Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2012 Mar 1.
Published in final edited form as: Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2010 Sep 24;214(1):55–70. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-2009-2

Effects of Early Life Stress on Cognitive and Affective Function: An Integrated Review of Human Literature

Pia Pechtel 1, Diego A Pizzagalli 1
PMCID: PMC3050094  NIHMSID: NIHMS254811  PMID: 20865251

Abstract

Rationale

The investigation of putative effects of early life stress (ELS) in humans on later behavior and neurobiology is a fast developing field. While epidemiological and neurobiological studies paint a somber picture of negative outcomes, relatively little attention has been devoted to integrating the breadth of findings concerning possible cognitive and emotional deficits associated with ELS. Emerging findings from longitudinal studies examining developmental trajectories of the brain in healthy samples may provide a new framework to understand mechanisms underlying ELS sequelae.

Objective

The goal of this review was two-fold. The first was to summarize findings from longitudinal data on normative brain development. The second was to utilize this framework of normative brain development to interpret changes in developmental trajectories associated with deficits in cognitive and affective function following ELS.

Results

Five principles of normative brain development were identified and used to discuss behavioral and neural sequelae of ELS. Early adversity was found to be associated with deficits in a range of cognitive (cognitive performance, memory, and executive functioning) and affective (reward processing, processing of social and affective stimuli, and emotion regulation) functions.

Conclusion

Three general conclusions emerge: (1) higher-order, complex cognitive and affective functions associated with brain regions undergoing protracted postnatal development are particularly vulnerable to the deleterious effects of ELS; (2) the amygdala is particularly sensitive to early ELS; and (3) several deficits, particularly those in the affective domain, appear to persist years after ELS has ceased and may increase risk for later psychopathology.

Keywords: Early life stress, brain, child abuse, cognitive function, emotion regulation

Introduction

When a person is challenged in their emotional or physical well-being to an extent that exceeds their ability to cope, stress ensues (Gunnar and Quevedo 2007). Early life stress (ELS) is the exposure to a single or multiple events during childhood that exceeds the child’s coping resources and leads to prolonged phases of stress. Commonly studied early childhood stressors include physical, sexual, emotional or verbal abuse, neglect, social deprivation, disaster or household dysfunctions (including witnessing of violence, criminal activity, parental separation, parental death or illness, poverty, substance abuse) (Brown et al. 2009).

Due to the array of stressors subsumed by the term ELS, obtaining a clear estimate of how many children experience ELS remains challenging. In 2007, 3.5 million (22.5%) children came to the attention of Child Protective Services in the United States alone. The rate of abuse and neglect, not considering household dysfunction, reached 10.6 % in 2007 (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services 2009). Due to low rates of disclosure, these figures likely underestimate the incidence of childhood trauma (Finklehor 1994; Pereda et al. 2009).

ELS can be studied behaviorally and biologically. While the acute activation of the body’s stress response systems is considered an adaptive mechanism that mobilizes resources to increase chances of survival, high or chronic levels of stress may disturb brain development and affect mental health (Anda et al. 2006; De Bellis et al. 1999a; De Bellis et al. 1999b; Lupien et al. 2009; Maniglio 2009; Pirkola et al. 2005; Spataro et al. 2004). Although epidemiological and neurobiological studies portray a somber picture of negative outcomes following ELS (De Bellis 2005; Grassi-Oliveira et al. 2008; Teicher et al. 2003; Weber and Reynolds 2004), relatively little attention has been devoted to evaluating putative cognitive and emotional outcomes of ELS. Such knowledge could prove pivotal in tailoring early intervention and preventing long-term sequelae. In this review, we adopt a developmental approach in which we summarize recent findings on normative brain development in childhood and adolescence to assist later discussion on the disruptions of developmental trajectories of the brain following ELS.

Goals

The objective of this review was to review and integrate changes in the neurobiological pathways associated with cognitive and emotional functioning following ELS within the framework of recent longitudinal studies of normal human brain development. Priority was given to studies examining structural or functional sequelae of early adversity in conjunction with cognitive or emotional correlates. While ELS may affect an array of functions, due to space constraints, we have focused on behavioral phenotypes most widely investigated in both the psychological and neuroscientific literature so that inferences about primary pathways of early neurobiological disruptions affecting later well-being could be advanced. In the first section, recent findings of longitudinal studies in healthy samples are reviewed to identify overarching principles of normative brain development. Mechanisms potentially disrupting the trajectory of brain development in those with a history of ELS are also discussed. In the second section, we specifically focus on structural and functional changes associated with deficits in cognitive function (cognitive performance, memory, and executive functioning) and affective functions (reward processing, processing of social and affective stimuli, and emotion regulation). In each section, behavioral, neuroimaging and psychophysiological data are integrated, and selected animal findings are discussed (see other contributions in this issue for more comprehensive reviews of the animal literature on ELS). The review ends with a discussion of future directions of research on ELS.

Early life stress and psychopathology

The recent National Comorbidity Survey highlighted that childhood adverse experiences explain nearly 32% of psychiatric disorders, and an even higher percentage (44%) of disorders with childhood onset (Green et al. 2010). Various forms of early adversity account for about 67% of the population-attributable risk for suicide (Dube et al. 2001). Exposure to multiple episodes of ELS can significantly increase the risk of mental illness and somatic disturbances (Anda et al. 1999; Anda et al. 2006; Anda et al. 2007; Chapman et al. 2004; Cutrona et al. 2005; Dong et al. 2004; Edwards et al. 2003; Pirkola et al. 2005). Notably, adults with more than six adverse childhood events had a higher likelihood of dying 20 years earlier than those without such histories (Anda et al. 2009; Brown et al. 2009). Others have emphasized the need for more differentiated research methods in order to investigate how common clusters of co-occurring ELS experiences are related to psychopathology (Green et al. 2010; Wright et al. 2009).

Developmental timing

The sequelae of ELS often depend on the type of adversity, the number of exposures, and, in particular, the age at the time of occurrence. For example, adults who were sexually abused in childhood after the age of 12 were 10 times more likely to develop severe symptoms of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) compared to those who experienced sexual abuse prior to the age of 12 (Schoedl et al. 2010). Teicher and colleagues conducted extensive studies on “sensitive periods” emphasizing that the timing of ELS may especially affect those brain regions undergoing specific growth spurts at the time (Teicher et al. 2006a, 2006b). For example, among females, experience of sexual abuse between the ages of 3–5 (and marginally between 11–13 years) was related to smaller hippocampal volume. Conversely, sexual abuse occurring between age 9–10 and 11–14 years was linked to dysfunctions in the corpus callosum and prefrontal cortex (PFC), respectively (Andersen et al. 2008). Based on these and other data, it was concluded that brain regions with extended postnatal development are particularly vulnerable to long-term effects of stress (Teicher et al. 2003).

While research has emphasized the outcomes of ELS on mental health, less attention has been devoted to explaining the link between cognitive and emotional sequelae of ELS and brain development. Before summarizing findings on normative brain development in childhood and adolescence, we would like to emphasize several key points relevant to understanding the limitation of the following discussion. First, neuroimaging research commonly focuses on changes in regional brain volume or cortical thickness. It should be kept in mind that while these volumetric changes are an important mean to identify possible abnormalities, little is known about the specific mechanisms underlying these changes. Disruptions in neurogenesis, myelination, and synaptic pruning are often invoked to explain structural changes, but modest empirical support exists for these claims (Gogtay and Thompson 2010). Furthermore, while delineating structural abnormalities in single regions is an important first step for investigating cognitive and affective deficits following ELS, our limited understanding of the effects of ELS on functional connectivity and development of neural circuits remains an important challenge for the future. Lastly, throughout the review, and consistent with recent publications, we refer to primary, lower-order functions vs. complex, higher-order functions (Gogtay and Thompson 2010; Marsh et al 2008). This differentiation should not imply that primary brain functions are deemed as less critical, especially as they become increasingly involved in complex computations during maturation. Instead we emphasize that regional brain development mirrors the immediate needs of each developmental stage which, in turn, may help to explain the diversity in outcomes depending on the timing of ELS.

Brain development in healthy children and adolescents

Brain development is an exceedingly complex process as the organism needs to respond to the challenges and demands of the environment (Teicher et al. 2006b). In this section, we highlight five principles of brain development derived from recent findings in healthy longitudinal and cross-sectional samples, which represent the framework along which ELS acts.

Principle 1: Human brain development is largely a nonlinear process

(Giedd et al. 1999; Gogtay and Thompson 2010; Shaw et al. 2008). While the developmental trajectory of grey matter (GM) volume follows an inverted U-shape, white matter (WM) volume increases steadily throughout childhood and adolescence and is likely to reflect improved connectivity and integration of disparate neural circuits (Gerber and Peterson. 2009; Giedd et al. 1999). More specifically, different brain regions have been shown to develop along three main patterns of maturation. Cubic developmental trajectories are characterized by an early increase in GM cortical thickness followed by a decrease in adolescence and stabilization in adulthood. This applies, for example, to lateral frontal, lateral temporal, parietal and occipital cortex (Shaw et al. 2008). Cortical thickness in the insula and anterior cingulate follows a quadratic developmental course, increasing over childhood, decreasing in adolescence but demonstrating no phase of stabilization of cortical thickness within first three decades of life. Finally, a linear developmental trajectory corresponding to increased cortical thickness with age is characteristic of a large number of brain regions (e.g., posterior orbitofrontal, frontal operculum, portions of piriform cortex, subgenual cingulate areas) (Shaw et al. 2008). With various brain regions undergoing different patterns of maturation, neural consequences of ELS depend on the developmental stage at which the stress occurred (Teicher et al. 2003). Stress may therefore have a greater impact in childhood and adolescence as the brain experiences critical changes compared to adulthood (e.g., cubic developmental trajectory).

