Abstract
We examined the developmental processes involved in peer problems among children (M age = 10.41 years) previously diagnosed with ADHD at study entry (N = 536) and a comparison group (N = 284). Participants were followed over a 6-year period ranging from middle childhood to adolescence. At four assessment periods, measures of aggression, social skills, positive illusory biases (in the social and behavioral domains), and peer rejection were assessed. Results indicated that children from the ADHD group exhibited difficulties in each of these areas at the first assessment. Moreover, there were vicious cycles among problems over time. For example, peer rejection was related to impaired social skills, which in turn predicted later peer rejection. Problems also tended to “spill over” into other areas, which in turn compromised functioning in additional areas across development, leading to cascading effects over time. The findings held even when controlling for age and were similar for males and females, the ADHD and comparison groups, and among ADHD treatment groups. The results suggest that the peer problems among children with and without ADHD may reflect similar processes; however, children with ADHD exhibit greater difficulties negotiating important developmental tasks. Implications for interventions are discussed.
Peer rejection is pervasive among children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), characterizing 52 to 82 percent of children with the disorder (Hoza, Mrug et al., 2005; Pelham & Bender, 1982), depending on the definition of rejection and the sample employed. Of great concern, peer rejection develops quickly in new social groups (Erhardt & Hinshaw, 1994), and, once established, is highly resistant to change. Even state-of-the-art intervention strategies that are successful at improving functioning in other areas have not been successful at normalizing the peer functioning of children with ADHD (Hoza, Gerdes et al., 2005). These difficulties characterize children with ADHD of both sexes and begin at an early age (e.g., age 7; Hoza, Mrug et al., 2005), with the strong potential of depriving these children of important developmental opportunities with peers.
Given the severe and persistent peer problems evident among children with ADHD, an important goal is to identify the processes that exacerbate or ameliorate ADHD children’s risk for poor relationships with peers. To address this question, we adopted a developmental psychopathology approach (Cicchetti & Cohen, 2006; Cicchetti & Rogosch, 2002; Sroufe, 1997), and applied it to a large existing sample that includes both children previously diagnosed with combined type ADHD, and a comparison group. A central tenet of this approach is that consideration of normal developmental processes is essential in understanding the emergence of disorder, associated impairments, and accumulating comorbidities over the life course; in effect, maladaptive functioning or disorder results from a failure to successfully negotiate developmentally-appropriate tasks (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 2002; Sroufe & Rutter, 1984). One implication of this approach is that comparisons to normative samples are important for understanding the processes involved in the emergence of peer problems among children with ADHD. A second implication is that the peer problems among children with ADHD may be the result of unsuccessful attempts to navigate developmental challenges. A third implication is that successful intervention may prevent the onset of accumulating difficulties.
In their evaluation of psychosocial developmental tasks, Masten and Coatsworth (1998) identified broad areas of competence and the developmental progression of adaptation typical in these areas. For example, one important area of functioning is engaging in socially appropriate conduct. During early childhood, children must develop strategies for self-control and learn to comply with the requests of others. By middle childhood, they are expected to follow societal rules regarding moral and prosocial behavior, including inhibiting aggressive and delinquent behaviors and engaging in positive behaviors such as helping others. Finally, during adolescence, they are expected to internalize rules regarding socially appropriate conduct and to engage in such behaviors in the absence of direct supervision (Masten & Coatsworth, 1998). Importantly, evidence suggests that children with ADHD fail to exhibit this typical developmental progression and struggle in these areas of functioning across developmental periods. For example, they display elevated levels of aggressive and delinquent behaviors (Hoza, Owens, & Pelham, 1999) and perform less skillfully with peers (Hinshaw, 2002), suggesting that they may be relatively unsuccessful in negotiating these important developmental tasks.
Another important developmental task is the formation of a cohesive sense of self (Masten & Coatsworth, 1998). One aspect of this process is developing an accurate view of one’s own abilities. In fact, during early childhood, children tend to overestimate their competencies in a variety of domains (Harter & Pike, 1984). By early adolescence, typical children are fairly accurate in their reports of their abilities (Harter, 1982; Marsh et al., 1998). However, many children with ADHD continue to exhibit inflated self-perceptions in areas such as social and behavioral competence (relative to actual competence as rated by teachers and parents) into early adolescence in that they approach normative self-perceptions while having actual competence below the norm (e.g., Hoza et al., in press). This suggests that these children have failed to negotiate this important developmental task (for additional details regarding developmental changes in biases across adolescence in this sample, see Hoza et al, in press).
