Abstract
Modulating glutamatergic neurotransmission induces alterations in conscious experience that mimic the symptoms of early psychotic illness. We review studies that use intravenous administration of ketamine, focusing on interindividual variability in the profundity of the ketamine experience. We will consider this individual variability within a hypothetical model of brain and cognitive function centered upon learning and inference. Within this model, the brains, neural systems, and even single neurons specify expectations about their inputs and responding to violations of those expectations with new learning that renders future inputs more predictable. We argue that ketamine temporarily deranges this ability by perturbing both the ways in which prior expectations are specified and the ways in which expectancy violations are signaled. We suggest that the former effect is predominantly mediated by NMDA blockade and the latter by augmented and inappropriate feedforward glutamatergic signaling. We suggest that the observed interindividual variability emerges from individual differences in neural circuits that normally underpin the learning and inference processes described. The exact source for that variability is uncertain, although it is likely to arise not only from genetic variation but also from subjects' previous experiences and prior learning. Furthermore, we argue that chronic, unlike acute, NMDA blockade alters the specification of expectancies more profoundly and permanently. Scrutinizing individual differences in the effects of acute and chronic ketamine administration in the context of the Bayesian brain model may generate new insights about the symptoms of psychosis; their underlying cognitive processes and neurocircuitry.
Keywords: glutamate, ketamine, psychosis, negative symptoms, prediction error
INTRODUCTION
A Brief History of Psychotomimesis and NMDA Blockade
Psychotomimetic drugs have a rich, though controversial, history in neuropsychopharmacology research. The term psychotomimetic was first used to describe the similarity between the subjective effects of lysergic acid diethylamide in healthy volunteers and the reports of patients with schizophrenia (Osmond, 1957). However, research with compounds that engender psychosis-like experiences has a much longer history; Moreau, (1845/1973) documented the effects of marijuana intoxication during the 1840s, noting that different subjects reacted quite differently to identical doses. Knauer and Maloney (1913) emphasized the importance of set (the individual subject's physiology) and setting (the situation in, and route by which the compound is administered) in explaining the variability in subjects' responses to psychotogenic compounds such as mescaline. Beringer (1927) first used the term ‘artificial model of psychosis' he emphasized the potential utility of testing subjects before, during, and after the intake of the agent, believing that inducing psychosis in this way, he could discern primary pathological mechanisms from secondary reactions to, and coping mechanisms for such pathology. These themes, emerging early in the history of drug models of psychosis, are ones that will guide much of this current review. While we view the central ideas as generalizing across a number of different psychotomimetic drugs (see Corlett et al, 2009a, 2009b and Box 1 for review), we focus here on drugs whose mode of action involves noncompetitive NMDA receptor antagonism.
Other psychotomimetic drugs.
We believe the effects of other psychotomimetic drugs might be explicable within the Bayesian framework (Corlett et al, 2009a).
Serotonergic drugs: Lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD) and other serotonergic hallucinogens act at 5-HT receptors; however, they also affect glutamatergic transmission (Aghajanian and Marek, 2000), enhancing glutamatergic responses to sensory stimuli in the locus coeruleus (Rasmussen and Aghjahanian, 1986). They induce profound hallucinations (Geyer and Vollenweider, 2008). Cortically, 5-HT stimulation enhances excitatory glutamatergic postsynaptic potentials (Aghajanian and Marek, 1997). The effect is blocked by MgluR2/3 agonists (Marek et al, 2000), suggesting that serotonergic hallucinogens induce an increase in presynaptic glutamate release in frontal cortex. However, LSD does not impair NMDA receptor signaling (Aghajanian and Marek, 2000), and may actually enhance it (Lambe and Aghajanian, 2006). Within the Bayesian framework, excessive AMPA signaling in the absence of NMDA impairment would lead to increased sensory noise in the context of normal priors leading to hallucinations; the persistence and strength of the feedforward signal suggests that there is something to be explained, and preserved (perhaps enhanced) top-down processes posit an hallucinatory explanation.
Cannabinoids: Δ-9-Tetrahydrocannabinol (Δ-9-THC) has psychotomimetic effects in healthy volunteers (D'Souza et al, 2004). A clue to the action of the Δ-9-THC comes from its impact upon perception of the hollow-mask illusion, which depends on the interaction between top-down expectancies and bottom-up experiences (Emrich, 1989; Koethe et al, 2006). Under normal conditions, a rotating face mask will continue to be experienced as convex, which, though objectively wrong, is the expected and more plausible percept. In healthy volunteers administered with Δ-9-THC, the illusion is attenuated (Koethe et al, 2006; Semple et al, 2003). This suggests an effect of cannabinoid signaling on top-down correction of perception (Emrich et al, 1991; Schneider et al, 1996). Ketamine has no effect on perception of the illusion, despite inducing perceptual changes and psychotic symptoms (Passie et al, 2003), perhaps because it has a predominant impact on bottom-up AMPA signaling in contrast to the predominant top-down effect of THC. Δ9-THC increases prefrontal glutamate release (Pistis et al, 2002). However, in the hippocampus, cannabinoids act presynaptically to inhibit Ca2+-induced release of glutamate and acetylcholine (Fujiwara and Egashira, 2004). We have argued that these two neurotransmitters are intimately involved in the specification of priors and, in the absence of strong priors/ one would predict a weaker hollow-mask illusion.
Dopamine agonists: Acute amphetamine administration increases striatal dopamine release in healthy humans (Kegeles et al, 1999; Laruelle et al, 1995) and ketamine pretreatment enhances this effect (Kegeles et al, 2000), suggesting that amphetamine-induced dopamine release may be under glutamatergic control. A single amphetamine administration leads to elevated mood, grandiose ideas, and hyperactivity (Jacobs and Silverstone, 1986). It also increases perceptual acuity of the whole visual field (Fillmore et al, 2005), unlike ketamine, which enhances the salience of discrete and apparently random objects, events, and stimuli (Corlett et al, 2007; Oye et al, 1992). Amphetamine may increase confidence in actual perceptual inputs and predictions through enhanced dopaminergic (Kegeles et al, 1999; Laruelle et al, 1995) and cholinergic function (Acquas and Fibiger, 1998). Dopamine also increases the signal-to-noise ratio of perceptual inputs, hence amphetamine increases the extent to which a bottom-up signal matches a strong top-down expectancy, leading to increased confidence in the capacity to predict and control the environment manifest as increased perceptual clarity and potentially grandiosity.
A richer pattern of psychopathology is induced through repeated amphetamine administration including profound delusions and hallucinations in humans (Angrist et al, 1970). Repeated administration of amphetamine alters striatal responsivity (Boileau et al, 2006). This may be due to changes in the morphology of dendritic spines in striatal medium spiny neurons (Robinson and Kolb, 1997, 1999), altered expression of NMDA and AMPA receptors (Lu et al, 1997; Lu and Wolf, 1999), and changes in prefrontal cholinergic function (Sarter et al, 2005; 2008). Hence repeated amphetamine administration engenders aberrant feedforward AMPA-mediated prediction error, but in the context of a sensitized dopamine system, prone to excessive and inappropriate dopamine and acetylcholine release in response to nonsalient events (Nelson et al, 2002). As these slower transmitters are critical in specifying top-down perceptual and attentional priors (Collerton, 2005; Friston, 2005), their inappropriate registration confers a sensory expectancy when there should be none, engendering hallucinations proper rather than the odd perceptions characteristic of ketamine (Pomarol-Clotet et al, 2006).
We aim to outline a model within which to understand how the administration of psychotomimetic compounds can induce changes in subjective experience that are redolent of psychosis. There is a wealth of work on the detailed physiological mechanisms through which psychotomimetics affect neural function (Aghajanian, 2009). We aim to build upon that work by outlining a set of principles that may allow us to extrapolate from perturbed synaptic function to disordered subjective experience. Although we focus on the cognitive aspects of psychotomimetic effects, but we believe that the Bayesian model that we outline may well provide a tool for translational investigations of the psychotic symptoms of schizophrenia (Corlett et al, 2010b).
NMDA receptor antagonists and human subjects
Phencyclidine (or PCP) and ketamine bind noncompetitively to NMDA receptors (Anis et al, 1983). At higher doses, these NMDA receptor antagonists produce a dissociative anesthesia: a profound analgesia coupled with a somnolent state in which the patient does not appear to be asleep or anesthetized but rather disconnected from his surroundings (Corssen et al, 1968). However, on emergence from anesthesia, subjects treated with PCP experienced psychotomimetic side effects (Luby et al, 1959), which resembled the effects of sensory deprivation (Meyer et al, 1959). These include ‘feelings of anxiety, depression or fear together with a difficulty in thinking and concentration. In higher doses illusional, delusional and hallucinatory experience and a sensation of displacement commonly occur', for example, subjects administered with PCP suffered persecutory thoughts, illusory experiences of objects, as well as their own bodies. PCP induced negative symptoms, including subjective thought block, anhedonia, catatonia, and waxy flexibility; ‘his limbs would persist if placed in abnormal postures for several minutes'. These states were postoperative, following the use of PCP as an anesthesia, and they were transient, lasting 12–72 h (Meyer et al, 1959).
Healthy subjects administered with a low subanesthetic dose of the drug experienced disturbances in body image and a difficulty distinguishing self and nonself. Their thinking was disorganized, manifest as concreteness in proverb interpretation. Subjects were apathetic, they were able to recall hearing questions and comprehending instructions, but felt no special compulsion to respond or comply with requests. They also described a peculiar state (described by Luby et al as Hypnagogic), reporting feeling as though they were in some specific setting from their personal past (for example a classroom) that they were able to describe in detail (eg, recognizing an old school friend); a phenomenon reminiscent of the rare reports of confabulation in head-injured patients with frontostriatal damage (Schnider, 2001).
Ketamine was first synthesized by Parke-Davis in 1963 (Jansen, 2004) and first administered to a human subject on 3 August 1964 (Domino, 1984). Then named CI-581, it was also found to have dissociative anesthetic properties but also induced emergence reactions including ‘marked changes in mood and affect', which varied across subjects, as well as ‘frank hallucinatory episodes'. These effects usually subsided within 30 min (Domino et al, 1965).
Unlike a number of other drugs used to model schizophrenia, NMDA receptor blockade can induce both positive and negative symptoms and therefore provide a more complete model of psychosis (Javitt and Zukin, 1991). These observations provided the foundation for the NMDA receptor hypofunction model of schizophrenia (Abi-Saab et al, 1998; Javitt and Zukin, 1991; Olney and Farber, 1995; Sharp et al, 2001; Tsai and Coyle, 2002). However, while the negative symptoms induced by the drug are related to its NMDA-binding capacity, positive symptoms are not (Stone et al, 2008), suggesting some non-NMDA component to the generation of positive symptoms. This may be mediated by postsynaptic AMPA receptors (Jackson et al, 2004; Moghaddam et al, 1997). Despite modeling both the positive and negative symptoms associated with schizophreniform psychosis, the acute ketamine model is not without its detractors. See Box 2 for discussion.
Controversies.
The model psychoses are not without their detractors (Langlitz, 2006) with relevant and important criticisms about the conclusions that one can draw about psychosis from ketamine research.
Hallucinations
There is some controversy as to whether ketamine induces frank auditory and visual hallucinations (Krystal et al, 1994). The available evidence suggests that ketamine certainly induces perceptual illusions, that is, it alters the experience of stimuli that are actually present. Some authors report auditory and visual hallucinations (Bowdle et al, 1998; Malhotra et al, 1996; Vollenweider et al, 1997a; Vollenweider et al, 1997b), others do not (Krystal et al, 1994). Newcomer et al (1999) states that that the hallucinatory behavior item on the BPRS-incorporated perceptual distortions, as well as hallucinations, as distortions are ‘perceptions without normal external stimulus correspondence…distinctly different from the thought and imagery processes of normal people' (Newcomer et al, 1999). The auditory illusions described by patients with schizophrenia early in their illness are precursors of hallucinations (Bowers, 1968; Bowers and Freedman, 1966; Chapman, 1966; Freedman and Chapman, 1973; Freedman, 1974; McGhie and Chapman, 1961). This possibility is illustrated by the following quote from a first episode patient:
‘I seem to get a little mixed up where sounds come from. Often I have to check up if someone speaks to me and several times I thought someone was shouting through the window then it was really the wireless at the front of the house.' (Patient 11, (McGhie and Chapman, 1961).
The auditory illusions characteristic of the ketamine experience may, if the neurochemical perturbation were maintained, become full-blown hallucinations (Allen and Young, 1978; Jentsch and Roth, 1999).
In our hands, acute ketamine administration to healthy volunteers occasionally engenders the hypnagogic phenomena described by Luby; subjects feel transported away from the experimental setting and into a new space. However, new space is often influenced by stimuli or thoughts present at the time of the experience; a polka dot dress becomes the night sky, and the noise of the functional neuroimaging scanner becomes a crowd singing at a football match (PR Corlett and JH Krystal, unpublished observations).
Delusions
There is also controversy regarding the effects of ketamine on beliefs; some investigators find that acute ketamine does not induce delusional ideation (Freeman et al, 2009). Alternatively even if the phenomena are observed, some feel that the beliefs expressed do not share enough of the key features of delusions for them to be considered delusions; they are transient, and, even when subjects endorse delusion-like ideas, they do so with varying degrees of conviction and, for the most part, they describe their experiences in relative terms (‘it feels/felt as if…').
First, there are dose effects on the phenomenology that people experience; higher doses more reliably induce delusional ideas (Bowdle et al, 1998), those investigators who claim that ketamine does not induce delusions often use lower doses (Freeman et al, 2009). Furthermore, given the individual differences in ketamine response that we described, the study design is also critically important, especially, when a between-group design is employed and there are small numbers of participants per cell; one runs the risk of not incorporating any delusion prone subjects in the group administered ketamine.
In addition, subjects administered with ketamine often use relative terms to describe their beliefs and experiences (‘it feels as if…'), although some subjects do report a high degree of certainty retrospectively (Pomarol-Clotet et al, 2006). Rather than delusions per se, ketamine may be a good model for the ‘delusional mood' (Schneider, 1930) that attends the very early phases of psychosis (Corlett et al, 2007). Here patients report: ‘Wherever you are looking, everything looks unreal.' (Gross and Huber, 1972). Furthermore: ‘People look confusing…they are almost like they're made up…people that I know…have masks on or they are disguising themselves. It's like a play…like a big production story' (Freedman and Chapman, 1973). Patients experiencing delusional mood use the same relative terms employed by subjects administered ketamine (Corlett et al, 2010a).
Negative symptoms
NMDA receptor antagonist psychotomimetic drugs are also known as dissociative anesthetics because of the unresponsive dissociated state that they induce (Domino et al, 1965). Given that they are anesthetics, the apathy, anhedonia, alogia, and executive dysfunctions that ketamine induces may be a rather nonspecific sedative effect (Pomarol-Clotet et al, 2006). We believe that the data we outlined minister against such a conclusion; if ketamine's cognitive effects were due to a general sedative effect, we would not have observed the predictive associations that we did; across cognitive domains, responses to specific cognitive processes within circumscribed neural circuits were predictive of particular symptoms in a manner outlined by cognitive neuropsychiatric accounts of those symptoms (Honey et al, 2008).
Experimental psychoses?
Finally, the ketamine experience has a defined cause (the drug infusion) and a definite end point. Although the reports of schizophrenic patients early in their illness resemble those of healthy volunteers administered with ketamine, the psychological effects of the drug are obviously attributable to its infusion. This may explain the degree of insight remained by ketamine-treated subjects.
‘Ketamine subjects—unlike schizophrenics—could at least in part recognize derealisation and depersonalization phenomena as abnormal experiences and attribute them to the drug.' (Vollenweider et al, 1997a)
‘Many schizophrenics have their times of heavenly happiness; but the fact that (unlike the mescaline taker) they do not know when, if ever, they will return to the reassuring banality of everyday experiences causes even heaven to seem appalling' (Huxley, 1956).
Potential resolutions
‘Remember that all models are wrong; the practical question is how wrong do they have to be to not be useful' (Box and Draper, 1987).