Principle 2: Higher-order association cortices develop only after lower-order sensorimotor cortices have matured in structure and function

(Gogtay et al. 2004; Gogtay and Thompson 2010; Shaw et al. 2008). During the process of development, essential structures such as sensorimotor cortex and occipital poles develop first. The remainder of the cortex matures in a back-to-front direction (parietal to frontal). This ensures the child’s early availability of primary motor and sensory function including vision, taste and smell followed by areas involving spatial orientation, speech, language, and attention. Higher-order structures that contain complex, association sites develop very late in the brain’s trajectory. Among the last areas to develop are frontal lobe structures (e.g. dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC)) involved in executive functioning, attention, motor coordination as well as heteromodal association regions (e.g., superior temporal cortex). Association areas mature until basic earlier-developing sensory regions, whose function they integrate, are developed (Gogtay et al. 2004; Gogtay and Thompson 2010; Shaw et al. 2008). It can thus be hypothesized that complex functions in higher-order structures are more susceptible to the effects of ELS due to protracted periods of postnatal development.

Principle 3: Ontogeny recapitulates phylogeny

(Gogtay et al. 2004; Gogtay and Thompson 2010; Lenroot et al. 2009; Shaw et al. 2008). The brain regions latest to mature in development – including the DLPFC, orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), temporal lobes, and superior parietal lobes – are the most recent areas from an evolutionary point of view (Lenroot et al. 2009). Again, these evolutionary ‘young’ structures are linked to higher-order, complex skills such as decision-making, executive functioning, and inhibition (Gogtay et al. 2004; Gogtay and Thompson 2010; Shaw et al. 2008) and are at greater risk of impairment following ELS.

Principle 4: Brain development is guided by genes but sculpted by the environment

(Lenroot and Giedd, 2008; Lenroot et al. 2009; Peper et al. 2009; Schmitt et al. 2008). While early experiences have pivotal effects on brain developmental, recent twin studies in healthy samples also identified substantial genetic influences. For example, a large research study using magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data from 308 twins, 64 siblings of twins, and 228 singletons with no twin siblings found that a single factor accounted for up to 60% of genetic variance in cortical thickness (Schmitt et al. 2008). Genetic effects were largest for frontal lobes while their influence gradually declined from temporal to parietal lobes with the occipital lobe showing the smallest degree of heritability (Schmitt et al. 2008). Moreover, later-maturing brain regions (e.g., DLPFC, superior parietal cortex, temporal cortex, language-associated regions) involved in complex cognitive processes (e.g., executive function, language) become more heritable with increasing age (Lenroot and Giedd 2008; Lenroot et al. 2009). As heritability varies with age, brain regions that develop earlier during development show stronger genetic influences earlier in life, while later developing brain regions are under greater genetic influences later in life (Lenroot et al. 2009). The effects of ELS on brain development may therefore be potentiated or buffered, depending on the strength and timing of genetic influences on specific brain regions.

Principle 5: Trajectories of brain development differ for females and males

(Gerber and Peterson. 2009; Lenroot et al. 2007; Lenroot and Giedd 2010; Marsh et al 2008). Gender differences can be found in overall brain volume, with males having a proportionally larger (between 9–12%) mean cerebral volume despite reaching GM peak volume 1–3 years later than females (Gerber and Peterson. 2009; Lenroot and Giedd, 2010). However, not all studies found gender differences in total brain volume, especially when controlling for intracranial volume (Peper et al. 2009). Gender specific difference in brain regions occur primarily in areas containing sex steroid receptors (e.g., hypothalamus) or regions with strong connections to areas with high sex steroids receptor density (e.g., amygdala, parts of the nucleus of the stria terminialis) (Lenroot and Giedd, 2010). As developmental trajectories differ for males and females, ELS occurring at the same time may lead to diverse, sex-specific outcomes.

Interim summary: normative brain development

Findings from healthy samples suggest that a given brain region may be affected by ELS differently at different times depending on its pattern of growth. Moreover, high-order cortices and complex functions develop after lower-order cortices and primary functions have matured. Genetic and sex influences jointly contribute to individual differences in brain development. The following section will review the specific mechanisms by which ELS can impact brain development.

Early life stress and brain development

Since the early 1990s, a large body of neurobiological and neuroendocrine studies has highlighted the impact of chronic stress on brain development. The most accepted explanation for alterations in brain structures postulates that ELS interferes with the critical waves of neurogenesis, synaptic overproduction, and pruning of synapses and receptors (Teicher et al. 2006). As the loss in GM often exceeds what can be explained by aberrant increases in synaptic pruning, an expansion in WM through an increase in myelination in intra-cortical axons may also contribute to the apparent loss of GM (for review, see Paus et al. 2008). Due to limited resolution of current MRIs, the cellular processes underlying such structural changes, however, remain impenetrable with in vivo neuroimaging techniques (Gogtay and Thompson 2010).

Mechanisms of stress

From a biological point of view, encountering a stressful situation activates the sympathetic nervous system and hypothalamic-pituitary adrenal (HPA) axis (for a review on HPA dysfunction, see Yehuda et al. in this issue; Gunnar and Quevedo 2007; Heim and Nemeroff 2001, Lupien et al. 2009). While the acute activation of stress response systems is considered an adaptive mechanism that mobilizes resources to increase chances of survival, high or chronic levels of stress can lead to aberrant reactivity of the HPA axis (Glaser 2000; Gunnar and Quevedo, 2007). When faced with a stressor, corticotrophin-releasing hormone and arginine vasopressin travel to the anterior pituitary where they stimulate the release of adrenocorticotropic hormone, which in turn interacts with receptors on the cortex of the adrenal gland (Gunnar and Quevedo 2007; Heim and Nemeroff 2001; Lupien et al. 2009). This cascade releases glucocorticoids throughout the brain and body binding to mineralocorticoid and glucocorticoid receptors. Most importantly, glucocorticoids acting via glucocorticoid receptors can impair neural plasticity (Gunnar and Quevedo 2007). This explains why brain regions with a particularly high density of glucocorticoid receptors and characterized by prolonged phases of postnatal development (e.g., PFC, hippocampus) are more susceptible to disturbances (Teicher et al. 2003; Tottenham and Sheridan 2010). Exactly how ELS affects cognitive functioning and emotional well-being via specific neurobiological pathways, however, remains poorly understood. The following sections review how particular cognitive and affective functions may be affected by disruptions in brain development following ELS.

ELS and cognitive functioning

Global cognitive performance

Impaired intellectual ability, worse academic performance, and greater need for individualized education programs have been noted in children who experienced ELS, including early institutionalization, neglect, or various forms of maltreatment (Cohen et al. 2008; DeBellis et al. 2009; Johnson et al. 2010; Loman et al. 2009; Nelson et al. 2007; van den Dries et al. 2010). Smaller intracranial volume, reduced hemispheric integration, and a smaller corpus callosum are some of the neural correlates of impaired global cognitive functioning following ELS (Noble et al. 2005; Schiffer et al. 1995; Teicher et al. 2004).

Intellectual performance

A recent study by DeBellis and colleagues (2009) used a battery of standardized tests to investigate neurocognitive functioning in neglected children with and without PTSD compared to non-traumatized controls. Regardless of their PTSD diagnosis, neglected children scored significantly lower on IQ than controls. Even when differences in IQ were statistically controlled, significant differences between neglected and non-neglected children were found in a range of cognitive functions including language, memory/learning, and attention/executive functioning (DeBellis et al. 2009).

Similar results emerged from a study of orphans. Children who spent the first part of life in institutionalized care showed decreased intellectual performance, language difficulties, poorer cognitive abilities and impaired psychomotor development compared to never institutionalized children (Cohen et al. 2008; Loman et al. 2009; Rutter and O’Connor 2004; van den Dries et al. 2010). The time spent in an institution was positively related to the extent of cognitive deficits (Loman et al. 2009; Noble et al. 2005; Rutter and O’Connor 2004). Confirming prior studies (e.g., Perez and Widom 1994), greater severity of abuse was linked to lower IQ (DeBellis et al. 2009).

Interestingly, early foster care at 10 months or older resulted in significant gains in cognitive functioning, commonly referred to as ‘catch up’ (Nelson et al. 2007). Findings are consistent with other studies describing a significant increase in cognitive functioning 6-months post-adoption (Cohen et al. 2008; van den Dries et al. 2010). Despite some functional gains, institutionalized children often still lag behind children raised by their biological parents even after years of quality care (Rutter and O’Connor 2004). It should be noted that most studies assessing IQ in abused or neglected children do not control for heritability factors. Accordingly, parents who abuse or neglect their children might be more likely to score lower on intelligence measures, which can introduce interpretative biases.

Cerebral volume

Several studies on ELS uncovered a relationship between amount of GM and IQ, with earlier onset and longer duration of abuse being correlated with decreased cerebral volumes (De Bellis et al. 2002b; Noble et al. 2005). As evident from research in healthy samples, cortical GM follows an inverted U-shaped developmental trajectory undergoing change from childhood to late adolescence (Principle 1; Gerber and Peterson. 2009; Gogtay and Thompson 2010). GM peaks earliest in primary sensorimotor areas and latest in association areas of the frontal cortex (e.g., DLPFC peaking at 10.5 years) and temporal cortex (e.g., superior temporal gyrus (STG) peaking at 14.9 years) (Gerber and Peterson. 2009; Gogtay et al. 2004; Shaw et al. 2008). Although the deprivation in institutionalized children takes place at an early age, complex association areas continue to mature once lower-order sensorimotor regions are developed (Principle 2; Gogtay and Thompson. 2010). The protracted development of GM association areas (DLPFC, STG) poses an increased risk of impairment and is likely to contribute to deficits in cognitive performance.

