Cognitive sequelae of cancer and its treatment have become an active area of investigation (Meyers and Perry, 2008), particularly the effects of adjuvant chemotherapy for breast cancer. Early retrospective neuropsychological studies demonstrated lower function in long-term survivors treated with chemotherapy than in patients not exposed to chemotherapy or healthy controls. These objective data, as well as patient-reported difficulties with cognition in daily life, led to the coining of terms such as ‘chemobrain', as alterations in cognitive functioning were presumed to reflect the neurotoxic effects of systemic chemoherapy. Prospective studies later documented lower cognitive performance in a subset of patients prior to adjuvant treatment. These effects remained after controlling for potentially related psychosocial factors (eg, mood, fatigue), suggesting that the cancer disease process may play an independent role. It has also been demonstrated that hormonal therapies may exert an adverse effect on cognition. Reported cognitive effects are generally modest; mean performance for the affected group (eg, chemotherapy-treated vs no chemotherapy, cancer vs noncancer, and hormonal therapy vs no hormonal therapy) typically remains within normal limits based on clinical criteria. Where cognitive changes are noted, the affected domains have been remarkably consistent, with greatest differences noted in processing speed, executive functions such as working memory, and aspects of episodic memory (Jansen et al, 2005). Psychometric and subjective findings are greatest during and shortly after treatment and resolve in most patients over time, with longitudinal studies showing residual cognitive sequelae in only a subset of patients several years post-treatment. Multiple candidate mechanisms for these effects have been proposed, including chemotherapy-induced DNA damage (directly or through increases in oxidative stress), individual variation in genes related to neural repair and/or plasticity, and/or chemotherapy-induced hormonal changes (Ahles and Saykin, 2007). Structural and functional neuroimaging has been applied to examine the neural substrate of these cognitive changes in both retrospective and prospective samples. Using voxel-based morphometry, a structural MRI technique that is capable of detecting alterations in gray matter density, our group has detected decreases in gray matter in bilateral frontal and temporal regions 1 month after chemotherapy relative to baseline (Figure 1 and Table 1) (McDonald et al, in press). These changes do not occur in breast cancer patients who are not treated with chemotherapy or healthy controls. One year later, these gray matter alterations show partial but not complete recovery, consistent with previous work in retrospective samples. fMRI and PET have also shown differences in brain function during tasks tapping episodic memory and executive functions, including working memory (de Ruiter et al, 2010). Mirroring the cognitive literature, altered patterns of brain activation have been found both prior to adjuvant treatment and following chemotherapy or hormonal treatment. Both structural and functional neuroimaging approaches have shown alteration in frontal brain regions, consistent with the most commonly affected cognitive processes in prior neuropsychological studies. Ongoing research examining variables that contribute to cancer- and treatment-related cognitive and brain changes (eg, genetic variability and other bioarkers, age, cognitive reserve, and other medical comorbidities) will be critical to identifying potential risk factors that may increase individual vulnerability (Ahles et al, in press). Preclinical research in animal models can be expected to further enhance the understanding of underlying mechanisms. Finally, identification of optimal treatment approaches will be an important future direction.
Table 1. Atlas Coordinates, Cluster Extents, P and T Values, and Region Descriptions.
Acknowledgments
The work described here was supported in part by National Cancer Institute Grants 5R01CA087845 and 5R01CA101318.
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
References
- Ahles TA, Saykin AJ. Candidate mechanisms for chemotherapy-induced cognitive changes. Nat Rev Cancer. 2007;7:192–201. doi: 10.1038/nrc2073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ahles TA, Saykin AJ, McDonald BC, Li Y, Furstenberg CT, Hanscom BS, et al. 2010Longitudinal assessment of cognitive changes associated with adjuvant treatment for breast cancer: the impact of age and cognitive reserve J Clin Oncolin press). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- de Ruiter MB, Reneman L, Boogerd W, Veltman DJ, van Dam FSAM, Nederveen AJ, et al. 2010Cerebral hyporesponsiveness and cognitive impairment 10 years after chemotherapy for breast cancer Hum Brain Mapping(in press); e-pub ahead of print 28 July 2010, at http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/hbm.21102/pdf . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Jansen CE, Miaskowski C, Dodd M, Dowling G, Kramer J. A metaanalysis of studies of the effects of cancer chemotherapy on various domains of cognitive function. Cancer. 2005;104:2222–2233. doi: 10.1002/cncr.21469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McDonald BC, Conroy SK, Ahles TA, West JD, Saykin AJ.2010Gray matter reduction associated with systemic chemotherapy for breast cancer: a prospective MRI study Breast Cancer Res Treat(in press); e-pub ahead of print 6 August 2010, at http://www.springerlink.com/content/5686768t1q25287p/fulltext.pdf . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Meyers CA, Perry JR.(eds) (2008Cognition and Cancer Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, MA [Google Scholar]