We concur with the engineer George Box, a sentiment that has been echoed by previous neuropsychopharmacologists and neurobiologists studying model psychoses; it would be extraordinary if an acute drug administration could transiently and reversibly engender the symptoms of schizophrenia in full (Laruelle et al, 2003), but what can we reasonably gain from studying the acute administration of psychotomimetic drugs? If we assume that the states induced by acute ketamine administration are redolent of the very early prodromal phases of psychosis (Corlett et al, 2010b; Corlett et al, 2007), we believe that the pseudodelusions and protohallucinations that acute ketamine engages can be used to explore a phase of illness that is not normally amenable to investigation, the transition from risk to illness; a phase in which the sufferer does not perceive that there is any problem and often gains pleasure from their new-found insights and abilities (McGhie and Chapman, 1961). For example, the hallucination-like experiences that are induced in some subjects often have an eliciting stimulus, suggesting that perhaps endogenous hallucinations too have an initial environmental basis. Likewise, the delusion-like ideas that ketamine engenders are often linked to some initial unexpected and inexplicable percept; might endogenous delusions form in the same way? Using ketamine, and appropriate cognitive tasks, we can refine our understanding of the psychology and neurobiology of symptom generation and, in so doing, we will be better equipped to design psychological and pharmacological treatments that are tailored to specific symptoms and that target the causes of those symptoms more directly.
Acute ketamine administration
Since its discovery, many studies have used acute, subanesthetic ketamine administration to induce psychotic symptoms (Bowdle et al, 1998; Ghoneim et al, 1985; Krystal et al, 1994; Oye et al, 1992), as well as cognitive deficits redolent of schizophrenia (eg, (Honey et al, 2005a, 2005b, 2005c; Krystal et al, 1994, 2000; Malhotra et al, 1996; Morgan et al, 2004a; Newcomer et al, 1999). The dose and pattern of ketamine delivery varies across studies, although the intravenous route is used almost exclusively. Single bolus injections of ketamine lead to rapid changes in subjective experience that decline as the drug is metabolized and excreted (Bowdle et al, 1998; Oye et al, 1992). In order to prolong the effects of the drug and allow for more rigorous characterization of psychotic symptoms, bolus injections can be followed by a maintenance dose (Bowdle et al, 1998; Breier et al, 1997; Krystal et al, 1994; Malhotra et al, 1996; Newcomer et al, 1999; Vollenweider et al, 1997a, 1997b, 2000) (Krystal et al, 1998). Maintenance doses are often applied using computer-controlled pumps to deliver ketamine at a particular rate (Honey et al, 2003, 2004, 2005a, 2005b, 2005c). The infusion schemes commonly employed can be summarized as BET; that is, a bolus (B), followed by a constant rate infusion to compensate for drug elimination (E), and an exponentially decreasing infusion to compensate for drug distribution or transfer (T) between the body's compartments (Kruger-Thiemer, 1968). The pharmacokinetic parameters for these computerized pumps came from early anesthetic studies by Domino et al, 1982 (Keefe et al, 1999). These models were based on volunteer subjects who were amputees, as they were compartment based (ie, their parameters were estimated based on compartment volumes that were significantly smaller than nonamputee volunteers), and their predictions were often inaccurate (Absalom et al, 2007). Furthermore, as the pharmacokinetic and pharmacodynamic parameters were based on higher anesthetic doses, and hence there were inaccuracies and inconsistencies at the lower subanesthetic doses favored in psychotomimetic ketamine research, newer models are available with a shorter time to peak concentration and a more accurate maintenance of ketamine plasma levels (Absalom et al, 2007).
A further potential source of variability across studies is the ketamine itself. Ketamine has two enantiomers, (S) and (R) ketamine. Although commonly administered as a racemic mixture (a 50 : 50 mix of both enantiomers), it appears that the (S) enantiomer is more psychotogenic than the (R) (Vollenweider et al, 1997a). Furthermore, the two enantiomers have different effects on brain metabolism; (S)-ketamine induces hyperfrontality that correlates with the severity of psychotomimetic effects (Vollenweider et al, 1997a), whereas (R)-ketamine induces hypoactivation of posterior cortical regions (Vollenweider et al, 1997a). If different batches of ketamine were composed of different proportions of these enantiomers, then variability in responses would be observed. However, there is still significant interindividual variability in the ketamine response even within a single study using one batch of ketamine.
Exploring Individual Variability: Relating Tasks to Symptoms
Given that we can reliably control the level of drug to which a subject is exposed, it may be possible to use modern neuroscientific techniques to dissociate the individual variability in symptoms from the variations in dose received. Following Beringer (1927), we argue that characterization of the individual differences in the psychotomimetic effects of ketamine enables us to test cognitive and neural models of how those symptoms arise (Kosslyn et al, 2002; Underwood, 1975). We can challenge subjects with psychological tasks that engage a number of key cognitive processes; we can capture the neural circuitry engaged by those processes using functional imaging; and, subsequently, we can administer controlled doses of ketamine that induce the psychotomimetic effects of interest. With these data, we can relate subjects' baseline neural and behavioral responses (in the absence of the drug and even when the participant has never before experienced the drug) to the psychopathology they experience; such an approach may be used to test hypotheses about the cognitive and neural bases of specific symptoms (Corlett et al, 2006; Honey et al, 2008; Umbricht et al, 2002) (Krystal et al, 2003).
Previous work employing this approach has found evidence in favor of such links for both positive (Corlett et al, 2006; Honey et al, 2008; Umbricht et al, 2002) and negative symptoms (Honey et al, 2008). These studies exploited the potential offered by functional neuroimaging for allowing observation of latent behavioral processes (Henson, 2006; Poldrack, 2006). For example, as we discuss in more detail below, we can use functional neuroimaging to provide an assay or brain marker for prediction error and thereby devise means of using variations in this marker as the basis for developing our understanding of the emergence of delusions.
A key concept, common to this and to the other examples below, is that these signals (and their variability across subjects) provide an opportunity for testing cognitive neuropsychiatric models of psychotic symptoms. This approach is centrally related to notions of efficiency and sensitivity in brain–behavior relationships during cognitive tasks. That is, for some tasks, more proficient subjects engage particular neural circuits to a lesser degree (Rypma and D'Esposito, 1999); for other tasks, greater activation in a set of regions may be associated with better task performance (Gray et al, 2003). These differences in relationship between magnitude of brain response and behavioral competence may reflect the engagement of different cognitive processes and neural circuits by specific task demands (Rypma and Prabhakaran, 2010). An appreciation of the relationship between magnitude of brain response and behavioral competence is critical in interpreting any correlation between brain responses and ketamine-induced symptoms. Thus, fMRI data that capture a particular process in intact individuals can be used to implicate that process in psychotic symptom generation by exploring the extent to which the presence or magnitude of the neural marker for the process predicts the severity of specific symptoms when those same individuals are administered a psychotomimetic drug. Combining cognitive neuroscience, psychopharmacology, and psychiatry in this way can enrich our understanding of the formation of psychotic symptoms; a disease process not readily amenable to empirical study (Corlett et al, 2007).
Exploring Individual Variability: Using Tasks to Predict Symptoms
Predicting delusions and perceptual aberrations
Beliefs emerge because we learn about associations in our environment. Formal learning theories relating to these associations appeal to prediction error or surprise as a driving force in learning and have been able to explain a number of key behavioral observations (Kamin, 1969; Rescorla and Wagner, 1972), However, prediction error remained a ‘latent' process whose presence was inferred but not observed for many years. Neurophysiological recording from midbrain dopamine neurons during reward conditioning in nonhuman primates revealed a prediction error signal in these neurons (Montague et al, 1996; Schultz, 1998; Waelti et al, 2001). Subsequent neuroimaging studies in human subjects have found evidence for similar signals in the midbrain (D'Ardenne et al, 2008), the striatum (McClure et al, 2003; O'Doherty et al, 2003), and the frontal cortex (Corlett et al, 2004; Fletcher et al, 2001; Turner et al, 2004).
Thus, prediction error signal in the brain can be estimated even though it does not have any explicit behavioural correlates. This has proven useful in exploring the brain basis for the anomalous perceptions and beliefs that occur in association with acute ketamine administration. With respect to positive symptoms, sensitivity to prediction error within right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex was predictive of the perceptual aberrations and delusional ideation suffered on a high dose of ketamine across subjects; those subjects with a larger prediction error response to events that violated their expectancies were more likely to suffer perceptual aberrations, attentional capture, and referential delusions under ketamine (Corlett et al, 2006); see Figure 1. Furthermore, a low subpsychotic dose of ketamine engendered aberrant prediction error responding during causal learning and inference in the midbrain, hippocampus, striatum, and DLPFC, which showed a trend toward predicting delusions and perceptual aberrations (Corlett et al, 2006).
A physiological measure with relevance to sensory prediction and prediction error is mismatch negativity (MMN). The MMN is an electrocortical response to rare deviant auditory or visual stimuli embedded in a stream of identical stimuli (Cammann, 1990; Naatanen et al, 1978). Umbricht et al (2002) found that a weaker auditory MMN response at baseline (in the absence of ketamine) was predictive of more severe positive symptoms under ketamine. That is, those subjects who showed a reduced electrocortical response to rare deviant tones in a stream of predictable tones scored more highly on an aberrant experiences scale when they were administered ketamine. This appears at odds with our observation; however, the paradigm used to engage the MMN response involves a predictable pattern of tones with a deviant occurring once every ten tones. Paradigms in which deviant events are more unpredictable observe larger MMN responses (Kimura et al, 2010; Sussman et al, 1998), suggesting that subjects (either implicitly or explicitly) learn the tone sequence. In formal learning theory terms, they use prediction errors; events that violate their expectancy to learn the structure of their environment (Rescorla and Wagner, 1972). As such, subjects with a smaller average MMN response might actually have an increased sensitivity to prediction error and learn the predictability of their sensory inputs more effectively. Of course this is speculative and there are other methodological differences between the studies, which may have contributed to the difference (such as different doses and methods of ketamine administration, as well as different symptom ratings scales).
Predicting negative symptoms
Negative symptoms involve social and cognitive disengagement, perhaps due to a reduction in processing capacity of prefrontal cortex (Silver and Feldman, 2005), which leads to difficulties in sustaining concentration and maintaining task set (Nuechterlein et al, 1986). If this is the case, those subjects who had inefficient prefrontal function during the attention and working memory tasks would show increased vulnerability to negative symptoms under high-dose ketamine. This was what we observed (Honey et al, 2008). For both an n-back working memory task and a continuous performance task (CPT) involving sustained attention, there was a significant association between task-related activation under placebo and the expression of negative symptoms on a high dose of ketamine. Regression analyses revealed a strong relationship between lateral prefrontal cortex activity under placebo and negative symptom scores under ketamine (see Figure 1). For the attention task (CPT), there was significant association between task-related activation and negative symptoms in the bilateral inferior frontal gyri and right middle frontal gyrus (see Figure 1). Both findings are consistent with our prediction that the efficiency of responses to working memory and attentional demands, particularly frontally mediated responses, would relate to negative symptoms under the drug.
Predicting thought disorder and (pseudo)hallucinations
Another cognitive domain that can be explored in relation to positive symptoms is self-monitoring; the ability to distinguish self from nonself related processing (Frith et al, 2000b). Self-monitoring has been implicated in the genesis of auditory hallucinations, where sufferers may be misattributing their own inner speech to an external source (Frith et al, 2000b). Furthermore, aberrations of self-monitoring have been invoked to explain delusions of passivity—in which the sufferer believes that their thoughts or actions are under the control of an external agent (Frith et al, 2000b). Finally, verbal self-monitoring is relevant to another cardinal psychotic symptom, formal thought disorder, in which a sufferer's expressive language is disrupted such that they produce tangential disconnected speech (Oh et al, 2002); sometimes subjects produce no speech at all (alogia) and they claim not have any thoughts (subjective thought block) (Berenbaum et al, 2008). Working memory and, in particular, contextual regulation of ongoing processing are particularly relevant to the induction of thought disorder (Barrera et al, 2005). We explored the idea that, if ketamine produces thought disorder and hallucination-like perceptual aberrations, then the extent to which it does may relate to subject's ability to perform tasks engaging self-monitoring and to the neural underpinnings of such a task. Subjects' were therefore required to imagine visually presented sentences being read in their own inner speech relative or in the voice of one of two ‘robots'. Imagining robotic speech preferentially engaged fontal and temporal cortices. Critically, those subjects in whom this underlying activation was greatest experienced greater changes in perceptual salience and severity of thought disorder when receiving ketamine outside the scanner on a separate occasion. The finding was considered in terms of a relative inefficiency of processing such that those who required the greatest level of activation to carry out the task were those most vulnerable to these particular effects of the drug. Furthermore, within the same task, we manipulated a requirement to generate the endings of sentences. Inefficiency of left frontal cortical engagement during this component of the task predicted the severity of thought disorder across subjects (Honey et al, 2008); see Figure 1.
It may be that the prediction error model that we have found useful in considering brain responses predictive of delusional ideas (above) is also useful here. Self-monitoring processes involve a predictive expectation and a comparison with experience in order to discern self from other (Blakemore et al, 2002; Frith et al, 2000a). Furthermore, sentence completion involves a predictive contextual process that constrains the set of words that may be employed to complete a particular sentence (Hoeks et al, 2004). Aberrant prediction-related processes may underpin individual susceptibility to thought disorder and auditory illusory phenomena (precursory to hallucinations).
Thus, there is evidence that individual variability in brain responses can be predictive of ketamine-induced symptoms and experiences. Moreover, these observations offer clues about the cognitive and neural basis for symptoms insofar as they highlight the process- and region-specific vulnerability markers for the array of drug effects. In the remainder of this article, we will attempt to bring these links between cognition, brain function, and psychiatric phenomenology under a common explanatory framework; the Bayesian model of brain function (Friston, 2005a, 2009) and psychosis (Corlett et al, 2009a; Fletcher and Frith, 2009; Stephan et al, 2009).
Individual Variability and the Bayesian Brain: a Model that Links Cognitive Variability to Symptom Susceptibility
Of course it is interesting, and, potentially, clinically useful to be able to predict how an individual will respond to a drug on the basis of their ‘baseline' measures of task-specific neural response. But it would be especially so if we could use this information to frame hypotheses about how the relationship between the cognitive architecture of that individual and his or her specific vulnerability (to say, negative symptoms or to perceptual disturbance) might show us how cognition and symptoms emerge from the same systems whose variable responsiveness predicts this vulnerability. We will consider whether this is possible by proposing a unifying account of the positive and negative symptoms engendered by ketamine (both acutely and chronically) expressed in terms of the increasingly influential Bayesian conception of the brain and behavior (Corlett et al, 2009a; Fletcher and Frith, 2009; Stephan et al, 2009). What we perceive and learn and how we comport ourselves is affected by the sensations incident upon us combined with our prior experiences and expectations about the causes of those sensations (Bayes, 1764); see Box 3. We add that this explanation may be generalized to other drug models (see Corlett et al, 2009a, 2009b and Box 1).
Bayes, the mind and the brain.
Thomas Bayes was an English mathematician and minister whose theorem on probabilities was published posthumously in 1764 (Bayes, 1764). In the paper, Bayes' stance on probability is as follows:
‘The probability of any event is the ratio between the value at which an expectation depending on the happening of the event ought to be computed, and the value of the thing expected upon its happening.'
Why should a neuropsychopharmacologist care about such an arcane mathematical theory? Bayes theorem might help us, as scientists, to reason about new data and evaluate our a priori hypotheses: according to Bayes rule, the probability that a hypothesis is true given the observed data (the posterior probability) is proportional to the likelihood (the probability of those data given that the hypothesis is true) multiplied by the prior probability (the probability that the hypothesis was correct before the data were seen). For simplicity, the ‘prior probability' is often abbreviated as the ‘prior'.
More formally, we can express the relationship between a hypothesis and some new data as:
Where H is the hypothesis and D is the data. P(H) is the prior probability of H: the probability that H is correct before the data D were observed. P(D∣H) is the probability off seeing the data D given that the hypothesis (H) is true. P(D∣H) is the likelihood. P(D) is the probability of observing the data D and P(H∣D) is the posterior probability.
Hence, surprising data might be discredited in the face of overwhelming prior experience, or they might demand explanation and an updating of our priors.
Aside from reasoning about data, we believe that this theory might help us understand cognition (Shanks, 2006), perception (Rao and Ballard, 1999), brain function (Friston, 2005a, 2009), and dysfunction (Corlett et al, 2009a; Fletcher and Frith, 2009; Hemsley and Garety, 1986). Herman Von Helmholtz was a physician and physicist who constructed a prescient theory of visual perception (Helmholtz, 1878/1971) that appears steeped in Bayes theorem; Helmholtz postulated that perception was a process of unconscious inference that the brain entertains hypotheses about possible inputs and compares them with incoming sensations (Gregory, 1997; Helmholtz, 1878/1971). Through experience, particular candidate hypotheses can come to be favored over others, based on their minimizing the uncertainty or free-energy associated with a given situation; that is, the mismatch between data and priors, or prediction error (see Figure 2). A compelling demonstration of this phenomenon involves observing a rotating hollow face mask; the binocular depth inversion illusion (Emrich, 1989). As our overwhelming experience of faces is that they are convex, the backside of the mask is perceived as convex when it rotates into view, even though the incoming sensory information indicates a concave object.