Interhemispheric integration and corpus callosum

As the largest WM structure in the brain, the corpus callosum is composed of myelinated axons allowing an interhemispheric integration of diverse cognitive, motor, and sensory processes. In healthy samples, increased growth patterns were found for the anterior part of the corpus callosum between the ages of 3–6, assisting in vigilance and planning of new actions (Thompson et al. 2000). As language functions and associative memory functions become more prevalent, a sharp increase in posterior corpus callosum growth was noted around puberty (6–13 years). An attenuated but continuous growth pattern was found thereafter, ceasing at about 29 years of age (Pujoe et al. 1993; Thompson et al. 2000). It should be noted that this trajectory reflects the corpus callosum’s substantial role in connecting various brain regions which are likely to affect various aspects of cognitive functioning at different stages throughout development.

In both non-human and human research, exposure to ELS has been associated with decreases in corpus callosum size. In a study by Sanchez and colleagues (1998), primates separated from their mother from 2–12 months of age were later characterized by a reduced volume of the corpus callosum, loss of WM in prefrontal and parietal region, and impairments in recognition memory and reversal learning. In line with these preclinical data, reduced hemispheric integration and a smaller size corpus callosum, particularly in the medial and posterior regions, has been described in adults maltreated as children (Navalta et al. 2006; Schiffer et al. 1995), with the corpus callosum in neglected children being 17% smaller than in controls, and 11% smaller than in non-abused, psychiatric children (Teicher et al. 2004). These findings have been strengthened by diffusion tensor imaging techniques, which have uncovered reduced fractional anisotropy in medial and posterior regions of the corpus callosum in maltreated children with PTSD (Jackowski et al. 2008) and in the genu of the corpus callosum in non-clinical adults with a history of ELS (Paul et al. 2008).

Despite these initial findings, additional studies are needed to investigate with greater precision the functional outcomes associated with a reduction in corpus callosum size. At this stage, one can conclude that the separate growth spurts of the anterior and posterior regions of the corpus callosum appear to subserve specific functions. The anterior corpus callosum develops and assists with functions needed earlier in life (e.g., vigilance, approach behavior) while other functions coinciding with posterior growth become more relevant later in development (e.g., language, associative memory). Generally, with the corpus callosum developing until the third decade of life, it can be suggested that this structure supports various higher-order functions vulnerable to effects of ELS (Principle 1 and 2).

Memory

Exposure to stress can have numerous effects on mnemonic function, including stress-induced potentiation of emotional memory (associated with increased amygdala activation) as well as disruption of neutral memory (Payne et al. 2007). In cases of ELS, multiple studies have found significant impairments in verbal, visual, and global memory function in adulthood (Bos et al. 2009; Bremner et al. 2003a; Bremner et al. 2003b; De Bellis et al. in press; Navalta et al. 2006; Tomoda et al. in press). For sexual abuse and mixed maltreatment, the extent of memory deficit appears to be moderated by the chronicity of the abuse, the number of incidents, and the relationship to the perpetrator (Anda et al. 2006; Navalta et al. 2006). For example, for those who reported four or more adverse life events, impairments in childhood memory was increased by 4.4-fold (Anda et al. 2006). The number of age periods affected by such memory gaps gradually increased with the number of adverse ELS experiences (Anda et al. 2006)

Memory and the hippocampus

While postnatal neuronal proliferation in the hippocampus begins at a very early age, it has been shown to continue into adulthood (Gogtay et al. 2006). The hippocampus is a heterogeneous structure, with sub-regions being responsible for different functions and developing along different growth trajectories. In healthy samples, the anterior portion of the hippocampus – which projects to the amygdala, PFC and HPA axis and plays a role in anxiety-related behavior, associative memory, and emotional processing – shows volume reduction with increasing age. Conversely, posterior regions (particularly the CA3 region) – which play a significant role in spatial learning and contextual memory – gradually increase over time (Gogtay et al. 2006).

In adults who experienced ELS, reduced volume of the hippocampus is a common finding (see meta-analyses by Karl et al. 2006; Woon and Hedges 2008; Woon et al. in press). Animal studies confirm the relationship between early life stress (e.g., maternal separation) and reduced hippocampal volume in late life, suggesting permanent impairments in hippocampus-dependent learning and memory (Karten et al. 2005). Morphometric changes may be due to elevated levels of glucocorticoids in the glucocorticoid receptor-rich hippocampus. Accordingly, cortisol secreted as a result of ELS would bind to these receptors and negatively impact neurogenesis, resulting in cell loss (Sapolsky 1996).

However, not all studies have found reduced hippocampal volume following ELS. Numerous studies have failed to demonstrate volumetric reductions in younger individuals with ELS (Carrion et al. 2001; De Bellis et al. 2002b; De Bellis et al. in press; Teicher et al. 2003; see meta-analyses by Karl et al. 2006; Woon and Hedges 2008; Woon et al. in press). Moreover, some studies failed to demonstrate a link between abnormal hippocampal volume and deficits in memory functioning following maltreatment (Pederson et al. 2004; Stein et al. 1997). In the next sections, we briefly explore the potential explanations for these divergent findings.

a. Prolonged exposure to stress

Some have argued that prolonged periods of cortisol secretion and HPA hyper-activation are needed for long-term hippocampal atrophies to be manifested at the level of resolution of current MRI technology (Carrion et al. 2007). This would imply that hippocampal volume loss may be detectable only years after trauma onset, thus explaining why imaging studies have described hippocampal volume decreases in adults but not children. Preliminary support for this hypothesis stems from a longitudinal study of traumatized children in which markers of stress (PTSD symptoms, cortisol levels) predicted reduced hippocampal volume from baseline to follow-up 12–18 months later (Carrion et al. 2007). However, it should be noted that this study used a very small sample of children (n = 15) with PTSD and a short time interval between initial and follow-up assessment.

b. Preexisting vulnerability

Could reduced hippocampal volume be a risk-factor for stress-related pathology rather a consequence of ELS? It is currently a matter of debate whether smaller hippocampi may be a risk factor for stress hypersensitivity. Non-human primate research illustrates that naturally occurring hippocampal volume differences predicted adrenocorticotropic cortisol levels following a restraining stressor (Lyons et al. 2007). Along similar lines, in a monozygotic twin study, Gilbertson and colleagues (2002) found that non-traumatized twin siblings of those who developed PTSD following combat exposure had smaller hippocampi. Although not directly related to ELS, these finding strengthens the argument that small hippocampi may be a risk factor for impaired regulation of the HPA axis and a vulnerability factor to stress-related psychopathology.

c. Impact on other neurobiological systems

Most recently, Tottenham and Sheridan (2010) argued that hippocampus development lags behind the developmental trajectory of the amygdala. In a study of the Israeli Defense forces, 50 healthy recruits were studied upon their entry to the military and 18 months after serving as combat paramedics during which they were exposed to high levels of stress (Admon et al. 2009). The authors found that pre-exposure amygdalar, but not hippocampal, reactivity predicted a greater increase in stress symptoms over time. Moreover, hippocampal alteration in response to stress-related pictures following the stress exposure was predicted by higher amygdala pre-stress reactivity (Admon et al. 2009). Although this military-related stress experience does not resemble ELS, it could be speculated that amygdala hyperreactivity precedes volumetric changes in the hippocampus following stress exposure (Tottenham and Sheridan 2010). This, in turn, could explain why research fails to detect changes in hippocampal volume in children. Similar to the argument of prolonged stress exposure, ELS may lead to hippocampal atrophy only in later life

d. Comorbidity

Hippocampal size may also be associated with psychiatric illness or substance abuse. Unfortunately, studies examining memory performance or hippocampal volume often fail to control for psychiatric illness, such as PTSD (Stein et al. 1997) or borderline personality disorder (Driessen et al. 2000). Only a few studies have controlled for the presence of clinical disorders, or used non-clinical samples in order to attribute reductions in hippocampal volume to ELS (DeBellis et al. 2009; Vythilingam et al. 2002). Some studies did not find volumetric differences even when controlling for common psychopathology such as PTSD (e.g., Pederson et al. 2004).

e. Genetic factors

Finally, animal studies have demonstrated that at least 54% of hippocampal size variance is attributable to genetics (Lyons et al. 2001). In humans, several genetic markers have been associated with smaller hippocampi (Principle 4). For example, Met carriers of the BDNF polymorphism were found to exhibit smaller hippocampal volumes in the presence of stress, trait depression and neuroticism (Joffe et al. 2009). Depressed adults exposed to ELS carrying the short allele of the serotonin transporter gene also displayed smaller hippocampal volumes than adults who had only one risk factor (ELS or short allele) (Frodl et al. 2010). Notably, among healthy controls, emotional neglect and 5HTTLPR allele jointly explained 57% of variance in hippocampal WM, while neither variable alone explained significant variance (Frodl et al. 2010).

In sum, based on data reviewed above, it is clear that longitudinal studies controlling for comorbidity and investigating genetic influences will be required to disentangle how ELS contributes to long-term structural changes in the hippocampus. Most importantly, more research is needed to explain how changes in different sub-regions of the hippocampus relate to specific impairments in memory function.

