objective evidence of cognitive complaints in Chronic Fatigue Syndrome: a BOLD fMRI study of verbal working memory. Neuroimage. 2005;26:513–24. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Caseras X, Mataix-Cols D, Giampietro V, Rimes KA, Brammer M, Zelaya F, Chalder T, Godfrey EL. Probing the working memory system in chronic fatigue syndrome: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study using the n-back task. Psychosom Med. 2006;68:947–55. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000242770.50979.5f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Cook DB, O’Connor PJ, Lange G, Steffener J. Functional neuroimaging correlates of mental fatigue induced by cognition among chronic fatigue syndrome patients and controls. Neuroimage. 2007;36:108–22. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Caseras X, Mataix-Cols D, Rimes KA, Giampietro V, Brammer M, Zelaya F, Chalder T, Godfrey E. The neural correlates of fatigue: an exploratory imaginal fatigue provocation study in chronic fatigue syndrome. Psychol Med. 2008;38:941–51. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708003450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.de Lange FP, Kalkman JS, Bleijenberg G, Hagoort P, van der Werf SP, van der Meer JW, Toni I. Neural correlates of the chronic fatigue syndrome--an fMRI study. Brain. 2004;127:1948–57. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Cleare AJ, Messa C, Rabiner EA, Grasby PM. Brain 5-HT1A receptor binding in chronic fatigue syndrome measured using positron emission tomography and [11C]WAY-100635. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57:239–46. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.10.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Decker MJ, Tabassum H, Lin JM, Reeves WC. Electroencephalographic correlates of Chronic Fatigue Syndrome. Behav Brain Funct. 2009;5:43. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-5-43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Armitage R, Landis C, Hoffmann R, Lentz M, Watson N, Goldberg J, Buchwald D. Power spectral analysis of sleep EEG in twins discordant for chronic fatigue syndrome. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2009;66:51–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2008.08.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Guilleminault C, Poyares D, Rosa Ad, Kirisoglu C, Almeida T, Lopes MC. Chronic fatigue, unrefreshing sleep and nocturnal polysomnography. Sleep Medicine. 2006;7:513–20. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2006.03.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Van Hoof E, De Becker P, Lapp C, Cluydts R, De Meirleir K. Defining the occurrence and influence of alpha-delta sleep in chronic fatigue syndrome. American Journal of the Medical Sciences. 2007;333:78–84. doi: 10.1097/00000441-200702000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Hakala M, Vahlberg T, Niemi PM, Karlsson H. Brain glucose metabolism and temperament in relation to severe somatization. Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. 2006;60:669–75. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2006.01581.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Garcia-Campayo J, Sanz-Carrillo C, Baringo T, Ceballos C. SPECT scan in somatization disorder patients: an exploratory study of eleven cases. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2001;35:359–63. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1614.2001.00909.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Hakala M, Karlsson H, Ruotsalainen U, Koponen S, Bergman J, Stenman H, Kelavuori JP, Aalto S, Kurki T, Niemi P. Severe somatization in women is associated with altered cerebral glucose metabolism. Psychol Med. 2002;32:1379–85. doi: 10.1017/s0033291702006578. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Hakala M, Karlsson H, Kurki T, Aalto S, Koponen S, Vahlberg T, Niemi PM. Volumes of the caudate nuclei in women with somatization disorder and healthy women. Psychiatry Res. 2004;131:71–8. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2004.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Rauch SL, Phillips KA, Segal E, Makris N, Shin LM, Whalen PJ, Jenike MA, Caviness VS, Kennedy DN. A preliminary morphometric magnetic resonance imaging study of regional brain volumes in body dysmorphic disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2003;122:13–9. doi: 10.1016/s0925-4927(02)00117-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Feusner JD, Townsend J, Bystritsky A, McKinley M, Moller H, Bookheimer S. Regional brain volumes and symptom severity in body dysmorphic disorder. Psychiatry Research - Neuroimaging. 2009;172:161–7. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.12.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Feusner JD, Townsend J, Bystritsky A, Bookheimer S. Visual information processing of faces in body dysmorphic disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:1417–25. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.12.1417. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.van Praag H, Kempermann G, Gage FH. Neural consequences of environmental enrichment. Nature Review Neuroscience. 2000;1:191–8. doi: 10.1038/35044558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Corlett PR, Aitken MR, Dickinson A, Shanks DR, Honey GD, Honey RA, Robbins TW, Bullmore ET, Fletcher PC. Prediction error during retrospective revaluation of causal associations in humans: fMRI evidence in favor of an associative model of learning. Neuron. 