Pavlov equated the learned conditioned reflexes that he observed with Helmholtz' learned perceptual expectancies (Barlow, 1990; Helmholtz, 1878/1971), an assertion supported by empirical evidence (Davies et al, 1982). Such conditioned expectancies are also important for causal inference and belief formation (Dickinson, 2001); in particular, we learn most in situations that violate our expectancies, inducing prediction error (Rescorla and Wagner, 1972); we attend most to features that are coincident with prediction error (Pearce and Hall, 1980) and we use prediction errors to learn the parameters of causal models as well as which causal models are able to explain the co-occurrence of particular events (Waldmann and Martignon, 1998).
Even single neurons may be Bayesian; that is, they may represent a distribution of expected inputs and respond to incident stimuli by changing that distribution (Deneve, 2008a). These processes could be implemented by shuttling receptors and ion channels into and out of the membrane to control the level of depolarization of the cell and its sensitivity to subsequent incoming stimuli (Fiorillo, 2008).
This Bayesian framework encompasses levels of explanation, from cellular physiology to cognitive psychology, making it an ideal candidate for a common language in translational neuroscience.
The model conceives of a general purpose for the brains, brain systems, and even single neurons; they aim to predict their subsequent inputs with increasing certainty (Deneve 2008a, 2008b; Fiorillo, 2008; Friston 2005a). This idea has its roots in formal associative and reinforcement learning theories (Rescorla and Wagner, 1972; Sutton and Barto, 1981), as well as in the work of Hermann Von Helmholtz, who theorized that perception was a constructive process based on learned expectancies (Helmholtz, 1878/1971). We attend to and learn about events that violate our expectancies such that we are less surprised subsequently (Kamin, 1969; Pearce and Hall, 1980; Rescorla and Wagner, 1972). This ‘prediction error' concept has been formally developed to account not only for reinforcement and causal learning (Dickinson, 2001; Fletcher et al, 2001; Schultz and Dickinson, 2000) but also the receptive field properties of the visual system (Rao and Ballard, 1999); the anatomy and physiology of cortical hierarchies (Friston, 2005a; Mesulam, 2008; Mumford, 1992), as well as the interactions between basal ganglia learning systems and sensory cortices that mediate perceptual learning (den Ouden et al, 2009).
Implementing Bayes in the Brain
The Bayesian hierarchical model makes explicit predictions about the roles of different neurotransmitter systems and subsystems in signaling predictions and prediction errors. This division of labor maps on to other conceptions of interneuronal signaling such as the notion of neural ‘drivers' and ‘modulators' (Sherman and Guillery, 1998); ‘drivers' send signals upward through the hierarchy and ‘modulators' specify predictions top-down (Friston, 2005a). While the originators of driving and modulation as interneural effects did not specify a currency for driving, they did speculate that modulation took place through metabotropic glutamate receptors (Sherman and Guillery, 1998). Subsequently, Friston suggested that the topography and signaling properties of glutamate receptor subtypes might be well suited to instantiating both driving and modulation (Angelucci et al, 2002a, 2002b; Bullier et al, 2001; Hupe et al, 1998, 2001a, 2001b); in an hierarchical cortical system in which representations become more abstract with increasing distance from the primary input, higher levels of the hierarchy specify top-down predictions through NMDA receptor signaling and any mismatches between expectancy and experience are conveyed upward through the hierarchy via rapid AMPA and GABA signaling (Friston, 2005a). There is some neurophysiological support for this contention (Self et al, 2008); however, it is unlikely that NMDA receptors only signal predictions and AMPA receptors signal prediction errors; rather, there may be a division of labor where AMPA receptors are relatively more engaged bottom-up and NMDA receptors are relatively more involved in top-down processes.
Considering the distribution of NMDA and AMPA receptors on the surface of neocortical neurons, the soma and proximal dendrite are highly sensitive to NMDA receptor manipulations, whereas more distal parts of the dendrite are more sensitive to manipulations of AMPA function (Dodt et al, 1998), implying that NMDA receptors near the soma might regulate the amplification of synaptic signals resulting from AMPA receptor activation on remote dendritic sites (Dodt et al, 1998). A similar division of labor is apparent in the connections between the striatum and frontal cortex, although with added complexity (Gittis et al, 2010).
Striatal GABAergic interneurons control the output of the striatum to frontal cortex via the direct and indirect pathways (which express D1 and D2 dopamine receptor subtypes, respectively (Shen et al, 2008)). The majority of control involves feedforward inhibition from local interneurons, which contain parvalbumin and are fast spiking (Tepper and Bolam, 2004). However, some inhibition is mediated by low-threshold spiking interneurons (Koos and Tepper, 1999). These two subtypes of interneurons have distinct signaling properties; fast spiking cells have large rectifying AMPA-mediated currents, but no detectable NMDA-mediated responses (Gittis et al, 2010). On the other hand, low-threshold spiking interneurons have currents indicative of a small AMPA current and an NMDA receptor-mediated component. The AMPA signaling FS interneurons preferentially target direct pathway medium spiny neurons over those in the indirect pathway (Gittis et al, 2010). This arrangement would amount to AMPA receptor stimulation driving and NMDA receptor signaling inhibiting driving outputs from the striatum to the prefrontal cortex. Furthermore, while NMDA receptor antagonism does not alter phasic dopamine release directly (Chesselet, 1984; Mount et al, 1989), top-down regulation of tonic extrasynaptic dopamine levels by glutamatergic afferents from the prefrontal cortices sets the tone on prediction error responses via NMDA receptors (Grace, 1991).
There are, of course, numerous other aspects to the glutamatergic synapse and its regulation; too numerous to treat them in all in detail here. We will briefly discuss some of those aspects with relevance to psychosis and the model under consideration. There are high steady-state glutamate levels in the extrasynaptic space, controlled by extrasynaptic metabotropic group II glutamate receptors (mglur2), which interact with an active exchange mechanism that shuttles cystine into and glutamate out of the glial cells that surround synapses (Baker et al, 2008). As mglur2s function as presynaptic autoreceptors (Baskys and Malenka, 1991), extrasynaptic glutamate negatively modulates the synaptic release of glutamate (Moran et al, 2005). Hence, the modulatory role of mglur receptors on feedforward driving inputs (Sherman and Guillery, 1998).
Another metabotropic receptor, subtype 5 (mglur5), has emerged as a close signaling partner with the NMDA receptor; the two physically interact via postsynaptic density scaffolding proteins homer and shank (Tu et al, 1999); dysfunctions in homer (Gauthier et al, 2010; Gilks et al, 2010), as well as numerous other components of the postsynaptic density (Hahn et al, 2006), have been associated with risk for schizophrenia. Activation of mglur5 receptors reverses the effects of NMDA antagonism on cortical function (Lecourtier et al, 2007). Crucially, mglur5-knockout mice display schizophrenia-like phenotypes (Gray et al, 2009) and mglur antagonists are psychotomimetic in human subjects (Friedmann et al, 1980), underlining the key role of decreased NMDA signaling in the generation of psychosis-like symptoms.
More broadly, these more detailed observations of the glutamatergic synapse underline the relevance of excess synaptic glutamate for the generation of psychotic symptoms (Moghaddam et al, 1997) and, within the context of our proposed translational model, how excess synaptic glutamate might arise either due to inappropriate feedforward signaling or an absence of the regulating processes that specify expected inputs.
Slower Neuromodulators—Precision, Uncertainty, and Learning
There is, of course, much more to cognition, perception, their dysfunction, and their underlying neurobiology than rapid neurotransmission; slower neuromoulators, such as dopamine, acetylcholine, serotonin, and noradrenaline, have a significant role (Greengard, 2001; Spitzer, 1995). Multiple neuromodulators, most notably dopamine, have been implicated in the pathophysiology of psychosis (Carlsson et al, 2001; Kapur, 2003). Where do slower neuromodulators fit into this Bayesian framework? An important distinction, within Bayesian hierarchical models of the brain, (Friston, 2005a, 2005b 2009, 2010) is between prediction errors per se and the precision or uncertainty about those errors. We (and others before us) propose that rapid glutamatergic and GABAergic neurotransmission represent prediction error and, depending on the neuroanatomical circuitry, slower neuromodulators encode the precision of prediction errors (Corlett et al, 2009a; Friston, 2005a); see Figure 2. Some theorists distinguish between these two neurochemical components as mediating inference and learning, respectively (Fiser et al, 2010; Friston, 2005a). While these processes are intimately connected, they are dissociable to come extent (see Figure 3). Inferences involve short-term decisions, such as unconscious perceptual inductions, in which one perceptual hypothesis is favored over another, for example, the Necker Cube (Barlow, 1990). On the other hand, learning represents the set of prior expectancies brought to bear on current processing (Fiser et al, 2010); the set of learned predictions that drives healthy individuals to perceive a hollow mask as a convex face (Emrich, 1989).
This distinction (between prediction error and uncertainty, inference, and learning) expands the role of phasic dopaminergic discharges beyond encoding reward prediction error, to include a more general role of dopamine in modulating or optimizing the precision of prediction errors that may or may not be reward related (ie, modulating the signal to noise response properties of neural units encoding prediction error). This notion fits with the proposed role of dopamine and glutamate interactions in controlling signal to noise ratio (Grace, 1991; Spitzer and Walter, 2003) and the numerous proposals that dopamine (at least in terms of its tonic discharge rates) encodes uncertainty or violation of expectations (Fiorillo et al, 2003; Preuschoff and Bossaerts, 2007). These models are also consistent with physiological recordings of prediction errors; for example, the slower dopaminergic response in cortex following a rapid glutamatergic surprise signal, perhaps involved in maintaining surprising events in working memory (Lavin et al, 2005), as well as updating of expectancies through changes in synaptic function (Schultz and Dickinson, 2000). Furthermore, frontostriatal prediction error signals guide the formation of sensory associations between nonrewarding stimuli (den Ouden et al, 2010, 2009).
Of course, the general model is a gross oversimplification, but it does provide a skeletal framework in which to consider learning, inference, perception, and, perhaps, consciousness in a manner conducive to generating and testing hypotheses. Below, we consider in more detail the relationship of this overarching view of the importance of prediction error and the symptoms of psychosis.
Prediction Error and Psychosis
The positive symptoms of psychosis involve gross misrepresentations of reality. In terms of the Bayesian model, delusions result when an individual experiences internally engendered aberrant prediction error coincident with what should be unsurprising and highly predictable events. Hallucinations involve excessively strong predictions within the hierarchical sensory cortices, conferring an apparent structure upon sensory noise such that the individual experiences a percept without sensory stimulus (Corlett et al, 2009a, 2009b). Passivity phenomena (experiencing one's own actions without intention and inferring that they are therefore under the control of an external agent) occupy a hinterland between hallucinations and delusions (Fletcher and Frith, 2009). Within the Bayesian model, they too involve a prediction error dysfunction (Corlett et al, 2009a; Fletcher and Frith, 2009; Stephan et al, 2009). In the absence of robust predictions about the sensory consequences of an intentional action, performance of the action feels surprising, and that surprise encourages an external agency attribution.
It is important to include in the model of psychotic symptoms a consideration of the characteristic prodromal phase of illness. During the early stages of psychosis, patients report changes in the intensity of their perceptual experience, such that background noises seem much louder and colors seem brighter (McGhie and Chapman, 1961; Bowers and Freedman, 1966; Freedman, 1974; Matussek, 1952). Further, they perceive inappropriate relatedness between external and internal stimuli and events (Schneider, 1930); (Freedman and Chapman, 1973; Freedman, 1974; Matussek, 1952). Within the framework of a model combining prior expectations with incoming sense data, these experiences can be conceived as arising from persistent and irreconcilable mismatch such that even mundane experiences are conferred a degree of salience (see Kapur, 2003) that renders them vivid, novel, and important.
In light of the above simplified account of the prepsychotic and psychotic state, we believe psychotomimetic drugs, and ketamine in particular, offer a potential resolution to this problem, allowing us to induce transient psychotic states by blocking NMDA receptors and enhancing AMPA receptor signaling (Corlett et al, 2007,2009a, 2009b). NMDA antagonists impair how current experience is constrained by learned expectation by blocking the top-down actions of NMDA receptors (Phillips and Silverstein, 2003), while, at the same time, encouraging aberrant prediction error responses to events that are merely coincident with enhanced AMPA receptor signaling (Jackson et al, 2004; Moghaddam et al, 1997). In this way, the world of an individual administered with NMDA receptor antagonists becomes highly unpredictable, different things seem important, and important things seem different, and, from this confusing state, delusions arise as explanatory schemes (Corlett et al, 2007).
Predictive Learning, Negative Symptoms, and Cognitive Control
Can prediction error also apply to negative symptoms? Negative symptoms involve deficits in motivated behavior and a lack of pleasure for previously enjoyed acts, events, and experiences (Andreasen, 1982). Recently, negative symptoms have been analyzed within the reinforcement learning framework (a special case of the Bayesian model, Dayan and Daw, 2008). Patients with negative symptoms appear to have problems predicting the ultimate consequences of a series of actions (Polgar et al, 2008) and using those predictions to guide goal-directed decision-making (Barch et al, 2003). This deficit may also involve working memory dysfunction; one of the cardinal cognitive dysfunctions associated with schizophrenia (Goldman-Rakic, 1994); that is, the short-term storage of salient information in a limited capacity storage system in service of behavior (Baddeley, 1981).
There is an important contextual aspect to working memory processes, such that behaviors, thoughts, and percepts are constrained to what is appropriate to the current situation (Barch et al, 2001; Braver et al, 1999). Working memory deficits also disturb contextual appropriateness (Barch et al, 2003) and lead to a reduction in behavioral engagement and output (Barch et al, 2003), as well as an inability to logically order thoughts and produce coherent and communicative speech (thought disorder, a positive symptom, although complete thought block and alogia are considered negative symptoms).
We contend that negative symptoms arise when no viable predictions can be produced or maintained, subtending a paucity of behavior, a disengagement, and withdrawal. Others have attempted to bring negative symptoms within the Bayesian framework (Stephan et al, 2009); these authors conceived of negative symptoms as a reaction to the unpredictability associated with positive symptoms and ‘impaired learning'. Our empirical work with ketamine (see above) contradicts this idea of negative symptoms as merely a reaction to positive symptoms; as subjects treated with ketamine experience a range of different responses and crucially, those who experience the most severe perceptual aberrations and delusion-like ideas are not the same subjects who experience the most severe negative symptoms (Honey et al, 2008).
Furthermore, social isolation (presumably related to negative symptoms in at least some patients) appears to incubate the generation of psychotic symptoms (Hoffman, 2007). Indeed, negative symptoms appear to precede the genesis of positive symptoms in some patients (Iyer et al, 2008) and to exist as endophenotypes in otherwise unaffected relatives of patients with schizophrenia (Laurent et al, 2000). Our account of negative symptoms as a paucity of motivated expectation (and not secondary to positive symptoms) would be entirely consistent with these observations.
Thus, we have considered a prediction error-based model of both the positive and negative symptoms of schizophrenia and shown how observations of neural responses and, critically, the individual differences therein, may help us to relate this theoretical framework more directly to the ketamine-induced psychosis. Clearly, a great deal of work remains and the existing literature is not without controversy (see Box 2).
EXTENDING THE MODEL IN TIME: LEARNING FROM THE EFFECTS OF CHRONIC AND REPEATED KETAMINE USE
The acute ketamine model is not without its critics (see Box 2). One important point is the surprising rapidity with which ketamine produces psychotomimetic effects (of the order of minutes; Bowdle et al, 1998). However, changes in synaptic function can happen that rapidly (Stephan et al, 2006). Ketamine effects also curtail rapidly. The drug is metabolized with a half-life between 10 and 15 min. Within 30 min, subjects administered a psychotomimetic dose of ketamine will have returned almost completely to normal (with a small minority experiencing residual effects for the next few hours). There is no evidence of adverse effects subsequent to a single acute ketamine challenge in healthy subjects, nor any significant sensitization of responses over multiple (up to 4) repeated ketamine administrations (Cho et al, 2005; Perry et al, 2007).