Executive functioning

Global deficits in executive functioning following ELS are frequently reported (Bos et al. 2009; Colvert et al. 2008; Pollak et al. 2010). Executive functions (e.g., inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility, sustained attention) are subserved by a network of brain regions, including various PFC and striatal regions (Leh et al. 2010). Accordingly, the development of executive functions is thought to coincide with growth spurts in the maturation of the frontal cortex occurring between birth and 2 years, 7–9 years, during adolescence, and continue into the third decade of life (Jurado and Rosselli 2007; Marsh et al. 2008, Shaw et al. 2008). Two developmental characteristics make the PFC particularly vulnerable to stress effects: First, PFC circuits progress in a back-to-front direction and which are marked by long developmental trajectories supporting higher-order functions (Principle 2; Gogtay et al. 2004). Second, the PFC has a high density of glucocorticoid receptors and dopaminergic projections that are stress-susceptible (Brake et al. 2000).

Frontostriatal circuits and inhibitory control

A recent study by Mueller and colleagues (in press) explored the perturbations in cognitive control and their underlying neural correlates in ELS. In an fMRI study, adolescents were asked to inhibit a prepotent response (Go) and initiate a non-prepotent alternative (Change). Individuals with ELS not only displayed longer reaction times on the Change trials, but also demonstrated greater activations in regions involved in cognitive control, in particular, the inferior frontal cortex (cognitive inhibitory control) and striatum (response control). Greater activation only occurred during the Change trials compared to Go trials, thus suggesting a specific impairment in inhibitory control rather than simple motor function (Mueller et al, in press). Interestingly, diminished inhibitory capacity was also found in a sample of high-functioning college women with sexual abuse histories (Navalta et al. 2006).

Inhibitory control is thought to improve with age due to increasing activity in the frontostriatal circuits; however, the development of these circuits may be affected by early stress experiences even in high functioning abuse survivors (Marsh et al. 2008). Both studies revealed impairments to aspects of cognitive control and prefrontal-striatal networks that can also be found in psychopathologies related to ELS such as depression (Langendecker et al. 2007) and PTSD (Falconer et al. 2008).

Cerebellum and planning

Bauer and colleagues (2009) found an association between deficits in planning and a smaller right superior-posterior cerebellar lobe in early institutionalized children. In addition to motor learning, balance, and coordination, the cerebellum is also involved in affective and cognitive processes such as language, visual-spatial learning and working memory (Tiemeier et al. 2010). Of note, the cerebellum demonstrates one of the most drawn-out developmental time courses (Gogtay and Thompson 2010), with total volume peaking at about 12 years for females and 16 years for males (Tiemeier et al. 2010). Interestingly, this implies that the cerebellum peaks approximately 2 years later than cerebral volume (Gerber and Peterson. 2009). It could be argued that the prolonged development of the cerebellum leaves it susceptible to long-term stress exposure and altered cytoarchitecture (Principle 2) and may therefore interfere with the later developing cognitive functions, e.g., planning (Bauer et al. 2009) or learning and working memory (Tiemeier et al. 2010) associated with the cerebellum.

Interim summary: cognitive functioning

A substantial number of studies have demonstrated that ELS can be associated with global cognitive difficulties, including decreased intellectual performance, academic success, language abilities and aspects of executive functioning (e.g., inhibitory control, planning). These higher-order functions are subserved by association sites (PFC, STG) and regions that undergo protracted development (corpus callosum, cerebellum), which could explain the increased risk for global impairment as a consequence of ELS (Principle 2 and 3). Findings of memory deficits and hippocampal volume reductions have been more inconsistent, particularly as hippocampal volume reduction has been found in adults but not in children.

ELS and affective functioning

Reward processing

The ability to evaluate and learn from rewarding outcomes and respond to reward-predicting cues is vital to survival and contributes to adaptive, goal-directed decision-making (Ernst and Paulus 2005). Key components of the reward circuit, including basal ganglia regions involving the ventral (e.g., nucleus accumbens) and dorsal (e.g., caudate) striatum, amygdala, and OFC, undergo significant changes throughout childhood and adolescence (Forbes and Dahl 2005; Giedd et al. 1999). Some basal ganglia structures (e.g., caudate) and the amygdala are also subject to volumetric sex differences possibly suggesting differences in reward responsiveness (Principle 5; Lenroot and Giedd 2010).

The mesolimbic dopaminergic (DA) system, in particular, plays a crucial role in various aspects of reward processing, including attributing incentive salience to reward-related stimuli (“wanting”; Berridge 2007). DA cell bodies in the ventral tegmental area project heavily to the nucleus accumbens, and both midbrain and striatal DA cells fire to both reward-predicting cues and unpredictable rewards. Blunted mesolimbic DA transmission has been postulated in depression, particularly in the presence of anhedonia, low energy, loss of libido and apathy (Dunlop and Nemeroff 2007; Hasler et al. 2008; Pizzagalli et al. 2009a, 2009b). Of major relevance to this review, animal studies have shown that chronic stressors and early adverse rearing environments lead to anhedonia-like behavior (including reduced motivation to pursue reward) and dysfunction in mesolimbic DA pathways in adulthood (Anisman and Matheson 2005; Matthews and Robbins 2003; Pryce et al. 2004; Strekalova et al. 2004). In light of these preclinical data, two recent studies have probed putative dysfunction in reward-related brain activation in adults exposed to ELS.

In a study with a longitudinal component, Dillon and colleagues (2009) found that, relative to non-abused individuals, maltreated subjects were characterized by decreased anticipatory reward-related activity in the left pallidus and putamen, two key regions in the basal ganglia that have been implicated in processing of reward-predicting cues (Figure 1). Maltreated subjects, who reported more symptoms of anhedonia than non-abused subjects, also rated reward cues as less positive. Based on prior data, we speculated that the abnormal pallidus activity might weaken the ability of reward-predicting cues to elicit goal-directed behavior.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Affective and neural dysfunction during a monetary incentive delay task in a longitudinal sample of young adults (mean age: 24.58, SD: 0.88) exposed to childhood adversities. Eight of the maltreated participants have been studied since infancy (mean age at enrollment: 8.88 months), and five have been studied since young adulthood (mean age at enrollment: 20.60 years). Relative to healthy controls (n = 31), maltreated individuals (n = 13) reported reduced positive affect (a) and were characterized by reduced activation in the left pallidus and left putamen during the anticipation of a potential reward (monetary gains) (b–d). Highlighting the specificity of these findings, groups did not differ in their affective ratings or neural responses while anticipating a potential loss (monetary penalty) or no-incentive outcome (no monetary changes) (for all variables, the Group × Condition interaction was significant.) Functional MRI signals were extracted from structurally defined basal ganglia regions, including the putamen and pallidus (see panel b)

Interestingly, decreased basal ganglia activity was found for the anticipatory but not the consummatory phase of reward processing. These findings were recently replicated in a study investigating Romanian adoptees who had experienced global early deprivation. Using a similar task, Mehta et al. (in press) also described reduced ventral striatal activation to reward-predicting cues in the deprived young adults (see also Guyer et al. 2006 for behavioral evidence of sub-optimal reward-related decision-making in a gambling task).

Overall, these findings are consistent with theoretical arguments (Charney 2004) and initial empirical evidence (Vythilingam et al. 2009) suggesting that robust brain reward function is a key component of resiliency following trauma. The fact that reward dysfunction in the two fMRI studies was specific to anticipatory component of the task is intriguing, based on (1) animal studies indicating that chronic and unpredictable stressors lead to a downregulation of mesolimbic DA (Cabib and Puglisi-Allegra 1996), and (2) evidence implicating DA in incentive motivation (“wanting”; Berridge 2007). Both groups speculated that fMRI findings might reflect reduced phasic DA transmission in maltreated individuals. Recent findings that BOLD signal during a similar reward task correlated with an indirect measure of endogenous DA release (Schott et al 2008), further provide evidence for this hypothesis. Critically, blunted responsiveness to reward-predicting cues might be associated with two opposite sequelae: 1) anhedonic behavior, which might increase the risk for mood disorders, and 2) compensatory reward-seeking behavior, which might increase the risk of maladaptive behaviors, including substance abuse. In this context, it is interesting to note that individuals with a history of ELS show more frequent, heavier, and earlier smoking behavior in adulthood compared to individuals without early adversity (Anda et al. 1999). Because nicotine is a potent modulator of the brain reward system (Kenny and Markou 2006), it is possible that substance use might be a self-medication attempt to ameliorate anhedonic symptoms.

Emotional perception

Emotion-modulated attentional biases

Maltreated and neglected infants and children show abnormalities in identifying and responding to facial expressions of negative valence (Ciccetti and Curtis 2005; Fries and Pollak 2004; Parker et al. 2005; Pollak et al. 2000). For example, event-related potential (ERP) studies have shown that, unlike children not exposed to physical maltreatment, those with such histories generated larger P3b amplitudes when presented with angry faces (or voices) compared to happy or fearful targets (Pollak et al. 2001; Shackman et al. 2007). Potentiated P3b was interpreted as indicating increased attentional resources devoted to angry cues in maltreated children (see also Pine et al. 2005 for similar behavioral evidence). Because children with maltreatment histories are more likely to witness anger and subsequent adversity, recruitment of more cognitive processes and selective attention to threat-related cues appears justified (see also Shackman et al. 2007). Fitting these interpretations, abused children have been found to require less visual information to detect anger in facial expressions, which might reflect their effort to identify potential threats as early as possible to minimize harm (Pollak and Kistler 2002). A number of studies have confirmed the presence of attentional bias in maltreated or institutionalized children of varying ages. The Bucharest Early Intervention Project, for example, showed that institutionalized children between 7 and 32 months of age exhibited greater N170 and smaller P250 amplitude to fearful expressions at both the midline and lateral electrode sites, suggesting potentiated encoding of threat-related faces (Parker et al. 2005; see also Cicchetti and Curtis 2005).