Finally, a third salient developmental task that gains even greater prominence during middle childhood and into adolescence is the formation of friendships and getting along with peers (Berndt, 1996; Bukowski & Kramer, 1986; Hartup, 1992; Masten & Coatsworth, 1998). A number of studies have demonstrated that children with ADHD have lower social preference and fewer friendships (Hoza, Mrug et al., 2005) than do agemates. Even when friendships are formed, these are less stable and of poorer quality than those of typically developing peers (Blachman & Hinshaw, 2002).
Although the peer problems of children with ADHD may simply reflect a failure to establish successful relationships with peers, it is possible that these difficulties also result from failures to negotiate other important developmental tasks across the lifespan. The hypothesis that compromised functioning in one area has implications for competence in other areas is not new. Moreover, investigators have recently adopted a “developmental cascades” analytical approach to demonstrate how failures in one area of functioning (e.g., social, academic, behavioral) can “spill over” into other areas across development (Burt et al., 2008; Masten et al., 2005). A developmental cascades analysis involves testing the cross-lagged associations among multiple areas of functioning over time. Importantly, these analyses control for the stability of each area of functioning across time as well as within-time correlations among variables. As such, this approach controls for the fact that functioning in one area is often correlated with functioning in other areas (by controlling for within-time correlations) and thus provides a strong empirical test of longitudinal associations among areas of functioning across development. For example, Masten and colleagues (2005) demonstrated that externalizing problems at age 10 were associated with lower academic competence at age 17 and that academic failures at age 20 predicted internalizing problems at age 30. These findings highlight how failures in one area can “spill over” into functioning in other areas across development, and also offer important insights about potential points for effective intervention.
A developmental cascades approach may provide insight regarding why peer problems are so pervasive, severe, and persistent among children with ADHD. For example, problems with inhibiting aggression, developing appropriate social skills, and developing accurate self-perceptions may all have implications for a child’s status in the peer group. In fact, a number of studies with normative samples have demonstrated that aggressive behavior is associated with peer problems such as rejection (Coie & Dodge, 1998; Ostrov & Crick, 2007; Vaillancourt & Hymel, 2006). Aggression may be associated with rejection for a number of reasons. First, children are likely to find aggression by peers objectionable, leading aggressive children to be increasingly disliked over time. Consistent with this perspective, researchers have demonstrated that aggressive behaviors predict later peer rejection (e.g., Hay et al., 2004; Little & Garber, 1995; Ostrov & Crick, 2007; Pope & Bierman, 1999). One longitudinal study indicated that childhood aggression in a sample of ADHD youth was inversely related to self-reported social acceptance in adolescence five years later (Bagwell, Molina, Pelham, & Hoza, 2001). However, rejection also may lead to aggression over time. For example, some children might respond to dislike and rejection by others with aggressive conduct. In fact, evidence suggests that rejection is associated with later aggression (Dodge et al., 2003; Hay et al., 2004; Ialongo, Vaden-Kiernan, & Kellam, 1998; Kupersmidt, Burchinal, & Patterson, 1995; Pettit, Clawson, Dodge, & Bates, 1996). Importantly, although some of these studies control for initial levels of the outcome variable when exploring longitudinal associations (e.g., Dodge et al., 2003; Ialongo et al., 1998), others do not (e.g., Ostrov & Crick, 2007; Pope & Bierman, 1999). Nevertheless, taken together, these studies suggest that a vicious cycle seems likely: Aggressive conduct may evoke high levels of peer rejection. In turn, rejection by peers may lead to even greater levels of aggression.
Failures to develop appropriate social skills may place children with ADHD at additional risk for rejection by peers. Social skills are defined as behaviors and abilities that allow children to succeed on social tasks, and include perspective-taking abilities, the tendency to initiate and maintain positive social exchanges with others, and engaging in socially positive and prosocial behaviors (see Rubin et al., 2006). A number of studies have demonstrated that poor social skills are associated with peer rejection (e.g., Harrist et al., 1997; Newcomb et al., 1993) whereas positive social skills such as friendly and cooperative behavior are associated with peer popularity (Newcomb et al., 1993; Rubin et al., 1998; Warden & Mackinnon, 2003). In one study of children with ADHD, behavioral frequencies of helping peers and following activity rules predicted small but significant positive changes in peer sociometric variables (such as acceptance and rejection) over the course of a summer treatment program (Mrug, Hoza, Pelham, Gnagy, & Greiner, 2007). The association between impaired social skills and peer rejection makes theoretical sense. Socially skilled children will have advantages in forming positive social relationships with their peers. In contrast, children with poor social skills will likely fail in their attempts at eliciting positive regard from their peers and, in their disappointment or in desperate attempts to elicit positive peer regard, may do things (such as enviously attacking more successful peers or unwarranted bragging) that lead to rejection by peers. Consistent with this view, one common component of interventions for rejected children is social skills training (see Bierman, 2004).