2004;44:877–88. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.11.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Pizzagalli DA, Jahn AL, O’Shea JP. Toward an objective characterization of an anhedonic phenotype: a signal-detection approach. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57:319–27. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Fu CH, Williams SC, Brammer MJ, Suckling J, Kim J, Cleare AJ, Walsh ND, Mitterschiffthaler MT, Andrew CM, Pich EM, Bullmore ET. Neural responses to happy facial expressions in major depression following antidepressant treatment. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164:599–607. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.4.599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Derbyshire SWG, Jones AKP, Devani P, Friston KJ, Feinmann C, Harris M, Pearce S, Watson JDG, Frackowiak RSJ. Cerebral responses to pain in patients with atypical facial pain measured by positron emission tomography. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1994;57:1166–72. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.57.10.1166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Mountz JM, Bradley LA, Modell JG, Alexander RW, Triana-Alexander M, Aaron LA, Stewart KE, Alarcon GS, Mountz JD. Fibromyalgia in women: abnormalities of regional cerebral blood flow in the thalamus and the caudate nucleus are associated with low pain threshold levels. Arthritis and Rheumatisim. 1995;38:926–38. doi: 10.1002/art.1780380708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Silverman DH, Munakata JA, Ennes H, Mandelkern MA, Hoh CK, Mayer EA. Regional cerebral activity in normal and pathological perception of visceral pain. Gastroenterology. 1997;112:64–72. doi: 10.1016/s0016-5085(97)70220-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Mertz H, Morgan V, Tanner G, Pickens D, Price R, Shyr Y, Kessler R. Regional cerebral activation in irritable bowel syndrome and control subjects with painful and nonpainful rectal distention. Gastroenterology. 2000;118:842–8. doi: 10.1016/s0016-5085(00)70170-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Naliboff BD, Derbyshire SW, Munakata J, Berman S, Mandelkern M, Chang L, Mayer EA. Cerebral activation in patients with irritable bowel syndrome and control subjects during rectosigmoid stimulation. Psychosom Med. 2001;63:365–75. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200105000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Derbyshire SWG, Jones AKP, Creed F, Starz T, Meltzer CC, Townsend DW, Peterson AM, Firestone L. Cerebral responses to noxious thermal stimulation in chronic low back pain patients and normal controls. Neuroimage. 2002;16:158–68. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Chang L, Berman S, Mayer EA, Suyenobu B, Derbyshire S, Naliboff B, Vogt B, FitzGerald L, Mandelkern MA. Brain responses to visceral and somatic stimuli in patients with irritable bowel syndrome with and without fibromyalgia. Am J Gastroenterol. 2003;98:1354–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1572-0241.2003.07478.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Verne GN, Himes NC, Robinson ME, Gopinath KS, Briggs RW, Crosson B, Price DD. Central representation of visceral and cutaneous hypersensitivity in the irritable bowel syndrome. Pain. 2003;103:99–110. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(02)00416-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Giesecke T, Gracely RH, Grant MA, Nachemson A, Petzke F, Williams DA, Clauw DJ. Evidence of augmented central pain processing in idiopathic chronic low back pain. Arthritis and Rheumatisim. 2004;50:613–23. doi: 10.1002/art.20063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Kwan CL, Diamant NE, Pope G, Mikula K, Mikulis DJ, Davis KD. Abnormal forebrain activity in functional bowel disorder patients with chronic pain. Neurology. 2005;65:1268–77. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000180971.95473.cc. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Andresen V, Bach DR, Poellinger A, Tsrouya C, Stroh A, Foerschler A, Georgiewa P, Zimmer C, Monnikes H. Brain activation responses to subliminal or supraliminal rectal stimuli and to auditory stimuli in irritable bowel syndrome. Neurogastroenterol Motil. 2005;17:827–37. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2982.2005.00720.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Mayer EA, Berman S, Suyenobu B, Labus J, Mandelkern MA, Naliboff BD, Chang L. Differences in brain responses to visceral pain between patients with irritable bowel syndrome and ulcerative colitis. Pain. 2005;115:398–409. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2005.03.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Lackner JM, Lou CM, Mertz HR, Wack DS, Katz LA, Krasner SS, Firth R, Mahl TC, Lockwood AH. Cognitive therapy for irritable bowel syndrome is associated with reduced limbic activity, GI symptoms, and anxiety. Behav Res Ther. 2006;44:621–38. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.05.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Stoeter P, Bauermann T, Nickel R, Corluka L, Gawehn J, Vucurevic G, Vossel G, Egle UT. Cerebral activation in patients with somatoform pain disorder exposed to pain and stress: an fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2007;36:418–30. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.01.052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Hillert L, Musabasic V, Berglund H, Ciumas C, Savic I. Odor processing in multiple chemical sensitivity. Hum Brain Mapp. 2007;28:172–82. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20266. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Gündel H, Valet M, Sorg C, Huber D, Zimmer C, Sprenger T, Tölle TR. Altered cerebral response to noxious heat stimulation in patients with somatoform pain disorder. Pain. 2008;137:413–21. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2007.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Berman SM, Naliboff BD, Suyenobu B, Labus JS, Stains J, Ohning G, Kilpatrick L, Bueller JA, Ruby K, Jarcho J, Mayer EA. Reduced brainstem inhibition during anticipated pelvic visceral pain correlates with enhanced brain response to the visceral stimulus in women with irritable bowel syndrome. J Neurosci. 2008;28:349–59. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2500-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Burgmer M, Pogatzki-Zahn E, Gaubitz M, Wessoleck E, Heuft G, Pfleiderer B. Altered brain activity during pain processing in fibromyalgia. Neuroimage. 2009;44:502–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Jensen KB, Kosek E, Petzke F, Carville S, Fransson P, Marcus H, Williams SCR, Choy E, Giesecke T, Mainguy Y, Gracely R, Ingvar M. Evidence of dysfunctional pain inhibition in Fibromyalgia reflected in rACC during provoked pain. Pain. 2009;144:95–100. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2009.03.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Pujol J, Lopez-Sola M, Ortiz H, Vilanova JC, Harrison BJ, Yucel M, Soriano-Mas C, Cardoner N, Deus J. Mapping brain response to pain in fibromyalgia patients using temporal analysis of FMRI. PLoS ONE. 2009;4:e5224. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005224. Electronic Resource. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Bernstein CN, Frankenstein UN, Rawsthorne P, Pitz M, Summers R, McIntyre MC. Cortical mapping of visceral pain in patients with GI disorders using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Am J Gastroenterol. 2002;97:319–27. doi: 10.1111/j.1572-0241.2002.05464.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Marshall JC, Halligan PW, Fink GR, Wade DT, Frackowiak RSJ. The functional anatomy of a hysterical paralysis. Cognition. 1997;64:B1–B8. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(97)00020-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Yazici KM, Kostakoglu L. Cerebral blood flow changes in patients with conversion disorder. Psychiatry Res. 1998;83:163–8. doi: 10.1016/s0925-4927(98)00039-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Spence SA, Crimlisk HL, Cope H, Ron MA, Grasby PM. Discrete neurophysiological correlates in prefrontal cortex during hysterical and feigned disorder of movement. Lancet. 2000;355:1243–4. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)02096-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Burgmer M, Konrad C, Jansen A, Kugel H, Sommer J, Heindel W, Ringelstein EB, Heuft G, Knecht S. Abnormal brain activation during movement observation in patients with conversion paralysis. Neuroimage. 2006;67:2036–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Stone J, Zeman A, Simonotto E, Meyer M, Azuma R, Flett S, Sharpe M. FMRI in patients with motor conversion symptoms and controls with simulated weakness. Psychosom Med. 2007;69:961–9. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31815b6c14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.de Lange FP, Roelofs K, Toni I. Increased self-monitoring during imagined movements in conversion paralysis. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:2051–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132.de Lange FP, Roelofs K, Toni I. Motor imagery: a window into the mechanisms and alterations of the motor system. Cortex. 2008;44:494–506. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2007.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133.Cojan Y, Waber L, Carruzzo A, Vuilleumier P. Motor inhibition in hysterical conversion paralysis. Neuroimage. 2009;47:1026–37. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134.Tiihonen J, Kuikka J, Viinamaki H, Lehtonen J, Partanen J. Altered cerebral blood flow during hysterical paresthesia. Biol Psychiatry. 1995;37:134–5. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(94)00230-Z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Vuilleumier P, Chicherio C, Assal F, Schwartz S, Skusman D, Landis T. Functional neuroanatomical correlates of hysterical sensorimotor loss. Brain. 2001;124:1077–90. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.6.1077. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136.Mailis-Gagnon A, Giannoylis I, Downar J, Kwan CL, Mikulis DJ, Crawley AP, Nicholson K, Davis KD. Altered central somatosensory processing in chronic pain patients with “hysterical” anesthesia. Neurology. 2003;60:1501–7. doi: 10.1212/wnl.60.9.1501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137.Werring DJ, Weston L, Bullmore ET, Plant GT, Ron MA. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of the cerebral response to visual stimulation in medically unexplained visual loss. Psychol Med. 2004;34:583–9. doi: 10.1017/S0033291703008985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 138.Ghaffar O, Staines WR, Feinstein A. Unexplained neurologic symptoms:an fMRI study of sensory conversion disorder. Neurology. 2006;67:2036–8. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000247275.68402.fc. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.