Despite a lack of sensitization across a small number of administrations, widely spaced in time and in the experimental setting, chronic ketamine abusers do show escalating cognitive changes and symptom experiences (Morgan et al, 2010). Admittedly their exposure involves more numerous and frequent ketamine administration than subjects in repeated ketamine studies (which will have been separated perhaps by years and most likely months). Recreational users usually administer ketamine intranasally, with an estimated duration of effect of up to 1 h (Siegel, 1978). In an evening, ketamine abusers will often self-administer several sequential doses of the drug in order to maintain psychotropic effects over time (Muetzelfeldt et al, 2008).
CHRONIC KETAMINE ABUSE
The recreational use of ketamine was first reported in 1970s in North America, (Petersen and Stillman, 1978). Renowned experimenters or ‘psychonauts' John C Lilly and Marcia Moore both published books on their experiences with ketamine in 1978 (Lilly, 1978; Moore and Altounian, 1978). The possibility for consciousness expansion and the exploration of new worlds provided by ketamine appealed to both of them; however, as ketamine use is continued, tolerance developed and they both spiraled toward larger and larger consumption (Jansen, 2004). Both Lilly and Moore perceived increasing numbers of coincidences or synchronicities that demanded explanation. Lilly conceived of a Coincidence Control Center and was increasingly preoccupied with communicating with cetaceans (themes to which he alludes in published academic work; Lilly, 1963). As each of them continues to use ketamine excessively, the degree of self-harm escalates; Lilly is interred in a state psychiatric hospital and suffers a near fatal drowning accident; Moore dies from exposure in a forest near her home (Jansen, 2004).
These instructive yet cautionary tales have not quelled enthusiasm for illicit ketamine use; from 1999 to 2003, prevalence of ketamine use among club goers in the UK rose from 25 to 40% (McCambridge et al, 2007). An increasing number of studies are examined cognition and phenomenology in chronic ketamine users (Curran and Monaghan, 2000, 2001; Freeman et al, 2009; Morgan et al, 2004b, 2004c, 2006, 2008, 2009, 2010). Ketamine users had high levels of dissociation and schizophrenia-like symptoms while on drug, and frequent users remained impaired when they were drug free (Curran and Monaghan, 2001). Although some cognitive processes remain relatively unaltered, for example, frequent ketamine users do not have severe occulomotor abnormalities redolent of schizophrenia (Morgan et al, 2009), chronic users do have semantic and episodic memory impairments that correlate with their degree of exposure (Morgan et al, 2010); moreover, relevant to the current thesis, chronic ketamine users show aberrant learning; they engage in predictive responding toward irrelevant stimuli, which is a superstitious responding style (note though that ketamine-naive subjects also show a considerable degree of superstition (Freeman et al, 2009)). Crucially, frequent ketamine use increases the severity of delusional ideation when subjects are followed up longitudinally for 1 year (Morgan et al, 2010). These beliefs remain, although attenuated, even in individuals who are abstinent (Morgan et al, 2010).
A further important question arises: what is the basis for these individual differences in acute and chronic effects upon which the Bayesian approach may capitalize?
FACTORS UNDERPINNING ACUTE AND CHRONIC KETAMINE EFFECTS
Genetic Effects
The idea that genetic variability between individuals might influence response to drugs was described and termed pharmacogenomics by Vogel (1959). Genetic variation confers differences in receptor function, cortical development, synapse formation, structure, or plasticity; this may underpin the individual differences we observed (Stephan et al, 2006). Although an association study between the acute effects of ketamine in healthy volunteers and genetic variability has yet to be published, there are a number of important leads.
Family history of alcoholism: Individuals with a family history of alcoholism show a blunted psychotomimetic response to ketamine, experiencing fewer dysphoric effects of ketamine, fewer positive symptoms, and milder negative symptoms compared with individuals without such a family history (Petrakis et al, 2004). As ketamine binds to NMDA receptors, it was assumed that this variability related to the function, structure, or regulation of NMDA receptor signaling in some way. Future studies will unveil the specific mechanisms of this effect.
Apolipoprotein E4: A ketamine challenge study in a group of remitted patients with schizophrenia (n=18) revealed a milder effect of ketamine in subjects who were carrying the epsilon 4 allele of apoliprotein E (n=7)—in particular, they had smaller ketamine-induced changes in unusual thought content (a measure of delusional ideation) compared with placebo (Malhotra et al, 1998). Patients with schizophrenia who carry this gene have less severe positive symptoms (Pickar et al, 1997). However, patients with Alzheimer's disease who carry this allele are more likely to suffer hallucinations and delusions (Zdanys et al, 2007). Apolipoprotein E4 is involved in lipid metabolism; however, data on its neural interactions are emerging, for example, ApoE4 modulates NMDA receptor function via effects on insulin-degrading enzymes and protein kinase A (Sheng et al, 2008), which is indicative of a potential role of cyclic AMP signaling in the psychotomimetic effects of ketamine. The relationship between the presence of the epsilon 4 allele and the severity of ketamine-induced psychosis in healthy volunteers has yet to be established empirically.
Personality
Initial studies into emergence phenomena following ketamine anesthestia explored personality as an explanatory factor (Biersner et al, 1977). These studies focused on the ‘big 5' personality measures with inconsistent results; high neuroticism, high psychotism, and low extraversion were related to ketamine-induced emergence phenomena; however, these studies were not placebo controlled and, given the initial use of ketamine in obstetric and gynecological anesthesia, female subjects predominated which may have biased the results (Jansen, 2004).
The lack of a significant association between schizotypal personality and ketamine responses (Krystal et al, 1994; PR Corlett, GD Honey, and PC Fletcher (unpublished observations)) suggests that schizotypy and acute ketamine-induced psychosis are mediated by different neurochemical mechanisms. However, such inferences should be made with caution; healthy volunteers who are invited to partake in ketamine research are carefully screened for a family history or personal history of mental illness (some groups even use high schizotypy scores as an exclusion criterion).
Although prolonged ketamine abuse may engender more convincing delusions than acute administration, this experimental model has not been examined prospectively (so far). That is, in the same way, we have found individual differences in behavioral and brain functions that predispose toward certain patterns of experience of acute ketamine; perhaps, there are predisposing factors related to chronic ketamine use. It is impossible to carry out a placebo-controlled study here. Indeed, it is possible that more schizotypal subjects (for example) are drawn to engage in repeated ketamine use. Exploring this idea would involve a prospective study with an extremely large initial population of subjects; some of whom would go on to abuse ketamine and in whom assessments had been made before the onset of ketamine use.
Future work should triangulate the relationship between ketamine-induced psychopathology, personality measures, and cortical neurochemistry. Other relevant personality measures ought to be explored, in particular suggestibility and absorption, which correlate with susceptibility to hypnosis (Braffman and Kirsch, 1999); a relatively recent model of psychosis that is beginning to be employed in delusion research (Barnier et al, 2008).
Other Candidate Mechanisms for Individual Variability in Ketamine Response
In addition to single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs), which are one marker for genetic variability, there are numerous biological mechanisms through which the observed variability in ketamine response may be mediated. For example, gene copy number repeat variation, which has been associated with schizophrenia (Walsh et al, 2008) and may be associated with responses to pharmacological interventions (Dhawan and Padh, 2009). In addition, alternative splicing provides a means through which variability can be introduced (Passetti et al, 2009), put simply, alternative splicing involves the formation of multiple alternative messenger RNAs (mRNAs) from the same gene (Gilbert, 1978), which can code for proteins of varying functionality (Black, 2000). Again alternative splicing has been implicated in the generation of interindividual differences in drug responses (Passetti et al, 2009). With respect to NMDA antagonist model psychoses, alternative splicing has been employed to alter the functionality of NMDA receptors in vitro (Rodriguez-Paz et al, 1995). In brief, NMDA receptors are assembled from a combination of subunits; in this study, the inclusion of an alternatively spliced insertion of 21 amino acids into the NMDAR1 subunit gives rise to receptors that were more sensitive to ketamine, an effect that was maintained when the alternatively spliced NR1 subunits were combined with NMDAR2B subunits into heteromeric receptors (Rodriguez-Paz et al, 1995). Furthermore, NMDAR2B-selective antagonists do not engender psychotomimetic effects in human subjects (Wang and Shuaib, 2005). The variability in effects of NMDA receptor antagonists on behavior (Gilmour et al, 2009) may be explicable in terms of their differing abilities to bind and activate NMDA receptors of varying subunit composition, as well as the variability in concentration of those different subunits in different brain regions (Wenzel et al, 1995).
Furthermore, variation in ketamine response in human subjects may be relatable to the subunit composition of their NMDA receptors, specifically the functionality of NR1 subunits affected by alternative splicing. However, the inbred Balb/c mouse strain that is more sensitive to NMDA receptor antagonists (Deutsch et al, 1997) does not have any significant alteration in mRNAs for NR1, NR2A, or NR2B subunits (Perera et al, 2008).
Another possible mechanism through which alternative splicing might influence ketamine responses is through interactions with microRNAs (miRNAs). miRNAs are noncoding RNAs that can block mRNA translation and affect mRNA stability and hence the function of ∼30% of protein-coding genes in the human genome (Rajewsky, 2006). miRNAs mediate their effects by targeting the 3′ untranslated region of mRNAs generated from the genes being regulated (Bartel, 2009). Variations in DNA sequence, such as SNPs, as well as variations in mRNA sequence generated by alternative splicing can effect an miRNAs ability to regulate its target (Georges et al, 2006). Hence variance in miRNA function could also be involved in the individual differences in ketamine response. More specifically, microRNA 219 (miR-219) is significantly reduced in rodent prefrontal cortex following NMDA antagonism and is involved in the expression of psychotomimetic effects of NMDA blockade (Kocerha et al, 2009). miR-219 has also been implicated in neuronal development, specifically myelination and the generation of oligodendrocytes (Dugas et al, 2010), as well as in the synaptic learning process long-term potentiation (Wibrand et al, 2010). Hence, variation in miR-219 function might confer susceptibility to the effects of NMDA antagonism, in particular those effects that involve aberrant neural learning.
There are multiple other potential sources of variability in the ketamine response; we highlight these mechanisms as potential future avenues of inquiry.
CONSILIENCE?
We aim to provide a consilient account of acute and chronic NMDA antagonist administration in terms of the Bayesian model we introduced.
To briefly rehearse, we and other have postulated that the brains, brain systems, and even single neurons function as Helmholtz machines (Dayan et al, 1995; Hinton and Dayan 1996); they try to minimize their uncertainty about subsequent inputs and stimulation by generating predictions, and responding to violations of their expectancy predictions are specified top-down via NMDA signaling, to lower layers of the hierarchy and lower layers signal prediction errors, bottom-up via AMPA and GABA receptor signaling. These errors are either ignored or accommodated; that is, fed forward through the hierarchy and update subsequent predictions with new learning (Dayan et al, 1995; Hinton and Dayan 1996).
In addition to the rapid glutamatergic (and GABAergic) signaling between layers in a cortical hierarchy, there are slower neuormodulatory effects, for example, dopamine and acetylcholine appear to code a confidence estimate or uncertainty in predictions and prediction errors (Preuschoff and Bossaerts, 2007; Yu and Dayan, 2002) in different hierarchies (Friston, 2005; Stephan et al, 2006, 2009). In the mesocortical dopamine system, dopamine and glutamate can be coreleased; dopamine levels rise over a slower timescale following a rapid glutamatergic prediction error signal, perhaps to ensure the maintenance of potentially explanatory information in working memory, perhaps promoting subsequent plasticity and new learning (Lavin et al, 2005). We and others make a distinction based on these two signaling modes, rapid and neuromodulatory, between inference and learning (Fiser et al, 2010; Friston, 2005a). These two processes complement one another, but, we posit, may be differentially affected by acute and chronic ketamine.
We posit that inference is glutamatergically mediated, and in the short term, briefly perturbs slower neuromodulatory function. However, with persistent changes in glutamatergic function, learning is engaged that alters the parameters of dopaminergic uncertainty coding and thus affects subsequent inferences (see Figures 3 and 4).
Acute ketamine then would impair the specification of top-down priors (by blocking NMDA receptors); furthermore, it would enhance presynaptic glutamate release (Jackson et al, 2004), engendering aberrant prediction error responses by stimulating AMPA receptors (Moghaddam et al, 1997; Jackson et al, 2004). These effects are primarily responsible for the psychotomimetic responses that ketamine engenders. Although acute ketamine may induce striatal dopamine release (Breier et al, 1998; Smith et al, 1998; Vollenweider et al, 2000), however, it does not always (Kegeles et al, 2000), and typical D2 dopamine blocking antipsychotic drugs do not alleviate the psychotomimetic effects of acute ketamine (Krystal et al, 2005). However, lamotrigine, which blocks presynaptic glutamate release, does reverse the psychotomimetic effects of ketamine (Anand et al, 2000).
Perhaps the psychosis-like experiences are transient because ketamine is rapidly metabolized by the liver and excreted. Homeostatic mechanisms involving glia retune glutamatergic synapses (Baker et al, 2008), and hence cognition and phenomenal experience return to normal.
Acute ketamine administration does increase cortical cholinergic function (Nelson et al, 2002), perhaps providing a mechanism for the perceptual aberrations and attentional capture that characterize its effects (Pomarol-Clotet et al, 2006). Furthermore, formal learning theories (Pearce and Hall, 1980) and preclinical behavioral neuroscience tell us that increasing cortical acetylcholine engages new explanatory learning and tunes attention to stimuli with unexpected and unpredictable consequences (Sarter et al, 2005). The engagement of cortical acetylcholine to aid uncertain perceptual inference may underpin the referential delusion-like ideas that a narrow, darting focus of attention happens upon under ketamine (Sarter et al, 2005).
These mechanistic differences may explain why acetylcholinersterase inhibitors are psychotogenic (Sarter et al, 2005). Furthermore, according to this model, acute ketamine should not engender hallucinations, because of its effects on cortical acetylcholine and the subsequent uncertainty of cortical inference—hallucinations would occur in the context of reduced cholinergic function when cortical inference was more certain; for example, following ingestion of anticholinergic drugs (Hall et al, 1977) or in disease states associated with reduced cholinergic activity (Collerton et al, 2005).
These primary effects on cortical inference (rather than more permanent learning effects) may explain the degree of insight that ketamine-treated subjects retain; they describe their experiences in relative terms and do not appear to experience longer-term effects of the drug once it has been metabolized (Pomarol-Clotet et al, 2006) (Corlett et al, 2010a; see Figure 3 and 4).
On the other hand, chronic ketamine administration involves dysfunction to the homeostatic regulation of glutamatergic synapses such that new learning takes place; both at the level of glutamate receptors, as well as the dopamine system; chronic NMDA blockade sensitizes the dopamine system (Jentsch and Roth, 1999). Intriguingly, longer-term ketamine administration does not sensitize cortical cholinergic function (Nelson et al, 2002), again, potentially explaining why chronic ketamine use is associated with delusions rather than hallucinations (Morgan et al, 2010); see Figures 3 and 4 and also see Box 1 for a brief discussion of the effects of binge use of amphetamine.
Continued frequent ketamine use is associated with delusions that worsen over time but not hallucinations (Morgan et al, 2010). Morgan et al speculate that delusion formation occurs during acute ketamine intoxication, but delusions crystallize into the drug-free state. We propose that the crystallization process involves interplay between sensitized dopamine function in the basal ganglia (especially the associative striatum (Kegeles et al, 2010)) and glutamatergic reinforcement of new explanatory priors (Friston, 2005a). Associative striatal dopamine sensitization may also explain the apparent increase in susceptibility to superstitious conditioning in chronic ketamine users (Freeman et al, 2009), as chaotic and hypersensitive dopamine signaling has been implicated in superstitious learning in animal models (King et al, 1984; Shaner, 1999).
Dopamine antagonism then, particularly with drugs that target D2 dopamine receptors, may ameliorate the psychopathology generated by chronic ketamine use, despite having no effect on the psychopathology induced by acute ketamine administration. A sensitized dopamine system may be prone to aberrant salience attribution (Kapur, 2003) either due to excess uncertainty (associated with bottom-up prediction error) that demands explanation (Pearce and Hall, 1980) or due to excessive top-down salience, based on inappropriately strong conditioned expectations (Mackintosh, 1975). Typical dopaminergic antipsychotics would modulate both of these effects and hence they ameliorate hallucinations and delusions (Hamamura and Harada, 2007).