It is also possible that peer rejection impairs the development of appropriate social skills, leading to a vicious cycle between functioning in these areas. In fact, developmental psychologists have long emphasized the importance of social interaction in the development of social skills (Hartup & Sicilio, 1996; Newcomb & Bagwell, 1995). Peer interaction may be essential for learning social norms, developing social-cognitive abilities, and practicing social skills (see Harrist et al., 1997). In fact, evidence suggests that close emotional ties to peers are associated with perspective taking skills (e.g., Hughes & Dunn, 1998; Maguire & Dunn, 1997), and longitudinal work indicates that peer rejection in childhood is associated with lower sociability in adulthood, although these findings were no longer significant when controlling for childhood social prominence (Bagwell et al., 1998). Overall, these findings suggest that rejected children may have fewer opportunities to interact with peers and thus learn and practice social skills, leading to impaired social skills and further rejection over time.
Finally, inflated self-perceptions regarding competence might place children at risk for rejection by peers via their influence on peer-based behaviors. Children who report high levels of competence relative to external criteria such as peer or teacher reports are considered to have overly positive self-perceptions (e.g., Diamantoppoulou et al., 2008; Hoza et al., 2004; Hoza et al. 2002). A number of researchers have found that children with these positively biased self-perceptions in areas such as social and behavioral competence are more likely than their peers to engage in aggressive behaviors (Diamantopoulou et al., 2008; Hoza et al., in press; Hymel et al., 1993; Rudolph & Clark, 2001; Zakriski & Coie, 1996), although these studies tend to be cross-sectional in design (for an exception, see Hoza et al., in press). Baumeister and colleagues (1996) argued that individuals with inflated self-perceptions may choose to behave aggressively against peers who challenge their positive self-views. In addition, Hoza and colleagues (2009) have proposed that accurate self-perceptions are essential in learning appropriate behaviors in the peer group. For example, children with behavioral problems and poor social skills who nonetheless maintain positive self-perceptions in these domains will likely be unmotivated to alter their behavior, leading to sustained problems across development. Thus, we expected that overly positive self-perceptions in the social and behavioral domains would be associated with the development of negative peer-based behaviors (i.e., poor social skills, increased aggression).
Previous research has generally explored domain-specific positively biased self-perceptions, such as biases in the social and behavioral domains (e.g., Hoza et al., in press). This approach has been adopted in part because self researchers have argued that self-views often vary across competence domains (Harter, 1999). Moreover, recent research with this sample has provided evidence of distinct trajectories of change in positive biases in the social and behavioral domains across development (Hoza et al., in press), further bolstering the case that these biases should be considered separately. It is possible, for example, that biases in the social and behavioral domains will have different effects on peer-based behavior. In fact, given Hoza et al.’s (2009) proposal that positively biased self-perceptions interfere with learning appropriate behaviors in the peer group, we expected that biases in the social domain would be more strongly related to poor social skills whereas biases in the behavioral domain would be more strongly related to aggressive conduct. For example, children with inflated views of their social competence should be especially unlikely to work to improve their social skills whereas children with inflated views of their behavioral competence should be especially unlikely to work to reduce their involvement in disruptive behaviors such as aggression. Finally, we expected that these negative behaviors would be associated with rejection by peers, suggesting an indirect role of positively biased self-perceptions in peer problems among children with ADHD. In fact, recent work (Kaiser, Hoza, Pelham, Gnagy, & Greiner, 2008) demonstrated that overly positive self-perceptions mediated the relation between ADHD status and frequency of socially problematic behaviors in a summer program setting.
Although overly positive self-perceptions may place children at risk for aggression and poor social skills, it is possible that negative peer-based behaviors also may lead to the development of positively biased self-perceptions. In fact, researchers have proposed that inflated self-perceptions may serve a self-protective function for some individuals (e.g., Baumeister et al., 1996). From this perspective, individuals with low competence may learn to cope with these failures by rejecting others’ evaluations of them and insisting that they are in fact successful in these domains. Thus, children with poor social skills and high levels of aggressive conduct may develop positively biased self-perceptions in an effort to bolster their self-views.