These behavioral and electrophysiological data have been complemented by neuroimaging findings. A large body of work has shown that recognizing and understanding emotions depends on the amygdala and its projections to occipitotemporal regions (e.g., fusiform gyrus), STG, thalamus, and OFC (Adolphs 2002). Individuals with a history of ELS show functional and structural abnormalities within these pathways. Accordingly, maltreated children with PTSD were found to have increased gray matter volume in the STG compared to non-maltreated children without PTSD (DeBellis 2005; DeBellis et al. 2002a). This is consistent with recent finding of an increase in GM volume of 14% in left STG in a group of adults who experienced parental verbal abuse (Tomoda et al. in press). The STG is a heteromodal structure critically implicated in receptive and nonverbal auditory processing and critically implicated in emotional perception (Adolphs 2002), which may be particularly vulnerable to effects of ELS due to its prolonged, cubic developmental trajectory (peak GM at about 15 years) (Principles 1, 2 and 3; Gogtay et al. 2004; Gogtay and Thompson 2010; Shaw et al. 2008). An increase in GM volume might therefore reflect abuse-related increases in sensitivity to nonverbal stimuli, thus contributing to attentional bias in processing affective facial expressions.

Abused children show selective attention to and difficulty disengaging from threat-related cues, including facial and vocal expressions of anger and fear. The fact that these dysfunctions do not extend to happy facial expressions argues against global information processing deficits. Emerging neuroimaging data indicate that abnormalities might be linked to dysfunction in subcortical and cortical regions critically implicated in processing emotional and social cues, including the amygdala and STG (Tomoda et al. in press; Tottenham et al. 2010). Although early and potentiated detection of anger may be an adaptive coping mechanism, it is important to emphasize that most of the children studied had been removed from their noxious environment. This suggests that while some of the previously described cognitive impairments following ELS are subject to a ‘catch-up’ in functioning, emotion-associated deficits may prove less plastic.

Emotional regulation

Children exposed to severe ELS experience significant emotion regulation difficulties, which are thought to confer risks for later psychopathology (Lyons-Ruth 2008; Tottenham et al. 2010). Research in healthy controls has emphasized the role of the DLPFC and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) in emotion regulation, and successful downregulation of negative affect has been associated with decreased amygdala activation (for review, see Ochsner and Gross 2005). Thus, emotional dysregulation in maltreated individuals might be associated with functional and structural abnormalities in frontocingulate and frontolimbic pathways.

The amygdala and emotional responsiveness

While DLPFC and ACC mature in early adolescence (peak cortical thickness ~ 10.5 years), the amygdala is present in its basic cytoarchitecture and function at birth. It develops rapidly thereafter, reaching its peak volume as early as age 4 in females (Shaw et al. 2008). Interestingly, the amygdala is not only functional at a very early age but also appears more reactive in childhood compared to adulthood. This early development may indicate a very early sensitive period in which ELS (e.g., early deprivation) can disrupt maturation (Tottenham et al. 2010). Overall, the amygdala is pivotal in learning and responding to social and emotional stimuli in the child’s environment. Disruptions in its developmental trajectory are likely to alter thresholds to emotional information and contribute to psychopathology, particularly anxiety (Tottenham and Sheridan, 2010; Kim and Whalen, 2009).

Providing initial evidence for some of these arguments, Tottenham and colleagues (2010) recently described in a sample of institutionalized but later adopted children morphometric differences in the limbic system accompanied by response inhibition impairments in the presence of negative distractors in an emotional go-nogo task. Compared to controls, previously institutionalized children showed greater amygdala volumes and made more false alarm errors when the nogo condition used negatively valenced faces. Moreover, reaction times of institutionalized children varied with slower responses to neutral stimuli in the context of positive valence and accelerated response to neutral stimuli in context of negative faces. Behavioral and neurobiological evidence therefore suggests that late-adopted children may be more influenced by emotional contexts, contributing to their emotion regulation difficulties (Tottenham et al. 2010). One might argue that this outcome could be attributed to the previously reviewed difficulties in recognizing emotions (Pollak et al. 2000). However, the majority of errors made in Tottenham’s sample (false alarm) suggest a hypersensitivity to emotional arousal rather than failure to identify the emotion (miss).

Interestingly, in contrast to global cognitive functions showing improvements after removal from the adverse environment (Nelson et al. 2007), the amygdala seems more resistant to environment change. It has been suggested that this pattern may contribute to persistence of emotional problems displayed by children who were relieved of their adverse environment over 10 years ago (see Tottenham and Sheridan 2010 for a review).

Interim summary: affective functioning

ELS has been linked to impairments in reward processing associated with disruptions in basal ganglia regions. In addition, individuals with a history of ELS are often characterized by increased attention and potentiated encoding of negative or threatening faces and larger STG, suggesting increased sensitivity to non-verbal stimuli. Larger amygdala volumes and increased emotional reactivity to negative stimuli are likely to contribute to psychopathology commonly seen following ELS (e.g., anxiety). Overall, affective impairments remain evident long after the removal of the stressor. An important avenue for future research will be to directly test the speculation that very early disruption affecting brain regions associated with affective deficits are more resistant to change than aspects of cognitive functioning. Moreover, as the amygdala continues to be a sexually dimorphic structure (earlier peak volume for females, larger total volume for males), future studies are needed to test whether ELS occurring at a given time may lead to gender-specific impairments and mental health problems (Principle 5, Lenroot and Giedd 2010).

Conclusion

In this review, we merged findings from two independent but complementary lines of research, namely studies on normative brain development and disruptions to brain development, to understand mechanism associated with cognitive and affective functioning following ELS. Using a developmental approach, we identified general principles of normative brain development that significantly add to our understanding on the sequelae of ELS. By integrating normative and ELS outcomes on brain development, we concluded that brain regions with prolonged developmental trajectories (e.g. PFC) are particularly vulnerable to ELS which may partially explain impairments in complex, higher-order functions (e.g., executive functioning). These dysfunctions could contribute to the progression of mental illness, for example, executive dysfunction may impair emotional regulation and foster rumination as seen in depression (Gotlib and Joormann 2010). Another interesting finding that requires attention is the extent of ‘catch up’ in cognitive and affective functioning following the relief of the stressor. While cognitive functions have shown some degree of recovery after relief from the stressful environment, deficits in affective functioning (e.g., emotional regulation) and associated brain regions (e.g., amygdala) appear more resistant. This may contribute to higher rates of anxiety and mood disorders commonly seen adults with ELS histories.

Finally, we would like to note that the structure of this review is guided by research developments in humans over the past two decades, often inspired by important findings in animal models. We acknowledge that discussing separate brain regions as contributing to specific emotional or cognitive deficits can appear simplistic. However, considering the plethora of studies investigating ELS using a vast range of designs, it was our objective to merge findings to highlight primary pathways of early neurobiological disruptions affecting later well-being. While past research commonly focused on single brain-behavior associations, we support more recent approaches investigating functionally connected networks (Lu et al. 2009). Moreover, although we focused on behavioral phenotypes most often investigated in clinical and basic studies (e.g., memory, executive functioning, and affective processing), ELS can negatively impact a wide range of functions (e.g., language), and further research exploring additional phenotypes is warranted.

Future directions

Partially due to relative recency of this line of work, there are several outstanding questions that remain unanswered and should be the topic of future studies. First, as developmental trajectories differ for males and females, studies will be needed to evaluate sex-specific outcomes of ELS occurring at particular developmental stages. This knowledge is critical since it might inform sex-specific prevention and intervention strategies. Second, initial evidence indicates that genetic influences on brain development might be region- and age-specific (Lenroot and Giedd 2008; Lenroot et al. 2009). Accordingly, studies will be needed to evaluate whether the effects of ELS on brain development may be moderated by the strength and timing of genetic influences on specific brain regions. Moreover, a stronger focus on potential Gene × Environment interactions might provide important clues about mechanisms and pathways associated with increased risk for psychopathology following ELS. Third, an important topic for future research will be to investigate whether permanent abnormalities of stress reactivity reviewed here may contribute to overall poor decision-making due to stress-related impairments in processing reward and punishment cues. In particular, adolescents and adults who have experienced ELS show increased odds of maladaptive behavior, including eating disorders (Rayworth et al. 2004), substance abuse (Bailey and McCloskey 2005), alcohol dependence (Nelson et al. 2002), high risk sexual behaviors or disorders (Mangilio 2009), dissociation (Wright et al. 2009), and self-harm or suicide (Dube et al. 2001; Paolucci et al. 2001). More recently, researchers have started to conceptualize these behaviors as maladaptive coping in an attempt to relieve or alleviate distress despite receiving negative feedback (Accident Compensation Corporation 2008). Future research should focus on the relationship between harmful behavior and cognitive deficits discussed above (e.g., reward processing) with their underlying structural and functional alterations in the brain (e.g., ACC, PFC, amygdala) as well as possible permanent changes to the neuroendocrine stress response system. This research is of particular practical importance, because it speaks to a vicious cycle that too often characterizes abuse. While abused children are generally at an increased risk of subsequent abuse (Mangilio 2009; Nelson et al. 2002), the number of maladaptive coping strategies is one of the strongest predictor of revictimization (Filipas and Ullman 2006) - a fact that may reflect a combination of impaired cognitive and affective functioning following ELS.

Acknowledgments

During the preparation of this paper, Pia Pechtel received research funding from the German Research Foundation (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft - DFG). DAP was supported by Award Numbers R01MH068376 and R21MH078979 from the National Institute of Mental Health and by a 2008 NARSAD Independent Investigator Award. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute of Mental Health, the National Institutes of Health, or NARSAD.