One further characteristic of repeated ketamine use that we wish to consider is the commonality of experience across exposures; in a phenomenological analysis of repeated ketamine use (Newcombe, 2008), one psychonaut describes common themes and experiences across multiple ketamine administrations. We believe that the same mechanisms may explain the maintenance of delusional themes across episodes of psychosis (even when periods of remission separate episodes of psychosis by years (Sinha and Chaturvedi, 1989)) and perhaps a means through which ketamine experiences crystallize into delusions. In brief, the components of a synapse that confer its strength and excitability are frequently completely recycled (Arshavsky, 2006). One relatively stable and information-rich candidate for storage of memories across the lifespan is DNA. Changes in how the genome can be expressed could mediate long-term retention of information by an organism (Crick, 1984; Holliday, 1999). Much of the genome may be silenced in a particular cell, whereas other portions will be highly expressed; a process mediated, in part, through changes in proteins called histones that combine with DNA to form chromatin (Levenson et al, 2004). We posit that one effect of repeated ketamine use is chromatin remodeling such that prior ketamine experiences are repeated on subsequent use.
In addition to altering synaptic connection weights (and perhaps chromatin structure) and thus inducing aberrant learning and memory, we should, of course, highlight that chronic ketamine use may also alter brain structure more macroscopically, for example, by engendering excitotoxic cell death. Olney, Faber, and coworkers found evidence of cortical lesions in rodents following treatment with NMDA receptor antagonists (Farber et al, 1995; Olney et al, 1989; Olney et al, 1991); lesions that impinged upon circuits relevant to schizophrenia (Sharp et al, 2001). Whether these lesions occur in primates has yet to be determined, but we note that structural brain changes following ketamine are a possible mediator of the more marked and persistent delusions associated with chronic self-administration.
In the context of the Bayesian hierarchical model, we speculate that negative symptoms result from a failure to specify coherent expectations with sufficient certainty in order to justify motivated behaviors. Although withdrawal from social interaction may result from uncertainty and unpredictability to some extent (Stephan et al, 2006, 2009), we propose that negative symptoms, such as alogia, amotivation, and anhedonia, result when sufferers cannot predict the positive, hedonic qualities of a valued goal, nor can they maintain that prediction online, or use it to guide their behavior (Heerey and Gold, 2007). Again, we might expect particular genes to predispose individuals to particularly severe negative symptoms on ketamine, for example, the val158met polymorphism of catechol-o-methyltransferase is associated with inefficient frontal cortical responses during working memory tasks and a paucity of exploratory decision-making during reward learning (Frank et al, 2009). Chronic ketamine use further compounds inefficient frontal cortical function (Narendran et al, 2005) and is associated with decreased mood (Morgan et al, 2010). This is worth underlining given the recent move toward treating depression with NMDA antagonists and the suggestions that chronic administration may prolong the short-lived antidepressant effect of acute ketamine (Machado-Vieira et al, 2009).
CLINICAL UTILITY OF THE MODEL
NMDA antagonist model psychoses inspired the development of the first antipsychotic drug without a direct effect on dopamine function; an mglur2/3 agonist that would act presynaptically to regulate glutamatergic tone, decreasing excess release and the stimulation of non-NMDA glutamate receptors, and thus ameliorating the positive symptoms of psychosis (Patil et al, 2007). Other adjunct treatments have been trialed as a result of the model, for example, lamotrigine, which reverses the neural and phenomenological effects of ketamine (Anand et al, 2000; Deakin et al, 2008). Lamotrigine has modest ameliorative effects as an adjunct treatment in schizophrenia (Dursun and Deakin, 2001). Glycine and -serine, two compounds that act as cofactors to NMDA receptor transmission, increase NMDA receptor function and have clinical utility as adjunct treatments (Heresco-Levy et al, 2004; Heresco-Levy et al, 2005).
But does our Bayesian analysis suggest any novel treatment options? In the context of the model, delusional beliefs may be conceptualized as inappropriately reinforced prior expectations that are constantly updated with novel information or reconsolidated (Corlett et al, 2009b). One novel approach therefore involves engaging the prior belief and administering a drug that destabilizes it, preventing its reconsolidation (Rubin, 1976). Propanolol, a β-adrenergic receptor antagonist, has been used in this manner in preclinical models to disrupt fear memories (Debiec and Ledoux, 2004) and drug memories (Milton et al, 2008) and recently to attenuate learned fear responses in human subjects (Kindt et al, 2009). With respect to the NMDA antagonist models under consideration, propanolol attenuates the excessive glutamate release engendered by NMDA antagonists (Narimatsu et al, 2002). Propanolol has been used to treat psychosis with varying results (Atsmon et al, 1972; Manchanda and Hirsch, 1986); one reason for the inconsistency may involve variability in the degree to which the prior mediating the psychotic symptoms is engaged at the time of treatment (Lee et al, 2006).
FUTURE DIRECTIONS
The Bayesian conception of psychosis, which we have outlined, provides a framework for us to consider the co-occurrence of positive and negative symptoms, with obvious relevance for endogenous psychotic illnesses. Empirical examinations of the model will either support the model or disconfirm it. We feel that there is already sufficient consilience between glutamatergic model psychoses and endogenous psychotic illnesses such that they may provide a useful step in developing novel antipsychotic medications. Although ketamine-induced symptoms are not reversed by typical antipsychotic drugs (Krystal et al, 1999), atypical antipsychotics ameliorate them (Malhotra et al, 1997), as do drugs that modulate presynaptic glutamate release (Anand et al, 2000) and synaptic glutamate processing (Krystal et al, 2005). In the context of the framework we have outlined, it may be possible to engage in experimental medicine, targeting specific psychotic symptoms both theoretically and empirically, by identifying individuals who will respond to acute ketamine with a particular profile of symptoms and perhaps using those symptoms as a test bed for novel antipsychotic treatments. By focusing on the individual differences in ketamine effects via the Bayesian model of psychosis, we may have a framework within which to explore psychosis translationally. Indeed, genetic association studies of the cognitive and neural bases of the model may well assist our understanding of the biological mechanisms of psychosis. Furthermore, the individual differences in ketamine response that we describe, and their prediction by the engagement of specific neural circuitry and cognitive processes, portend the use of fMRI and cognitive science in clinical decision-making and perhaps targeted, personalized medicine.
CONCLUSION
NMDA receptor antagonists provide a useful model of psychosis following both acute and chronic administration. Acute administration permits the transient and reversible engagement of both positive and negative psychotic symptoms. With repeated chronic (self-) administration, delusions develop and persist even after the ketamine has been metabolized (Morgan et al, 2010).
We outlined a series of data that explored and exploited the individual differences in response to acute ketamine, emphasizing the utility of quantifying brain responses to theoretically relevant cognitive tasks and what they might tell us about the brain and behavioral bases of psychotic symptoms.
We interpreted these effects in terms of a hierarchical Bayesian model of cognition, comportment, and brain function. By comparing and contrasting the phenomenology and neurobiology of acute and chronic ketamine administration, we were able to explain, among other things, why ketamine administration does not engender hallucinations (either acutely or chronically), why the psychotomimetic effects of ketamine are not reversed by haloperidol, and why there is no strong association between schizotypy and the acute effects of ketamine, but at the same time, highly schizotypal individuals may be particularly susceptible to chronic ketamine use. Furthermore, we outline a potential explanation for negative symptoms in terms of aberrant predictions and ineffective maintenance of those predictions in service of goal-directed behavior. We find this simplifying framework a very powerful means to relate a number of disparate observations relevant to psychosis. This exercise has generated a number of testable predictions both in terms of the generation of psychosis and strategies that may be used to ameliorate it.
Acknowledgments
We thank Douglas Guarnieri and three anonymous reviewers for helpful comments and criticism.
Philip Corlett reports receiving research support from AstraZeneca Pharmaceuticals, LP, and consulting for Pfizer Pharmaceuticals. Garry Honey is employed by Pfizer Pharmaceuticals. John Krystal reports consulting for AstraZeneca Pharmaceuticals, LP, Cypress Bioscience, HoustonPharma, Schering-Plough Research Institute, Shire Pharmaceuticals, and Pfizer Pharmaceuticals; Advisory Boards: Bristol-Myers Squibb, Eli Lilly and Co., Forest Laboratories, GlaxoSmithKline, Lohocla Research Corporation, Merz Pharmaceuticals, Takeda Industries, and Transcept Pharmaceuticals; Exercisable Warrant Options: Tetragenex Pharmaceuticals; Research Support: Janssen Research Foundation; Pending Patents: glutamatergic agents for psychiatric disorders (depression, OCD), antidepressant effects of oral ketamine, and oral ketamine for depression. Paul Fletcher has received consultancy fees from GlaxoSmithKline and an honorarium for a lecture from Astra Zeneca.
References
- Abi-Saab WM, D′Souza DC, Moghaddam B, Krystal JH. The NMDA antagonist model for schizophrenia: promise and pitfalls. Pharmacopsychiatry. 1998;31 (Suppl 2:104–109. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-979354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Absalom AR, Lee M, Menon DK, Sharar SR, De Smet T, Halliday J, et al. Predictive performance of the Domino, Hijazi, and Clements models during low-dose target-controlled ketamine infusions in healthy volunteers. Br J Anaesth. 2007;98:615–623. doi: 10.1093/bja/aem063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Acquas E, Fibiger HC. Dopaminergic regulation of striatal acetylcholine release: the critical role of acetylcholinesterase inhibition. J Neurochem. 1998;70:1088–1093. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.1998.70031088.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aghajanian GK, Marek GJ. Serotonin induces excitatory postsynaptic potentials in apical dendrites of neocortical pyramidal cells. Neuropharmacology. 1997;36:589–599. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3908(97)00051-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aghajanian GK, Marek GJ. Serotonin model of schizophrenia: emerging role of glutamate mechanisms. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 2000;31:302–312. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(99)00046-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aghajanian GK. Modeling ‘psychosis' in vitro by inducing disordered neuronal network activity in cortical brain slices. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2009;206:575–585. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1484-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allen RM, Young SJ. Phencyclidine-induced psychosis. Am J Psychiatry. 1978;135:1081–1084. doi: 10.1176/ajp.135.9.1081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anand A, Charney DS, Oren DA, Berman RM, Hu XS, Cappiello A, et al. 2000Attenuation of the neuropsychiatric effects of ketamine with lamotrigine: support for hyperglutamatergic effects of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonists Arch Gen Psychiatry 57270–276.Blocking presynaptic glutamate release blocks the psychotomimetic effects of ketamine in healthy volunteers. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andreasen NC. Negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Definition and reliability. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1982;39:784–788. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1982.04290070020005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Angelucci A, Levitt JB, Lund JS. Anatomical origins of the classical receptive field and modulatory surround field of single neurons in macaque visual cortical area V1. Prog Brain Res. 2002a;136:373–388. doi: 10.1016/s0079-6123(02)36031-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Angelucci A, Levitt JB, Walton EJ, Hupe JM, Bullier J, Lund JS. Circuits for local and global signal integration in primary visual cortex. J Neurosci. 2002b;22:8633–8646. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-19-08633.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Angrist BM, Schweitzer JW, Gershon S, Friedhoff AJ. Mephentermine psychosis: misuse of the Wyamine inhaler. Am J Psychiatry. 1970;126:1315–1317. doi: 10.1176/ajp.126.9.1315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anis NA, Berry SC, Burton NR, Lodge D. The dissociative anaesthetics, ketamine and phencyclidine, selectively reduce excitation of central mammalian neurones by N-methyl-aspartate. Br J Pharmacol. 1983;79:565–575. doi: 10.1111/j.1476-5381.1983.tb11031.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arshavsky YI. ‘The seven sins' of the Hebbian synapse: can the hypothesis of synaptic plasticity explain long-term memory consolidation. Prog Neurobiol. 2006;80:99–113. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2006.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Atsmon A, Blum I, Steiner M, Latz A, Wijsenbeek H. Further studies with propranolol in psychotic patients. Relation to initial psychiatric state, urinary catecholamines and 3-methoxy-4-hydroxyphenylglycol excretion. Psychopharmacologia. 1972;27:249–254. doi: 10.1007/BF00422805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley A. The concept of working memory: a view of its current state and probable future development. Cognition. 1981;10:17–23. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(81)90020-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baker DA, Madayag A, Kristiansen LV, Meador-Woodruff JH, Haroutunian V, Raju I.2008Contribution of cystine-glutamate antiporters to the psychotomimetic effects of phencyclidine Neuropsychopharmacology 331760–1772.Underlines the importance of extrasynaptic glutamate in the effects of NMDA antagonists. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barch DM, Carter CS, Braver TS, Sabb FW, MacDonald A, III, Noll DC, et al. Selective deficits in prefrontal cortex function in medication-naive patients with schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58:280–288. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.3.280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barch DM, Carter CS, MacDonald AW, III, Braver TS, Cohen JD. Context-processing deficits in schizophrenia: diagnostic specificity, 4-week course, and relationships to clinical symptoms. J Abnorm Psychol. 2003;112:132–143. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barlow H. Conditions for versatile learning, Helmholtz's unconscious inference, and the task of perception. Vision Res. 1990;30:1561–1571. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(90)90144-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnier AJ, Cox RE, O'Connor A, Coltheart M, Langdon R, et al. Developing hypnotic analogues of clinical delusions: mirrored-self misidentification. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2008;13:406–430. doi: 10.1080/13546800802355666. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrera A, McKenna PJ, Berrios GE. Formal thought disorder in schizophrenia: an executive or a semantic deficit. Psychol Med. 2005;35:121–132. doi: 10.1017/s003329170400279x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bartel DP. MicroRNAs: target recognition and regulatory functions. Cell. 2009;136:215–233. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2009.01.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baskys A, Malenka RC. Agonists at metabotropic glutamate receptors presynaptically inhibit EPSCs in neonatal rat hippocampus. J Physiol. 1991;444:687–701. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1991.sp018901. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bayes T.1764An essay toward solving a problem in the doctrine of chances Philos Trans R Soc Lond 53370–418.The original description of this powerful theory. [Google Scholar]
- Berenbaum H, Kerns JG, Vernon LL, Gomez JJ. Cognitive correlates of schizophrenia signs and symptoms: I. Verbal communication disturbances. Psychiatry Res. 2008;159:147–156. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2007.08.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beringer K. Der Meskalinrausch. Springer: Berlin; 1927. [Google Scholar]
- Biersner RJ, Harris JA, Ryman DH. Emotional predisposition to psychotropic-drug effects. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1977;45:943–945. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.45.5.943. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Black DL. Protein diversity from alternative splicing: a challenge for bioinformatics and post-genome biology. Cell. 2000;103:367–370. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)00128-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blakemore SJ, Wolpert DM, Frith CD. Abnormalities in the awareness of action. Trends Cogn Sci. 2002;6:237–242. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(02)01907-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boileau I, Dagher A, Leyton M, Gunn RN, Baker GB, et al. Modeling sensitization to stimulants in humans: an [11C]raclopride/positron emission tomography study in healthy men. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63:1386–1395. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.12.1386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowdle TA, Radant AD, Cowley DS, Kharasch ED, Strassman RJ, Roy-Byrne PP. Psychedelic effects of ketamine in healthy volunteers: relationship to steady-state plasma concentrations. Anesthesiology. 1998;88:82–88. doi: 10.1097/00000542-199801000-00015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowers MB., Jr Pathogenesis of acute schizophrenic psychosis. An experimental approach. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1968;19:348–355. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1968.01740090092009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowers MB, Jr, Freedman DX. ‘Psychedelic' experiences in acute psychoses. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1966;15:240–248. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1966.01730150016003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Box GEP, Draper NR. Empirical Model-Building and Response Surfaces. Wiley: New York; 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Braffman W, Kirsch I. Imaginative suggestibility and hypnotizability: an empirical analysis. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1999;77:578–587. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.77.3.578. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braver TS, Barch DM, Cohen JD. Cognition and control in schizophrenia: a computational model of dopamine and prefrontal function. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;46:312–328. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00116-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Breier A, Adler CM, Weisenfeld N, Su TP, Elman I, et al. Effects of NMDA antagonism on striatal dopamine release in healthy subjects: application of a novel PET approach. Synapse. 1998;29:142–147. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2396(199806)29:2<142::AID-SYN5>3.0.CO;2-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Breier A, Malhotra AK, Pinals DA, Weisenfeld NI, Pickar D. Association of ketamine-induced psychosis with focal activation of the prefrontal cortex in healthy volunteers. Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154:805–811. doi: 10.1176/ajp.154.6.805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bullier J, Hupe JM, James AC, Girard P. The role of feedback connections in shaping the responses of visual cortical neurons. Prog Brain Res. 2001;134:193–204. doi: 10.1016/s0079-6123(01)34014-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cammann R. Is there no MMN in the visual modality. Behav Brain Sci. 1990;13:234. [Google Scholar]
- Carlsson A, Waters N, Holm-Waters S, Tedroff J, Nilsson M, Carlsson ML. Interactions between monoamines, glutamate, and GABA in schizophrenia: new evidence. Annu Rev Pharmacol Toxicol. 2001;41:237–260. doi: 10.1146/annurev.pharmtox.41.1.237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chapman J. The early symptoms of schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry. 1966;112:225–251. doi: 10.1192/bjp.112.484.225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chesselet MF. Presynaptic regulation of neurotransmitter release in the brain: facts and hypothesis. Neuroscience. 1984;12:347–375. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(84)90058-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cho HS, D'Souza DC, Gueorguieva R, Perry EB, Madonick S, et al. Absence of behavioral sensitization in healthy human subjects following repeated exposure to ketamine. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005;179:136–143. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-2066-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collerton D, Perry E, McKeith I.2005Why people see things that are not there: a novel perception and attention deficit model for recurrent complex visual hallucinations Behav Brain Sci 28737–757.; discussion 757-794. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corlett PR, Aitken MR, Dickinson A, Shanks DR, Honey GD, et al. Prediction error during retrospective revaluation of causal associations in humans: fMRI evidence in favor of an associative model of learning. Neuron. 2004;44:877–888. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.11.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corlett PR, D'Souza DC, Krystal JH. Capgras syndrome induced by ketamine in a healthy subject. Biol Psychiatry. 2010a;68:e1–e2. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.02.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corlett PR, Frith CD, Fletcher PC. From drugs to deprivation: a Bayesian framework for understanding models of psychosis. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2009a;206:515–530. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1561-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corlett PR, Honey GD, Aitken MR, Dickinson A, Shanks DR, et al. Frontal responses during learning predict vulnerability to the psychotogenic effects of ketamine: linking cognition, brain activity, and psychosis. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63:611–621. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.6.611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corlett PR, Honey GD, Fletcher PC. From prediction error to psychosis: ketamine as a pharmacological model of delusions. J Psychopharmacol. 2007;21:238–252. doi: 10.1177/0269881107077716. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corlett PR, Krystal JH, Taylor JR, Fletcher PC. Why do delusions persist. Front Hum Neurosci. 2009b;3:12. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.012.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corlett PR, Taylor JR, Wang XJ, Fletcher PC, Krystal JH.2010bToward a neurobiology of delusions Prog Neurobiol(E-pub ahead of print) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Corssen G, Miyasaka M, Domino EF. Changing concepts in pain control during surgery: dissociative anesthesia with CI-581. A progress report. Anesth Analg. 1968;47:746–759. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crick F. Memory and molecular turnover. Nature. 1984;312:101. doi: 10.1038/312101a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Curran HV, Monaghan L. In and out of the K-hole: a comparison of the acute and residual effects of ketamine in frequent and infrequent ketamine users. Addiction. 2001;96:749–760. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2001.96574910.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Curran HV, Morgan C. Cognitive, dissociative and psychotogenic effects of ketamine in recreational users on the night of drug use and 3 days later. Addiction. 2000;95:575–590. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2000.9545759.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- D'Ardenne K, McClure SM, Nystrom LE, Cohen JD. BOLD responses reflecting dopaminergic signals in the human ventral tegmental area. Science. 2008;319:1264–1267. doi: 10.1126/science.1150605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- D'Souza DC, Perry E, MacDougall L, Ammerman Y, Cooper T, et al. The psychotomimetic effects of intravenous delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol in healthy individuals: implications for psychosis. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:1558–1572. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davies P, Davies GL, Bennett S. An effective paradigm for conditioning visual perception in human subjects. Perception. 1982;11:663–669. doi: 10.1068/p110663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dayan P, Daw ND. Decision theory, reinforcement learning, and the brain. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2008;8:429–453. doi: 10.3758/CABN.8.4.429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dayan P, Hinton GE, Neal RM, Zemel RS. The Helmholtz machine. Neural Comput. 1995;7:889–904. doi: 10.1162/neco.1995.7.5.889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deakin JF, Lees J, McKie S, Hallak JE, Williams SR, Dursun SM. Glutamate and the neural basis of the subjective effects of ketamine: a pharmaco-magnetic resonance imaging study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65:154–164. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2007.37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Debiec J, Ledoux JE. Disruption of reconsolidation but not consolidation of auditory fear conditioning by noradrenergic blockade in the amygdala. Neuroscience. 2004;129:267–272. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.08.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- den Ouden HE, Daunizeau J, Roiser J, Friston KJ, Stephan KE.2010Striatal prediction error modulates cortical coupling J Neurosci 303210–3219.Striatal error signals forge non-reward related sensory associations by strengthening inter-regional connectivity. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- den Ouden HE, Friston KJ, Daw ND, McIntosh AR, Stephan KE. A dual role for prediction error in associative learning. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19:1175–1185. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn161. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deneve S. Bayesian spiking neurons I: inference. Neural Comput. 2008a;20:91–117. doi: 10.1162/neco.2008.20.1.91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deneve S. Bayesian spiking neurons II: learning. Neural Comput. 2008b;20:118–145. doi: 10.1162/neco.2008.20.1.118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deutsch SI, Rosse RB, Paul SM, Riggs RL, Mastropaolo J. Inbred mouse strains differ in sensitivity to ‘popping' behavior elicited by MK-801. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1997;57:315–317. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(96)00347-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dhawan D, Padh H. Pharmacogenetics: technologies to detect copy number variations. Curr Opin Mol Ther. 2009;11:670–680. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickinson A. The 28th Bartlett Memorial Lecture. Causal learning: an associative analysis. Q J Exp Psychol B. 2001;54:3–25. doi: 10.1080/02724990042000010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dodt HU, Frick A, Kampe K, Zieglgansberger W. NMDA and AMPA receptors on neocortical neurons are differentially distributed. Eur J Neurosci. 1998;10:3351–3357. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1998.00338.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Domino EF. This Week's Citation Classic, Domino EF, Chodoff P, and Corssen G: Pharmacologic effects of CI-581, a new dissociative anesthetic in man. Clin Pharmacol Ther6:279–291, 1965. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 1984;25:306. doi: 10.1002/cpt196563279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Domino EF, Chodoff P, Corssen G. Pharmacologic effects of Ci-581, a new dissociative anesthetic, in man. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 1965;6:279–291. doi: 10.1002/cpt196563279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Domino EF, Zsigmond EK, Domino LE, Domino KE, Kothary SP, Domino SE. Plasma levels of ketamine and two of its metabolites in surgical patients using a gas chromatographic mass fragmentographic assay. Anesth Analg. 1982;61:87–92. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dugas JC, Cuellar TL, Scholze A, Ason B, Ibrahim A, et al. Dicer1 and miR-219 are required for normal oligodendrocyte differentiation and myelination. Neuron. 2010;65:597–611. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dursun SM, Deakin JF. Augmenting antipsychotic treatment with lamotrigine or topiramate in patients with treatment-resistant schizophrenia: a naturalistic case-series outcome study. J Psychopharmacol. 2001;15:297–301. doi: 10.1177/026988110101500409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Emrich HM. A three-component-system hypothesis of psychosis. Impairment of binocular depth inversion as an indicator of a functional dysequilibrium. Br J Psychiatry (Suppl 5) 1989. pp. 37–9. [PubMed]
- Emrich HM, Weber MM, Wendl A, Zihl J, von Meyer L, Hanisch W. Reduced binocular depth inversion as an indicator of cannabis-induced censorship impairment. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1991;40:689–690. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(91)90383-d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farber NB, Wozniak DF, Price MT, Labruyere J, Huss J, et al. Age-specific neurotoxicity in the rat associated with NMDA receptor blockade: potential relevance to schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 1995;38:788–796. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(95)00046-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fillmore MT, Rush CR, Abroms BD. d-Amphetamine-induced enhancement of inhibitory mechanisms involved in visual search. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2005;13:200–208. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.13.3.200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fiorillo CD. Towards a general theory of neural computation based on prediction by single neurons. PLoS ONE. 2008;3:e3298. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fiorillo CD, Tobler PN, Schultz W.2003Discrete coding of reward probability and uncertainty by dopamine neurons Science 2991898–1902.In addition to prediction errors, midbrain dopamine neurons code uncertainty. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fiser J, Berkes P, Orban G, Lengyel M. Statistically optimal perception and learning: from behavior to neural representations. Trends Cogn Sci. 2010;14:119–130. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.01.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fletcher PC, Anderson JM, Shanks DR, Honey R, Carpenter TA, et al. Responses of human frontal cortex to surprising events are predicted by formal associative learning theory. Nat Neurosci. 2001;4:1043–1048. doi: 10.1038/nn733. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fletcher PC, Frith CD. Perceiving is believing: a Bayesian approach to explaining the positive symptoms of schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10:48–58. doi: 10.1038/nrn2536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frank MJ, Doll BB, Oas-Terpstra J, Moreno F. Prefrontal and striatal dopaminergic genes predict individual differences in exploration and exploitation. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12:1062–1068. doi: 10.1038/nn.2342. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freedman B, Chapman LJ. Early subjective experience in schizophrenic episodes. J Abnorm Psychol. 1973;82:46–54. doi: 10.1037/h0034952. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freedman BJ. The subjective experience of perceptual and cognitive disturbances in schizophrenia. A review of autobiographical accounts. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1974;30:333–340. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1974.01760090047008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freeman TP, Morgan CJ, Klaassen E, Das RK, Stefanovic A, et al. Superstitious conditioning as a model of delusion formation following chronic but not acute ketamine in humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2009;206:563–573. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1564-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friedmann CTH, Davis LJ, Ciccone PE, Rubin RT. Phase-II double-blind controlled-study of a new anxiolytic, fenobam (McN-3377) vs placebo. Curr Ther Res Clin E. 1980;27:144–151. [Google Scholar]
- Friston K.2005aA theory of cortical responses Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 360815–836.A Bayesian brain theory that explains cortical function in terms of preditions and prediction errors. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston K. The free-energy principle: a rough guide to the brain. Trends Cogn Sci. 2009;13:293–301. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston K. The free-energy principle: a unified brain theory. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11:127–138. doi: 10.1038/nrn2787. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston KJ.2005bHallucinations and perceptual inherence Behav Brain Sci 28764–766.An explanation of hallucinations in terms of Bayesian brain theory. [Google Scholar]
- Frith CD, Blakemore S, Wolpert DM. Explaining the symptoms of schizophrenia: abnormalities in the awareness of action. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 2000a;31:357–363. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(99)00052-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frith CD, Blakemore SJ, Wolpert DM. Abnormalities in the awareness and control of action. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2000b;355:1771–1788. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2000.0734. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fujiwara M, Egashira N. New perspectives in the studies on endocannabinoid and cannabis: abnormal behaviors associate with CB1 cannabinoid receptor and development of therapeutic application. J Pharmacol Sci. 2004;96:362–366. doi: 10.1254/jphs.fmj04003x2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gauthier J, Champagne N, Lafreniere RG, Xiong L, Spiegelman D, et al. De novo mutations in the gene encoding the synaptic scaffolding protein SHANK3 in patients ascertained for schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2010;107:7863–7868. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0906232107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Georges M, Clop A, Marcq F, Takeda H, Pirottin D, et al. Polymorphic microRNA-target interactions: a novel source of phenotypic variation. Cold Spring Harb Symp Quant Biol. 2006;71:343–350. doi: 10.1101/sqb.2006.71.056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geyer MA, Vollenweider FX. Serotonin research: contributions to understanding psychoses. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 2008;29:445–453. doi: 10.1016/j.tips.2008.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ghoneim MM, Hinrichs JV, Mewaldt SP, Petersen RC. Ketamine: behavioral effects of subanesthetic doses. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 1985;5:70–77. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilbert W. Why genes in pieces. Nature. 1978;271:501. doi: 10.1038/271501a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilks WP, Allott EH, Donohoe G, Cummings E, Gill M, et al. Replicated genetic evidence supports a role for HOMER2 in schizophrenia. Neurosci Lett. 2010;468:229–233. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilmour G, Pioli EY, Dix SL, Smith JW, Conway MW, et al. Diverse and often opposite behavioural effects of NMDA receptor antagonists in rats: implications for ‘NMDA antagonist modelling' of schizophrenia. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2009;205:203–216. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1530-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gittis AH, Nelson AB, Thwin MT, Palop JJ, Kreitzer AC. Distinct roles of GABAergic interneurons in the regulation of striatal output pathways. J Neurosci. 2010;30:2223–2234. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4870-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldman-Rakic PS. Working memory dysfunction in schizophrenia. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1994;6:348–357. doi: 10.1176/jnp.6.4.348. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grace AA.1991Phasic vs tonic dopamine release and the modulation of dopamine system responsivity: a hypothesis for the etiology of schizophrenia Neuroscience 411–24.A classic description of the dopamine system and its potential disruption in schizophrenia. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray JR, Chabris CF, Braver TS. Neural mechanisms of general fluid intelligence. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6:316–322. doi: 10.1038/nn1014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray L, van den Buuse M, Scarr E, Dean B, Hannan AJ. Clozapine reverses schizophrenia-related behaviours in the metabotropic glutamate receptor 5 knockout mouse: association with N-methyl-D-aspartic acid receptor up-regulation. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2009;12:45–60. doi: 10.1017/S1461145708009085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greengard P. The neurobiology of slow synaptic transmission. Science. 2001;294:1024–1030. doi: 10.1126/science.294.5544.1024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gregory RL. Knowledge in perception and illusion. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1997;352:1121–1127. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1997.0095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gross G, Huber G. [Sensory disorders in schizophrenia] Arch Psychiatr Nervenkr. 1972;216:119–130. doi: 10.1007/BF00346414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hahn CG, Wang HY, Cho DS, Talbot K, Gur RE, et al. Altered neuregulin 1-erbB4 signaling contributes to NMDA receptor hypofunction in schizophrenia. Nat Med. 2006;12:824–828. doi: 10.1038/nm1418. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hall RC, Popkin MK, McHenry LE. Angel's Trumpet psychosis: a central nervous system anticholinergic syndrome. Am J Psychiatry. 1977;134:312–314. doi: 10.1176/ajp.134.3.312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamamura T, Harada T. Unique pharmacological profile of aripiprazole as the phasic component buster. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2007;191:741–743. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0654-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heerey EA, Gold JM. Patients with schizophrenia demonstrate dissociation between affective experience and motivated behaviour. J Abnorm Psychol. 2007;116 2:268–278. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.2.268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Helmholtz H, Hermann Von1878/1971The facts of perceptionK Russell (ed).Selected Writings of Hermann von Helmholtz Wesleyan University Press: Middletown CT [Google Scholar]
- Hemsley DR, Garety PA.1986The formation of maintenance of delusions: a Bayesian analysis Br J Psychiatry 14951–56.The first Bayesian analysis of delusions. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henson R. What has (neuro)psychology told us about the mind (so far)? A reply to Coltheart (2006) Cortex. 2006;42:387–392. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70365-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heresco-Levy U, Ermilov M, Lichtenberg P, Bar G, Javitt DC. High-dose glycine added to olanzapine and risperidone for the treatment of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55:165–171. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(03)00707-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heresco-Levy U, Javitt DC, Ebstein R, Vass A, Lichtenberg P, et al. D-serine efficacy as add-on pharmacotherapy to risperidone and olanzapine for treatment-refractory schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57:577–585. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hinton GE, Dayan P. Varieties of Helmholtz machine. Neural Netw. 1996;9:1385–1403. doi: 10.1016/s0893-6080(96)00009-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoeks JC, Stowe LA, Doedens G. Seeing words in context: the interaction of lexical and sentence level information during reading. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2004;19:59–73. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2003.10.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoffman RE. A social deafferentation hypothesis for induction of active schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2007;33:1066–1070. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbm079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holliday R. Is there an epigenetic component in long-term memory. J Theor Biol. 1999;200:339–341. doi: 10.1006/jtbi.1999.0995. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Honey GD, Corlett PR, Absalom AR, Lee M, Pomarol-Clotet E, et al. 2008Individual differences in psychotic effects of ketamine are predicted by brain function measured under placebo J Neurosci 286295–6303.Across multiple tasks, the magnitude of brain responses in specific citcuits predicts the severity of symptoms suffered ona high dose of ketamine. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Honey GD, Honey RA, O'Loughlin C, Sharar SR, Kumaran D, et al. Ketamine disrupts frontal and hippocampal contribution to encoding and retrieval of episodic memory: an fMRI study. Cereb Cortex. 2005a;15:749–759. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Honey GD, Honey RA, Sharar SR, Turner DC, Pomarol-Clotet E, et al. Impairment of specific episodic memory processes by sub-psychotic doses of ketamine: the effects of levels of processing at encoding and of the subsequent retrieval task. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005b;181:445–457. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0001-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Honey GD, O'Loughlin C, Turner DC, Pomarol-Clotet E, Corlett PR, Fletcher PC. The effects of a subpsychotic dose of ketamine on recognition and source memory for agency: implications for pharmacological modelling of core symptoms of schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005c;31:413–423. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300846. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Honey GD, Sharma T, Suckling J, Giampietro V, Soni W, et al. The functional neuroanatomy of schizophrenic subsyndromes. Psychol Med. 2003;33:1007–1018. doi: 10.1017/s0033291703007864. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Honey RA, Honey GD, O'Loughlin C, Sharar SR, Kumaran D, et al. Acute ketamine administration alters the brain responses to executive demands in a verbal working memory task: an FMRI study. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:1203–1214. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300438. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hupe JM, James AC, Girard P, Bullier J. Response modulations by static texture surround in area V1 of the macaque monkey do not depend on feedback connections from V2. J Neurophysiol. 2001a;85:146–163. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.85.1.146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hupe JM, James AC, Girard P, Lomber SG, Payne BR, Bullier J. Feedback connections act on the early part of the responses in monkey visual cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2001b;85:134–145. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.85.1.134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hupe JM, James AC, Payne BR, Lomber SG, Girard P, Bullier J. Cortical feedback improves discrimination between figure and background by V1, V2 and V3 neurons. Nature. 1998;394:784–787. doi: 10.1038/29537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huxley A. Heaven and Hell. Harper and Row: New York; 1956. [Google Scholar]
- Iyer SN, Boekestyn L, Cassidy CM, King S, Joober R, Malla AK. Signs and symptoms in the pre-psychotic phase: description and implications for diagnostic trajectories. Psychol Med. 2008;38:1147–1156. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708003152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jackson ME, Homayoun H, Moghaddam B. NMDA receptor hypofunction produces concomitant firing rate potentiation and burst activity reduction in the prefrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2004;101:8467–8472. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0308455101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacobs D, Silverstone T. Dextroamphetamine-induced arousal in human subjects as a model for mania. Psychol Med. 1986;16:323–329. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700009132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jansen K. Ketamine: Dreams & Realities. Multidisciplinary Association for Psychedlic Studies: Sarasota, FL; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Javitt DC, Zukin SR. Recent advances in the phencyclidine model of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 1991;148:1301–1308. doi: 10.1176/ajp.148.10.1301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jentsch JD, Roth RH. The neuropsychopharmacology of phencyclidine: from NMDA receptor hypofunction to the dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1999;20:201–225. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(98)00060-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kamin LJ.1969Predictability, surprise, attention and conditioningIn: R Church, B Campbell (eds).Punishment and aversive behavior 279–296.New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts; Demonstrating that surprise is key to learning, no surprise, no learning. [Google Scholar]
- Kapur S. Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: a framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:13–23. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.1.13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keefe RS, Arnold MC, Bayen UJ, Harvey PD. Source monitoring deficits in patients with schizophrenia; a multinomial modelling analysis. Psychol Med. 1999;29:903–914. doi: 10.1017/s0033291799008673. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kegeles LS, Abi-Dargham A, Frankle WG, Gil R, Cooper TB, et al. Increased synaptic dopamine function in associative regions of the striatum in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67:231–239. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kegeles LS, Abi-Dargham A, Zea-Ponce Y, Rodenhiser-Hill J, Mann JJ, et al. Modulation of amphetamine-induced striatal dopamine release by ketamine in humans: implications for schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2000;48:627–640. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)00976-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kegeles LS, Zea-Ponce Y, Abi-Dargham A, Rodenhiser J, Wang T, et al. Stability of [123I]IBZM SPECT measurement of amphetamine-induced striatal dopamine release in humans. Synapse. 1999;31:302–308. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2396(19990315)31:4<302::AID-SYN9>3.0.CO;2-A. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kimura M, Widmann A, Schroger E. Human visual system automatically represents large-scale sequential regularities. Brain Res. 2010;1317:165–179. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.12.076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kindt M, Soeter M, Vervliet B. Beyond extinction: erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12:256–258. doi: 10.1038/nn.2271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- King R, Barchas JD, Huberman BA. Chaotic behavior in dopamine neurodynamics. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1984;81:1244–1247. doi: 10.1073/pnas.81.4.1244. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knauer A, Maloney WJ. A preliminary note on the psychic action of mescalin, with special reference to the mechanims of visual hallucinations. J Nerv Ment Disord. 1913;40:425–436. [Google Scholar]
- Kocerha J, Faghihi MA, Lopez-Toledano MA, Huang J, Ramsey AJ, et al. MicroRNA-219 modulates NMDA receptor-mediated neurobehavioral dysfunction. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2009;106:3507–3512. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0805854106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koethe D, Gerth CW, Neatby MA, Haensel A, Thies M, et al. Disturbances of visual information processing in early states of psychosis and experimental delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol altered states of consciousness. Schizophr Res. 2006;88:142–150. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.07.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koos T, Tepper JM. Inhibitory control of neostriatal projection neurons by GABAergic interneurons. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:467–472. doi: 10.1038/8138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kosslyn SM, Cacioppo JT, Davidson RJ, Hugdahl K, Lovallo WR, et al. Bridging psychology and biology. The analysis of individuals in groups. Am Psychol. 2002;57:341–351. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kruger-Thiemer E. Continuous intravenous infusion and multicompartment accumulation. Eur J Pharmacol. 1968;4:317–324. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(68)90100-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krystal JH, Abi-Saab W, Perry E, D'Souza DC, Liu N, et al. Preliminary evidence of attenuation of the disruptive effects of the NMDA glutamate receptor antagonist, ketamine, on working memory by pretreatment with the group II metabotropic glutamate receptor agonist, LY354740, in healthy human subjects. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005;179:303–309. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-1982-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krystal JH, Bennett A, Abi-Saab D, Belger A, Karper LP, et al. Dissociation of ketamine effects on rule acquisition and rule implementation: possible relevance to NMDA receptor contributions to executive cognitive functions. Biol Psychiatry. 2000;47:137–143. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00097-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krystal JH, D'Souza DC, Karper LP, Bennett A, Abi-Dargham A, et al. Interactive effects of subanesthetic ketamine and haloperidol in healthy humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1999;145:193–204. doi: 10.1007/s002130051049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krystal JH, D'Souza DC, Mathalon D, Perry E, Belger A, Hoffman R. NMDA receptor antagonist effects, cortical glutamatergic function, and schizophrenia: toward a paradigm shift in medication development. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2003;169:215–233. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1582-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krystal JH, Karper LP, Bennett A, D'Souza DC, Abi-Dargham A, et al. Interactive effects of subanesthetic ketamine and subhypnotic lorazepam in humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1998;135:213–229. doi: 10.1007/s002130050503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krystal JH, Karper LP, Seibyl JP, Freeman GK, Delaney R, et al. 1994Subanesthetic effects of the noncompetitive NMDA antagonist, ketamine, in humans Psychotomimetic, perceptual, cognitive, and neuroendocrine responses Arch Gen Psychiatry 51199–214.The first detailed exploration of the effects of ketamine on cognition and experience in human subjects. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lambe EK, Aghajanian GK. Hallucinogen-induced UP states in the brain slice of rat prefrontal cortex: role of glutamate spillover and NR2B-NMDA receptors. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:1682–1689. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300944. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Langlitz N. Ceci n'est pas une psychose. Toward a historical epistemology of model psychoses. Biosocieties. 2006;1:159–180. [Google Scholar]
- Laruelle M, Abi-Dargham A, van Dyck CH, Rosenblatt W, Zea-Ponce Y, et al. SPECT imaging of striatal dopamine release after amphetamine challenge. J Nucl Med. 1995;36:1182–1190. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laruelle M, Kegeles LS, Abi-Dargham A. Glutamate, dopamine, and schizophrenia: from pathophysiology to treatment. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2003;1003:138–158. doi: 10.1196/annals.1300.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laurent A, Biloa-Tang M, Bougerol T, Duly D, Anchisi AM, et al. Executive/attentional performance and measures of schizotypy in patients with schizophrenia and in their nonpsychotic first-degree relatives. Schizophr Res. 2000;46:269–283. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(99)00232-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lavin A, Nogueira L, Lapish CC, Wightman RM, Phillips PE, Seamans JK. Mesocortical dopamine neurons operate in distinct temporal domains using multimodal signaling. J Neurosci. 2005;25:5013–5023. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0557-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lecourtier L, Homayoun H, Tamagnan G, Moghaddam B. Positive allosteric modulation of metabotropic glutamate 5 (mGlu5) receptors reverses N-Methyl-D-aspartate antagonist-induced alteration of neuronal firing in prefrontal cortex. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62:739–746. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.12.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee JL, Milton AL, Everitt BJ. Reconsolidation and extinction of conditioned fear: inhibition and potentiation. J Neurosci. 2006;26:10051–10056. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2466-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levenson JM, O'Riordan KJ, Brown KD, Trinh MA, Molfese DL, Sweatt JD. Regulation of histone acetylation during memory formation in the hippocampus. J Biol Chem. 2004;279:40545–40559. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M402229200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lilly JC. Productive and creative research with man and dolphin. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1963;8:111–116. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1963.01720080001001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lilly JC. The Scientist: A Metaphysical Autobiography. Ronin Publishing: Berkeley, CA; 1978. [Google Scholar]
- Lu W, Chen H, Xue CJ, Wolf ME. Repeated amphetamine administration alters the expression of mRNA for AMPA receptor subunits in rat nucleus accumbens and prefrontal cortex. Synapse. 1997;26:269–280. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2396(199707)26:3<269::AID-SYN8>3.0.CO;2-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lu W, Wolf ME. Repeated amphetamine administration alters AMPA receptor subunit expression in rat nucleus accumbens and medial prefrontal cortex. Synapse. 1999;32:119–131. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2396(199905)32:2<119::AID-SYN5>3.0.CO;2-F. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luby ED, Cohen BD, Rosenbaum G, Gottlieb JS, Kelley R. Study of a new schizophrenomimetic drug; sernyl. AMA Arch Neurol Psychiatry. 1959;81:363–369. doi: 10.1001/archneurpsyc.1959.02340150095011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Machado-Vieira R, Manji HK, Zarate CA. The role of the tripartite glutamatergic synapse in the pathophysiology and therapeutics of mood disorders. Neuroscientist. 2009;15:525–539. doi: 10.1177/1073858409336093. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mackintosh NJ. A theory of attention: variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement. Psychological Review. 1975;82:276–298. [Google Scholar]
- Malhotra AK, Adler CM, Kennison SD, Elman I, Pickar D, Breier A. Clozapine blunts N-methyl-D-aspartate antagonist-induced psychosis: a study with ketamine. Biol Psychiatry. 1997;42:664–668. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(96)00546-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malhotra AK, Breier A, Goldman D, Picken L, Pickar D. The apolipoprotein E epsilon 4 allele is associated with blunting of ketamine-induced psychosis in schizophrenia A preliminary report. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1998;19:445–448. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(98)00031-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malhotra AK, Pinals DA, Weingartner H, Sirocco K, Missar CD, et al. NMDA receptor function and human cognition: the effects of ketamine in healthy volunteers. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1996;14:301–307. doi: 10.1016/0893-133X(95)00137-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manchanda R, Hirsch SR. Does propranolol have an antipsychotic effect? A placebo-controlled study in acute schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry. 1986;148:701–707. doi: 10.1192/bjp.148.6.701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marek GJ, Wright RA, Schoepp DD, Monn JA, Aghajanian GK. Physiological antagonism between 5-hydroxytryptamine(2A) and group II metabotropic glutamate receptors in prefrontal cortex. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2000;292:76–87. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matussek P.1952Studies in delusional perceptionIn: J Cutting, M Shepherd (eds).The clinical roots of the schizophrenia concept. Transaltions of seminal European contributions on schizophrenia 89–103.Cambridge University Press: Cambridge [Google Scholar]
- McCambridge J, Winstock A, Hunt N, Mitcheson L. 5-Year trends in use of hallucinogens and other adjunct drugs among UK dance drug users. Eur Addict Res. 2007;13:57–64. doi: 10.1159/000095816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McClure SM, Berns GS, Montague PR. Temporal prediction errors in a passive learning task activate human striatum. Neuron. 2003;38:339–346. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00154-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGhie A, Chapman J.1961Disorders of attention and perception in early schizophrenia Br J Med Psychol 34103–116.A fascinating account of the effects of schizophrenia on perception and attention. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mesulam M. Representation, inference, and transcendent encoding in neurocognitive networks of the human brain. Ann Neurol. 2008;64:367–378. doi: 10.1002/ana.21534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer JS, Greifenstein F, Devault M. A new drug causing the symptoms of sensory deprivation. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1959;129:54–61. [Google Scholar]
- Milton AL, Lee JL, Everitt BJ. Reconsolidation of appetitive memories for both natural. Learn Mem. 2008;15:88–92. doi: 10.1101/lm.825008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moghaddam B, Adams B, Verma A, Daly D.1997Activation of glutamatergic neurotransmission by ketamine: a novel step in the pathway from NMDA receptor blockade to dopaminergic and cognitive disruptions associated with the prefrontal cortex J Neurosci 172921–2927.NMDA receptor antagonists increase presynaptic glutamate release. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Montague PR, Dayan P, Sejnowski TJ. A framework for mesencephalic dopamine systems based on predictive Hebbian learning. J Neurosci. 1996;16:1936–1947. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-05-01936.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore M, Altounian H. Journeys into the Bright World. Para research: Massachussetts; 1978. [Google Scholar]
- Moran MM, McFarland K, Melendez RI, Kalivas PW, Seamans JK. Cystine/glutamate exchange regulates metabotropic glutamate receptor presynaptic inhibition of excitatory transmission and vulnerability to cocaine seeking. J Neurosci. 2005;25:6389–6393. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1007-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moreau JJ. Du Haschisch et l'alienation mentale. Raven Press: New York; 1845/1973. [Google Scholar]
- Morgan CJ, Curran HV. Acute and chronic effects of ketamine upon human memory: a review. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2006;188:408–424. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0572-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morgan CJ, Huddy V, Lipton M, Curran HV, Joyce EM. Is persistent ketamine use a valid model of the cognitive and oculomotor deficits in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;65:1099–1102. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.10.045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morgan CJ, Mofeez A, Brandner B, Bromley L, Curran HV. Acute effects of ketamine on memory systems and psychotic symptoms in healthy volunteers. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004a;29:208–218. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morgan CJ, Monaghan L, Curran HV. Beyond the K-hole: a 3-year longitudinal investigation of the cognitive and subjective effects of ketamine in recreational users who have substantially reduced their use of the drug. Addiction. 2004b;99:1450–1461. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2004.00879.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morgan CJ, Muetzelfeldt L, Curran HV.2010Consequences of chronic ketamine self-administration upon neurocognitive function and psychological wellbeing: a 1-year longitudinal study Addiction 105121–133.Docments the effects of repeated ketamine abuse. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morgan CJ, Rees H, Curran HV. Attentional bias to incentive stimuli in frequent ketamine users. Psychol Med. 2008;38:1331–1340. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707002450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morgan CJ, Riccelli M, Maitland CH, Curran HV. Long-term effects of ketamine: evidence for a persisting impairment of source memory in recreational users. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2004c;75:301–308. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2004.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morgan CJ, Rossell SL, Pepper F, Smart J, Blackburn J, et al. Semantic priming after ketamine acutely in healthy volunteers and following chronic self-administration in substance users. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;59:265–272. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.06.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mount H, Welner S, Quirion R, Boksa P. Glutamate stimulation of [3H]dopamine release from dissociated cell cultures of rat ventral mesencephalon. J Neurochem. 1989;52:1300–1310. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.1989.tb01879.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muetzelfeldt L, Kamboj SK, Rees H, Taylor J, Morgan CJ, Curran HV. Journey through the K-hole: phenomenological aspects of ketamine use. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2008;95:219–229. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.01.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mumford D. On the computational architecture of the neocortex. II. The role of cortico-cortical loops. Biol Cybern. 1992;66:241–251. doi: 10.1007/BF00198477. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Naatanen R, Gaillard AW, Mantysalo S. Early selective-attention effect on evoked potential reinterpreted. Acta Psychol (Amst) 1978;42:313–329. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(78)90006-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Narendran R, Frankle WG, Keefe R, Gil R, Martinez D, et al. Altered prefrontal dopaminergic function in chronic recreational ketamine users. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162:2352–2359. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.12.2352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Narimatsu E, Kawamata Y, Kawamata M, Fujimura N, Namiki A. NMDA receptor-mediated mechanism of ketamine-induced facilitation of glutamatergic excitatory synaptic transmission. Brain Res. 2002;953:272–275. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(02)03375-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelson CL, Burk JA, Bruno JP, Sarter M. Effects of acute and repeated systemic administration of ketamine on prefrontal acetylcholine release and sustained attention performance in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2002;161:168–179. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1004-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newcombe R. Ketamine case study: the phenomenology of a ketamine experience. Addict Res Theory. 2008;16:209–215. [Google Scholar]
- Newcomer JW, Farber NB, Jevtovic-Todorovic V, Selke G, Melson AK, et al. Ketamine-induced NMDA receptor hypofunction as a model of memory impairment and psychosis. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1999;20:106–118. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(98)00067-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nuechterlein KH, Edell WS, Norris M, Dawson ME. Attentional vulnerability indicators, thought disorder, and negative symptoms. Schizophr Bull. 1986;12:408–426. doi: 10.1093/schbul/12.3.408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Doherty JP, Dayan P, Friston K, Critchley H, Dolan RJ. Temporal difference models and reward-related learning in the human brain. Neuron. 2003;38:329–337. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00169-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oh TM, McCarthy RA, McKenna PJ. Is there a schizophasia? A study applying the single case approach to formal thought disorder in schizophrenia. Neurocase. 2002;8:233–244. doi: 10.1093/neucas/8.3.233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olney JW, Farber NB. Glutamate receptor dysfunction and schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;52:998–1007. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1995.03950240016004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olney JW, Labruyere J, Price MT. Pathological changes induced in cerebrocortical neurons by phencyclidine and related drugs. Science. 1989;244:1360–1362. doi: 10.1126/science.2660263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olney JW, Labruyere J, Wang G, Wozniak DF, Price MT, Sesma MA. NMDA antagonist neurotoxicity: mechanism and prevention. Science. 1991;254:1515–1518. doi: 10.1126/science.1835799. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Osmond H. A review of the clinical effects of psychotomimetic agents. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1957;66:418–434. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1957.tb40738.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oye I, Paulsen O, Maurset A. Effects of ketamine on sensory perception: evidence for a role of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1992;260:1209–1213. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Passetti F, Ferreira CG, Costa FF. The impact of microRNAs and alternative splicing in pharmacogenomics. Pharmacogenomics J. 2009;9:1–13. doi: 10.1038/tpj.2008.14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Passie T, Karst M, Borsutzky M, Wiese B, Emrich HM, Schneider U. Effects of different subanaesthetic doses of (S)-ketamine on psychopathology and binocular depth inversion in man. J Psychopharmacol. 2003;17:51–56. doi: 10.1177/0269881103017001698. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patil ST, Zhang L, Martenyi F, Lowe SL, Jackson KA, et al. 2007Activation of mGlu2/3 receptors as a new approach to treat schizophrenia: a randomized Phase 2 clinical trial Nat Med 131102–1107.A successful clinical trial in schizophrenia with a drug that acts upon metabotropic glutamate receptors. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pearce JM, Hall G. A model for Pavlovian learning: variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. Psychol Rev. 1980;87:532–552. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perera PY, Lichy JH, Mastropaolo J, Rosse RB, Deutsch SI. Expression of NR1, NR2A and NR2B NMDA receptor subunits is not altered in the genetically-inbred Balb/c mouse strain with heightened behavioral sensitivity to MK-801, a noncompetitive NMDA receptor antagonist. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2008;18:814–819. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2008.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perry EB, Jr, Cramer JA, Cho HS, Petrakis IL, Karper LP, et al. Psychiatric safety of ketamine in psychopharmacology research. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2007;192:253–260. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-0706-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petersen RC, Stillman RC. Phencyclidine: an overview. NIDA Res Monogr. 1978;21:1–17. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petrakis IL, Limoncelli D, Gueorguieva R, Jatlow P, Boutros NN, et al. Altered NMDA glutamate receptor antagonist response in individuals with a family vulnerability to alcoholism. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:1776–1782. doi: 10.1176/ajp.161.10.1776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips WA, Silverstein SM.2003Convergence of biological and psychological perspectives on cognitive coordination in schizophrenia Behav Brain Sci 2665–82.discussion—137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pickar D, Malhotra AK, Rooney W, Breier A, Goldman D. Apolipoprotein E epsilon 4 and clinical phenotype in schizophrenia. Lancet. 1997;350:930–931. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)63266-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pistis M, Ferraro L, Pira L, Flore G, Tanganelli S, et al. Delta(9)-tetrahydrocannabinol decreases extracellular GABA and increases extracellular glutamate and dopamine levels in the rat prefrontal cortex: an in vivo microdialysis study. Brain Res. 2002;948:155–158. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(02)03055-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poldrack RA.2006Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data Trends Cogn Sci 1059–63.An important consideration of what we can and can′t infer from functional neuroimaging data. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polgar P, Farkas M, Nagy O, Kelemen O, Rethelyi J, et al. How to find the way out from four rooms? The learning of ‘chaining' associations may shed light on the neuropsychology of the deficit syndrome of schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2008;99:200–207. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.06.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pomarol-Clotet E, Honey GD, Murray GK, Corlett PR, Absalom AR, et al. Psychological effects of ketamine in healthy volunteers. Phenomenological study. Br J Psychiatry. 2006;189:173–179. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.105.015263. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Preuschoff K, Bossaerts P. Adding prediction risk to the theory of reward learning. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2007;1104:135–146. doi: 10.1196/annals.1390.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rajewsky N. microRNA target predictions in animals. Nat Genet. 2006;38 (Suppl:S8–S13. doi: 10.1038/ng1798. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rao RP, Ballard DH. Predictive coding in the visual cortex: a functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:79–87. doi: 10.1038/4580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rasmussen K, Aghajanian GK. Effect of hallucinogens on spontaneous and sensory-evoked locus coeruleus unit activity in the rat: reversal by selective 5-HT2 antagonists. Brain Res. 1986;385:395–400. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(86)91090-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla RA, Wagner AR.1972A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and non-reinforcementIn: AH Black, WF Prokasy (eds).Classical Conditioning II: Current Research and Theory Appleton-Century-Crofts: New York. A classic in learning theory, emphasizes the role of prediction error in learning. [Google Scholar]
- Robinson TE, Kolb B. Persistent structural modifications in nucleus accumbens and prefrontal cortex neurons produced by previous experience with amphetamine. J Neurosci. 1997;17:8491–8497. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-21-08491.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson TE, Kolb B. Alterations in the morphology of dendrites and dendritic spines in the nucleus accumbens and prefrontal cortex following repeated treatment with amphetamine or cocaine. Eur J Neurosci. 1999;11:1598–1604. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00576.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodriguez-Paz JM, Anantharam V, Treistman SN. Block of the N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor by phencyclidine-like drugs is influenced by alternative splicing. Neurosci Lett. 1995;190:147–150. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(95)11524-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rubin RD. Clinical use of retrograde amnesia produced by electroconvulsive shock. A conditioning hypothesis. Can Psychiatr Assoc J. 1976;21:87–90. doi: 10.1177/070674377602100205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rypma B, D'Esposito M. The roles of prefrontal brain regions in components of working memory: effects of memory load and individual differences. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1999;96:6558–6563. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.11.6558. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rypma B, Prabhakaran V. When less is more and more is more: the mediating roles of speed in brain-behavior efficiency. Intelligence. 2010;37:207–222. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2008.12.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sarter M, Martinez V, Kozak R. A neurocognitive animal model dissociating between acute illness and remission periods of schizophrenia. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Sarter M, Nelson CL, Bruno JP. Cortical cholinergic transmission and cortical information processing in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2005;31:117–138. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbi006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider C. Die Psychologie der Schizophrenen. Germany Thieme: Leipzig; 1930. [Google Scholar]
- Schneider U, Leweke FM, Sternemann U, Weber MM, Emrich HM. Visual 3D illusion: a systems-theoretical approach to psychosis. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1996;246:256–260. doi: 10.1007/BF02190277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schnider A. Spontaneous confabulation, reality monitoring, and the limbic system--a review. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 2001;36:150–160. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(01)00090-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schultz W. Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. J Neurophysiol. 1998;80:1–27. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.80.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schultz W, Dickinson A.2000Neuronal coding of prediction errors Annu Rev Neurosci 23473–500.Discusses the importance of prediction errors for brain function and suggests that they are a basic mode of brain function. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Self MW, Super H, Roelfsema PR. Feedforward and feedback visual processing utilise different glutamate receptors. Soc Neurosci. 2008.
- Semple DM, Ramsden F, McIntosh AM. Reduced binocular depth inversion in regular cannabis users. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2003;75:789–793. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(03)00140-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaner A. Delusions, superstitious conditioning and chaotic dopamine neurodynamics. Med Hypotheses. 1999;52:119–123. doi: 10.1054/mehy.1997.0656. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shanks DR. Bayesian associative learning. Trends Cogn Sci. 2006;10:477–478. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sharp FR, Tomitaka M, Bernaudin M, Tomitaka S. Psychosis: pathological activation of limbic thalamocortical circuits by psychomimetics and schizophrenia. Trends Neurosci. 2001;24:330–334. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(00)01817-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shen WX, Flajolet M, Greengard P, Surmeier DJ. Dichotomous dopaminergic control of striatal synaptic plasticity. Science. 2008;321:848–851. doi: 10.1126/science.1160575. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheng Z, Prorok M, Brown BE, Castellino FJ. N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor inhibition by an apolipoprotein E-derived peptide relies on low-density lipoprotein receptor-associated protein. Neuropharmacology. 2008;55:204–214. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.05.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sherman SM, Guillery RW. On the actions that one nerve cell can have on another: distinguishing ‘drivers' from ‘modulators'. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1998;95:7121–7126. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.12.7121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siegel RK. Phencyclidine and ketamine intoxication: a study of four populations of recreational users. NIDA Res Monogr. 1978;21:272–288. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silver H, Feldman P. Evidence for sustained attention and working memory in schizophrenia sharing a common mechanism. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2005;17:391–398. doi: 10.1176/jnp.17.3.391. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sinha VK, Chaturvedi SK. Persistence of delusional content among psychotics over consecutive episodes. Psychopathology. 1989;22:208–212. doi: 10.1159/000284599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith GS, Schloesser R, Brodie JD, Dewey SL, Logan J, et al. Glutamate modulation of dopamine measured in vivo with positron emission tomography (PET) and 11C-raclopride in normal human subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1998;18:18–25. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(97)00092-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spitzer M. A neurocomputational approach to delusions. Compr Psychiatry. 1995;36:83–105. doi: 10.1016/s0010-440x(95)90103-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spitzer M, Walter H.2003The cognitive neuroscience of agency in schizophreniaIn: David A, Kircher T (eds).The Self in Neuroscience and Psychiatry 436–444.Cambridge University Press: Cambridge [Google Scholar]
- Stephan KE, Baldeweg T, Friston KJ. Synaptic plasticity and dysconnection in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;59:929–939. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stephan KE, Friston KJ, Frith CD. Dysconnection in schizophrenia: from abnormal synaptic plasticity to failures of self-monitoring. Schizophr Bull. 2009;35:509–527. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stone JM, Erlandsson K, Arstad E, Squassante L, Teneggi V, et al. Relationship between ketamine-induced psychotic symptoms and NMDA receptor occupancy: a [(123)I]CNS-1261 SPET study. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2008;197:401–408. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-1047-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sussman E, Ritter W, Vaughan HG., Jr Predictability of stimulus deviance and the mismatch negativity. Neuroreport. 1998;9:4167–4170. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199812210-00031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sutton RS, Barto AG. Toward a modern theory of adaptive networks: expectation and prediction. Psychol Rev. 1981;88:135–170. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tepper JM, Bolam JP. Functional diversity and specificity of neostriatal interneurons. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2004;14:685–692. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2004.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsai G, Coyle JT. Glutamatergic mechanisms in schizophrenia. Annu Rev Pharmacol Toxicol. 2002;42:165–179. doi: 10.1146/annurev.pharmtox.42.082701.160735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tu JC, Xiao B, Naisbitt S, Yuan JP, Petralia RS, et al. Coupling of mGluR/Homer and PSD-95 complexes by the Shank family of postsynaptic density proteins. Neuron. 1999;23:583–592. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80810-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turner DC, Aitken MR, Shanks DR, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW, et al. The role of the lateral frontal cortex in causal associative learning: exploring preventative and super-learning. Cereb Cortex. 2004;14:872–880. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Umbricht D, Koller R, Vollenweider FX, Schmid L. Mismatch negativity predicts psychotic experiences induced by NMDA receptor antagonist in healthy volunteers. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;51:400–406. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01242-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Underwood BJ. Individual differences as a crucible in theory construction. Am Psychol. 1975;30:128–134. [Google Scholar]
- Vogel F. Moderne probleme der human-genetik. Ergebnisse Inneren Medizin und Keinderheilkunde. 1959;12:52–125. [Google Scholar]
- Vollenweider FX, Leenders KL, Oye I, Hell D, Angst J. Differential psychopathology and patterns of cerebral glucose utilisation produced by (S)- and (R)-ketamine in healthy volunteers using positron emission tomography (PET) Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 1997a;7:25–38. doi: 10.1016/s0924-977x(96)00042-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vollenweider FX, Leenders KL, Scharfetter C, Antonini A, Maguire P, et al. Metabolic hyperfrontality and psychopathology in the ketamine model of psychosis using positron emission tomography (PET) and [18F]fluorodeoxyglucose (FDG) Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 1997b;7:9–24. doi: 10.1016/s0924-977x(96)00039-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vollenweider FX, Vontobel P, Oye I, Hell D, Leenders KL. Effects of (S)-ketamine on striatal dopamine: a [11C]raclopride PET study of a model psychosis in humans. J Psychiatr Res. 2000;34:35–43. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3956(99)00031-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waelti P, Dickinson A, Schultz W.2001Dopamine responses comply with basic assumptions of formal learning theory Nature 41243–48.An elegant demonstration that midbrain dopamine neurons code a prediction error signal that drives learning. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waldmann MR, Martignon L. Proceedings of the Twentieth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Earlbaum: Mahwah, NJ; 1998. A Bayesian network model of causal learning; pp. 1102–1107. [Google Scholar]
- Walsh T, McClellan JM, McCarthy SE, Addington AM, et al. Rare structural variants disrupt multiple genes in neurodevelopmental pathways in schizophrenia. Science. 2008;320:539–543. doi: 10.1126/science.1155174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang CX, Shuaib A. NMDA/NR2B selective antagonists in the treatment of ischemic brain injury. Curr Drug Targets CNS Neurol Disord. 2005;4:143–151. doi: 10.2174/1568007053544183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wenzel A, Scheurer L, Kunzi R, Fritschy JM, Mohler H, Benke D. Distribution of NMDA receptor subunit proteins NR2A, 2B, 2C and 2D in rat brain. Neuroreport. 1995;7:45–48. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wibrand K, Panja D, Tiron A, Ofte ML, Skaftnesmo KO, et al. Differential regulation of mature and precursor microRNA expression by NMDA and metabotropic glutamate receptor activation during LTP in the adult dentate gyrus in vivo. Eur J Neurosci. 2010;31:636–645. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07112.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yu AJ, Dayan P. Acetylcholine in cortical inference. Neural Netw. 2002;15:719–730. doi: 10.1016/s0893-6080(02)00058-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zdanys KF, Kleiman TG, MacAvoy MG, Black BT, Rightmer TE, et al. Apolipoprotein E epsilon4 allele increases risk for psychotic symptoms in Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;32:171–179. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]