Overall, these findings suggest that peer rejection may result from vicious cycles and cascading effects due to failures to successfully negotiate important developmental tasks. Specifically, failures to learn socially appropriate behavior (e.g., inhibiting aggression and developing social skills) may place children at risk for rejection by peers. In addition, children who are unsuccessful in developing accurate self-views may be at risk for rejection via changes in their peer-based behavior (e.g., increased aggression). Importantly, reciprocal effects are likely to occur, with rejection by peers leading to aggressive conduct and low social skills. Vicious cycles are also expected, in which failures in one area lead to problems in other areas, which in turn exacerbate problems in the original area of functioning. Given the documented difficulties negotiating each of these developmental tasks among children with ADHD, the relatively high levels of peer rejection among these children may in part reflect cascading effects from failures in other areas of functioning.
Hypotheses
This study tested the hypothesis that peer rejection among children with ADHD results from failures to successfully negotiate important developmental tasks, including inhibiting aggression, learning appropriate social skills, and developing accurate self-views. Data to test these predictions were drawn from a longitudinal sample that included both children with ADHD and a comparison group. Assessments of peer rejection, aggression, social skills, and positively biased self-perceptions were available at four assessment periods across a 6-year time span. We conducted a series of four nested model comparisons to test study hypotheses. Each successive model included all paths estimated in the previous model. A separate set of models were conducted for positive illusions in the social and behavioral domains, respectively, since self-perceptions are thought to be domain-specific (Harter, 1985, 1988), and to make findings comparable to previous research (e.g., Hoza et al., 2002, 2004).
In Model 1, we tested whether ADHD status (initial assignment to either the ADHD or comparison group) predicted peer rejection, aggressive behavior, poor social skills, and positively biased self-perceptions at the first assessment period given evidence that ADHD is associated with impairments in each of these areas of functioning. Consistent with previous cascade analyses (e.g., Burt et al., 2008; Masten et al., 2005), we calculated stability in each area of functioning and within-time correlations across areas. We expected moderate to high stability of study variables across time, and we expected that, within each time point, functioning in each area would be significantly correlated with functioning in the other three areas. Model 2 estimated the cross-lagged paths between rejection, aggression, and social skills given strong evidence suggesting reciprocal associations among these three variables. We expected that both aggression and impaired social skills would be associated with increases in rejection over time. In addition, we expected that peer rejection would lead to increases in aggression and decreases in social skills. Finally, although not central to our study hypotheses, we expected that poor social skills and aggression would exhibit reciprocal effects across development, given evidence that aggressive children lack social skills such as the ability to generate prosocial goals (e.g., Crick & Dodge, 1994).
In Model 3, we estimated the bidirectional association between positively biased self-perceptions (in the social and behavioral domains) and both aggression and social skills across the first time lag (from Time 1 to Time 2). We focused on this first time lag to examine whether the effects of self-perceptions on behavior are especially likely during early adolescence, when a focus on others’ views of the self becomes especially salient (see Harter, 1999). We expected that biases in the social domain would be more strongly related to social skills whereas biases in the behavioral domain would be more strongly related to aggression. In Model 4, we estimated cross-lagged paths between positively biased self-perceptions and both aggression and social skills across the remaining two time lags (Time 2 to Time 3 and Time 3 to Time 4). This final model was exploratory, as we did not have specific predictions about whether biased self-perceptions would be associated with aggression and social skills during later adolescence. Finally, we conducted additional analyses to test whether our findings remained when controlling for age and whether significant paths differed by sex, ADHD status (ADHD vs. comparison group), or by previous experiences of treatment among the ADHD sample.
Method
Participants
A total of 820 8–13 year-old children (M = 10.41 years, SD = .93) from the Multimodal Treatment Study of Children with ADHD (MTA) participated in this study. Of these 820 participants, 536 were diagnosed previously with ADHD. Specifically, two years prior to assessments for the current study, a total of 579 participants were recruited for the MTA intervention study for children with ADHD. At this time, these children were diagnosed as ADHD-Combined Type by MTA staff using procedures outlined in Hinshaw et al. (1997). The sample was selected to be widely representative of children seen in clinical practice (MTA Cooperative Group, 1999); as a result, a number of ADHD participants had comorbid disorders (e.g., 39.9% had oppositional defiant disorder and another 14% had conduct disorder, see MTA Cooperative Group, 1999). In total, 536 of the 579 ADHD participants had data on relevant study measures for at least one of the assessment periods for this study and were thus included in analyses.