Footnotes

Disclosures Dr. Pizzagalli has received research support from ANT North America Inc. (Advanced Neuro Technology); consulting fees from ANT North America Inc. and AstraZeneca; and honoraria from AstraZeneca.

References

  1. Accident Compensation Coporation. Sexual abuse and mental injury: Practice guidelines for Aotearoa/New Zealand. Wellington, New Zealand: ACC; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  2. Admon R, Lubin G, Stern O, Rosenberg K, Sela L, Ben-Ami H, et al. Human vulnerability to stress depends on amygdala’s predisposition and hippocampal plasticity. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2009;106:14120–14125. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0903183106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Adolphs R. Neural systems for recognizing emotion. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2002;12:169–177. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(02)00301-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Anda R, Croft JB, Felitti V, Nordenberg D, Giles W, Wiliamson DF, et al. Adverse childhood experiences and smoking during adolescence and adulthood. JAMA. 1999;228:1652–1658. doi: 10.1001/jama.282.17.1652. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Anda R, Dong M, Brown D, Felitti V, Giles W, Perry G, et al. The relationship of adverse childhood experiences to a history of premature death of family members. BMC Public Health. 2009;9:106. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-9-106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Anda R, Felitti V, Bremner J, Walker J, Whitfield C, Perry B, et al. The enduring effects of abuse and related adverse experiences in childhood. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2006;256:174–186. doi: 10.1007/s00406-005-0624-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Anda RF, Brown DW, Felitti VJ, Bremner JD, Dube SR, Giles WH. Adverse childhood experiences and prescribed psychotropic medications in adults. Am J Prev Med. 2007;32:389–394. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2007.01.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Andersen SL, Tomada A, Vincow ES, Valente E, Polcari A, Teicher MH. Preliminary evidence for sensitive periods in the effect of childhood sexual abuse on regional brain development. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2008;20:292–301. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.20.3.292. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Anisman H, Matheson K. Stress, depression, and anhedonia: Caveats concerning animal models. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2005;29:525–546. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bailey JA, McCloskey LA. Pathways to adolescent substance use among sexually abused girls. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2005;33:39–53. doi: 10.1007/s10802-005-0933-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bauer PM, Hanson JL, Pierson RK, Davidson RJ, Pollak SD. Cerebellar volume and cognitive functioning in children who experienced early deprivation. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66:1100–1106. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.06.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Berridge KC. The debate over dopamine’s role in reward: the case for incentive salience. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2007;191:391–431. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0578-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bos KJ, Fox N, Zeanah CH, Nelson CA. Effects of early psychosocial deprivation on the development of memory and executive function. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2009:3. doi: 10.3389/neuro.08.016.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Brake WG, Sullivan RM, Gratton A. Perinatal distress leads to lateralized medial prefrontal cortical dopamine hypofunction in adult rats. J Neurosci. 2000;20:5538–5543. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-14-05538.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bremner JD, Vythilingam M, Vermetten E, Southwick SM, McGlashan T, Nazeer A, et al. MRI and PET study of deficits in hippocampal structure and function in women with childhood sexual abuse and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2003a;160:924–932. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.5.924. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bremner JD, Vythilingam M, Vermetten E, Southwick SM, McGlashan T, Staib LH, et al. Neural correlates of declarative memory for emotionally valenced words in women with posttraumatic stress disorder related to early childhood sexual abuse. Bio Psychiatry. 2003b;53:879–889. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01891-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Brown DW, Anda RF, Tiemeier H, Felitti VJ, Edwards VJ, Croft JB, et al. Adverse childhood experiences and the risk of premature mortality. Am J Prev Med. 2009;37:389–396. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2009.06.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cabib S, Puglisi-Allegra S. Stress, depression and the mesolimbic dopamine system. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1996;128:331–42. doi: 10.1007/s002130050142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Carrion VG, Weems CF, Eliez S, Patwardhan A, Brown W, Ray RD, et al. Attenuation of frontal asymmetry in pediatric posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;50:943–951. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01218-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Carrion VG, Weems CF, Reiss AL. Stress predicts brain changes in children: A pilot longitudinal study on youth stress, posttraumatic stress disorder, and the hippocampus. Pediatrics. 2007;119:509–516. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-2028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Chapman DP, Whitfield CL, Felitti VJ, Dube SR, Edwards VJ, Anda RF. Adverse childhood experiences and the risk of depressive disorders in adulthood. J Affect Disord. 2004;82:217–225. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2003.12.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Charney DS. Psychobiological mechanisms of resilience and vulnerability: Implications for successful adaptation to extreme stress. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:195–216. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.2.195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Ciccetti D, Curtis WJ. An event-related potential study of the processing of affective facial expressions in young children who experienced maltreatment during the first year of life. Dev Psychopathol. 2005;17:641–677. doi: 10.1017/S0954579405050315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Cohen NJ, Lojkasek M, Zadeh ZY, Pugliese M, Kiefer H. Children adopted from China: a prospective study of their growth and development. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008;49:458–468. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01853.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Colvert E, Rutter M, Kreppner J, Beckett C, Castle J, Groothues C, et al. Do theory of mind and executive function deficits underlie the adverse outcomes associated with profound early deprivation?: Findings from the English and Romanian Adoptees Study. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2008;36:1057–1068. doi: 10.1007/s10802-008-9232-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Cutrona CE, Russell DW, Brown PA, Clark LA, Hessling RM, Gardner KA. Neighborhood context, personality, and stressful life events as predictors of depression among African American women. J Abnorm Psychol. 2005;114:3–15. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.1.3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. De Bellis MD. The psychobiology of neglect. Child Maltreat. 2005;10:150–172. doi: 10.1177/1077559505275116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. De Bellis MD, Baum AS, Birmaher B, Keshavan MS, Eccard CH, Boring AM, et al. Developmental traumatology part I: biological stress systems. Biol Psychiatry. 1999a;45:1259–1270. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00044-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. De Bellis MD, Hooper SR, Spratt EG, Woolley DP. Neuropsychological findings in childhood neglect and their relationships to pediatric PTSD. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2009;15:868–878. doi: 10.1017/S1355617709990464. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. De Bellis MD, Hooper SR, Woolley DP, Shenk C. Demographic, maltreatment, and neurobiological correlates of PTSD symptoms in children and adolescents. J Pediatr Psychol. 2010 Jun 16; doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsp116. (in press) [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. De Bellis MD, Keshavan MS, Clark DB, Casey BJ, Giedd JN, Boring AM, et al. Developmental traumatology part II: brain development. Biol Psychiatry. 1999b;45:1271–1284. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00045-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. De Bellis MD, Keshavan MS, Frustaci K, Shifflett H, Iyengar S, Beers SR, et al. Superior temporal gyrus volumes in maltreated children and adolescents with PTSD. Biol Psychiatry. 2002a;51:544–552. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01374-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. De Bellis MD, Keshavan MS, Shifflett H, Iyengar S, Beers SR, Hall J, et al. Brain structures in pediatric maltreatment-related posttraumatic stress disorder: a sociodemographically matched study. Biol Psychiatry. 2002b;52:1066–1078. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01459-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Dillon DG, Holmes AJ, Birk JL, Brooks N, Lyons-Ruth K, Pizzagalli DA. Childhood adversity is associated with left basal ganglia dysfunction during reward anticipation in adulthood. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66:206–213. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.02.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Dong M, Giles WH, Felitti VJ, Dube SR, Williams JE, Chapman DP, et al. Insights Into causal pathways for ischemic heart disease: Adverse Childhood Experiences Study. Circulation. 2004;110:1761–1766. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.0000143074.54995.7F. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Driessen M, Herrmann J, Stahl K, Zwaan M, Meier S, Hill A, et al. Magnetic resonance imaging volumes of the hippocampus and the amygdala in women with borderline personality disorder and early traumatization. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57:1115–1122. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.12.1115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Dube SR, Anda RF, Felitti VJ, Chapman DP, Williamson DF, Giles WH. Childhood abuse, household dysfunction, and the risk of attempted suicide throughout the life span: Findings from the adverse childhood experiences study. JAMA. 2001;286:3089–3096. doi: 10.1001/jama.286.24.3089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Dunlop BW, Nemeroff CB. The role of dopamine in the pathophysiology of depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:327–337. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.3.327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Edwards VJ, Holden GW, Felitti VJ, Anda RF. Relationship between multiple forms of childhood maltreatment and adult mental health in community respondents: Results from the Adverse Childhood Experiences Study. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:1453–1460. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.8.1453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Ernst M, Paulus MP. Neurobiology of decision making: A selective review from a neurocognitive and clinical Perspective. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;58:597–604. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Falconer E, Bryant R, Felmingham KL, Kemp AH, Gordon E, Peduto A, et al. The neural networks of inhibitory control in posttraumatic stress disorder. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2008;33:413–422. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Filipas HH, Ullman SE. Child sexual abuse, coping responses, self-blame, posttraumatic stress disorder, and adult sexual revictimization. J Interpers Violence. 2006;21:652–72. doi: 10.1177/0886260506286879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Finklehor D. The international epidemiology of child sexual abuse. Child Abuse Negl. 1994;18:409–417. doi: 10.1016/0145-2134(94)90026-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Forbes EE, Dahl RE. Neural systems of positive affect: Relevance to understanding child and adolescent depression? Dev Psycholpathol. 2005;17:827–850. doi: 10.1017/S095457940505039X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Fries ABW, Pollak SD. Emotion understanding in postinstitutionalized Eastern European children. Dev Psychopathol. 2004;16:355–369. doi: 10.1017/S0954579404044554. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Frodl T, Reinhold E, Koutsouleris N, Donohoe G, Bondy B, Reiser M, et al. Childhood stress, serotonin transporter gene and brain structures in Major Depression. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010 Feb 10;2010 doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.8. [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Gerber A, Peterson S. Anatomical brain magnetic resonance imaging of typically developing children. Applied Brain Imaging. 2009;48:465–470. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e31819f2715. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Giedd J, Blumenthal J, Jeffries N, Castellanos F, Liu H, Zijdenbos A, et al. Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:861–863. doi: 10.1038/13158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Gilbertson MW, Shenton ME, Ciszewski A, Kasai K, Lasko NB, Orr SP, et al. Smaller hippocampal volume predicts pathologic vulnerability to psychological trauma. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5:1242–1247. doi: 10.1038/nn958. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Glaser D. Child abuse and neglect and the brain - A review. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2000;41:97–116. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Gogtay N, Giedd J, Lusk L, Hayashi K, Greenstein D, Vaituzis A, et al. Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2004;101:8174–8179. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0402680101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Gogtay N, Nugent TF, Herman DH, Ordonez A, Greenstein D, Hayashi KM, et al. Dynamic mapping of normal human hippocampal development. Hippocampus. 2006;16:664–72. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Gogtay N, Thompson PM. Mapping gray matter development: implications for typical development and vulnerability to psychopathology. Brain Cogn. 2010;72:6–15. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.08.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Gotlib IH, Joormann J. Cognition and depression: current status and future directions. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2010;6:285–312. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131305. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Grassi-Oliveira R, Ashy M, Stein LM. Psychobiology of childhood maltreatment: effects of allostatic load? Rev Bras Psiquiatr. 2008;30:60–68. doi: 10.1590/s1516-44462008000100012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Green JG, McLaughlin KA, Berglund PA, Gruber MJ, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, et al. Childhood adversities and adult psychiatric disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication I: Associations with first onset of DSM-IV sisorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67:113–123. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Gunnar M, Quevedo K. The neurobiology of stress and development. Annu Rev Psychol. 2007;58:145–173. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Guyer AE, Kaufman J, Hodgdon H, Masten C, Jazbec S, Pine DS, et al. Behavioral alterations in reward system function: the role of childhood maltreatment and psychopathology. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2006;45:1059–1067. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000227882.50404.11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Hasler G, Fromm S, Carlson PJ, Luckenbaugh DA, Waldeck T, Geraci M, et al. Neural response to catecholamine depletion in unmedicated subjects with major depressive disorder in remission and healthy subjects. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65:521–531. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.5.521. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Heim C, Nemeroff CB. The role of childhood trauma in the neurobiology of mood and anxiety disorders: preclinical and clinical studies. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;49:1023–1039. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01157-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Jackowski AP, Douglas-Palumberi H, Jackowski M, Win L, Schultz RT, Staib LW, et al. Corpus callosum in maltreated children with posttraumatic stress disorder: A diffusion tensor imaging study. Psychiatry Res: Neuroimaging. 2008;162:256–261. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2007.08.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Joffe RT, Gatt JM, Kemp AH, Grieve S, Dobson-Stone C, Kuan SA, et al. Brain derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met polymorphism, the five factor model of personality and hippocampal volume: Implications for depressive illness. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30:1246–1256. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20592. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Johnson DE, Guthrie D, Smyke AT, Koga SF, Fox NA, Zeanah CH, et al. Growth and associations between auxology, caregiving environment, and cognition in socially deprived Romanian children randomized to foster vs gngoing institutional care. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2010 Apr 5;2010 doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2010.56. [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Jurado M, Rosselli M. The elusive nature of executive functions: A review of our current understanding. Neuropsychol Rev. 2007;17:213–233. doi: 10.1007/s11065-007-9040-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Karl A, Schaefer M, Malta LS, Dörfel D, Rohleder N, Werner A. A meta-analysis of structural brain abnormalities in PTSD. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2006;30:1004–1031. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Karten YJG, Olariu A, Cameron HA. Stress in early life inhibits neurogenesis in adulthood. Trends Neurosci. 2005;28:171–172. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2005.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Kenny PJ, Markou A. Nicotine self-administration acutely activates brain reward systems and induces a long-lasting increase in reward sensitivity. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:1203–1211. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300905. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Kim MJ, Whalen PJ. The structural integrity of an amygdala-prefrontal pathway predicts trait anxiety. J Neurosci. 2009;29:11614–8. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2335-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Langenecker SA, Kennedy SE, Guidotti LM, Briceno EM, Own LS, Hooven T, et al. Frontal and limbic activation during inhibitory control predicts treatment response in major depressive disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62:1272–1280. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.02.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Leh S, Petrides M, Strafella A. The neural circuitry of executive functions in healthy subjects and Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:70–85. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.88. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Lenroot RK, Giedd JN. The changing impact of genes and environment on brain development during childhood and adolescence: initial findings from a neuroimaging study of pediatric twins. Dev Psychopathol. 2008;20:1161–75. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000552. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Lenroot RK, Giedd JN. Sex differences in the adolescent brain. Brain Cogn. 2010;72:46–55. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.10.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Lenroot RK, Gogtay N, Greenstein DK, Wells EM, Wallace GL, Clasen LS, et al. Sexual dimorphism of brain developmental trajectories during childhood and adolescence. Neuroimage. 2007;36:1065–73. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Lenroot RK, Schmitt JE, Ordaz SJ, Wallace GL, Neale MC, Lerch JP, et al. Differences in genetic and environmental influences on the human cerebral cortex associated with development during childhood and adolescence. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30:163–74. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20494. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Loman MM, Wiik KL, Frenn KA, Pollak SD, Gunnar MR. Postinstitutionalized children’s development: Growth, cognitive, and language outcomes. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2009;30:426–434. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181b1fd08. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Lu LH, Dapretto M, O’Hare ED, Kan E, McCourt ST, Thompson PM, et al. Relationships between brain activation and brain structure in normally developing children. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19:2595–2604. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Lupien SJ, McEwen BS, Gunnar MR, Heim C. Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10:434–45. doi: 10.1038/nrn2639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Lyons-Ruth K. Contributions of the mother-infant relationship to dissociative, borderline, and conduct symptoms in young adulthood. Infant Mental Health Journal. 2008;29:203–218. doi: 10.1002/imhj.20173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Lyons DM, Parker KJ, Zeitzer JM, Buckmaster CL, Schatzberg AF. Preliminary evidence that hippocampal volumes in monkeys predict stress levels of adrenocorticotropic hormone. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62:1171–1174. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.03.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Lyons DM, Yang C, Sawyer-Glover AM, Moseley ME, Schatzberg AF. Early life stress and inherited variation in monkey hippocampal volumes. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58:1145–1151. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.12.1145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Maniglio R. The impact of child sexual abuse on health: A systematic review of reviews. Clin Psychol Rev. 2009;29:647–657. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Marsh R, Gerber AJ, Peterson BS. Neuroimaging studies of normal brain development and their relevance for understanding childhood neuropsychiatric disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2008;47:1233–1251. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e318185e703. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Matthews K, Robbins TW. Early experience as a determinant of adult behavioural responses to reward: the effects of repeated maternal separation in the rat. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2003;27:45–55. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(03)00008-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Mehta M, Gore-Langton E, Golembo N, Colvert E, Williams S, Sonuga-Barke E. Hyporesponsive reward anticipation in the basal ganglia following severe institutional deprivation early in life. J Cogn Neurosci. 2009 Nov 23; doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21394. (in press) [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Mueller SC, Maheu FS, Dozier M, Peloso E, Mandell D, Leibenluft E, et al. Early-life stress is associated with impairment in cognitive control in adolescence: An fMRI study. Neuropsychologia. 2010 Jun 16; doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.06.013. (in press) [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Navalta CP, Polcari A, Webster DM, Boghossian A, Teicher MH. Effects of childhood sexual abuse on neuropsychological and cognitive function in college women. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2006;18:45–53. doi: 10.1176/jnp.18.1.45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Nelson CA, Zeanah CH, Fox NA, Marshall P, Smyke AT, Guthrie D. Cognitive recovery in socially deprived young children: The Bucharest Early Intervention Project. Science. 2007;318:1937–1940. doi: 10.1126/science.1143921. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Nelson EC, Heath AC, Madden PAF, Cooper ML, Dinwiddie SH, Bucholz KK, et al. Association between self-reported childhood sexual abuse and adverse psychosocial outcomes: Results from a twin study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002;59:139–145. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.2.139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Noble KG, Tottenham N, Casey BJ. Neuroscience perspectives on disparities in school readiness and cognitive achievement. Future Child. 2005;15:71–89. doi: 10.1353/foc.2005.0006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Ochsner KN, Gross JJ. The cognitive control of emotion. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9:242–249. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Paus T, Keshavan M, Giedd JN. Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence? Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:947–57. doi: 10.1038/nrn2513. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Paolucci E, Genuis M, Violato C. A meta-analysis of the published research on the effects of child sexual abuse. J Psychol. 2001;135:17–36. doi: 10.1080/00223980109603677. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Parker SW, Nelson CA Bucharest Early Intervention Project Core Group. An event-related potential study of the impact of institutional rearing on face recognition. Dev Psychopathol. 