Abstract
Students were taught an algorithm for solving a new class of mathematical problems. Occasionally in the sequence of problems they encountered exception problems that required that they extend the algorithm. Regular and exception problems were associated with different patterns of brain activation produced. Some regions showed a Cognitive pattern of being active only until the problem was solved and no difference between regular or exception problems. Other regions showed a Metacognitive pattern of greater activity for exception problems and activity that extended into the post-solution period, particularly when an error was made. The Cognitive regions included some of parietal and prefrontal regions associated with the triple-code theory of Dehaene et al (2003) and associated with algebra equation solving in the ACT-R theory (Anderson, 2005). Metacognitive regions included the superior prefrontal gyrus, the angular gyrus of the triple-code theory, and frontopolar regions.
This research is concerned with identifying brain regions that are engaged when students have to deploy their mathematical knowledge in non-routine versus routine ways. Our goal is to acquire information that will guide development of a model of metacognitive activity in mathematical problem solving. We will both exmine predefined brain regions (see Figure 1) that past theory has related to routine mathematical tasks and identify additional regions that are engaged by the non-routine tasks in this research.
Considerable research has studied the neural basis of what might be characterized as routine use of mathematical knowledge. The greatest amount of research has gone into understanding the role of various parietal regions in arithmetic tasks (e.g., Castelli et al., 2006; Eger et al., 2003; Isaacs et al., 2001; Molko et al., 2003; Naccache & Dehaene, 2001; Piazza et al., 2004; Pinel et al., 2004). The triple-code theory (e.g., Dehaene & Cohen, 1997) proposes that basic numerical knowledge is distributed over three brain regions that code for different aspects of number knowledge: the horizontal intraparietal sulcus (HIPS) that processes numerical quantities, a left perisylvian language network that is involved in the verbal processing of numbers, and a ventral occipital-parietal region that processes visual representations of digits. In related work, Dehaene et al (2003) identified three parietal regions that will be of interest to us. In addition to HIPS, there is the angular gyrus (ANG) that is part of the perisylvian language network, and the posterior superior parietal lobule (PSPL, not part of the original triple-code theory) that supports attentional orientation on the mental number line and other spatial processing.
The prefrontal cortex is also involved in mathematical performance. Broca’s area is part of the perisylvian language network identified in the triple-code theory. There is a region of the lateral inferior prefrontal cortex (LIPFC) that is particularly involved in more advanced tasks involving topics like algebra, geometry, or calculus (e.g., Krueger et al., 2008; Qin et al., 2004; Ravizza et al., 2008; Sohn et al., 2004). It also appears to play a key role in retrieval of arithmetic facts and semantic facts (Danker & Anderson, 2007; Menon, et al., 2000).
We have developed ACT-R theory (Anderson et al., 2004; Anderson, 2007) of equation solving (Anderson, 2005; Ravizza et al., 2008) and mental multiplication (Rosenberg-Lee et al., 2009). These models particularly emphasize the contribution of the LIPFC and a region of the posterior parietal cortex (PPC), which is about 2 centimeters away from each of the three parietal regions (HIPS, PSPL, and ANG) identified by Dehaene et al (2003). This region is activated when mental representations are being manipulated (e.g., Carpenter et al., 1999; Zacks et al., 2002). In a variety of experiments studying tasks like algebra equation solving and geometry proof generation (see Anderson, 2007, for a review), activity in the PPC proves to be the best correlate of problem complexity, while activity in the LIPFC proves to be the best correlate of student proficiency. According to the ACT-R theory the connection between complexity and PPC holds because its activity reflects how much the mental representation of the problem is manipulated in solving iti. The connection between proficiency and LIPFC holds because its activity reflects amount of declarative retrieval, which decreases as students develop a procedural mastery of a new algorithm and drop out the need for retrieval of tasks instructions.
The predefined regions that we will work with (Figure 1) are those that have been localized in Dehaene et al (2003) or the ACT-R algebra studies. The predefined regions for the triple-code theory were of similar size as the ACT-R region and were centered at the coordinates reported in Dehaene et al (2003) and Cohen et al. (2008). Rosenberg-Lee et al (2009) compared the activity in HIPS, PSPL, ANG, PPC, and LIPFC in mental multi-digit multiplication. They found that, while there were differences, all four of the LIPFC, PPC, HIPS, and PSPL showed strong engagement that increased with task difficulty. In contrast, ANG was deactivated and was not affected by condition. As in all of our research on algebra, the left hemisphere LIPFC, PPC, HIPS, and PSPL gave stronger responses than their right hemisphere homologs although the right gave similar responses. The left ANG is often activated in language tasks, and the triple-code theory only concerns left ANG (in contrast to HIPS and PSPL which are bilateral). Rosenberg-Lee et al did not find differential response between left and right ANG. Inspection of algebra data from our laboratory (e.g., Anderson et al., 2003) confirms the result of deactivation in the region of the angular gyrus and no differential left-right response.
The common feature of most of these math tasks studied is that they involve participants executing a known algorithm to find a result. However, all use of mathematics is not algorithmic and it is often necessary to adapt ones mathematical knowledge to novel situations. Success in solving novel problems involves the metacognitive processes of monitoring and reflecting on the problem solving process. Metacognition has been a topic of interest in research on mathematics education, in particular if and how students monitor their problem solving, adapt their solution methods, and check their solutions for whether they make sense (e.g., Schoenfeld, 1987).
This paper will study how the regions in Figure 1 respond to routine problems and non-routine problems that require modifying solution methods. We will also see if other regions are engaged by our non-routine problems. Given that the regions in Figure 1 were identified in studies of routine mathematical activities, we suspect they will be again engaged by our routine problems. There is some reason to suspect that the ANG may also be engaged to serve the metacognitive activities of monitoring and reflecting. Regions close to the right ANG have been found to play a variety of metacognitive functions (Decety & Lamm, 2007, for a review), although there are a number of other ways of characterizing the function of the right temporoparietal junction (e.g., Legrande & Ruby, 2009).
Another region that is potentially involved in metacognitive function is Brodmann Area 10 or frontopolar cortex (FPC), particularly its lateral portion (Christoff & Gabrieli, 2000: Fletcher and Henson, 2001). A number of converging lines of research suggest that this region of the brain may be critical in the ability to extend knowledge. Ramani & Owen (2004) stress that this region is unique in being exclusively connected only with supramodal regions of the cortex. They suggest that this region is involved in the integration of information from multiple regions. There are numerous other theories of the role of this region, all of which have elements of metacognition – such as monitoring of memory retrieval (e.g., Buckner, 2003; Rugg et al., 2003), branching (Koechlin & Hyafil, 2007), shifting between external and internal processes (Burgess et al., 2006), relational integration (Christoff et al., 2001), and after-task processing in memory experiments (e.g., Johnson et al. 2005; Raye et al. 2007; Reynolds et al., 2006).
In an effort to contrast cognitive and metacognitive processes in mathematics we developed pyramid problems. Table 1 reproduces the instruction provided to participants for solving these problems. These problems require only middle-school mathematics to solve. As students work with pyramid problems they quickly master the algorithm for solving these problems. Nonetheless, students can still be placed in situations that require they extend this knowledge and most students can do so with at least some success.
Table 1.
There is a notation for writing repeated addition where each term added is one less than the previous: |
For instance is written as 4 + 3 + 2 = 4$3 |
Since 4 + 3 + 2 = 9 we would evaluate 4$3 as 9 and write 4$3 = 9. The parts of 4 $ 3 are given names: |
4 is the base and reflects the number you start with |
3 is the height and reflects the total number of items you add, including the base |
4 $ 3 is called a pyramid |
You will see a variety of problems in which you will need to solve for the variable. Here are three examples: |
4$3 = x you are to type "9" the answer |
x$3=9 you are to type "4" the base |
4$x=9 you are to type "3" the height |
Anderson (2007) reported an initial behavioral study with these problems. Students had little difficulty solving regular versions of these problems in the three forms:
8$4 = x (solve-for-value)
7$x = 18 (solve-for-height)
x$4 = 22 (solve-for-base)
Students initially solve both solve-for-value and solve-for-height problems by performing repeated addition. In the solve-for-value case, students keep adding until the number of additions equals the height while in the solve-for-height case they keep adding until the sum equals the value. The solve-for-base problems cannot be dealt with so simply because there is no base to start the addition. Most students start by using a guess-and-check strategy – guessing a base, checking that, and guessing again if necessary. Their initial guesses are close (for instance, 6 for x$4 = 22) and they sensibly adjust wrong guesses (e.g. since 6 produces too small an answer for x$4 = 22, they try 7 -- which they often do not even test but just assume will be correct).
The experiment to be reported here contrasts students solving regular problems like the above with exception problems. Regular problems are characterized by involving only small positive integer values for base and height with a single unknown. Exception problems involve going outside these constraints. There are a substantial variety of exception problems and each requires the student to think about how to extend their understanding of pyramid problems. We use four types of exception problems – problems involving fractions for bases, problems involving negative numbers for bases or heights, problems involving large numbers for bases or heights, and problems involving a repetition of the variable as in x$x = 15.
We collected verbal protocols from pilot students solving these problems. The following verbal protocol of one student solving 6$5 illustrates the solution of a regular problem:
“Six plus four plus three plus two – four, fifteen, three, eighteen plus two is 20” That is, the student basically gives a partial record of his calculations. The student said nothing further upon receiving confirmatory feedback and just went on to the next problem. In contrast, consider the protocol of the same student as he successfully solved −9$4:
“So wait it's one less so it's minus ten…minus nine plus minus ten plus minus eleven is three and then negative… minus 42”
In this case, in addition to the record of calculations there is an initial recognition of difficulty. Also after solving the problem the student continues to think about it even though he got it right:
“You need to explain the rule”
Consider another student on the X$4=X which he solved incorrectly entering −2 rather than 2:
“different… Oh, that’s interesting…x, 3x +6”
and after the feedback:
“no, oh 2, 2, 2! Shit!”
Two metacognitive features show up in the protocols for exception problems, both of which are intuitively reasonable – an early recognition that this was a different kind of problem and various assessment remarks after the solution. We expected that these exception problems would engage brain regions in addition to those engaged for regular problems and that this engagement would extend after the problem was solved. This research will contrast regions engaged during execution of a routine procedure with regions engaged when the problem involved rethinking the existing procedure. We will call these Cognitive and Metacognitive regions. The Cognitive regions might be somewhat more active in solving exception problems than routine problems because exception problems tend to involve somewhat more complex computations, but their activity should drop after solving the problem. In contrast, Metacognitive regions should show much greater activation for exception problems and maintain that activation into after-task reflection. Less than 15% of all problems were exceptions and they were quite varied so that they would continue to engage metacognitive activities throughout the experiment.
Our research will contrast regions that reflect a Cognitive or a Metacognitive pattern of activity. As Figure 2 illustrates, participants are given feedback after their response and time to consider this feedback. Letting “Before” denote activity before the answer is generated and “After” the activity after the answer is generated (so, for instance, Before-Regular denotes activity for regular problems before the answer is generated) we expect one of the following two orderings of the conditions:
Cognitive Pattern: Before-Regular > After-Exception – Cognitive regions should cease engagement upon solution of a problem and, even if Exception problems involve somewhat more computation, there should be relatively little activation after the solution of the problem.
Metacognitive Pattern: After-Exception > Before-Regular – Exception problems should evoke reflection after as well as during the solution of the problem. In contrast, regular problems should involve little reflection at any point.
Thus, the contrast between After-Exception and Before-Regular offers a simple diagnostic of whether a region is Cognitive or Metacognitive. We will restrict this contrast to correctly solved problems to avoid any complications that error processing might add to the analysis, but we will also look at activity on error trials.
Methods
Participants
Twenty right-handed participants (8 female and 12 male, 18–31 years of age, mean = 23.8) were recruited from subject population at Carnegie Mellon University. Each participant took part in one fMRI scan that lasted approximately 90 minutes.
Procedure
Participants responded by using a mouse to select numbers from a numeric keypad on the screen. They were given practice on using the keypad when in the scanner. A scanning session consisted of 7 blocks. Each block involved a series of problems presented according the procedure in Figure 2. The first block was a warm-up block that involved 18 regular problems (i.e., small positive numbers solving for the value, base, and height) and was presented during the structural image acquisitions. The subsequent 6 blocks occurred during the functional scanning. Each block contained a mixture of 21 problems. The first problem was always a regular problem for warm-up purposes and was not analyzed. The remaining problems were a randomly ordered sequence of the following problem types:
Regular: Twelve problems were created using small positive integers for base (4 to 9) and height (2 to 5). These consisted of 4 problems each that required solving for base, solving for height, and solving for value.
- Exception: Four problems consisted of one each of the following:
- A problem with a large base (in the hundreds) or large exponent (in the teens or twenties) (e.g., 151$2 = X or 8$21=X),
- A problem with a fractional base (e.g. 5 ½ $4=X),
- A problem with a negative bases or height (e.g., −9$3=X, 9$-3 = X),
- A problem with a repetition of X in two positions (e.g., X$4=X, X$X=21).
- Over the course of the 6 blocks, two problems in categories (a) – (c) would involve solving for base, solving for height, and solving for value. For category (d), the variable appeared twice each pair of positions. These subcategories within the 4 exception categories were randomly ordered over the blocks.
Font: To have regular problems that would be surprising like exception problems but not pose a cognitive challenge we created four very odd fonts and presented regular problems in these fonts and with different colors other than the normal black. Over the course of the experiment each font-color choice would be tested twice solving for base, height, and value. These problems proved indistinguishable from regular problems in their behavioral and imaging profiles in the predefined regions (Figure 1) and were collapsed with regular problems in all reported analyses.
fMRI Analysis
Images were acquired using gradient echo-echo planar image (EPI) acquisition on a Siemens 3T Allegra Scanner using a standard RF head coil (quadrature birdcage), with 2 s. repetition time (TR), 30 ms. echo time (TE), 70° flip angle, and 20 cm. field of view (FOV). The experiment acquired 34 axial slices on each TR using a 3.2 mm thick, 64×64 matrix. This produces voxels 3.2 mm high and 3.125 × 3.125 mm2. The anterior commissure-posterior commissure (AC-PC) line was on the 11th slice from the bottom scan slice. Acquired images were analyzed using the NIS system. Functional images were motion-corrected using 6-parameter 3D registration (AIR; Woods et al., 1998). All images were then co-registered to a common reference structural MRI by means of a 12-parameter 3D registration and smoothed with an 6 mm full-width-half-maximum 3D Gaussian filter to accommodate individual differences in anatomy.
All of our analysis will be on percent change in the BOLD response from the first scan of the 4-second fixation period. To test the predictions outlined in the introduction we extracted an estimate of the engagement of these regions before and after the response. To separate these two estimates from any activity that occurred during the actual response generation, we used three regressors – one for the variable period up to 2 seconds before response completion, one for the 2 seconds up to response completion, and one for the 8 seconds after response completion. These regressors were created by taking boxcar functions for the three periods and convolving them with a hemodynamic response function. We used the standard hemodynamic function in the ACT-R model (Anderson et al., 2008), a gamma function with an index parameter of 6 and a scale parameter of 0.75 secondsii. Three such regressors were entered into a design matrix (Friston, 2006) for each of five conditions (regular versus exception crossed with correct versus error, plus a fifth condition to deal with cases where participants times out and never generated a response). Finally a set of regressors for a quadratic function was added for each block to extract any general trends. Ignoring the time outs and block trends, this yielded 12 beta weights for each participant – the 4 conditions crossed with the three intervals. Analyses (random-effects model at the group level of analysis) will focus on the beta weights for the variable period before the response and the 8-second period after the response. We refer to these beta weights as estimates of engagement for that region.
We performed three analyses on the imaging data:
Predefined Regions: The first analysis focused on the six regions in Figure 1: HIPS, PSPL, and ANG from the triple-code theory and a Motor region controlling the hands, LIPFC, and PPC from the ACT-R theory. The ACT-R regions have been used many times (for a review see Anderson, 2007). The locations of triple-code regions are from Dehaene et al. (2003) and were first used in our research in Rosenberg-Lee et al. (2009). However, we updated the coordinates of the HIPS region to reflect the larger meta analysis in on Cohen et al. (2008)
408 Regions Exploratory: This analysis used 408 regions that we have used in a multivariate pattern recognition study (e.g., Anderson et al., 2010). These regions were created by evenly distributing 4×4×4 voxel cubes (a voxel is 3.2 mm high by 3.125 mm long and wide) over the 34 slices of the 64×64 acquisition matrix. Between region spacing was 1 voxel in the x- and y- directions in the axial plane, and one slice in the z-direction. We applied a mask of the structural reference brain and excluded regions where less than 70% of the region’s original 64 voxels survived.
Voxel-Level Exploratory: The other exploratory analyses will be at the individual voxel level. They include a contrast of Before-Exception and before Before-Regular. This exploratory analysis looked for regions of at least 15 contiguous voxels that showed a voxel-wise significance of 0.00001 for the difference between Before-Exception and Before-Regular. Using these values results in a brain-wide significance estimated to be less than 0.00001 by simulation (Cox, 1996; Cox & Hyde, 1997).
Results
Behavioral Results
Participants were correct on 90.3% of the regular problems but only 42.9% of the exception problems, a highly significant difference (t(19)=12.55, p < .0001). The error rate on exception problems ranged relatively uniformly across participants from 17% to 79%. We excluded from further analysis the 4.9% of problems that resulted in errors due to timing out. Of the remaining problems, correct regular averaged 9.77 s., correct exceptions average 16.02 s., incorrect regulars average 12.75 s., and incorrect exceptions averaged 19.02 sec. These times display a main effect of problem type (F(1,19) = 113.36, p < .0001) and correctness (F(1,19) = 48.21, p < .0001) and no significant interaction F(1,19) = 0.01).
Imaging Analysis: Predefined Regions
The first analysis will look at the BOLD response in the left and right homologs of 6 predefined regions given in Figure 1. While the real interest is in the other regions, we will use the motor region to illustrate how the analysis applies. Figure 3a illustrates the analysis for the left motor region for regular and exception problems that were solved correctly. In this figure and others like it, for illustrative purposes only, the BOLD values from all trials have been rescaled (see Anderson et al., 2008) so that the scan of the response is always the mean duration of that condition – that is, 5 scans for correct regular problems and 8 scans for correct exception problems (marked by stars in the figure). This makes the illustrative BOLD responses both stimulus-locked and response-locked. The BOLD values are plotted for the 2 scans before the problem (during which the warning appeared) through 10 scans after the response. The measure plotted is the percent change from a baseline defined by the first and last scan in the graph. The last scan is the beginning of the warning for the next trial. Also plotted is mean magnitude of engagement during the three periods (Before response, Response period, and After response), estimated as described in the Methods section). The solid line is the predicted hemodynamic response scaled to the mean length of the conditions as the empirical data. The good fit to the data suggests that our estimation of the three engagement magnitudes captures the main effects in the data. Sensibly, engagement is low before the response, spikes during the period of response generation, and is low again during the 8 seconds after the response.
While the hemodynamic responses in Figure 3a are illustrative, the statistical analyses will be on estimates of engagement during the Before and After intervals (ignoring the Response engagement for the 2 seconds before the response was completed). Figure 3b illustrates the mean engagements, averaged over participants, both for left and right motor regions and for both correct and incorrect answers. For this and subsequent regions we performed a 2×2×2×2 analysis of variance (see Table 2) where the factors were Hemisphere (left versus right), Accuracy (correct versus error), Type (regular versus exception), and Period (Before versus After). The analysis in the case of the motor region produced only one strongly significant effect – that of hemisphere (F(1,19) = 22.10, p < .0005). Not surprisingly, since the responding hand is the right, the right motor region shows little activity. While the Before and After engagement in the left hemisphere is significantly greater than in the right, it is small relative to the left’s engagement during the Response period (see Figure 3a).
Table 2.
(a) Predefined Regions | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Motor | PPC | LIPFC | PSPL | HIPS | Ang | |
Hemisphere | 22.10*** | 0.61 | 0.09 | 1.99 | 3.93 | 3.19 |
Correctness | 0.00 | 17.97*** | 34.59*** | 12.39** | 26.81*** | 6.69* |
HxC | 2.58 | 1.39 | 1.61 | 0.12 | 0.59 | 0.75 |
Type | 3.04 | 15.23*** | 8.90** | 1.79 | 9.73** | 46.55*** |
HxT | 0.79 | 0.01 | 2.93 | 0.00 | 0.10 | 0.28 |
CxT | 1.29 | 2.24 | 2.82 | 5.36* | 2.40 | 1.24 |
HxCxT | 4.86* | 1.97 | 1.63 | 0.02 | 1.22 | 0.14 |
Period | 2.20 | 13.04** | 37.18*** | 19.95*** | 27.76*** | 0.78 |
HxP | 0.00 | 1.79 | 39.63*** | 0.86 | 1.93 | 1.43 |
CxP | 0.00 | 34.43*** | 32.80*** | 4.71* | 31.08*** | 7.99* |
HxCxP | 0.55 | 0.26 | 8.76** | 0.00 | 0.01 | 0.16 |
TxP | 2.92 | 2.91 | 0.02 | 0.92 | 2.20 | 3.47 |
HxTxP | 4.37 | 1.29 | 0.44 | 3.46 | 0.07 | 1.81 |
CxTxP | 0.01 | 0.36 | 0.02 | 0.06 | 0.12 | 1.01 |
HxCxTxP | 0.75 | 1.26 | 0.80 | 0.94 | 1.73 | 2.24 |
After-Ex minus Before-Reg | −1.84 | −2.54* | −4.04*** | −3.15** | −3.98*** | 3.37** |
(b) Exploratory Regions | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Premotor | APFC | Fusiform | SPFG | |
Hemisphere | 0.12 | na | na | 0.00 |
Correctness | 10.15** | 6.76* | 10.45** | 29.73*** |
HxC | 2.95 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 9.00** |
Type | 0.00 | 22.33*** | 13.04** | 38.62*** |
HxT | 5.48* | na | na | 0.53 |
CxT | 4.46* | 0.13 | 5.78* | 3.77 |
HxCxT | 7.19* | na | na | 2.30 |
Period | 67.18*** | 43.15*** | 20.90*** | 0.04 |
HxP | 0.00 | na | na | 0.97 |
CxP | 8.70** | 9.86** | 26.11*** | 16.08*** |
HxCxP | 2.25 | na | na | 0.84 |
TxP | 0.24 | 0.39 | 17.69*** | 4.01 |
HxTxP | 0.28 | na | na | 2.70 |
CxTxP | 0.28 | 5.28* | 0.04 | 0.40 |
HxCxTxP | 0.32 | na | na | 4.53* |
After-Ex minus Before-Reg | −7.17*** | 7.64*** | 5.30*** | 2.34* |
Significance (2-tailed)
p < .05
p < .01
p< .001
The results in Figure 3 can be viewed as a sanity check on the analysis, as there is no reason to expect any effects of factors other than hemisphere for the motor region and a strong reason to expect a larger effect in the left hemisphere that would spike during the response interval. The relatively small Before engagement probably reflects mouse movements, perhaps in anticipation of response generation. Also, the relatively small After engagement probably reflects mouse movements, perhaps in anticipation of the repetition detection task.
The results of complete statistical analyses for the 6 predefined regions are in Table 2a. The results for the other regions are quite different from the motor region. None show significant effects of hemisphere. In contrast, they show significant or near significant effects of correctness (errors greater than corrects), type (exception greater than regular), and (except for ANG) of period (Before greater than After). They all show a significant period-by-correctness interaction such that the greater response to errors only occurs after the response has been made. Table 2b also reports the results of a 2-tailed t-test of the difference between After-Exception and Before-Regular which is the critical test identified in the introduction for distinguishing Cognitive and Metacognitive regions. The PPC, the LIPFC, and the HIPS show significant effects)in the Cognitive direction of Before-Regular being greater than After-Exception. The effect in the PSPL is in the same direction but marginal statistically (p < .1). In contrast, ANG shows a strong effect in the opposite direction (p < .005).
The PPC, LIPFC, PSPL, and HIPS all show the same direction of effects and are strongly intercorrelated (mean correlation of the 8 Before and After engagement values (averaging over left and right) is .933, with a range from .841 to .981) while they are much more weakly correlated with ANG (mean .423, range from .185 to .591). Figure 4 illustrates the BOLD values for the left LIPFC, which is quite representative of the intercorrelated 4 regions, and Figure 5 illustrates the pattern in right ANG, which is different. The LIPFC (Figure 4) shows strong engagement during the problem solving and low engagement afterwards. The mean .04% engagement after successful solution of regular problems is only marginally different from 0 (t(19)=2.01, p < .1). While there is a difference between exception and regular problems, it is relatively small.
The results for ANG (Figure 5) are quite different than the LIPFC. For corrects (Figure 5a) the Before engagement is not significantly different than the After engagement. The difference between exceptions and regulars is quite large and results in a highly significant difference between Before-Regular and After-Exception, but in the opposite direction of the other regions. The magnitude of the left engagement is marginally larger (p < .1) than in right but hemisphere is involved in no interactionsiii. The left ANG is often distinguished from the right in many theories including the triple code, but the pattern of ANG effects in this experiment is basically the same in the two hemispheres.
Imaging Analysis: Exploratory Analysis with 408 Regions
The ANG is the only predefined region to display the Metacognitive Pattern defined in the introduction. To find further Metacognitive (or Cognitive) regions we performed an exploratory analysis using 408 regions that cover the entire brain as described in the Methods section. For each of the 408 regions we calculated the difference between the critical conditions of Before-Regular and After-Exception for the correct data. Using the Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons, a t of +/− 4.81 is required for significance. Eleven regions exceeded this threshold (see Table 3). Four of these regions were located in the bilateral premotor areas and showed significantly greater activation for the Before period for regular problems than the After period for exception problems and they all give very similar patterns of engagement. Three parietal regions, the last two of which overlap with the predefined left and right ANG, show patterns of engagement similar to Figure 5. Two regions in the frontopolar cortex (FPC) and two visual regions also show greater engagement for After-Exceptions than Before-Regulars. One of the visual regions is the left fusiform and the other is an occipital region just posterior to the fusiform. These visual areas overlap substantially with the region in the triple-code theory of Dehaene and Cohen that processes visual representations of digits. Having already examined the ANG, we will report the effects for the premotor regions, frontopolar, and fusiform in more detail. We averaged the two premotor regions on the left, the two on the right, the two FPC regions, and the two fusiform regions. Complete results of statistical tests involving these regions are included in Table 2b.
Table 3.
Region of Interest | Brodmann Area(s) |
Coordinates (x,y,z) |
Voxel Count |
t-value |
---|---|---|---|---|
(a) Exploratory (408 regions): Before-Regular>After-Exception | ||||
R. Premotor | 6 | 2,7,49 | 64 | −5.31 |
L. Premotor | 6 | −13,7,49 | 64 | −6.19 |
R. Premotor | 6 | 17,−7,51 | 64 | −5.63 |
L. Premotor | 6 | −28,−7,51 | 64 | −6.84 |
(b) Exploratory (408 regions): After-Exception>Before-Regular | ||||
L. Inf Parietal | 40 | −59,−52,42 | 64 | 5.68 |
R. Angular Gyrus | 39 | 48,−67,44 | 64 | 5.02 |
L. Angular Gyrus | 39 | −44,−67,44 | 64 | 5.38 |
L. Frontopolar | 10 | −13,59,−6 | 64 | 6.84 |
R. Inferior Prefrontal | 47 | 48,29,−2 | 64 | 5.23 |
L. Fusiform | 37 | −44,−58,−5 | 64 | 5.32 |
L. Occipital | 19 | −44,−73,−3 | 64 | 4.83 |
(c) Exploratory (voxel level): Before-Exception>Before-Regular | ||||
R. Sup Frontal Gyrus | 8 | 40,17,46 | 120 | 1.20 |
L. Sup Frontal Gyrus | 8 | −39,20,46 | 95 | 2.99 |
M. Sup Frontal Gyrus | 8 | 1,28,45 | 54 | 0.05 |
L. IPL Ang Gyrus | 39/40 | −46,−53,47 | 146 | 3.08 |
R. IPL Ang Gyrus | 39/40 | 44,−59,45 | 115 | 2.78 |
L. Frontopolar | 10/46 | −42,46,6 | 36 | 0.92 |
R. Frontopolar | 10/46 | 46,39,1 | 33 | 3.89 |
R. Temporal Gyrus | 21/37 | 56,−42,−6 | 36 | 3.01 |
L. Temporal Gyrus | 21/37 | −57,−40,−6 | 49 | 4.49 |
Figure 6 presents the results for the left and right premotor regions. The pattern of engagement in the Before and After periods shows substantial correlation with PPC, LIPFC, PSPL, and HIPS (mean correlation .892) and much correlation with ANG (mean .061). For corrects the premotor areas are strongly engaged in the Before period and very weakly engaged in the After period. The mean After engagement for correct regulars (.02%) is not significantly different than zero (t(19) = 1.16, p > .25). These regions contrast with the other regions in showing no significant difference between regular problems and exceptions. This pattern of results yields a highly significant contrast between Before-Regular and After-Exception.
Figure 7 provides information about the average engagement of the two frontopolar regions and the two visual-fusiform regions, which give somewhat similar effects. The mean engagement in the fusiform correlates .907 with the frontopolar engagement. Their mean correlation with ANG is .742 while their mean correlation with the other non-motor predefined regions is just −.101. Both are more engaged by errors, by exceptions, and in the After period, and show a strong interaction between correctness and period. Figure 7a illustrates the mean response of FPC for corrects. This region actually shows a negative engagement in the Before period for regular problems which is marginally significant (t(19) = − 1.93, p < .10).
The exploratory regions in Figures 6 and 7 provide a striking contrast. While there are small effects of errors in Figure 6, the majority of variance among conditions is due to the greater activity before than after the response. On the other hand, the regions in Figure 7 show strong effects of error and problem type and show more activation after the response. To determine how typical these regions might be of general activity in the 408 regions we performed a factor analysis of the 8 magnitudes of engagement (excluding the magnitude for the response scan between the Before and After periods) for each of the 408 regions. Figure 8a shows, after a rotation to produce the best illustration, the scores for the first two factors (Factor 1 and Factor 2) that account for 72% of the variance. Factor 1 is much like the pattern in Figure 6 for the frontopolar regions. The figure also shows a hypothetical Cognitive Factor that is simply +1 before the response and −1 after. The correlation between Factor 1 and the Cognitive Factor is .974. Factor 2 shows a pattern somewhat like Figure 7 for the frontopolar and fusiform regions. Figure 8a also illustrates a Metacognitive Factor that is engaged by exceptions and by errors in the After period. The Metacognitive factor adds the exception engagement to the error engagement for the last condition (After response to exception errors) in Figure 8a. The correlation between the Metacognitive factor and Factor 2 is .913. Unlike Factors 1 and 2, the Cognitive and Metacognitive factors are not perfectly orthogonal but show a negative correlation of −.378. Factor 2 may be the Metacognitive factor distorted under the constraint of extracting orthogonal factors in the factor analysis.
Individual Voxel Analysis of the Cognitive and Metacognitive Factors
While this final analysis is at the voxel level, the intent is to shed more light on the global organization of the brain in service of both cognition and metacognition. Given the interrelatedness of these functions, and the prior research discussed below in the general discussion, the expectation is that various cognitive-to-metacognitive gradients should be observed. To this end, we calculated the correlation of each voxel with the Cognitive and Metacognitive factors in Figure 8a. Figure 8b provides a scatterplot of the two correlations for all 51,239 voxels. The outer boundary in this scatterplot reflects the theoretical bound on the maximum correlations. It has a negative principal axis because of the negative correlation between the two factors. Figure 8c presents only those voxels for which the mean engagement over the 8 conditions was greater than zero and only focus on those with R2 greater than .841, which corresponds to a significance of p<0.01. Of the 32,820 positively responding voxels, 11,994 of these have R2 > .841. Since we would only expect about 328 voxels with R2 > .841 by chance, it is clear that these two factors account for real variance in the brain– just as the factor analysis implied.
Figure 8c breaks these voxels into 6 color-coded 60-degree regions:
Cognitive: The 60-degree region from −45° to +15° where the correlation with the Cognitive factor is near 1.
Mixed: The 60-degree region from +15° to +75° where the correlations with the Cognitive and Metacognitive factors are about equal.
Metacognitive. The 60-degree region from +75° to +135° where the correlation with the Metacognitive factor is near 1.
Anti-cognitive. The 60-degree region from +135° to +195° where the correlation with the Cognitive factor is near −1.
Negative: The 60-degree region from +195° to +255° where the correlation is about equally negative with the Cognitive and Metacognitive factors.
Anti-metacognitive. The 60-degree region from +255° to +315° (or −45°) where the correlation with the Metacognitive factor is near −1.
Many of voxels in the Anti-cognitive to Anti-Metacognitive range in Figure 8b are eliminated in Figure 8c because they had a negative mean response.
Figure 8d shows the locations of all voxels with R2 > .841, whether positively or negatively responding. The positively responding regions use the same color-coding as in Figure 8c, while the negatively responding voxels are in black. Many of the negatively responding voxels are in classic default network regions (e.g., Fair et al., 2008; Raichle & Snyder, 2007) such as the medial prefrontal and posterior cingulate. The positively responding voxels include many of the regions already identified. The motor region is not among the regions that show strong correlation with the Cognitive and Metacognitive factors. The LIPFC, PPC, PSPL, HIPS overlap Cognitive and Mixed voxels. The premotor is a purely Cognitive region. The figure also shows three regions that have already been found to show metacognitive profiles – ANG, FPC, and fusiform. In every case the patterns are roughly bilaterally symmetric. Figure 8d indicates an additional Metacognitive region that has not come out in the prior analysis and this is part of the superior prefrontal gyrus (SPFG) that is Brodmann Area 8. Separate left, right, and medial SPFG regions show the metacognitive pattern.
SPFG regions are also identified in an exploratory analysis that compares the difference Before-Exception and Before-Regular (see Table 3c). Figure 9 illustrates the response in the SPFG. While the SPFG has a strong correlation with the Metacognitive profile (r = .883) and shows similar effects to other metacognitive regions, it differs from the ANG (Figure 5) and FPC regions (Figure 8) in that it shows significantly greater engagement in the Before period for corrects (t(19) = 3.29, p < .005). This results in a smaller After-Exception and Before-Regular contrast for corrects, which is why it was not identified in the earlier 408-region exploratory analysis. In fact, the contrast only reaches conventional levels of significance for the left SPG (Table 3c).
General Discussion
The analyses in this paper depend on comparing engagement before and after response generation. We separately estimated an engagement for response generation not to contaminate either of these measures with the role the region might play in response generation. The ability to clearly separate the engagement of the motor region into expected components in Figure 3 is one indication of the success of the procedure. Another indication of the success of this separation is that, while many regions showed differences between error and non-errors in the After period, no region showed a significant difference in engagement to corrects versus errors in the Before period, when participants did not yet know the accuracy of their response.
These pyramid problems nicely separate regions that show a Cognitive profile (strong engagement during problem solving, independent of challenge and disengagement afterwards) and regions that show a Metacognitive profile (strong engagement during challenging parts of problem solving and continued reflection afterwards). Figure 8d reveals a cognitive-to-metacognitive gradient in the frontal cortex moving posterior to anterior. Others have noted a similar concrete-to-abstract organization of the prefrontal cortex (e.g., Badre, 2008; Buckner, 2003; Christof & Gabrieli, 2000; Petrides, 2006). Figure 8d would also appear to reveal another cognitive-to-metacognitive gradient in the parietal cortex going medial to lateral. There are some reports of a similar organization in other tasks (e.g., Asari et al. 2005). There are projections between many prefrontal regions and parietal regions (e.g., Petrides & Pandya, 1984; Selemon & Goldman-Rakic, 1988) including connections between premotor and superior parietal (Wise et al., 1997), but apparently no direct parietal connections from frontopolar to the parietal (Petrides & Pandya, 2007).
It would be wrong to conclude all of all of the Cognitive and Metacognitive regions are equivalent. On the Cognitive side, the ACT-R theory implies different roles for Cognitive regions like the LIPFC and PPC and the triple-code theory implies distinct functions for the HIPS and PSPL. On the Metacognitive side, it is interesting that two of the regions identified are part of the triple-code theory – ANG and the fusiform gyrus. The fusiform gyrus is supposed to encode for visual form and has been shown to be active in many reading tasks (McCandliss et al., 2003), particularly on the left. This region is almost certainly not performing a metacognitive function per se but rather is being recruited in this experiment for more visual inspection in service of metacognitive functions. According to the triple-code theory, the left ANG is involved in verbal processing of numbers, also a relatively routine cognitive activity. Perhaps the greater activity in the left ANG reflects this verbal processing. The bilateral Metacognitive pattern may reflect a second function of this region that is overlaid on the verbal processing in the left.
It is of interest that the three of the regions identified as metacognitive in this task, the ANG, the FPC, and the SPFG, are adjacent to regions typically identified as default network and which respond negatively in our experiment (see Figure 8d). These default regions have also been associated with cognitive functions like theory of mind and prospection (Buckner & Carroll, 2007; Spreng et al., 2009). This suggests there may be an abstraction continuum that goes beyond the three metacognitive regions we found and that the highest levels of abstraction may not be activated by any of our mathematical problems.
We think the three Metacognitive regions identified in this experiment are serving general higher-level functions, but these functions can be separated, just as the functions of the different Cognitive regions have been separated. Even in this experiment the three regions show differences in their pattern of engagement to correct exception problems. The ANG (Figure 5) shows no significant difference in engagement before versus after response generation. The FPC (Figure 7) shows significantly greater engagement after than before. The SPFG (Figure 9) shows significantly greater engagement before response generation than afterwards. Below we will discuss these three regions and their possible functions in more detail.
Although the left ANG shows somewhat stronger engagement than the right, the profiles for the two hemispheres are highly correlated (r = .955 for Figure 5b). We have suggested that the left ANG is engaged both for verbal processing and metacognitive processing. The right ANG is close to the regions of the right temporoparietal junction that have been associated with theory of mind and reorientation of attention. While the right ANG tends to be about two centimeters away from the center cited for the temporoparietal junction (ANG is a little higher and more posterior) its coordinates do overlap with some studies reported in the meta-analysis of Decety & Lamm (2007). Theory of mind studies typically require participants to interpret the intentions of others. For instance, one theory-of-mind study (Ruby & Decety, 2003) found this region was more active when participants took another’s perspective rather than their own. This suggests that activation in the region might reflect trying to understand the experimenter’s intentions in the definition of pyramid problems in order to correctly extend it to these exception problems. Certainly the verbal protocols contained reference to the experimenter intentions as in “You need to explain the rule”. The reorientation of attention function attributed to the right temporoparietal junction may also be involved because these exception problems typically involve taking a different approach to their solution. In addition, a right ANG region, basically identical to the predefined region, is activated by a mismatch between one’s actions and the consequences of these actions (Farrer et al., 2008). Under these various interpretations it makes sense that the ANG is heavily engaged both before and after response generation.
The greater involvement of the FPC region in the After period is consistent with other studies that find it highly engaged in reflection. In episodic memory experiments it also gives a late positive fMRI response that can peak after the motor area controlling response generation peaks (e.g., Reynolds et al., 2005; Rugg et al., 2003). In other memory research Johnson et al. (2005) and Raye et al. (2007) describe a “refreshing” process that shares features with reflection on problem solving. Their paradigm involves presenting participants with a sequence of items, usually a new word to study, but sometimes a cue to refresh the previous word. If the FPC region principally serves a reflection function, then it is reasonable that it should be much more engaged after response generation. However, it is not just a region that is engaged after any response, since it does not show significant engagement after a correct response to a regular problem.
The SPFG region has been shown to play a role in spatial working memory tasks (e.g., Casey et al., 1998; Courtney et al., 1996; Postle et al., 2000; Rowe & Passingham, 2001) and reasoning tasks (e.g., Goel & Dolan, 2004; Parsons & Osherson, 2001), particularly inductive reasoning. Neither of these processes are what one would consider classic metacognitive functions. One might speculate the SPFG is activated by the need to reason about how to extend regular solutions to exception cases by analogy to the solution for regular problems. Alternatively, this region might be engaged in spatial reasoning about the pyramid metaphor for these mathematical problems. Psychometric studies tend to find substantial correlations among spatial ability, reasoning, and mathematical ability (Lehman, 1988). Both of the reasoning and the spatial interpretations would imply that the SPFG would be engaged more by the immediate challenge of solving a pyramid problem and so we might expect it to be more active before the response is determined.
Our suggested interpretations for the ANG, FPC, and SPFG are consistent with other interpretations in the literature and these interpretations generally see these regions as having a role in higher-level cognitive processing. The postulated functions for each region can be interpreted as general-purpose computation that is not specific to metacognitive processing. In this experiment the critical metacognitive demands involve reflecting on ones processing, both concurrently to adapt the solution method for an exception problem and after the fact to tune one’s methods given the feedback. These regions are like the fusiform in that they perform general-purpose functions that get called upon to meet the metacognitive demands of solving these pyramid problems.