Two years prior to their participation in the present study, the ADHD participants were randomly assigned to one of four treatment conditions: (1) Medication Management; (2) Behavior Therapy; (3) Combined Treatment (treatment including both Medication Management and Behavior Therapy); or (4) Community Care (families selected treatment(s) from options available in their communities, including the possibility of no treatment). Following the treatment phase, MTA children participated in a 10-month follow-up phase. During this period, no further treatments were provided by the MTA; however, participants were free to seek treatment in the community.
At the 24 month assessment (i.e., the start of the present study), a comparison group of 289 were recruited from the same schools and grades as the ADHD sample. These children were selected randomly, in the same sex proportions as the ADHD group, and were intended to be broadly representative of children in the local communities. Hence, they were not screened for ADHD or other problems, but only for factors that might limit their valid participation (e.g., non-English speaking; low IQ). Of these 289 participants, 284 (231 males and 53 females) had data on relevant study measures for at least one assessment point and were thus included in study analyses. In sum, the present study included 820 participants (536 ADHD and 284 from the comparison group).
Data for this study were collected at 4 different time points. The first time point analyzed represents the 24 month assessment in the main MTA study. For purposes of the present investigation, this first time point is referred to as Time 1. Three follow-up assessments also were conducted: a 1 year follow-up (Time 2, 3 years after original MTA baseline), a four-year follow-up (Time 3, 6 years after original baseline), and a 6-year follow-up (Time 4, 8 years after original baseline).
All children who had completed relevant study measures during at least one assessment period were included. Teacher reports of children’s behavior at Time 4 were only included in analyses if children were still in school at this assessment period. Sixty-two percent of the sample was Caucasian, 18% African-American, 11% Hispanic or Latino, 7% was biracial, and 2% was “other”. The average household income of participants was $40,001–$50,000 (income ranged from less than $10,000 to $75,000 or more).
Positive Illusory Biases
To assess participants’ positive illusory biases, participants and teachers completed measures of competence in both the social and behavioral domains. These domains were selected given evidence that positively biased self-perceptions in these domains are associated with peer-based behavior such as aggression (e.g., Hoza et al., in press). Participants rated their perceptions regarding their social acceptance (e.g., “some kids are popular with other kids their age…”) and behavioral conduct (e.g., “some kids usually get in trouble because of things they do…”, reverse-scored) on a scale of one to four using the appropriate subscales from the Harter Self-Perception Profile for Children (Harter, 1985) at Time 1 and Time 2 of the study and the Harter Self-Perception Profile for Adolescents (Harter, 1988) at Time 3 and Time 4. Scores for items in each subscale were averaged to yield a competence score in each domain, with higher scores reflecting higher self-reported competence. A subset of participants (N = 94) completed the Harter Self-Perception Profile for Adults (Messer & Harter, 1989) at Time 4, because they were 18 years old. This measure is substantially different from the measure for children and adolescents; therefore these scores were excluded from analyses and treated as missing data. In the present sample, the internal consistencies of both the self-reported social acceptance and the behavioral conduct scales were acceptable at all time points (αs ranged from .72 to .80).
Teachers rated participants’ social acceptance (e.g., “this child is popular with others his/her age…”) and behavioral conduct (e.g., “this child is usually well-behaved…”) on a scale of one to four using the teacher version of the Harter (1985) scale for children at Time 1 and Time 2 and the teacher version of the Harter (1988) scale for adolescents at Time 3 and Time 42. Scores in each subscale were averaged to yield a competence scale in each domain. When more than one teacher reported on a participating child, the scores from all teachers were averaged to create a single teacher-report score. The internal consistencies of both subscales were acceptable at all time points (αs ranged from .81 to .95).
Positive illusory bias scores were calculated by subtracting teacher reports of participants’ competence in the social and behavioral domains from children’s own self-reported competence from the parallel domain. Thus, positive bias scores reflected inflated self-perceptions whereas negative scores reflected an underestimation of one’s competence. Because of missing self or teacher reports, 16% of the sample was missing positive illusory bias scores regarding social and behavioral competence at Time 1. Also, because of missing data and/or attrition, 20%, 31%, and 36% of the sample was missing positive illusory bias scores at Times 2, 3, and 4, respectively.
Of note, despite debate in the literature regarding the use of difference scores (for reviews and discussions of this debate, see Colvin, Block, & Funder, 1996; Owens et al., 2007), we chose to operationalize bias in this manner for the following reasons. First, suggested alternatives to difference scores (e.g., residual scores) have equally serious statistical limitations, plus the added problem of interpretive difficulty (Colvin, Block, & Funder, 1996). Second, although difference scores have been criticized on the basis of lower reliability, residual scores often have comparable or even lesser reliability than difference scores (Rogosa, 1988, as cited in Colvin et al., 1996). As noted previously, “unreliability will only attentuate and never spuriously inflate correlations” (Colvin et al., 1996, p. 1253); hence the resultant effect of unreliability of difference scores would be to underestimate relations among variables. Importantly, then, when significant relations are found, they are likely to be meaningful. Third, separately analyzing components of a difference scores, such as self and teacher reports of competence, tests conceptually different questions than analysis of the difference score itself (Tisak & Smith, 1994a, b) and, therefore, is not considered a conceptually suitable alternative. For these reasons, we retained difference scores as measures of bias.
Bivariate correlations between biases in the social and behavioral domain were moderate in size (correlations ranged in size from .30 to .45 across the four assessment points), suggesting that these biases are related but distinct constructs.
Aggressive and Antisocial Behavior
Parents reported on participants’ aggressive and antisocial behaviors using the DSM-IV Conduct Disorder Checklist (Hinshaw et al., 1997). This 38-item instrument assesses the frequency of aggressive behaviors (e.g., “hit other children”) and antisocial behaviors (e.g., stealing, lying, property destruction) on a scale of one (never) to four (often). Items were averaged to yield an overall score, hereafter (for simplicity) referred to as “aggression.” Most participants (93% of participants with parent reports of aggression at Time 1) had reports from the biological mother; however many participants had reports from biological fathers (49% at Time 1), stepfathers (5% at Time 1), adoptive mothers (2% at Time 1), or other caretakers (e.g., foster mothers; 4% at Time 1). Given the relatively high correlation between scores by different reporters (e.g., r = .59, p < .001 between biological mothers and biological fathers at Time 1), scores were averaged to create one caretaker-report score if more than one caretaker reported on the participant. The internal consistency of this scale was good (αs ranged from .84 to .89). Because of missing data and/or attrition, 6%, 11%, 15%, and 29% of the sample was missing aggression scores at Times 1, 2, 3, and 4, respectively.
Social Skills
Teachers provided reports of participants’ social skills using the Social Skills Rating System – Teacher Report2 (Gresham & Elliott, 1990). The K-6 and 7–12 versions were used based on the grade of the participant at each assessment period. Teachers rated how often participants engaged in a variety of socially desirable behaviors for the social conduct factor of this measure (10 items; e.g., “cooperates with peers without prompting”) on a scale from 0 (never) to 2 (very often). Although other subscales are available from this measure (e.g., problem behaviors), only the social conduct subscale was included given our focus on social skills. When more than one teacher reported on a participating child, the scores from all teachers were averaged to create a single teacher-report score. Internal consistency of this scale was good at all time points (alphas ranged from .82 to .90). Because of missing data and/or attrition, 9%, 19%, 32%, and 42% of the sample was missing social skills scores at Times 1, 2, 3, and 4, respectively.
Peer Rejection
Teachers provided reports of participants’ rejection in the classroom using the Dishion Social Acceptance Scale (Dishion, 1990).2 Previous research has demonstrated moderate correlations between this scale and peer-based sociometric scores (Dishion, 1990), and recent research has used this measure to assess social functioning in the peer group (e.g., Owens et al., 2009). Teachers rated the proportion of children who disliked or rejected each child on a scale from 1 (very few: less than 25%) to 5 (almost all: more than 75%). Because of missing data and/or attrition, 12%, 19%, 30%, and 42% of the sample was missing rejection scores at Times 1, 2, 3, and 4, respectively.