2005;17:621–639. doi: 10.1017/S0954579405050303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Paul RH, Henry L, Stuart MG, Guilmette T, Niaura R, Bryant R, et al. The relationship between early life stress and microstructural integrity of the corpus callosum in a non-clinical population. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2008;4:193–201. doi: 10.2147/ndt.s1549. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Payne J, Jackson E, Hoscheidt S, Ryan L, Jacobs W, Nadel L. Stress administered prior to encoding impairs neutral but enhances emotional long-term episodic memories. Lern Mem. 2007;17:861–868. doi: 10.1101/lm.743507. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Pederson CL, Maurer SH, Kaminski PL, Zander KA, Peters CM, Stokes-Crowe LA, et al. Hippocampal volume and memory performance in a community-based sample of women with Posttraumatic Stress Disorder secondary to child abuse. J Trauma Stress. 2004;17:37–40. doi: 10.1023/B:JOTS.0000014674.84517.46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Peper JS, Schnack HG, Brouwer RM, Van Baal GC, Pjetri E, Székely E, et al. Heritability of regional and global brain structure at the onset of puberty: a magnetic resonance imaging study in 9-year-old twin pairs. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30:2184–2196. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20660. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Pereda N, Guilera G, Forns M, Gómez-Benito J. The international epidemiology of child sexual abuse: A continuation of Finkelhor (1994) Child Abuse Negl. 2009;33:331–342. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2008.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Perez CM, Widom CS. Childhood victimization and long-term intellectual and academic outcomes. Child Abuse Negl. 1994;18:617–633. doi: 10.1016/0145-2134(94)90012-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Pine DS, Mogg K, Bradley BP, Montgomery L, Monk CS, McClure E, et al. Attention bias to threat in maltreated children: Implications for vulnerability to stress-related psychopathology. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162:291–296. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.2.291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Pirkola S, Isometsä E, Aro H, Kestilä L, Hämäläinen J, Veijola J, et al. Childhood adversities as risk factors for adult mental disorders. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2005;40:769–777. doi: 10.1007/s00127-005-0950-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Pizzagalli DA, Holmes AJ, Dillon DG, Goetz EL, Birk JL, Bogdan R, et al. Reduced caudate and nucleus accumbens response to rewards in unmedicated subjects with Major Depressive Disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2009a;166:702–710. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08081201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Pizzagalli DA, Iosifescu D, Hallett LA, Ratner KG, Fava M. Reduced hedonic capacity in major depressive disorder: Evidence from a probabilistic reward task. J Psychiatr Res. 2009b;43:76–87. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.03.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Pollak SD, Cicchetti D, Hornung K, Reed A. Recognizing emotion in faces: Developmental effects of child abuse and neglect. Dev Psychol. 2000;36:679–688. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.36.5.679. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Pollak SD, Kistler DJ. Early experience is associated with the development of categorical representations for facial expressions of emotion. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2002;99:9072–9076. doi: 10.1073/pnas.142165999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Pollak SD, Klorman R, Thatcher JE, Cicchetti D. P3b reflects maltreated children’s reactions to facial displays of emotion. Psychophysiology. 2001;38:267–274. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Pollak SD, Nelson CA, Schlaak MF, Roeber BJ, Wewerka SS, Wiik KL, et al. Neurodevelopmental effects of early deprivation in postinstitutionalized children. Child Dev. 2010;81:224–236. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01391.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Pryce CR, Dettling AC, Spengler M, Schnell CR, Feldon J. Deprivation of parenting disrupts development of homeostatic and reward systems in marmoset monkey offspring. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;56:72–79. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Pujol J, Vendrell P, Junque C, Marti-Vilalta JL, Capdevila A. When does human brain development end? Evidence of corpus callosum growth up to adulthood. Ann Neurol. 1993;34:71–75. doi: 10.1002/ana.410340113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Rayworth B, Wise L, Harlow B. Childhood abuse and risk of eating disorders in women. Epidemiology. 2004;15:271–278. doi: 10.1097/01.ede.0000120047.07140.9d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Rutter M, O’Connor TG. Are there biological programming effects for psychological development? Findings from a study of Romanian adoptees. Dev Psychol. 2004;40:81–94. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.1.81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Sánchez MM, Hearn EF, Do D, Rilling JK, Herndon JG. Differential rearing affects corpus callosum size and cognitive function of rhesus monkeys. Brain Res. 1998;812:38–49. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(98)00857-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Sapolsky RM. Stress, glucocorticoids, and damage to the nervous system: the current state of confusion. Stress. 1996;1:1–19. doi: 10.3109/10253899609001092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Schiffer F, Teicher M, Papanicolaou A. Evoked potential evidence for right brain activity during the recall of traumatic memories. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1995;7:169–175. doi: 10.1176/jnp.7.2.169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Schmitt JE, Lenroot RK, Wallace GL, Ordaz S, Taylor KN, Kabani N, et al. Identification of genetically mediated cortical networks: a multivariate study of pediatric twins and siblings. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18:1737–47. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Schoedl AF, Costa MC, Mari JJ, Mello MF, Tyrka AR, Carpenter LL, et al. The clinical correlates of reported childhood sexual abuse: an association between age at trauma onset and severity of depression and PTSD in adults. J Child Sex Abus. 2010;19:156–70. doi: 10.1080/10538711003615038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Schott BH, Minuzzi L, Krebs RM, Elmenhorst D, Lang M, Winz OH, et al. Mesolimbic functional magnetic resonance imaging activations during reward anticipation correlate with reward-related ventral striatal dopamine release. J Neurosci. 2008;28:14311–14319. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2058-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Shackman JE, Shackman AJ, Pollak SD. Physical abuse amplifies attention to threat and increases anxiety in children. Emotion. 2007;7:838–852. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.838. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Shaw P, Kabani NJ, Lerch JP, Eckstrand K, Lenroot R, Gogtay N, et al. Neurodevelopmental trajectories of the human cerebral cortex. J Neurosci. 2008;28:3586–94. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5309-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Spataro J, Mullen PE, Burgess PM, Wells DL, Moss SA. Impact of child sexual abuse on mental health: Prospective study in males and females. Br J Psychiatry. 2004;184:416–421. doi: 10.1192/bjp.184.5.416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Stein MB, Koverola C, Hanna C, Torchia MG, McClarty B. Hippocampal volume in women victimized by childhood sexual abuse. Psychol Med. 1997;27:951–959. doi: 10.1017/s0033291797005242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Strekalova T, Spanagel R, Bartsch D, Henn FA, Gass P. Stress-induced anhedonia in mice is associated with deficits in forced swimming and exploration. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:2007–2017. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Teicher M, Andersen S, Polcari A, Anderson C, Navalta C, Kim D. The neurobiological consequences of early stress and childhood maltreatment. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2003;27:33–44. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(03)00007-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Teicher M, Dumont N, Ito Y, Vaituzis C, Giedd J, Andersen S. Childhood neglect is associated with reduced corpus callosum area. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;56:80–85. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.03.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Teicher M, Samson J, Polcari A, McGreenery C. Sticks, stones, and hurtful words: Relative effects of various forms of childhood maltreatment. Am J Psychiatry. 2006a;163:993–1000. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.6.993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Teicher M, Tomoda A, Andersen S. Neurobiological consequences of early stress and childhood maltreatment: Are results from human and animal studies comparable? Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2006b;1071:313–323. doi: 10.1196/annals.1364.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Tiemeier H, Lenroot RK, Greenstein DK, Tran L, Pierson R, Giedd JN. Cerebellum development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal morphometric MRI study. Neuroimage. 2010;49:63–70. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Thompson PM, Giedd JN, Woods RP, MacDonald D, Evans AC, Toga AW. Growth patterns in the developing brain detected by using continuum mechanical tensor maps. Nature. 2000;404:190–193. doi: 10.1038/35004593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Tomoda A, Sheu YS, Rabi K, Suzuki H, Navalta CP, Polcari A, et al. Exposure to parental verbal abuse is associated with increased gray matter volume in superior temporal gyrus. Neuroimage. 2010 May 17; doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.027. (in press) [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Tottenham N, Hare TA, Quinn BT, McCarry TW, Nurse M, Gilhooly T, et al. Prolonged institutional rearing is associated with atypically large amygdala volume and difficulties in emotion regulation. Development Sci. 2010;13:46–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00852.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Tottenham N, Sheridan MA. A review of adversity, the amygdala and the hippocampus: a consideration of developmental timing. Front Hum Neurosci. 2010;8(3):68. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.068.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Child Maltreatment 2007. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office; 2009. Administration on Children, Youth and Families. [Google Scholar]
  133. van den Dries L, Juffer F, van IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ. Infants’ physical and cognitive development after international adoption from foster care or institutions in China. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2010;31:144–150. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181cdaa3a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Vythilingam M, Heim C, Newport J, Miller AH, Anderson E, Bronen R, et al. Childhood trauma associated with smaller hippocampal volume in women with Major Depression. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159:2072–2080. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.12.2072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Vythilingam M, Nelson EE, Scaramozza M, Waldeck T, Hazlett G, Southwick SM, et al. Reward circuitry in resilience to severe trauma: An fMRI investigation of resilient special forces soldiers. Psychiatry Res. 2009;172:75–77. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.06.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Weber D, Reynolds C. Clinical perspectives on neurobiological effects of psychological trauma. Neuropsychol Rev. 2004;14:115–129. doi: 10.1023/b:nerv.0000028082.13778.14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Woon FL, Hedges DW. Hippocampal and amygdala volumes in children and adults with childhood maltreatment-related posttraumatic stress disorder: A meta-analysis. Hippocampus. 2008;18:729–736. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Woon FL, Sood S, Hedges DW. Hippocampal volume deficits associated with exposure to psychological trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder in adults: A meta-analysis. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2010 Jun 21; doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.06.016. (in press) [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Wright MOD, Crawford E, Del Castillo D. Childhood emotional maltreatment and later psychological distress among college students: The mediating role of maladaptive schemas. Child Abuse Negl. 2009;33:59–68. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2008.12.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES