Abstract
The niche construction model postulates that human bio-social evolution is composed of three inheritance domains, genetic, cultural and ecological, linked by feedback selection. This paper argues that many kinds of archaeological data can serve as proxies for human niche construction processes, and presents a method for investigating specific niche construction hypotheses. To illustrate this method, the repeated emergence of specialized reindeer (Rangifer tarandus) hunting/herding economies during the Late Palaeolithic (ca 14.7–11.5 kyr BP) in southern Scandinavia is analysed from a niche construction/triple-inheritance perspective. This economic relationship resulted in the eventual domestication of Rangifer. The hypothesis of whether domestication was achieved as early as the Late Palaeolithic, and whether this required the use of domesticated dogs (Canis familiaris) as hunting, herding or transport aids, is tested via a comparative analysis using material culture-based phylogenies and ecological datasets in relation to demographic/genetic proxies. Only weak evidence for sustained niche construction behaviours by prehistoric hunter–gatherer in southern Scandinavia is found, but this study nonetheless provides interesting insights into the likely processes of dog and reindeer domestication, and into processes of adaptation in Late Glacial foragers.
Keywords: niche construction, southern Scandinavia, Late Palaeolithic, comparative method, Canis familiaris, Rangifer
1. Niche construction and archaeology
Niche construction (NC) has been defined as the evolutionary process whereby organisms modify their own and other organisms' environments in such a way that selection pressures on the current and subsequent generations are altered significantly [1]. From this point of view, adaptation can be the result of two processes: (i) environment > selection > adapted organism, or (ii) organism > NC > modified environment. The end-result of both pathways is a fit between organism and environment (adaptation), but, importantly, the process differs. This distinction was flagged-up by Lewontin [2,3] some time ago, but it is only recently that its wider implications with regards to the evolutionary trajectories in a range of species are being explored in quantitative detail (e.g. [4–9]). Odling-Smee et al. [10] have compiled a long list of potential niche-constructing behaviours found across most taxonomic groups. They have also noted that humans in particular are adept niche constructers, and that many human genes may be the result of recent, culturally modified selection pressures [11]. Yet, despite considerable efforts to model human biological and cultural evolution in relation to NC (e.g. [12–23]), there are only relatively few quantitative studies of human NC [14,24]. One of the reasons for this lack may be that few disciplines have access to information on the sustained and long-term modification of ecologically relevant environmental parameters and their subsequent selective repercussions. However, ‘archaeology provides unique quantitative information on population-level distributions of cultural attributes over long periods of time. This information concerns not only socially transmitted cultural traditions but also the ongoing process of niche construction’ [25, p. 177]. I argue here that the tools of the comparative method, together with archaeological data, can be used to investigate hypotheses about specific prehistoric NC processes.
The NC model recognizes three domains of inheritance (cf. [26])—genetic, ecological and cultural—and archaeology can provide proxy information on all three domains. Archaeological data on craft traditions can be used to track cultural inheritance, thereby plotting the historical relationships among past communities of teachers and learners. Patterns of social information transmission among traditional societies tend to be conservative, operating largely within family groups [27,28], often with considerable pedagogical involvement by adults [29]. This conservatism is reflected in the many well-known typological sequences, indicating high degrees of cultural inheritance stability over many generations. Phylogenetic methods are increasingly being used to describe and analyse these patterns of material culture diversity (e.g. [30–32]). The key advantage of cultural phylogenetics over traditional typological methods is that a given phylogenetic tree represents an explicit and quantitative hypothesis of how given archaeological ‘taxa’ [33] are related historically. Cultural phylogenetics thus opens the door to formal studies of adaptation using the tools of the so-called comparative method [34,35].
In addition, much archaeological data pertains directly to human modifications of the biotic and abiotic environments, both locally and transiently as well as at larger geographical and temporal scales [36,37]. Culture, it has long been argued, constitutes the human niche [38,39], and environmental archaeologists have made human niche modification and its consequences their primary concern [40]. This includes the domestication of animals and plants [41], as well as ‘domesticated landscapes’ [42, p. 323], and even ‘transported landscapes’ [43, p. 217]: landscape modifications and built environments brought about by collective human efforts as well as entire economies/ecologies taken from one place to another during dispersals. The ecological transmission of physical resources in the form of modified environments, domesticated plants and animals is particularly relevant to an archaeological application of NC theory, because traditional human economies, subsistence practices and land-use strategies can be described well with reference to ecological inheritance (e.g. [44–46]). Sterelny [47, pp. 151–152] underlines that ‘to the extent that information does flow collectively, niche construction is our best model of the generation-by-generation accumulation of skill, technology and information’ in human societies. This collective information transmission is echoed in Oswalt's [48] distinction between weapons/instruments on the one hand, and facilities on the other. In this view, material culture that reflects personal transmission of information and use, such as projectile points, basketry or pottery (weapons/instruments), provides information on cultural inheritance in the strict sense. Material culture that reflects a collective transmission of information and use, such as tents and housing structures, fishing platforms, fortifications as well as field systems (facilities), can conceptually and analytically be framed as part of the ecological inheritance passed from generation to generation [49]. Alternatively, such features could be viewed as part of the human extended phenotype [50,51], but their selective relevance is via modified environments, particularly in subsequent generations born into a niche that already is modified in a given way [18,52]. Furthermore, facilities often have a use-life longer than a single human generation, and are continuously or periodically modified and changed. These evident ecological modifications cannot be readily related to the genotypes of those who played no part in putting them in place. Instead, they reference the collectively held stock of ecological knowledge and its implementation: a modified environment that constitutes the ontogenetic niche for subsequent generations.
Finally, the adaptive or maladaptive effects of human NC processes should be reflected in genetic inheritance patterns of the niche-constructing populations as well as the animals and plants whose niches are affected. The increasing availability of ancient and population genetic data facilitates inferences about, and direct insights into, past demographic processes [53]. Gene frequency patterns in both modern and ancient DNA can, for instance, be used to demonstrate the evident success of early farmers and their NC behaviours involving a range of domesticated animals and plants (e.g. [54–56]). However, genetic change is merely the endpoint of what is best thought of as a continuum of processes [57–59]. Archaeologists have access to datasets that reflect the (conscious or unconscious) manipulation of the behaviour or distribution of candidate domesticates long before genetic changes take place and become widely established in the target population [59]. In addition, demographic success of the niche-constructing population itself is also reflected in a range of archaeological proxies, such as range expansion, increases in the number and/or size of sites or the number of 14C dates in a given period [60,61].
In the following section, I will provide some examples of human NC that leave archaeologically visible traces. I will contrast the NC behaviours of farmers and foragers, arguing that the domestication of plants and animals [41] as well as the lasting modification or ‘domestication’ of landscapes [42,62] sets the benchmark for effective NC. I will then go on to explore in more detail a case study of prehistoric forager NC involving domesticated dogs (Canis familiaris) and reindeer (Rangifer tarandus) hunting/herding strategies in the Late Glacial of southern Scandinavia (approx. 14.7–11.5 kyr BP), where I use comparative methods to investigate the feedback relations between these two NC behaviours.
2. Archaeological signatures of human niche construction
A first example, the human occupation of Greenland between about AD 950 and 1500 serves to contrast the NC behaviours of farmers and foragers. Greenland was first settled by hunter–gatherer groups from North America and Siberia in several waves beginning sometime after approximately 4.5 kyr BP [63]. Around AD 950, the southern tip of Greenland was also settled by Viking (Norse) farmers from Iceland. The expanding Greenland Norse brought with them their agricultural niche package fine-tuned to Norwegian conditions, including animals and crops. They rapidly transformed local landscapes to suit their traditional niche requirements [64,65]. During milder climatic episodes—the first few hundred years of occupation—they fared sufficiently well. When the climate in the Northern Hemisphere turned colder as well as stormier during the Medieval Cold Period, traditional crops and herding techniques began to fail [66]. Anthropogenic landscape changes such as deforestation aggravated the conditions [67,68]. The deteriorating niche quality, coupled with unfavourable climate change, isolation and an insistence on maintaining a social and economic/ecological adaptation ill-suited to High Arctic environments, culminated in the local extinction of the Greenland Norse [69]. Meanwhile, Inuit groups of the Thule culture thrived [63]. The impact of these groups on the landscape was subtler and took the form of various facilities (e.g. drive lines, hunting stands and marked pathways: [70–72]) placed strategically in the landscape in order to facilitate travel and the management of reindeer movements.
The fate of the Greenland Norse serves to highlight that initially successful NC can have negative adaptive consequences in the longer term [73]. Generally, however, dispersing farming populations provide good examples of extensive NC involving both landscape modifications and the domestication of plants and animals. Their genetic and linguistic legacies can be traced worldwide [74,75]. The first dispersal of farming populations to Europe in particular has been studied intensively and much data on the changing plant and animal ecologies of these pioneering groups are available [76–79]. Shennan ([25], pp. 180–181; my emphasis) further adds that the dispersal of agricultural populations into Europe ‘is a classic example of natural selection acting on people through an inherited cultural tradition, which gave a selective advantage to those who adopted it and passed it on to their children. In fact, the process involved not simply the inheritance of a tradition but also the transmission of a new niche, because the actual descendants of the cereal crops and animals that had originally been domesticated were being carried along as part of the dispersal’. The evolutionary success of farmers under this model is largely due to the powerful positive NC that is part and parcel of most agricultural societies [55]. Equally, however, environmental archaeological data also indicate the collapse of such early agricultural societies in some regions because farming practices destroyed the environment [80]. Continuous settlement then required further NC (e.g. slope terracing) to counteract these negative effects (e.g. [81]).
Turning again to foragers, a recent ethnographic study by Bliege Bird et al. ([82]; see also [83]) provides detailed insights into the kind of landscape modifications that prehistoric hunter–gatherers may also have practised, and their adaptive outcomes. They show that episodic and systematic landscape burning by Australian Aboriginals increases hunting success measured in caloric yield per unit land. The effects of this burning on the species composition of that habitat is also documented. They demonstrate how this behaviour is underwritten and maintained via a stock of knowledge transmitted from generation to generation along with the physical niche component, the territory. Indeed, it has been argued that this behaviour has considerable time-depth in Australia [84–86].
Using detailed analyses of pollen profiles, fungal spores and charcoal traces in the vicinity of prehistoric settlement sites, it has likewise been argued that Mesolithic and Late Palaeolithic hunter–gatherers in Europe and elsewhere had similar burning practices (e.g. [87–92]), and that these had important adaptive benefits by increasing access to animals [93] and plants [94]. Increasingly, the notion of pre-Neolithic agriculture is being considered [95], and fire undoubtedly played an important role in the formation of the human niche in the long term [45,96].
It is such landscape manipulations that provide the context for the most evident process of prehistoric NC, the domestication of plants and animals [41,42]. The primary domesticate found among hunter–gatherers is the dog (C. familiaris). Numerous theories for why humans first began to domesticate dogs have been put forward [97,98], with many workers stressing the adaptive benefits of using dogs as hunting companions (e.g. [99,100]). The considerable extent to which dog-use enhances hunting success has recently been quantified within a behavioural ecological framework [101]. Keeping dogs also involves additional tools and trappings (tethers, leashes, pens, etc.) that together make up the constructed niche to which they are so well adapted physically, physiologically and cognitively [102–105]. From approximately 14 kyr BP onwards, the archaeological record is speckled with dog burials, signalling the symbiotic relationship and the important social as well as ecological role played by this first domesticate [98,106]. The effects of this long association between humans and dogs are evidenced by the significantly altered genetic composition, cognition, distribution and ecology of domesticated dogs, especially when compared with their now nearly extinct ancestor Canis lupus.
The most recent suggestions regarding the geographical origin and timing of dog domestication based on genetic data alone point to present-day southern China and argue that all extant dog breeds originated there less than approximately 16.3 kyr BP [107]. This stands in direct opposition to reports of domesticated dogs in Upper Palaeolithic contexts in Europe dating to 31.7 kyr BP [108,109] and would require an extremely rapid diffusion of this resource even if only the next-oldest European specimens were considered [99]. If, as argued above, domestication is considered as a process—and the domestication of Canis should be no exception in this respect [110]—then these seemingly opposing positions can be reconciled. Behavioural, morphological and genetic markers of domestication are not linked in a lock-step fashion [59]. The presence of C. familiaris in a range of Late Glacial archaeological sites in northern Europe [99,106,111] indicates that, at the very least, these hunter–gatherer groups were beginning to engage in symbiotic relations with wolves. Possibly, these early breeds never became fully (i.e. genetically) domesticated. Alternatively, breeds brought to Europe by dispersing farming populations later replaced these earlier lineages [112]. Either way, important questions about the process of dog domestication remain open, and southern Scandinavia constitutes a useful testing ground for investigating the emergence of this important NC behaviour.
Paralleling the first finds of C. familiaris, Palaeolithic foragers also began to engage in intense specialized economic/ecological relations with reindeer. Abundant reindeer bones that comprise practically 100 per cent of all faunal remains at some Late Glacial sites in southern Scandinavia (reviewed in the next section) document the repeated emergence of such specialized economies [113]. In fact, it has been suggested that the economic specialization seen in these earliest Late Glacial pioneers indicates a move towards reindeer herd management and domestication [114–117]. Specialized reindeer herding economies are widespread in high-latitude Eurasia today, and these practices have resulted in the partial domestication of this species [118]. While the intentional breeding of reindeer is a comparatively recent phenomenon, herd management is argued to go back a very long time indeed [119–121].
Faunal remains from key archaeological sites of this period do not support the notion of comprehensive herd control [122], but domesticated and wild reindeer herds are difficult to distinguish morphologically and demographically as even managed herds roam freely much of the time. Recent genetic studies indicate multiple independent domestication events in extant Rangifer populations in northern Europe [123], and standard zooarchaeological techniques may therefore not be able to readily detect incipient or small-scale reindeer herd management. Reindeer hunting and reindeer herding have much in common from an ecological perspective [124]. Istomin and Dwyer [125, p. 613] describe the relationship between humans and Rangifer as a kind of ‘dynamic mutual adaptation’, where humans impact the behaviour and biology of wild and domesticated reindeer, while they in turn influence their human counterparts. In addition, Ingold [126] and Ventsel [127] stress the continuity between techniques and technologies of Rangifer hunting and herding. These technologies—drive lines and pathways, for instance—are signatures of forager NC involving reindeer, and are found in Scandinavia from at least the Early Holocene [128,129]. Ingold also notes that reindeer economies provide excellent opportunities for canine companions, and Müller-Wille et al. [121] point out that northern European reindeer economies not only are associated with but also require the use of dogs. In the following, I therefore explore the specific hypothesis that intense reindeer specializations akin to herd management evolved already during the Late Glacial in southern Scandinavia. I ask whether the domestication or introduction of C. familiaris facilitated the repeated emergence of this kind of economy, which in terms of its use of dogs and in its relationship with reindeer reflects the NC behaviours of these hunter–gatherer groups.
Odling-Smee et al. [10, p. 344] have suggested that such ‘signatures of past human cultural niche construction’ can be investigated using the tools of the comparative method, a powerful and well-described suite of methods for the analysis of adaptation and causal correlations in biology [35]. The comparative method requires phylogenies in order to control for the historical relatedness and attendant statistical non-independence of the units under study, known as ‘Galton's Problem’. What such comparative analyses allow is to establish whether two given NC trait evolve in a correlated manner, where one trait, the niche-constructing trait, drives change in another trait, the recipient trait whose fitness depends on the effects of previous NC [18]. Such correlated evolution over time establishes the feedback relations between niche modifications that are at the heart of the NC model.
Material culture phylogenies are here constructed using Bayesian phylogenetic methods and lithic projectile point data, which reflect patterns of teaching and learning in Late Palaeolithic southern Scandinavia [130,131]. Such data, in non-phylogenetic formats, have traditionally provided the backbone for the culture-history in this region [132]. Information on the presence/absence of domesticated dogs and on reindeer specialization is plotted onto these phylogenies, and the resulting pattern is queried for correlations among the selected traits. In formal terms, this paper addresses the following hypotheses:
(H0) Dog-use and successful specialized reindeer economies evolve independently.
(H1) Dog-use and successful specialized reindeer economies evolve in concert.
If support for H1 can be found, it can be addressed which of the traits is the primary niche-constructing and which the recipient trait:
(H1a) Dog-use facilitate the evolution of specialized reindeer economies (dog-use = niche-constructing trait, reindeer specialization = recipient trait).
(H1b) The adoption of specialized reindeer economies necessitate dogs (reindeer specialization = niche-constructing trait, dog-use = recipient trait).
I first briefly review southern Scandinavian Late Palaeolithic culture-history, the evidence for specialized reindeer economies and the use of domesticated dogs in this period. Then, a methodology is presented that examines the correlation between reindeer specialization and dog-use across Bayesian material culture phylogenies reflecting the major culture-historical trends of this period. The likelihood ratio (LR) test and Bayes factors (BFs) are used to assess whether these two traits are correlated. These analyses indicate that specialized reindeer hunting without dogs was probably an unstable strategy tenable only under favourable climatic conditions during the Late Glacial, and that domesticated dogs were a necessary component of successful specialized reindeer economies, as suggested by Müller-Wille et al. [121]. They also support the idea that domesticated dogs during this period were a costly resource that, while conferring important adaptive advantages in hunting, also required significant maintenance and training costs [133]. The periodic absence of dogs during the Late Glacial colonization of northern Europe—known also from the later human colonization of High Arctic Greenland [134]—indicates that despite the evident adaptive benefits of dog-use, they may have dropped out of the cultural repertoire on occasion. In addition to the relationship between the two NC traits under study, archaeological proxies as well as recent population genetic data indicate that a demographically viable, continuous human presence in Scandinavia was only possible during the later part of the Late Glacial when dogs were firmly established in the ecologically inherited repertoire of these forager groups.
3. The southern scandinavian late palaeolithic
At the height of the Last Ice Age, southern Scandinavia was almost fully covered by ice and devoid of people. The reappearance of hunter–gatherer groups just before 14.7 kyr BP was part of the general human re-expansion from their glacial refugia. Gamble and colleagues [135–137] have provided a framework for this process incorporating insights from environmental science, population genetics and archaeology (table 1). Although southern Scandinavia became part of the human world from about 14.7 kyr BP, it remained demographically marginal until the beginning of the Holocene climatic amelioration. Population densities were low [140] and probably fluctuated in accord with environmental changes [141,142].
Table 1.
event stratigraphy |
GRIP ice core years of onset (BP) | chronozones | population events | techno-complexes | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Holocene | 11 500 | ||||
GS-1 | Younger Dryas | 5. contraction | Ahrensburgian | ||
GI-1a | 12 650 | Late Allerød | Bromme culture | ||
Laacher See eruption | |||||
GI-1b | 12 900 | Intra-Allerød Cold Phase | 4. stasis and population decline | Federmesser-Gruppen (FMG) | |
GI-1c | 13 150 | Early Allerød | |||
GI-1d | 13 900 | Older Dryas | 3. colonization, abandonment and re-colonization | Hamburgian (Havelte and Classic phases) | |
GI-1e | 14 500 | Bølling | |||
GS-2 | GS-2a | 16 900 | 2. initial expansions into central Europe | Southern Scandinavia not settled | |
GS-2b | 19 500 | ||||
GS-2c | 21 200 | ||||
GI-2 | 21 800 | Last Glacial Maximum | 1. refugium | Southern Scandinavia not settled |
Traditionally, four cultures or techno-complexes are recognized in the region, arranged in chronological succession: the Hamburgian (divided into an earlier Classic, and a later Havelte phase), the Federmesser-Gruppen (FMG), the Bromme and Ahrensburgian cultures, each with a characteristic stone tool repertoire (figure 1). Numerous hypotheses regarding the process of re-colonization have been proposed. Eriksen [143, p. 169], for instance, suggests that this sequence represents ‘a continuous and largely endogenous cultural development’, while Petersen [144] argues for a discontinuous human presence. Other recent studies have also suggested that these techno-complexes may represent discrete expansion–retraction pulses [145,146], and much recent work has focused on better understanding the underlying processes of culture change. Riede [147,148] has suggested that the origin of the Bromme should be seen in relation to the eruption of the Laacher See volcano around 13 kyr BP and the subsequent isolation experienced by peripheral groups belonging to the FMG techno-complex. Follow-up studies have explored different forcing mechanisms responsible for the relocation of animal and human populations away from areas affected by the volcanic ash fall-out [149,150]. Economically, however, neither the FMG nor the Bromme are characterized by specialized reindeer hunting. In contrast, both the preceding Hamburgian techno-complex and the later Ahrensburgian cultures are seen as specialized reindeer hunters, based on faunal evidence from a range of sites [151–153]. Late Palaeolithic hunter–gatherers may also have controlled or domesticated reindeer, as discussed above, and they may have manipulated the landscape by building cairns, flag lines, hunting stands etc. in order to steer the movements of herds for their own advantage [151,154], and to facilitate travel in an otherwise relatively featureless landscape [155].
In sum, ‘northern Europe is an extraordinary laboratory for the investigation of human colonization and adaptation’ [156, p. 185]. Adaptation and range expansion are possible outcomes of NC [157]. Specialized reindeer economies emerged in the Hamburgian and Ahrensburgian, but these two cultures are separated in time by nearly 1 kyr. It is essential to take account of the historical relatedness or otherwise of these groups when discussing whether their reindeer specializations were, in fact, adaptive and whether they relate to other NC behaviours such as dog-use.
4. Material and methods
As in many other regions, characteristic projectile points have provided the backbone for culture-historical reconstruction in southern Scandinavian prehistory [132]. Because of their historical sensitivity, projectile points have also been analysed, with increasing frequency, using phylogenetic methods [158]. Cultural phylogenetics has advantages over traditional typological approaches in that a given phylogeny constitutes a quantitative hypothesis of the historical relatedness among the chosen units of analysis [159]. Such hypotheses can then be evaluated statistically and in relation to external datasets, such as stratigraphic, geographical or radiocarbon dating information. While a phylogenetic quantification of material culture relations alone can reveal important new insights in its own right, phylogenies can also be used in additional comparative analyses. Here, a method is presented that uses these tools to detect Late Palaeolithic hunter–gatherer NC.
(a). Construction of archaeological taxonomic units
The method of taxon construction largely follows that described by O'Brien et al. [160,161] and Darwent & O'Brien [162], although an initial analysis also used phylogenetic networks [163,164] to explore potential instances of blending and reticulation [159]. A total of 607 specimens were measured for a variety of characters (figure 2), which were divided into discrete character states using exploratory statistics and lithic analytical principles as guidelines [131]. Twelve of these attributes were used to construct the phylogenies presented here, and these reflect the size and shape of the projectiles as well as manufacturing methods (table 2). Each taxon comprises at least five specimens identical in their attribute compositions, thus reflecting recurrently taught and learned flint-knapping behaviour [29,131]. This approach yielded 16 taxa, of which the taxon associated with the oldest 14C date was chosen as outgroup.
Table 2.
character | character state | character | character state |
---|---|---|---|
I maximum length (mm) | 0. ≤45 | VII tang retouch direction | 0. opposing |
1. 45–68 | 1. none | ||
2. > 68 | 2. same side | ||
II maximum width (mm) | 0. <19 | VIII tang symmetry | 0. >2.5 |
1. ≥19 | 1. 1.5–2.5 | ||
2. 1.0–1.4 | |||
III maximum thickness (mm) | 0. <5 | IX tip retouch | 0. none |
1. ≥5 | 1. unilateral | ||
2. bilateral | |||
IV volumea | 0. <39 | X combined tang/body ratioc | 0. <23 |
1. 39–58 | 1. 23–42 | ||
2. 59–166 | 2. >42 | ||
3. >166 | |||
V tang/body ratiob | 0. unilateral retouch | XI retouch extent ratiod | 0. 4–18 |
1. no tang | 1. 19–40 | ||
2. <2.0 | 2. >40 | ||
3. ≥2.0 | |||
VI percussion bulb morphology | 0. faint bulb | XII tang retouch symmetry | 0. ≤1.4 |
1. pronounced bulb | 1. no tang | ||
2. distinct bulb with scarring | 2. >1.4 |
aVolume = length × width × thickness.
bTang/body ratio = length/shortest tang retouch.
cLength/tang/body.
dTotal retouch extent/(length × width).
(b). Phylogeny construction
Bayesian Markov chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) methods are used to retrieve a sample of trees from the most likely regions of the universe of possible topologies [165]. The model of evolution used is a simple multistate model (KSTATES), where the rates of character state change are presumed to be equal, and only minimal assumptions about mode, tempo and direction of character change are introduced. The trees were rooted using the taxon associated with the oldest radiocarbon date. A total of 10 000 k iterations were run and the tree universe sampled at every 40 kth iteration to minimize autocorrelation among the trees in the final sample. The Markov chain quickly converged on the most likely tree configurations, and after a few thousand iterations, only minor fluctuations in the likelihood score of each tree are observed (figure 3). Figure 4 shows the consensus tree of the resulting tree sample (n = 251). This tree differs little from those produced using other maximum-likelihood (ML) and parsimony-based approaches (F. Riede 2007, unpublished PhD thesis), and the major techno-complexes recognized by traditional typological analysis are represented in the phylogeny, albeit not all as monophyletic clades [157]. The branch lengths reflect both the chronological sequence of diversification and the degree to which these groups experienced isolation (especially the Bromme clade). Even rates of cultural change cannot, however, be assumed. At times, craftsmen deliberately introduce variation into the manufacturing process [171], which can rapidly increase branch length. Note that the consensus tree is not used in further analyses. Instead, the uncertainties associated with the tree-building procedure are incorporated into subsequent analyses by querying the entire sample of 251 trees.
(c). Examining correlated trait change
Each taxon is associated with the two traits (dog-use/reindeer specialization) under investigation, scored as presence (1)/absence (0), at the level of their associated techno-complex. The ML and Bayesian algorithms implemented in BayesTraits [172,173] were used to approximate trait correlations. The posterior log-likelihoods for an ML model (iterations = 1000 k; rate deviation = 80; multiple tries = 25; sample period = 20) in which the two traits evolve independently are differently distributed than those in which the two traits are assumed to evolve in concert (figure 5). The LR test, described by Pagel [172] and calculated by 2*[log-Lh(D) − log-Lh(I)], can be used to statistically evaluate trait correlations in each tree sample. This test indicates that the mean of these likelihood (Lh) distributions is not statistically significant (p > 0.21, χ2, d.f. = 4). In the more appropriate Bayesian framework, the so-called Bayes factor can be used to assess relative support for one over the other model (see [173] for a detailed description). This analysis (parameter settings as above; burn-in = 50 k) returns a log BF (=2*[log(harmonic mean(D)) − log(harmonic mean(I))]) of 0.7, i.e. positive if very weak evidence in favour of the dependent model [174].
(d). Transition rate analysis
The transition rate probabilities provided as part of the output by BayesTraits (table 3) give clues as to the order of correlated change, and such correlations establish which trait is the active niche-constructing trait and which the recipient trait (figure 6). In the ML-dependent model, the distributions of transition rates for each trait combination across the 251 trees show some patterning (electronic supplementary material, figure S1). The transition rates for the evolution of non-dog-using economic generalists to non-dog-using reindeer specialists are the lowest and contrast with those for the transition from dog-using generalists to dog-using specialists (q12 ≠ q34), indicating that it was unlikely for reindeer specialization to emerge in the absence of dogs. In addition, dog-use appears to have facilitated economic flexibility as expressed in the more frequent transitions between generalist and specialized economies in the presence of dogs (q43, q43), while itself being a labile trait with a relatively high likelihood of becoming lost (q42, q31).
Table 3.
independent model | ||||
dog-use | 0 | 1 | ||
0 | — | q01: 13.11 | ||
1 | q10: 6.22 | — | ||
reindeer specialization | 0 | 1 | ||
0 | — | q01: 2.75 | ||
1 | q10: 3.08 | — | ||
dependent model | ||||
dog-use/reindeer specialization | 0/0 | 0/1 | 1/0 | 1/1 |
0/0 | – | q12: 0.07 | q13: 0.03 | — |
0/1 | q21: 0.43 | — | — | q24: 11.13 |
1/0 | q31: 1.66 | — | — | q34: 28.96 |
1/1 | — | q42: 19.92 | q43: 20.49 | — |
5. Discussion and conclusion
The foregoing analysis has found limited positive support for H1, a correlated evolution of the use of domesticated dogs as hunting/herding/transport aids and the repeated emergence of specialized reindeer economies. Both traits can be understood as a constructed niche dimension and interpreted within the NC framework. However, this support is not statistically significant. Why is the evidence for a correlated evolution of dog-use and specialized reindeer hunting not stronger, given the dependency of reindeer economies upon dogs observed today? The results of this study support the notion that prehistoric forager NC was in fact limited and intermittent, with little impact on target animals or landscapes. In this view, the fragmentary record of domesticated dogs in the northern European Late Glacial may not be the result of poor preservation, but rather of the fact that dog domestication/use was not a core part of the cultural repertoire at the time, much like in prehistoric Greenland [134]. It is possible, for instance, that early tamed or domesticated dogs did not provide significant advantages in food procurement [133]. If so, the costs of keeping these pets would regularly outweigh their benefits, particularly perhaps during times of food crisis. It is also worth noting that the increasingly close association of humans and dogs may already at this point have resulted in the zoonotic emergence of a range of infectious diseases [175], which can be seen as a negative NC effect.
In addition, and in contrast to some recent suggestions, this study finds no support for the notion of reindeer herd management during the earliest phases (Hamburgian) of human presence in southern Scandinavia during the Late Glacial. The adoption of a specialized reindeer economy possibly involving a degree of herd management or incipient domestication by Ahrensburgian groups was contingent on the adoption of domesticated dogs. Further distinguishing between successful and unsuccessful reindeer economies may aid in interpreting these results. Both Petersen [144] and Riede [155,176] have argued that the Hamburgian occupation of southern Scandinavia was ultimately unsuccessful. Both mitochondrial and non-recombining Y-chromosome data in Scandinavia indicate that a demographically viable colonization of the region is linked to the Ahrensburgian [177–179], implying that earlier colonization attempts had been unsuccessful. The Ahrensburgian is accordingly associated with a range expansion as well as an increase in the number and size of settlements (e.g. [180,181]). The emergence of specialized reindeer economies together with dog-use in the Ahrensburgian can thus be seen as an example of positive cultural NC that enabled an efficient adaptation to the harsh GS-1 conditions.
In contrast, the appearance of Hamburgian hunters seems strongly correlated with a pronounced abundance of Rangifer during the initial stages of faunal succession, first in the southern part of Scandinavia, and then increasingly northwards [176,182]. The disappearance of the Hamburgian techno-complex, in this view, represents a concrete example of negative NC, i.e. the failure of an adaptive system: ‘even the most adaptable of creatures will experience limits to its tolerance space, outside of which it is unable to behave adaptively’ ([183], p. 98). In the absence of dogs, and coupled with the climatic downturn at GI-1d most probably associated with a pronounced drop in reindeer populations across the region, Hamburgian foragers were no longer able to uphold the cultural buffer mechanisms protecting their niche space from larger scale, independent changes in the environment. Figure 7 summarizes the NC processes investigated in this study.
With regards to the methods used here, it should be noted that the search for correlations in analyses with few taxa is difficult [172]. Also, posterior branch support values for some clades used in this study are quite low. Larger, more robust phylogenies will increase our confidence in subsequent trait correlation analyses, yet ‘even partial phylogenetic information provides a better model of the variance in the data than completely ignoring phylogeny and assuming independence’ [34, p. 717]. A particular advantage of the time-depth provided by archaeological data is that it offers the opportunity to integrate stratigraphic and phylogenetic information, and to give directionality to both independent and dependent comparative models. Assigning particular trait states to known or reconstructed nodes would constrain comparative models, facilitating hypothesis testing. Finally, if the disappearance of the Hamburgian techno-complex at the GI-1d (Older Dryas) cold spell does represent a cultural ‘extinction’, then the inclusion of such ‘extinct’ taxa in the correlation analysis might distort the results of any comparative analyses [184,185]. Future work could profitably tackle these methodological issues, paralleling similar efforts in evolutionary biology [184], and by making use of some of the freely available software packages discussed by Freckleton [186].
Yet, despite the caveat of statistical non-significance, a comparative analysis of early dog domestication/use in northern Europe and the repeated emergence of specialized reindeer economies has highlighted several interesting features of this process, and a series of additional hypotheses are generated: Did the Late Glacial dogs that dispersed into northern Europe with their human partners belong to the currently dominant lineages? If yes, current interpretations about the timing and geography of the earliest dog domestication may have to be revised. If not, then repeated but incomplete domestication events have to be considered. Ancient DNA analysis of selected Late Glacial dog remains could be used to establish the relationship between prehistoric and present dog breeds. If extinct breeds can be found, these could be used to track the expansion of human groups in Europe and to investigate the interactions between indigenous populations and later immigrants [187,188]. Likewise, the genetic analysis of Late Glacial reindeer from different periods could aid in exploring their relation to each other, and to extant wild and domestic herds. In addition, targeted archaeological fieldwork might unearth technologies more directly associated with the keeping of dogs or the herding of reindeer.
This paper has built on previous efforts to identify and track prehistoric hunter–gatherer NC signatures [189–191] by exploring a quantitative method that searches for evolutionary correlations of ecologically inherited traits across material culture phylogenies. This methodology—potentially applicable across a wide range of archaeological datasets that act as proxies for past human NC—allows a discrimination of niche-constructing and recipient traits. Given the inherent difficulties in investigating human NC experimentally, archaeological data may provide useful quantitative data on such long-term processes and their evolutionary consequences. The present analysis has not found statistically significant support for sustained NC practices by Late Glacial hunter–gatherers in southern Scandinavia, and future analysis of prehistoric NC should perhaps focus on the more extensive environmental modifications and domestication efforts of past farming populations. In line with the arguments presented here, palaeontologists [192,193] as well as those concerned with the formation of soils [194,195] and landforms [196–199] have suggested that organisms play a demonstrable role in shaping the physical and adaptive landscapes in which they live, at scales ranging from the geological to the microscopic, from the long term to the transient. Supplementing these disciplines, prehistoric archaeology provides information on specifically human NC at specifically prehistoric timescales.
Acknowledgements
Research for this paper was conducted while working as British Academy Postdoctoral Fellow (PDF/2007/462) at the AHRC Center for the Evolution of Cultural Diversity (UCL). The support of Stephen Shennan and all colleagues at the CECD is gratefully acknowledged. I also thank the two anonymous reviewers for their comments and Jamie Tehrani and Jeremy Kendall for a stimulating workshop.
Footnotes
One contribution of 13 to a Theme Issue ‘Human niche construction’.
References
- 1.Laland K., Odling-Smee J., Feldman M., Kendal J. 2009. Conceptual barriers to progress within evolutionary biology. Found. Sci. 14, 195–216 10.1007/s10699-008-9153-8 (doi:10.1007/s10699-008-9153-8) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Lewontin R. C. 1978. Adaptation. Sci. Am. 239, 156–169 10.1038/scientificamerican0978-212 (doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0978-212) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Lewontin R. C. 1983. Gene, organism and environment. In Evolution from molecules to men (ed. Bendall D. S.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press [Google Scholar]
- 4.Boni M. F., Feldman M. W. 2005. Evolution of antibiotic resistance by human and bacterial niche construction. Evolution 59, 477–491 10.1043/0014-3820(2005)059(0477:EOARBH)2.0.CO;2 (doi:10.1043/0014-3820(2005)059(0477:EOARBH)2.0.CO;2) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Donohue K. 2005. Niche construction through phenological plasticity: life history dynamics and ecological consequences. New Phytol. 166, 83–92 10.1111/j.1469-8137.2005.01357.x (doi:10.1111/j.1469-8137.2005.01357.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Donohue K., Dorn L., Griffith C., Kim E., Aguilera A., Polisetty C. R., Schmitt J. 2005. Niche construction through germination cueing: life-history responses to timing of germination in Arabidopsis thaliana. Evolution 59, 771–785 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2005.tb01752.x (doi:10.1111/j.0014-3820.2005.tb01752.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Donohue K., Polisetty C. R., Wender N. J. 2005. Genetic basis and consequences of niche construction: plasticity-induced genetic constraints on the evolution of seed dispersal in Arabidopsis thaliana. Am. Nat. 165, 537–550 10.1086/429162 (doi:10.1086/429162) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Iriki A., Sakura O. 2008. The neuroscience of primate intellectual evolution: natural selection and passive and intentional niche construction. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 363, 2229–2241 10.1098/rstb.2008.2274 (doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.2274) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Schwilk D. W. 2003. Flammability is a niche construction trait: canopy architecture affects fire intensity. Am. Nat. 162, 725–733 10.1086/379351 (doi:10.1086/379351) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Odling-Smee F. J., Laland K. N., Feldman M. W. 2003. Niche construction. The neglected process in evolution. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press [Google Scholar]
- 11.Laland K. N., Odling-Smee J. F., Myles S. 2010. How culture shaped the human genome: bringing genetics and the human sciences together. Nat. Genet. 11, 137–148 10.1038/nrg2734 (doi:10.1038/nrg2734) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Aunger R. 2009. Human communication as niche construction. In Pattern and process in cultural evolution (ed. Shennan S. J.), pp. 33–43 Berkeley, CA: University of California Press [Google Scholar]
- 13.Bardone E., Magnani L. 2007. Sharing representations through cognitive niche construction. Data Sci. J. 6, S87–S91 10.2481/dsj.6.S87 (doi:10.2481/dsj.6.S87) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Borenstein E., Kendal J., Feldman M. 2006. Cultural niche construction in a metapopulation. Theor. Popul. Biol. 70, 92–104 10.1016/j.tpb.2005.10.003 (doi:10.1016/j.tpb.2005.10.003) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Bulbulia J. 2008. Meme infection or religious niche construction? An adaptationist alternative to the cultural maladaptationist hypothesis. Meth. Theor. Stud. Relig. 20, 67–107 10.1163/157006808x260241 (doi:10.1163/157006808x260241) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Heft H. 2007. The social constitution of perceiver-environment reciprocity. Ecol. Psychol. 19, 85–105 10.1080/10407410701331934 (doi:10.1080/10407410701331934) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Kerr B. 2007. Niche construction and cognitive evolution. Biol. Theory 2, 250–262 10.1162/biot.2007.2.3.250 (doi:10.1162/biot.2007.2.3.250) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Laland K., Kendal J., Brown G. 2007. The niche construction perspective: implications for evolution and human behaviour. J. Evol. Psychol. 5, 51–66 10.1556/JEP.2007.1003 (doi:10.1556/JEP.2007.1003) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Laland K. N. 2008. Exploring gene–culture interactions: insights from handedness, sexual selection and niche-construction case studies. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 363, 3577–3589 10.1098/rstb.2008.0132 (doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.0132) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Laland K. N., Odling-Smee F. J., Feldman M. W. 2000. Niche construction, biological evolution, and cultural change. Behav. Brain Sci. 23, 131–175 10.1017/S0140525X00002417 (doi:10.1017/S0140525X00002417) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Laland K. N., Odling-Smee F. J., Feldman M. W. 2001. Cultural niche construction and human evolution. J. Evol. Biol. 14, 22–33 10.1046/j.1420-9101.2001.00262.x (doi:10.1046/j.1420-9101.2001.00262.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Ross D. 2007. H. sapiens as ecologically special: what does language contribute? Lang. Sci. 29, 710–731 10.1016/j.langsci.2006.12.008 (doi:10.1016/j.langsci.2006.12.008) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Sterelny K. 2007. Social intelligence, human intelligence and niche construction. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 362, 719–730 10.1098/rstb.2006.2006 (doi:10.1098/rstb.2006.2006) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Ihara Y. W., Feldman M. 2004. Cultural niche construction and the evolution of small family size. Theor. Popul. Biol. 65, 105–111 10.1016/j.tpb.2003.07.003 (doi:10.1016/j.tpb.2003.07.003) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Shennan S. J. 2006. From cultural history to cultural evolution: an archaeological perspective on social information transmission. In Social information transmission and human biology (eds Wells J. C. K., Strickland S., Laland K. N.), pp. 173–190 London, UK: CRC Press [Google Scholar]
- 26.Odling-Smee F. J. 2007. Niche inheritance: a possible basis for classifying multiple inheritance systems in evolution. Biol. Theory 2, 276–289 10.1162/biot.2007.2.3.276 (doi:10.1162/biot.2007.2.3.276) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Shennan S. J. 1996. Social inequality and the transmission of cultural traditions in forager societies. In The archaeology of human ancestry: power, sex and tradition (eds Steele J., Shennan S. J.), pp. 365–379 London, UK: Routledge [Google Scholar]
- 28.Shennan S. J., Steele J. 1999. Cultural learning in hominids: a behavioural ecological approach. In Mammalian social learning: comparative and ecological perspectives (eds Box H. O., Gibson K. R.), pp. 367–388 Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press [Google Scholar]
- 29.Tehrani J. J., Riede F. 2008. Towards an archaeology of pedagogy: learning, teaching and the generation of material culture traditions. World Archaeol. 40, 316–331 10.1080/00438240802261267 (doi:10.1080/00438240802261267) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Collard M., Shennan S. J. 2008. Patterns, process, and parsimony: studying cultural evolution with analytical techniques from evolutionary biology. In Cultural transmission and material culture (eds Stark M. T., Bowser B. J., Horne L.), pp. 17–33 Tucson, AZ: The University of Tucson Press [Google Scholar]
- 31.Collard M., Shennan S. J., Tehrani J. J. 2006. Branching, blending, and the evolution of cultural similarities and differences among human populations. Evol. Hum. Behav. 27, 169–184 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.07.003 (doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.07.003) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Shennan S. J. 2008. Evolution in archaeology. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 37, 75–91 10.1146/annurev.anthro.37.081407.085153 (doi:10.1146/annurev.anthro.37.081407.085153) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Foley R. A. 1987. Hominid species and stone tools assemblages: how are they related? Antiquity 61, 380–392 [Google Scholar]
- 34.Freckleton R. P., Harvey P. H., Pagel M. 2002. Phylogenetic analysis and comparative data: a test and review of evidence. Am. Nat. 160, 712–726 10.1086/343873 (doi:10.1086/343873) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Harvey P. H., Pagel M. D. 1991. The comparative method in evolutionary biology. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press [Google Scholar]
- 36.Butzer K. W. 1982. Archaeology as human ecology. Method and theory for a contextual approach. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press [Google Scholar]
- 37.Butzer K. W. 1990. The realm of cultural-human ecology: adaptation and change in historical perspective. In The Earth as transformed by human action: global and regional changes in the biosphere over the past 300 years (eds Turner B. L. I., Clark W. C., Kates R. W., Richards J. F., Mathews J. T., Meyer W. B.), pp. 685–701 Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press [Google Scholar]
- 38.Hardesty D. L. 1972. The human ecological niche. Am. Anthropol. 74, 458–466 10.1525/aa.1972.74.3.02a00150 (doi:10.1525/aa.1972.74.3.02a00150) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Hardesty D. L. 1975. The niche concept: suggestions for its use in human ecology. Hum. Ecol. 3, 71–85 10.1007/BF01552263 (doi:10.1007/BF01552263) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.O'Connor T. P. 2001. Economic prehistory or environmental archaeology? On gaining a sense of identity. In Environmental archaeology: meaning and purpose (ed. Albarella U.), pp. 17–27 London, UK: Kluwer Academic Publishers [Google Scholar]
- 41.Smith B. D. 2007. Niche construction and the behavioral context of plant and animal domestication. Evol. Anthropol. 16, 188–199 10.1002/evan.20135 (doi:10.1002/evan.20135) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Terrell J. E., et al. 2003. Domesticated landscapes: the subsistence ecology of plant and animal domestication. J. Archaeol. Method Theory 10, 323–368 10.1023/B:JARM.0000005510.54214.57 (doi:10.1023/B:JARM.0000005510.54214.57) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Kirch P. V. 1997. The Lapita peoples. Ancestors of the oceanic world. Oxford, UK: Blackwell [Google Scholar]
- 44.Smith B. D. 2009. Resource resilience, human niche construction, and the long-term sustainability of Pre-Columbian subsistence economies in the Mississippi river valley corridor. J. Ethnobiol. 29, 167–183 10.2993/0278-0771-29.2.167 (doi:10.2993/0278-0771-29.2.167) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Smith B. D. 2011. General patterns of niche construction and the management of ‘wild’ plant and animal resources by small-scale pre-industrial societies. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 366, 836–848 10.1098/rstb.2010.0253 (doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0253) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Smith C. S., McNees L. M. 1999. Facilities and hunter–gatherer long-term land use patterns: an example from southwest Wyoming. Am. Antiquity 64, 117–136 10.2307/2694349 (doi:10.2307/2694349) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Sterelny K. 2006. Memes revisited. Br. J. Phil. Sci. 57, 145–165 10.1093/bjps/axi157 (doi:10.1093/bjps/axi157) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Oswalt W. H. 1976. An anthropological analysis of food-getting technology. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons [Google Scholar]
- 49.Riede F. 2010. Niche construction theory and human prehistory. Using artefact phylogenies and comparative methods to study past human ecosystem engineering. In Clasificación y arqueología: enfoques y métodos taxonómicos a la luz de la evolución darwiniana (eds García Rivero D., Escacena Carrasco J. L., García Fernández F. J.), pp. 175–204 Seville, Spain: University of Seville Press [Google Scholar]
- 50.Dawkins R. 1982. The extended phenotype. The long reach of the gene. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press [Google Scholar]
- 51.Dawkins R. 2004. Extended phenotype—but not too extended. A reply to Laland, Turner and Jablonka. Biol. Phil. 19, 377–396 10.1023/B:BIPH.0000036180.14904.96 (doi:10.1023/B:BIPH.0000036180.14904.96) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Laland K. N., Odling-Smee F. J., Feldman M. W. 2005. On the breath and significance of niche construction: a reply to Griffiths, Okasha and Sterelny. Biol. Phil. 20, 37–55 10.1007/s10539-004-6834-8 (doi:10.1007/s10539-004-6834-8) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Shennan S. J. 2008. Archaeology, genes and demographic histories. In Simulations, genetics and human prehistory (eds Matsumura S., Forster P., Renfrew C.), pp. 173–175 Cambridge, UK: McDonald Institute [Google Scholar]
- 54.Burger J., Kirchner M., Bramanti B., Haak W., Thomas M. G. 2007. Absence of the lactase-persistence-associated allele in early Neolithic Europeans. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 104, 3736–3741 10.1073/pnas.0607187104 (doi:10.1073/pnas.0607187104) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Gerbault P., Liebert A., Itan Y., Powell A., Currat M., Burger J., Swallow D. M., Thomas M. G. 2011. Evolution of lactase persistence: an example of human niche construction. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 366, 863–877 10.1098/rstb.2010.0268 (doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0268) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Itan Y., Powell A., Beaumont M. A., Burger J., Thomas M. G. 2009. The origins of lactase persistence in Europe. PLoS Comp. Biol. 5, e1000491. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000491 (doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000491) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Mannion A. M. 1999. Domestication and the origins of agriculture: an appraisal. Prog. Phys. Geog. 23, 37–56 10.1177/030913339902300102 (doi:10.1177/030913339902300102) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Zeder M. 2006. Central questions in the domestication of plants and animals. Evol. Anthropol. 15, 105–117 10.1002/evan.20101 (doi:10.1002/evan.20101) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Zeder M. A., Emshwiller E., Smith B. D., Bradley D. G. 2006. Documenting domestication: the intersection of genetics and archaeology. Trends Genet. 22, 139–155 10.1016/j.tig.2006.01.007 (doi:10.1016/j.tig.2006.01.007) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Chamberlain A. 2009. Archaeological demography. Hum. Biol. 81, 275–286 10.3378/027.081.0309 (doi:10.3378/027.081.0309) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Riede F. 2009. Climate and demography in early prehistory: using calibrated 14C dates as population proxies. Hum. Biol. 81, 309–337 10.3378/027.081.0311 (doi:10.3378/027.081.0311) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Kareiva P., Watts S., McDonald R., Boucher T. 2007. Domesticated nature: Shaping landscapes and ecosystems for human welfare. Science 316, 1866–1869 10.1126/science.1140170 (doi:10.1126/science.1140170) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Gulløv H. C., Andreasen C., Grønnow B., Jensen J. F., Appelt M., Arenborg J., Berglund J. 2004. Grønlands forhistorie. Copenhagen, Denmark: Gyldendal [Google Scholar]
- 64.Schofield J. E., Edwards K. J., Christensen C. 2008. Environmental impacts around the time of Norse landnám in the Qorlortoq valley, Eastern Settlement, Greenland. J. Archaeol. Sci. 35, 1643–1657 10.1016/j.jas.2007.11.004 (doi:10.1016/j.jas.2007.11.004) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Edwards K. J., Schofield J. E., Mauquoy D. 2008. High resolution paleoenvironmental and chronological investigations of Norse landnám at Tasiusaq, Eastern Settlement, Greenland. Quat. Res. 69, 1–15 10.1016/j.yqres.2007.10.010 (doi:10.1016/j.yqres.2007.10.010) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 66.McGovern T. H. 1974. The economics of extinction in Norse Greenland. In Climate and history (eds Wrigley T. M. L., Ingram M. J., Farmer G.), pp. 404–433 Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press [Google Scholar]
- 67.Amorosi T., Buckland P., Dugmore A., Ingimundarson J. H., McGovern T. H. 1997. Raiding the landscape: human impact in the Scandinavian North Atlantic. Hum. Ecol. 25, 491–518 10.1023/A:1021879727837 (doi:10.1023/A:1021879727837) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 68.McGovern T. H., Bigelow G. F., Amorosi T., Russell D. 1988. Northern islands, human error, and environmental degradation: a preliminary model for social and ecological change in the Medieval North Atlantic. Hum. Ecol. 16, 45–105 10.1007/BF00888447 (doi:10.1007/BF00888447) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Vésteinsson O., McGovern T. H., Keller C. M. 2002. Enduring impacts: social and environmental aspects of Viking Age settlement in Iceland and Greenland. Archaeol. Islandica 2, 98–136 [Google Scholar]
- 70.Brink J. W. 2005. Inukshuk: Caribou drive lanes on southern Victoria Island, Nunavut, Canada. Arctic Anthropol. 42, 1–28 [Google Scholar]
- 71.Grønnow B. 2009. Caribou hunting structures and hunting grounds of the Thule culture in Angujaartorfiup Nunaa, West Greenland. In On the track of the Thule culture from Bering Strait to east Greenland. Proc. SILA conf. ‘The Thule culture—new perspectives in Inuit prehistory’, Copenhagen, 26–28 October 2006. Papers in honour of Hans Christian Gulløv (ed. Grønnow B.), pp. 201–210 Copenhagen, Denmark: National Museum [Google Scholar]
- 72.Odgaard U. 2007. On the trail of the Caribou hunters: archaeological surveys in western Greenland. In Prehistoric journeys (eds Johnson R., Cummings V.), pp. 21–32 Oxford, UK: Oxbow [Google Scholar]
- 73.Lynnerup N., Norby S. 2004. The Greenland Norse: bones, graves, computers, and DNA. Polar Rec. 40, 107–111 10.1017/S0032247402002875 (doi:10.1017/S0032247402002875) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Bellwood P. 2005. First farmers: the origins of agricultural societies. Oxford, UK: Blackwell [Google Scholar]
- 75.Smith B. D. 2007. The ultimate ecosystem engineers. Science 315, 1797–1798 10.1126/science.1137740 (doi:10.1126/science.1137740) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Colledge S., Connolly J., Shennan S. J. 2006. The evolution of Neolithic farming from SW Asian origins to NW European limits. Eur. J. Archaeol. 8, 137–156 10.1177/1461957105066937 (doi:10.1177/1461957105066937) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Colledge S., Conolly J., Shennan S. J. 2004. Archaeobotanical evidence for the spread of farming in the eastern Mediterranean. Curr. Anthropol. 45, S35–S45 10.1086/422086 (doi:10.1086/422086) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Conolly J., Colledge S., Shennan S. 2008. Founder effect, drift, and adaptive change in domestic crop use in Early Neolithic Europe. J. Archaeol. Sci. 35, 2797–2804 10.1016/j.jas.2008.05.006 (doi:10.1016/j.jas.2008.05.006) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Coward F., Shennan S. J., Colledge S., Conolly J., Collard M. 2008. The spread of Neolithic plant economies from the Near East to northwest Europe: a phylogenetic analysis. J. Archaeol. Sci. 35, 42–56 10.1016/j.jas.2007.02.022 (doi:10.1016/j.jas.2007.02.022) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Kirch P. V. 2005. Archaeology and global change: the Holocene record. Annu. Rev. Environ. Resour. 30, 409–440 10.1146/annurev.energy.29.102403.140700 (doi:10.1146/annurev.energy.29.102403.140700) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 81.van Andel T. H., Runnels C. N., Pope K. O. 1986. Five thousand years of land use and abuse in the Southern Argolid. Hesperia 55, 103–128 10.2307/147733 (doi:10.2307/147733) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Bliege Bird R. B., Bird D. W., Codding B. F., Parker C. H., Jones J. H. 2008. The ‘fire stick farming’ hypothesis: Australian Aboriginal foraging strategies, biodiversity, and anthropogenic fire mosaics. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 105, 14 796–14 801 10.1073/pnas.0804757105 (doi:10.1073/pnas.0804757105) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Bird D. W., Bliege Bird R., Parker C. H. 2005. Aboriginal burning regimes and hunting strategies in Australia's Western Desert. Hum. Ecol. 33, 443–464 10.1007/s10745-005-5155-0 (doi:10.1007/s10745-005-5155-0) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Black M. P., Mooney S. D., Attenbrow V. 2008. Implications of a 14 200 year contiguous fire record for understanding human–climate relationships at Goochs Swamp, New South Wales, Australia. Holocene 18, 437–447 10.1177/0959683607087933 (doi:10.1177/0959683607087933) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Pyne S. J. 1991. Burning bush: a fire history of Australia. New York, NY: Henry Holt [Google Scholar]
- 86.Pyne S. J. 1998. Forged in fire: history, land, and anthropogenic fire. In Advances in historical ecology (eds Balée W., Crumley C. L.), pp. 64–103 New York, NY: Columbia University Press [Google Scholar]
- 87.Bos J. A. A., Janssen C. R. 1996. Local impact of Palaeolithic man on the environment during the end of the Last Glacial in the Netherlands. J. Archaeol. Sci. 23, 731–739 10.1006/jasc.1996.0069 (doi:10.1006/jasc.1996.0069) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Colombaroli D., Vanniere B., Emmanuel C., Magny M., Tinner W. 2008. Fire–vegetation interactions during the Mesolithic–Neolithic transition at Lago dell'Accesa, Tuscany, Italy. Holocene 18, 679–692 10.1177/0959683608091779 (doi:10.1177/0959683608091779) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Innes J. B., Blackford J. J. 2003. The ecology of Late Mesolithic woodland disturbances: model testing with fungal spore assemblage data. J. Archaeol. Sci. 30, 185–194 10.1006/jasc.2002.0832 (doi:10.1006/jasc.2002.0832) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Kay C. E. 2007. Are lightning fires unnatural? A comparison of aboriginal and lightning ignition rates in the United States. In Proc. 23rd Tall Timbers Fire Ecology Conf.: Fire in Grassland and Shrubland Ecosystems (eds Masters R. E., Galley K. E. M.), pp. 16–28 Tallahassee, FL: Tall Timbers Research Station [Google Scholar]
- 91.Moore J. 2000. Forest fire and human interaction in the early Holocene woodlands of Britain. Palaeogeogr. Palaeoecol. 164, 125–137 10.1016/S0031-0182(00)00180-2 (doi:10.1016/S0031-0182(00)00180-2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Vannière B., Colombaroli D., Chapron E., Leroux A., Tinner W., Magny M. 2008. Climate versus human-driven fire regimes in Mediterranean landscapes: the Holocene record of Lago dell'Accesa (Tuscany, Italy). Quat. Sci. Rev. 27, 1181–1196 10.1016/j.quascirev.2008.02.011 (doi:10.1016/j.quascirev.2008.02.011) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Mellars P. A. 1976. Fire ecology, animal populations and man: a study of some ecological relationships in prehistory. Proc. Preh. Soc. 42, 15–42 [Google Scholar]
- 94.Mason S. L. R. 2000. Fire and Mesolithic subsistence—managing oaks for acorns in northwest Europe? Palaeogeogr. Palaeoecol. 164, 139–150 10.1016/S0031-0182(00)00181-4 (doi:10.1016/S0031-0182(00)00181-4) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Tinner W., Nielsen E. H., Lotter A. F. 2007. Mesolithic agriculture in Switzerland? A critical review of the evidence. Quat. Sci. Rev. 26, 1416–1431 10.1016/j.quascirev.2007.02.012 (doi:10.1016/j.quascirev.2007.02.012) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Wrangham R. W. 2009. Catching fire: how cooking made us human. New York, NY: Basic Books [Google Scholar]
- 97.Manwell C., Baker C. M. A. 1984. Domestication of the dog: hunter, food, bed-warmer, or emotional object? J. Anim. Breed. Genet. 101, 241–256 10.1111/j.1439-0388.1984.tb00043.x (doi:10.1111/j.1439-0388.1984.tb00043.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Munt G., Meiklejohn C. 2007. The symbiotic dog. Why is the earliest domesticated animal also important symbolically? In On the road. Studies in honour of Lars Larsson (eds Hårdh B., Jennbert K., Olausson D.), pp. 165–169 Stockholm, Sweden: Almqvist & Wicksell [Google Scholar]
- 99.Musil R. 2000. Evidence for the domestication of wolves in central European Magdalenian sites. In Dogs through time: an archaeological perspective (ed. Crockford S. J.), pp. 21–28 Oxford, UK: Archaeopress [Google Scholar]
- 100.Petersen P. V. 2009. Stortandede harpuner—og jagt på hjortevildt til vands. Årbøger for nordisk Oldkyndighed og Historie 2009, 43–54 [Google Scholar]
- 101.Koster J. M. 2008. Hunting with dogs in Nicaragua: an optimal foraging approach. Curr. Anthropol. 49, 935–944 10.1086/592021 (doi:10.1086/592021) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Bleed P. 2006. Living in the human niche. Evol. Anthropol. 15, 8–10 10.1002/evan.20084 (doi:10.1002/evan.20084) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Hare B., Brown M., Williamson C., Tomasello M. 2002. The domestication of social cognition in dogs. Science 298, 1634–1636 10.1126/science.1072702 (doi:10.1126/science.1072702) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Price E. O. 1999. Behavioral development in animals undergoing domestication. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 65, 245–271 10.1016/S0168-1591(99)00087-8 (doi:10.1016/S0168-1591(99)00087-8) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Serpell J. (ed.) 1995. The domestic dog: its evolution, behaviour, and interactions with people. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press [Google Scholar]
- 106.Morey D. F. 2006. Burying key evidence: the social bond between dogs and people. J. Archaeol. Sci. 33, 158–175 10.1016/j.jas.2005.07.009 (doi:10.1016/j.jas.2005.07.009) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Pang J.-F., et al. 2009. mtDNA data indicate a single origin for dogs south of Yangtze River, less than 16 300 years ago, from numerous wolves. Mol. Biol. Evol. 26, 2849–2864 10.1093/molbev/msp195 (doi:10.1093/molbev/msp195) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Germonpré M., Sablin M. V., Stevens R. E., Hedges R. E. M., Hofreiter M., Stiller M., Després V. R. 2009. Fossil dogs and wolves from Palaeolithic sites in Belgium, the Ukraine and Russia: osteometry, ancient DNA and stable isotopes. J. Archaeol. Sci. 36, 473–490 10.1016/j.jas.2008.09.033 (doi:10.1016/j.jas.2008.09.033) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Sablin M. V., Khlopachev G. A. 2002. The earliest ice age dogs: evidence from Eliseevichi 1. Curr. Anthropol. 43, 795–799 10.1086/344372 (doi:10.1086/344372) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Morey D. F. 1994. The early evolution of the domestic dog. Am. Sci. 82, 336–347 [Google Scholar]
- 111.Benecke N. 1987. Studies on early dog remains from Northern Europe. J. Archaeol. Sci. 14, 31–49 10.1016/S0305-4403(87)80004-3 (doi:10.1016/S0305-4403(87)80004-3) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Malmström H., Vila C., Gilbert M. T., Storå J., Willerslev E., Holmlund G., Gotherström A. 2008. Barking up the wrong tree: modern northern European dogs fail to explain their origin. BMC Evol. Biol. 8, 71. 10.1186/1471-2148-8-71 (doi:10.1186/1471-2148-8-71) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Bratlund B. 1996. Hunting strategies in the Late Glacial of Northern Europe: a survey of the faunal evidence. J. World Prehist. 10, 1–48 10.1007/BF02226070 (doi:10.1007/BF02226070) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Grøn O. 2005. A Siberian perspective on the north European Hamburgian culture: a study in applied hunter–gatherer ethnoarchaeology. Before Farming 2005, 35–64 [Google Scholar]
- 115.Pohlhausen H. 1953. Nachweisbare Ansätze zum Wanderhirtentum in der niederdeutschen Mittelsteinzeit. Zeitschr. Ethnol. 78, 64–82 [Google Scholar]
- 116.Pohlhausen H. 1972. Standpunkte zur Diskussion über das Alter der Viehzucht. Anthropos 67, 176–195 [Google Scholar]
- 117.Sturdy D. A. 1975. Some reindeer economies in prehistoric Europe. In Palaeoeconomy (ed. Higgs E.), pp. 55–95 Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press [Google Scholar]
- 118.Forbes B. C., Bölter M., Müller-Wille L., Hukkinen J., Müller F., Gunslav N., Kostantinov Y. (eds) 2006. Reindeer management in northernmost Europe. Berlin, Germany: Springer [Google Scholar]
- 119.Bahn P. 1984. Preneolithic control of animals in western Europe: the faunal evidence. In Animals and archaeology: 4. Husbandry in Europe (eds Clutton-Brock J., Grigson G.), pp. 27–34 Oxford, UK: Oxbow [Google Scholar]
- 120.Gordon B. 2003. Rangifer and man: an ancient relationship. Rangifer 14, 15–28 [Google Scholar]
- 121.Müller-Wille L., Heinrich D., Lehtola V.-P., Aikio P., Kostantinov Y., Vladimirova V. 2006. Dynamics in human–reindeer relations: reflections on prehistoric, historic and contemporary practices in northernmost Europe. In Reindeer management in northernmost Europe (eds Forbes B. C., Bölter M., Müller-Wille L., Hukkinen J., Müller F., Gunslav N., Kostantinov Y.), pp. 27–45 Berlin, Germany: Springer [Google Scholar]
- 122.Weinstock J. 2000. Osteometry as a source of refined demographic information: sex-ratios of reindeer, hunting strategies, and herd control in the Late Glacial site of Stellmoor, northern Germany. J. Archaeol. Sci. 27, 1187–1195 10.1006/jasc.1999.0542 (doi:10.1006/jasc.1999.0542) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Røed K. H., Flagstad Ø., Nieminen M., Holand Ø., Dwyer M. J., Røv N., Vilá C. 2008. Genetic analyses reveal independent domestication origins of Eurasian reindeer. Proc. R. Soc. B 275, 1849–1855 10.1098/rspb.2008.0332 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.0332) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Benecke N. 1994. Der Mensch und seine Haustiere. Die Geschichte einer Jahrtausendealten Beziehung. Stuttgart, Germany: Theiss [Google Scholar]
- 125.Istomin K., Dwyer M. 2010. Dynamic mutual adaptation: human–animal interaction in reindeer herding pastoralism. Hum. Ecol. 38, 613–623 10.1007/s10745-010-9341-3 (doi:10.1007/s10745-010-9341-3) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Ingold T. 1986. Reindeer economies and the origins of pastoralism. Anthropol. Today 2, 5–10 10.2307/3032710 (doi:10.2307/3032710) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Ventsel A. 2006. Hunter–herder continuum in Anabarski district, NW Sakha, Siberia, Russian Federation. Nomadic Peoples 10, 68–86 10.3167/np.2006.100205 (doi:10.3167/np.2006.100205) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Helskog K. 1977. Et reingjerde fra steinalderen. Ottar 101, 25–29 [Google Scholar]
- 129.Helskog K. In press Reindeer corrals 4700–4200 BC: myth or reality? Quat. Int. (doi:10.1016/j.quaint.2010.10.001) [Google Scholar]
- 130.Fischer A. 1989. A Late Palaeolithic ‘school’ of flint-knapping at Trollesgave, Denmark. Results from refitting. Acta Archaeol. 60, 33–49 [Google Scholar]
- 131.Riede F. 2006. Chaîne opèratoire—Chaîne evolutionaire. Putting technological sequences in evolutionary context. Archaeol. Rev. Camb. 21, 50–75 [Google Scholar]
- 132.Eriksen B. V. 2000. Patterns of ethnogeographic variability in Late Pleistocene western Europe. In Regional approaches to adaptation in Late Pleistocene western Europe (eds Peterkin G. L., Price H. A.), pp. 147–168 Oxford, UK: Oxbow [Google Scholar]
- 133.Müller W. 2005. The domestication of the wolf—the inevitable first? In First steps of animal domestication (eds Vigne J.-D., Peters J., Helmer D.), Oxford, UK: Oxbow [Google Scholar]
- 134.Morey D. F., Aaris-Sørensen K. 2002. Paleoeskimo dogs of the eastern Arctic. Arctic 55, 44–56 [Google Scholar]
- 135.Gamble C., Davies W., Pettitt P., Hazelwood L., Richards M. 2006. The Late Glacial ancestry of Europeans: combining genetic and archaeological evidence. Doc. Praehist. 13, 1–10 [Google Scholar]
- 136.Gamble C., Davies W., Pettitt P., Richards M. 2004. Climate change and evolving human diversity in Europe during the last glacial. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 359, 243–254 10.1098/rstb.2003.1396 (doi:10.1098/rstb.2003.1396) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137.Gamble C., Davies W., Pettitt P., Richards M. 2005. The archaeological and genetic foundations of the European population during the Late Glacial: implications for ‘agricultural thinking’. Camb. Archaeol. J. 15, 193–223 10.1017/S0959774305000107 (doi:10.1017/S0959774305000107) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 138.Björck S., Walker M. J. C., Cwynar L. C., Johnsen S., Knudsen K.-L., Lowe J. J., Wohlfarth B. & INTIMATE members 1998. An event stratigraphy for the last termination in the North Atlantic region based on the Greenland ice-core record: a proposal by the INTIMATE group. J. Quat. Sci. 13, 283–292 (doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-1417(199807/08)13:4<283::AID-JQS386>3.0.CO;2-A) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 139.Lowe J. J., Rasmussen S. O., Bjorck S., Hoek W. Z., Steffensen J. P., Walker M. J. C., Yu Z. C. 2008. Synchronisation of palaeoenvironmental events in the North Atlantic region during the Last Termination: a revised protocol recommended by the INTIMATE group. Quat. Sci. Rev. 27, 6–17 10.1016/j.quascirev.2007.09.016 (doi:10.1016/j.quascirev.2007.09.016) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 140.Bocquet-Appel J.-P., Demars P.-Y., Noiret L., Dobrowsky D. 2005. Estimates of Upper Palaeolithic meta-population size in Europe from archaeological data. J. Archaeol. Sci. 32, 1656–1668 10.1016/j.jas.2005.05.006 (doi:10.1016/j.jas.2005.05.006) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 141.Boone J. L. 2002. Subsistence strategies and early human population history: an evolutionary ecological perspective. World Archaeol. 34, 6–25 10.1080/00438240220134232 (doi:10.1080/00438240220134232) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142.Price T. D. 1999. Human population in Europe during the Mesolithic. In Den Bogen spannen … Festschrift für Bernhard Gramsch (eds Cziesla E., Kersting T., Pratsch S.), pp. 185–195 Weissbach, Germany: Beier & Beran [Google Scholar]
- 143.Eriksen B. V. 1999. Late Palaeolithic settlement in Denmark—how do we read the record? In Post-pleniglacial re-colonisation of the Great European Lowland (eds Kobusiewicz M., Kozlowski J. K.), pp. 157–174 Kraków, Poland: Polska Akademia Umiejetnosci [Google Scholar]
- 144.Petersen E. B. 2009. The human settlement of southern Scandinavia 12 500–8700 cal BC. In Humans, environment and chronology of the late glacial of the North European Plain (eds Street M., Barton R. N. E., Terberger T.), pp. 89–129 Mainz, Germany: Verlag des Römisch-Germanischen Zentralmuseums [Google Scholar]
- 145.Madsen B. 1996. Late Palaeolithic cultures of south Scandinavia: tools, traditions and technology. In The earliest settlement of Scandinavia and its relationship with neighbouring areas (ed. Larsson L.), pp. 61–73 Stockholm, Sweden: Almqvist & Wiksell [Google Scholar]
- 146.Pettitt P. 2008. The British Upper Palaeolithic. In Prehistoric Britain (ed. Pollard J.), pp. 18–57 Oxford, UK: Blackwell [Google Scholar]
- 147.Riede F. 2007. Der Ausbruch des Laacher See-Vulkans vor 12.920 Jahren und urgeschichtlicher Kulturwandel am Ende des Alleröd. Eine neue Hypothese zum Ursprung der Bromme Kultur und des Perstunien. Mitteilungen der Gesellschaft für Urgeschichte 16, 25–54 [Google Scholar]
- 148.Riede F. 2008. The Laacher See—eruption (12 920 BP) and material culture change at the end of the Allerød in Northern Europe. J. Archaeol. Sci. 35, 591–599 10.1016/j.jas.2007.05.007 (doi:10.1016/j.jas.2007.05.007) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 149.Riede F., Bazely O. 2009. Testing the ‘Laacher See hypothesis’: a health hazard perspective. J. Archaeol. Sci. 36, 675–683 10.1016/j.jas.2008.10.013 (doi:10.1016/j.jas.2008.10.013) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 150.Riede F., Wheeler J. M. 2009. Testing the ‘Laacher See hypothesis’: tephra as dental abrasive. J. Archaeol. Sci. 36, 2384–2391 10.1016/j.jas.2009.06.020 (doi:10.1016/j.jas.2009.06.020) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 151.Baales M. 1996. Umwelt und Jagdökonomie der Ahrensburger Rentierjäger im Mittelgebirge. Bonn, Germany: Verlag Rudolf Habelt GmbH [Google Scholar]
- 152.Baales M. 1999. Economy and seasonality in the Ahrensburgian. In Tanged point cultures in Europe. Read at the International Archaeological Symposium, Lublin, 13–16 September 1993 (eds Kozlowski S. K., Gurba J., Zaliznyak L. L.), pp. 64–75 Lublin, Poland: Maria Curie-Sklodowska University Press [Google Scholar]
- 153.Bratlund B. 1999. A survey of the Ahrensburgian faunal assemblage of Stellmoor. In Tanged point cultures in Europe. Read at the International Archaeological Symposium, Lublin, 13–16 September 1993 (eds Kozlowski S. K., Gurba J., Zaliznyak L. L.), pp. 47–59 Lublin, Poland: Maria Curie-Sklodowska University Press [Google Scholar]
- 154.Bokelmann K. 1991. Some new thoughts on old data on humans and reindeer in the Ahrensburg tunnel valley in Schleswig-Holstein, Germany. In Late Glacial in north-west Europe: human adaptation and environmental change at the end of the Pleistocene (eds Barton R. N. E., Roberts A. J., Roe D. A.), pp. 72–81 Oxford, UK: Council for British Archaeology [Google Scholar]
- 155.Riede F. 2007. ‘Stretched thin, like butter on too much bread … ’: some thoughts about journeying in the unfamiliar landscapes of late Palaeolithic Southern Scandinavia. In Prehistoric journeys (eds Johnson R., Cummings V.), pp. 8–20 Oxford, UK: Oxbow [Google Scholar]
- 156.Price T. D. 1991. The view from Europe: concepts and questions about Terminal Pleistocene societies. In First Americans: search and research (eds Dillehay T. D., Meltzer D.), pp. 185–208 Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press [Google Scholar]
- 157.Day R. L., Laland K. N., Odling-Smee J., Feldman M. W. 2003. Rethinking adaptation: the niche construction perspective. Perspect. Biol. Med. 46, 80–95 10.1353/pbm.2003.0003 (doi:10.1353/pbm.2003.0003) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 158.O'Brien M. J., Lyman R. L. (eds) 2003. Style, function, transmission: evolutionary archaeological perspectives. Salt Lake City, UT: University of Utah Press [Google Scholar]
- 159.Riede F. In press Steps towards operationalizing an evolutionary archaeological definition of culture. In Investigating archaeological cultures. Material culture, variability, and transmission (eds Roberts B., Vanderlinden M.). Berlin, Germany: Springer [Google Scholar]
- 160.O'Brien M. J., Lyman R. L. 2000. Applying evolutionary archaeology. A systematic approach. New York, NY: Kluwer Academic/Plenum [Google Scholar]
- 161.O'Brien M. J., Lyman R. L., Glover D. S., Darwent J. 2003. Cladistics and archaeology. Salt Lake City, UT: University of Utah Press [Google Scholar]
- 162.Darwent J., O'Brien M. J. 2006. Using cladistics to construct lineages of projectile points from northeastern Missouri. In Mapping our ancestors. Phylogenetic approaches in anthropology and prehistory (eds Lipo C. P., O'Brien M. J., Collard M., Shennan S. J.), pp. 185–208 New Brunswick, NJ: AldineTransaction [Google Scholar]
- 163.Bandelt H.-J., Forster P., Röhl A. 1999. Median-joining networks for inferring intraspecific phylogenies. Mol. Biol. Evol. 16, 37–48 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 164.Morrison D. A. 2005. Networks in phylogenetic analysis: new tools for population biology. Int. J. Parasitol. 35, 567–582 10.1016/j.ijpara.2005.02.007 (doi:10.1016/j.ijpara.2005.02.007) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 165.Pagel M., Meade A. 2004. A phylogenetic mixture model for detecting pattern-heterogeneity in gene sequence or character-state data. Syst. Biol. 53, 571–581 10.1080/10635150490468675 (doi:10.1080/10635150490468675) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 166.Baales M. 1992. Überreste von Hunden aus der Ahrensburger Kultur am Karstein, Nordeifel. Archäol. Korrespondezbl. 22, 461–471 [Google Scholar]
- 167.Nobis G. 1986. Die Wildsäugetiere in der Umwelt des Menschen von Oberkassel bei Bonn und das Domestikationsproblem bei Wölfen im Jungpaläolithikum. Bonner Jahrb. 186, 367–376 10.1594/PANGAEA.64577 (doi:pangaea.de/10.1594/PANGAEA.64577) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 168.Staesche U. 1994. Die Tierreste aus dem Buntsandsteinabris im Leinebergland bei Göttingen. In Die Abris im südlichen Leinebergland bei Göttingen. Archäologische Befunde zum Leben unter Felsschutzdächern in urgeschichtlicher Zeit (ed. Grote K.), pp. 101–140 Oldenburg, Germany: Isensee Verlag [Google Scholar]
- 169.Street M. 2002. Ein Wiedersehen mit dem Hund von Bonn-Oberkassel. Bonn. Zool. Beitr. 50, 269–290 [Google Scholar]
- 170.Walker D. N. 2000. Preliminary bibliography on dogs and wolves, stressing their prehistoric and historic occurrence, hybridisation, and domestication. In Dogs through time: An archaeological perspective (ed. Crockford S. J.), pp. 313–343 Oxford, UK: Archaeopress [Google Scholar]
- 171.Eerkens J., Lipo C. P. 2007. Cultural transmission theory and the archaeological record: providing context to understanding variation and temporal changes in material culture. J. Archaeol. Res. 15, 239–274 10.1007/s10814-007-9013-z (doi:10.1007/s10814-007-9013-z) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 172.Pagel M. 1994. Detecting correlated evolution on phylogenies—a general method for the comparative analysis of discrete characters. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 255, 37–45 10.1098/rspb.1994.0006 (doi:10.1098/rspb.1994.0006) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 173.Pagel M., Meade A. 2005. Bayesian estimation of correlated evolution across cultures: a case study of marriage systems and wealth transfer at marriage. In The evolution of cultural diversity. A phylogenetic approach (eds Mace R., Holden C. J., Shennan S. J.), pp. 235–256 London, UK: UCL Press [Google Scholar]
- 174.Kass R. E., Raftery A. E. 1995. Bayes factors. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 90, 773–795 10.2307/2291091 (doi:10.2307/2291091) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 175.Anonymous 1987. Editorial: Man, dogs, and hydatid disease. Lancet 329, 21–22 10.1016/S0140-6736(87)90706-9 (doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(87)90706-9) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 176.Riede F. 2009. Climate change, demography and social relations: an alternative view of the Late Palaeolithic pioneer colonization of Southern Scandinavia. In Mesolithic Horizons. Papers presented at the 17th Int. Conf. on the Mesolithic in Europe, Belfast 2005. vol. 1 (eds McCartan S., Woodman P. C., Schulting R. J., Warren G.), pp. 3–10 Oxford, UK: Oxbow [Google Scholar]
- 177.Karlsson A. O., Wallerström T., Götherström A., Holmlund G. 2006. Y-chromosome diversity in Sweden—a long-time perspective. Eur. J. Hum. Genet. 14, 963–970 10.1038/sj.ejhg.5201651 (doi:10.1038/sj.ejhg.5201651) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 178.Lappalainen T., Salmela E., Andersen P. M., Dahlman-Wright K., Sistonen P., Savontaus M.-L., Schreiber S., Lahermo P., Kere J. 2010. Genomic landscape of positive natural selection in Northern European populations. Eur. J. Hum. Genet. 18, 471–478 10.1038/ejhg.2009.184 (doi:10.1038/ejhg.2009.184) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 179.Pereira L., et al. 2005. High-resolution mtDNA evidence for the late-glacial resettlement of Europe from an Iberian refugium. Genome Res. 15, 19–24 10.1101/gr.3182305 (doi:10.1101/gr.3182305) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 180.Cziesla E. 1992. Ahrensburger Jäger in Südwestdeutschland? Archäol. Korrespondezbl. 22, 13–26 [Google Scholar]
- 181.Cziesla E. 2007. Robbenjagd in Brandenburg? Gedanken zur Verwendung großer Widerhakenspitzen. Ethnogr.-archäol. Zeitschr. 48, 1–48 [Google Scholar]
- 182.Riede F., Grimm S. B., Weber M.-J., Fahlke J. M. 2010. Neue Daten für alte Grabungen. Ein Beitrag zur spätglazialen Archäologie und Faunengeschichte Norddeutschlands. Archäol. Korrespondezbl. 40, 297–316 [Google Scholar]
- 183.Laland K. N., Brown G. R. 2006. Niche construction, human behavior, and the adaptive-lag hypothesis. Evol. Anthropol. 15, 95–104 10.1002/evan.20093 (doi:10.1002/evan.20093) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 184.Freckleton R. P., Phillimore A. B., Pagel M. 2008. Relating traits to diversification: a simple test. Am. Nat. 172, 102–115 10.1086/588076 (doi:10.1086/588076) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 185.Maddison W. P. 2006. Confounding asymmetries in evolutionary diversification and character change. Evolution 60, 1743–1746 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2006.tb00517.x (doi:10.1111/j.0014-3820.2006.tb00517.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 186.Freckleton R. P. 2009. The seven deadly sins of comparative analysis. J. Evol. Biol. 22, 1367–1375 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2009.01757.x (doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2009.01757.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 187.Crockford S. J. 2000. A commentary on dog evolution: regional variation, breed development and hybridisation with wolves. In Dogs through time: an archaeological perspective (ed. Crockford S. J.), pp. 295–312 Oxford, UK: Archaeopress [Google Scholar]
- 188.Leonard J. A., Wayne R. K., Wheeler J., Valadez R., Guillen S., Vila C. 2002. Ancient DNA evidence for Old World origin of New World dogs. Science 298, 1613–1616 10.1126/science.1076980 (doi:10.1126/science.1076980) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 189.Riede F. 2005. Darwin vs. Bourdieu. Celebrity deathmatch or postprocessual myth? Prolegomenon for the reconciliation of agentive-interpretative and ecological-evolutionary archaeology. In Investigating prehistoric hunter–gatherer identities: case studies from Palaeolithic and Mesolithic Europe (eds Cobb H., Price S., Coward F., Grimshaw L.), pp. 45–64 Oxford, UK: Oxbow [Google Scholar]
- 190.Riede F. 2005. To boldly go where no (hu-)man has gone before. Some thoughts on the pioneer colonisations of pristine landscapes. Archaeol. Rev. Camb. 20, 20–38 [Google Scholar]
- 191.Riede F. 2009. Niche construction applied: triple-inheritance insights into the pioneer Late Glacial colonization of southern Scandinavia. In Theoretical and methodological issues in evolutionary archaeology. Toward a unified Darwinian paradigm. Proc. XV World Congress UISPP (Lisbon, 4–9 September 2006). Session 20—W22 (eds Muscio H. J., López G. E. J.), pp. 83–94 Oxford, UK: Archaeopress [Google Scholar]
- 192.Erwin D. H. 2008. Macroevolution of ecosystem engineering, niche construction and diversity. Trends Ecol. Evol. 23, 304–310 10.1016/j.tree.2008.01.013 (doi:10.1016/j.tree.2008.01.013) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 193.Krakauer D. C., Page K. M., Erwin D. H. 2009. Diversity, dilemmas, and monopolies of niche construction. Am. Nat. 173, 26–40 10.1086/593707 (doi:10.1086/593707) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 194.Phillips J. D. 2009. Soils as extended composite phenotypes. Geoderma 149, 143–151 10.1016/j.geoderma.2008.11.028 (doi:10.1016/j.geoderma.2008.11.028) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 195.Wilkinson M. T., Richards P. J., Humphreys G. S. 2009. Breaking ground: pedological, geological, and ecological implications of soil bioturbation. Earth Sci. Rev. 97, 254–269 10.1016/j.earscirev.2009.09.005 (doi:10.1016/j.earscirev.2009.09.005) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 196.Corenblit D., Gurnell A. M., Steiger J., Tabacchi E. 2008. Reciprocal adjustments between landforms and living organisms: extended geomorphic evolutionary insights. Catena 73, 261–273 10.1016/j.catena.2007.11.002 (doi:10.1016/j.catena.2007.11.002) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 197.Corenblit D., Steiger J., Gurnell A. M., Naiman R. J. 2009. Plants intertwine fluvial landform dynamics with ecological succession and natural selection: a niche construction perspective for riparian systems. Global Ecol. Biogeogr. 18, 507–520 10.1111/j.1466-8238.2009.00461.x (doi:10.1111/j.1466-8238.2009.00461.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 198.Corenblit D., Steiger J., Gurnell A. M., Tabacchi E. 2007. Darwinian origin of landforms. Earth Surf. Proc. Land. 32, 2070–2073 10.1002/esp.1536 (doi:10.1002/esp.1536) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 199.Corenblit D., Tabacchi E., Steiger J., Gurnell A. M. 2007. Reciprocal interactions and adjustments between fluvial landforms and vegetation dynamics in river corridors: a review of complementary approaches. Earth Sci. Rev. 84, 56–86 10.1016/j.earscirev.2007.05.004 (doi:10.1016/j.earscirev.2007.05.004) [DOI] [Google Scholar]