The results of this experiment have important implications for how to model metacognitive processing in mathematics. Just as processing of regular problems proved to require the postulation of multiple modules in either the triple-code theory or ACT-R, these results suggest that to one needs to incorporate multiple separate modules that perform different functions when confronted with a situation that evokes metacognition. We might conjecture that the exception problems in this experiment evoke a “theory of mind” module (ANG) to interpret the intentions of the experimenter in defining pyramid problems, a “reasoning” module (SPFG) to determine the implications of the definition of a pyramid problem, and a “reflection” module (APFC) to modify one’s understanding given the feedback. Figure 8d suggests that these pyramid problems evoke a wide pattern of activity with many different brain regions performing the distinct functions required to solve these problems.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health grant MH068243 to J. R. A. We would like to thank Julie Fiez for her comments on this research and Hee Seung Lee and Andrea Stocco for their comments on the paper. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to John R Anderson, Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213 (ja@cmu.edu)
Footnotes
While not explicitly associated with an ACT-R region, we have found in other research (e.g., Anderson, 2005) that premotor regions often show similar responses in problem-solving tasks to representational demands. Premotor regions will similarly co-activate with PPC in this research. Curtis & D’Esposito (2003) discuss the idea that premotor regions may serve a representational role along with the idea that prefrontal regions may serve more executive functions.
This produces a hemodynamic function that peaks at 4.5 seconds. It correlates .971 with the standard SPM hemodynamic function (Friston et al., 1998) but lacks the small undershoot of that function.
Given that the triple-code model predicts this difference, one might use a 1-tailed test in which case the difference would be significant.
Contributor Information
John R. Anderson, Carnegie Mellon University
Shawn Betts, Carnegie Mellon University.
Jennifer L. Ferris, Carnegie Mellon University
Jon M. Fincham, Carnegie Mellon University
References
- Anderson JR. Human symbol manipulation within an integrated cognitive architecture. Cognitive Science. 2005;29:313–342. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog0000_22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson JR. How Can the Human Mind Occur in the Physical Universe? New York: Oxford University Press; 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Anderson JR, Betts S, Ferris JL, Fincham JM. Neural Imaging to Track Mental States while Using an Intelligent Tutoring System. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA. 2010;107:7018–7023. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1000942107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson JR, Bothell D, Byrne MD, Douglass S, Lebiere C, Qin Y. An integrated theory of mind. Psychological Review. 2004;111:1036–1060. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.111.4.1036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson JR, Carter CS, Fincham JM, Ravizza SM, Rosenberg-Lee M. Using fMRI to test models of complex cognition. Cognitive Science. 2008;32:1323–1348. doi: 10.1080/03640210802451588. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson JR, Albert MV, Fincham JM. Tracing Problem Solving in Real Time: fMRI Analysis of the Subject-Paced Tower of Hanoi. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2005;17:1261–1274. doi: 10.1162/0898929055002427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson JR, Qin Y, Sohn M-H, Stenger VA, Carter CS. An information-processing model of the BOLD response in symbol manipulation tasks. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2003;10:241–261. doi: 10.3758/bf03196490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Asari T, Konishi S, Jimura K, Miyashita Y. Multiple components of lateral posterior parietal activation associated with cognitive set shifting. Neuroimage. 2005;26:694–702. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Badre D. Cognitive control, hierarchy, and the rostro-caudal axis of the prefrontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Science. 2008;12:193–200. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buckner RL. Functional-anatomic correlates of control processes in memory. Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;23:3999–4004. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-10-03999.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buckner RL, Carroll DC. Self-projection and the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2007;11:49–57. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buckner RL, Wheeler ME. The cognitive neuroscience of remembering. National Review Neuroscience. 2001;2:624–634. doi: 10.1038/35090048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burgess PW, Gilbert SJ, Okuda J, Simons JS. Rostral prefrontal brain regions (area 10): A gateway between inner thought and the external world? In: Prinz W, Sebanz N, editors. Disorders of Volition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2006. pp. 373–396. [Google Scholar]
- Carpenter PA, Just MA, Keller TA, Eddy W, Thulborn K. Graded function activation in the visuospatial system with the amount of task demand. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 1999;11:9–24. doi: 10.1162/089892999563210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Casey BJ, Cohen JD, O’Craven K, Davidson RJ, Irwin W, Nelson CA, Noll DC, Hu X, Lowe MJ, Rosen BR, Truwitt CL, Turski PA. Reproducibility of fMRI results across four institutions using a spatial working memory task. NeuroImage. 1998;8:249–261. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Castelli F, Glaser DE, Butterworth B. Discrete and analogue quantity processing in the parietal lobe: a functional MRI study. Proc. National Academy of Sciences USA. 2006;103:4693–4698. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0600444103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christoff K, Gabrieli JDE. The frontopolar cortex and human cognition: evidence for a rostrocaudal hierarchical organization within the human prefrontal cortex. Psychobiology. 2000;28:168–186. [Google Scholar]
- Christoff K, Prabhakaran V, Dorfman J, Zhao Z, Kroger JK, Holyoak KJ, Gabrieli JDE. Rostrolateral prefrontal cortex involvement in relational integration during reasoning. NeuroImage. 2001;14(5):1136–1149. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0922. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen L, Kadosh R, Lammertyn J, Izard V. Are numbers special? An overview of chronometric, neuroimaging, developmental, and comparative studies of magnitude representation. Progress in Neurobiology. 2008;84:132–147. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2007.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Courtney SM, Ungerleider LG, Keil K, Haxby JV. Object and spatial visual working memory activate separate neural systems in human cortex. Cerebral Cortex. 1996;6:39–49. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.1.39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cox R. AFNI: Software for Analysis and Visualization of Functional Magnetic Resonance Neuroimages. Computers and Biomedical Research. 1996;29:162–173. doi: 10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cox R, Hyde JS. Software tools for analysis and visualization of fMRI data. NMR in Biomedicine. 1997;10:171–178. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1099-1492(199706/08)10:4/5<171::aid-nbm453>3.0.co;2-l. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Curtis CE, D'Esposito M. Persistent activity in the prefrontal cortex during working memory". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2003;7:415–423. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00197-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Decety J, Lamm C. The role of the right temporoparietal junction in social interaction: How low-level computational processes contribute to metacognition. Neuroscientist. 2007;13:580–593. doi: 10.1177/1073858407304654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dehaene S, Cohen L. Cerebral pathways for calculation: double dissociation between rote verbal and quantitative knowledge of arithmetic. Cortex. 1997;33:219–250. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70002-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dehaene S, Piazza M, Pinel P, Cohen L. Three parietal circuits for number processing. Cognitive Neuropsychology. 2003;20:487–506. doi: 10.1080/02643290244000239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Danker J, Anderson J. The roles of prefrontal and posterior parietal cortex in algebra problem solving: a case of using cognitive modeling to inform neuroimaging data. Neuroimage. 2007;35:1365–1377. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.01.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eger E, Sterzer P, Russ MO, Giraud AL, Kleinschmidt A. A supramodal number representation in human intraparietal cortex. Neuron. 2003;37:719–725. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00036-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fair DA, Cohen AL, Dosenbach NU, Church JA, Miezin FM, Barch DM, Raichle ME, Petersen SE, Schlaggar BL. The maturing architecture of the brain's default network. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105:4028–4032. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0800376105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fletcher PC, Henson RNA. Frontal lobes and human memory: insights from functional neuroimaging. Brain. 2001;124:849–881. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.5.849. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston KJ. Statistical Parametric Mapping. London: The Analysis of Functional Brain Academic Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Friston KJ, Fletcher P, Josephs O, Holmes AP, Rugg MD, Turner R. Event-related fMRI: characterising differential responses. NeuroImage. 1998;7:30–40. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goel V, Dolan RJ. Differential involvement of left prefrontal cortex in inductive and deductive reasoning. Cognition. 2004;93:B109–B121. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Isaacs EB, Edmonds CJ, Lucas A, Gadian DG. Calculation difficulties in children of very low birth weight: A neural correlate. Brain. 2001;124:1701–1707. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.9.1701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson MK, Raye CL, Mitchell KJ, Greene EJ, Cunningham WA, Sanislow CA. Using fMRI to investigate a component process of reflection: Prefrontal correlates of refreshing a just-activated representation. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2005;5:339–361. doi: 10.3758/cabn.5.3.339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kao Y, Douglass S, Fincham J, Anderson JR. Traveling the Second Bridge: Using fMRI to Assess an ACT-R Model of Geometry Proof. Cognitive Science; (in revison) [Google Scholar]
- Koechlin E, Ody C, Kouneiher F. The Architecture of Cognitive Control in the Human Prefrontal Cortex. Science. 2003;302:1181–1185. doi: 10.1126/science.1088545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koechlin E, Hyafil A. Anterior Prefrontal Function and the Limits of Human Decision-Making. Science. 2007;318:594–598. doi: 10.1126/science.1142995. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krueger F, Spampinato MV, Pardini M, Pajevic S, Wood JN, Weiss GH, Landgraf S, Grafman J. Integral calculus problem solving: An fMRI investigation. Neuroreport. 2008;19:1095–1099. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e328303fd85. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Legrand D, Ruby P. What is self-specific? Theoretical investigations and critical review of neuroimaging results. Psychological Review. 2009;116:252–282. doi: 10.1037/a0014172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lohman DF. Spatial abilities as traits, processes, and knowledge. In: Sternberg RJ, editor. Advances in the Psychology of Human Intelligence. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1988. pp. 181–248. [Google Scholar]
- McCandliss BD, Cohen L, Dehaene S. The Visual Word Form Area: Expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2003;7:293–299. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00134-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Menon V, Rivera SM, White CD, Glover GH, Reiss AL. Dissociating prefrontal and parietal cortex activation during arithmetic processing. Neuroimage. 2000;12:357–365. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Molko N, Cachia A, Riviere D, Mangin JF, Bruandet M, Le Bihan D, Cohen L, Dehaene S. Functional and structural alterations of the intraparietal sulcus in a developmental dyscalculia of genetic origin. Neuron. 2003;40:847–858. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00670-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Naccache L, Dehaene S. The Priming method: Imaging unconscious repetition priming reveals an abstract representation of number in the parietal lobes. Cerebral Cortex. 2001;11:966–974. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.10.966. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parsons LM, Osherson D. New Evidence for Distinct Right and Left Brain Systems for Deductive versus Probabilistic Reasoning. Cerebral Cortex. 2001;11:954–965. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.10.954. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petrides M. Lateral prefrontal cortex: architectonic and functional organization. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2005;360:781–795. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1631. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petrides M, Pandya DN. Projections to the frontal cortex from the posterior parietal region in the rhesus monkey. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1984;228:105–116. doi: 10.1002/cne.902280110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piazza M, Izard V, Pinel P, Le Bihan D, Dehaene S. Tuning curves for approximate numerosity in the human intraparietal sulcus. Neuron. 2004;44:547–555. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.10.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pinel P, Piazza M, Le Bihan D, Dehaene S. Distributed and overlapping cerebral representations of number, size, and luminance during comparative judgments. Neuron. 2004;41:983–993. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(04)00107-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Qin Y, Anderson JR, Silk E, Stenger VA, Carter CS. The change of the brain activation patterns along with the children's practice in algebra equation solving. Proceedings of National Academy of Sciences, USA. 2004;101:5686–5691. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0401227101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Postle BR, Berger JS, Taich AM, D’Esposito M. Activity in human frontal cortex associated with spatial working memory and saccadic behavior. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2000;12:2–14. doi: 10.1162/089892900564028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raichle ME, Snyder AZ. A default mode of brain function: a brief history of an evolving idea. Neuroimage. 2007;37:1083–1090. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramnani N, Owen AM. Anterior prefrontal cortex: Insights into function from anatomy and neuroimaging. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2004;5:184–194. doi: 10.1038/nrn1343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ravizza SM, Anderson JR, Carter CS. Errors of mathematical processing: The relationship of accuracy to neural regions associated with retrieval or representation of the problem state. Brain Research. 2008;1238:118–126. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.08.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raye CL, Johnson MK, Mitchell KJ, Greene EJ, Johnson MR. Refreshing: A minimal executive function. Cortex. 2007;43:135–145. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70451-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reynolds JR, McDermott KB, Braver TS. A direct comparison of anterior prefrontal cortex involvement in episodic retrieval and integration. Cerebral Cortex. 2006;16:519–528. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenberg-Lee M, Lovett M, Anderson JR. Neural Correlates of Arithmetic Calculation Strategies. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience. 2009;9:270–285. doi: 10.3758/CABN.9.3.270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rowe JB, Toni I, Josephs O, Frackowiak RSJ, Passingham RE. The prefrontal cortex: Response selection or maintenance within working memory? Science. 2000;288:1656–1660. doi: 10.1126/science.288.5471.1656. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rugg MD, Henson RN, Robb WG. Neural correlates of retrieval processing in the prefrontal cortex during recognition and exclusion tasks. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41:40–52. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00129-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoenfeld AH. What's all the fuss about metacognition? In: Schoenfeld AH, editor. Cognitive Science and Mathematics Education. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1987. pp. 189–215. [Google Scholar]
- Selemon L, Goldman-Rakic PS. Common cortical and subcortical target areas of the dorsolateral prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices in the rhesus monkey: evidence for a distributed neural network subserving spatially guided behavior. Journal of Neuroscience. 1988;8:4049–4068. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.08-11-04049.1988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sohn M-H, Goode A, Koedinger KR, Stenger VA, Carter CS, Anderson JR. Behavioral equivalence does not necessarily imply neural equivalence: Evidence in mathematical problem solving. Nature Neuroscience. 2004;7:1193–1194. doi: 10.1038/nn1337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spreng RN, Mar RA, Kim AS. The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind and the default mode: A quantitative meta-analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2009;21:489–510. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.21029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wise SP, Boussaoud D, Johnson PB, Caminiti R. Premotor and parietal cortex: corticocortical connectivity and combinatorial computations. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 1997;20:25–42. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.20.1.25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woods RP, Grafton ST, Holmes CJ, Cherry SR, Mazziotta JC. Automated image registration: I. General methods and intrastudent intramodality validation. Journal of Computer Assisted Tomography. 1998;22:139–152. doi: 10.1097/00004728-199801000-00027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zacks JM, Ollinger JM, Sheridan MA, Tversky B. A parametric study of mental spatial transformation of bodies. NeuroImage. 2002;16:857–872. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]