Statistical Analysis
The statistical analyses for the present study were conducted using path analysis MPlus version 5 (Muthén & Muthén, 2007). Examination of the study variables indicated significant skewness for aggression and rejection (skewness for these variables ranged from 1.51 – 3.46; Kline, 2005); thus, the maximum likelihood-robust (MLR) estimator was used to accommodate the non-normality of these variables (see Chapter 15 in Muthén & Muthén, 2007). Maximum likelihood estimation procedures were used to accommodate missing data. The Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), and the Comparative Fit Index (CFI) were used to evaluate model fit (Hu & Bentler, 1999). In general, a cutoff value of .06 or lower for the RMSEA and .95 or higher for the CFI suggest good fit with the observed data (Hu & Benter, 1999), although lower thresholds are generally adopted for acceptable fit (e.g., CFI = .90, Little et al., 2003; see Hu & Benter, 1999). Relative fit for models was compared using a chi-square difference test for non-normally distributed data (Satorra, 2000).
For measures of positive illusory biases in the social and behavioral domain, respectively, a series of four theoretically-informed nested models were compared to assess which model provided the best fit. Model 1 (the baseline model) included within-time correlations (i.e., positive illusory biases, aggression, social conduct, and rejection) and first-lag continuity (e.g., continuity between aggression at T1 and T2). Model 2 included cross-lagged paths among social conduct, aggression, and rejection across the four time points. Model 3 allowed the cross-lagged paths between positive illusory biases and social conduct and aggression between Time 1 and Time 2 to vary. Finally, Model 4 allowed the remaining cross-lagged paths between positive illusory biases and social conduct and aggression to vary (i.e., between Time 2 and Time 4). In all models, ADHD status (initial classification in ADHD versus comparison group) predicted positive illusory biases, aggression, social conduct, and aggression at Time 1. Figure 1 depicts the paths freed in the nested models. Analyses were run separately by positive illusory domain (i.e., social and behavioral), given theory suggesting that self-concept is distinct across domains (Harter, 1985, 1988) and given the moderate correlation of positive illusions in the social and behavioral domains in the present sample, r = .45, p < .001. It is important to note that cross-lagged associations control for cross-sectional correlations and previous levels of functioning; thus, significant cross-lagged associations suggest that competence in one area predicts changes in functioning in another over time.
Results
Missing Data
Six-hundred and nineteen of the 820 participants (76% of the sample) remained in the study through Time 4, in terms of providing data on at least some measures at this time point. We examined whether there were differences between participants who remained in the study compared to participants for whom data were not available (or who were lost to attrition) by Time 4. Results indicated that attrition was not significantly associated with illusory biases in the social domain at Time 1, F(1, 690) = 2.28, p = .13, illusory biases in the behavioral domain at Time 1, F(1, 692) = 2.60, p = .11, peer rejection at Time 1, F(1, 720) = .93, p = .34, social skills at Time 1, F(1, 744) = 1.66, p = .20, family income, F(1, 798) = 2.68, p = .10, or race (1=Caucasian; 2=African-American; 3=Other), χ2(2, N = 820) = 2.17, p = .34. However, attrition was associated with aggressive conduct at Time 1, F(1, 768) = 6.15, p < .05, and ADHD status, χ2(1, N = 820) = 7.10, p < .01. Specifically, participants with ADHD and those with higher levels of aggression were less likely to have continued in the follow-up than were their peers. Among ADHD participants, attrition was also associated with treatment group, χ2(3, N = 536) = 12.09, p < .01; participants in the Combined Treatment and Behavioral Therapy were more likely to remain in the study than participants in the Medication Management and Community Care treatment groups.3 In addition, there was a significant association between attrition and age, F(1, 749) = 32.67, p < .01, with older participants being less likely to have data available at Time 4. Maximum likelihood estimation procedures were used to accommodate missing data.
Characteristics of the MTA and Comparison Groups
Analyses of variance comparing participants in the ADHD and the comparison groups at each time point indicated that children in the ADHD sample exhibited elevated levels of dysfunction across the six years of the study (see Table 1). Specifically, ADHD participants exhibited lower social skills at Time 1, F(1, 744) = 118.10, p < .001, Time 2, F(1, 659) = 125.70, p < .001, Time 3, F(1, 557) = 50.15, p < .001, and Time 4, F(1, 471) = 20.91, p < .001. In addition, participants with ADHD also were more rejected than their peers at Time 1, F(1, 720) = 69.68, p < .001; Time 2, F(1, 664) = 59.40, p < .001; Time 3, F(1, 571) = 38.31,p < .001; and Time 4, F(1, 473) = 39.40, p < .001. In addition, previous research with this sample has demonstrated that the children diagnosed with ADHD exhibited higher levels of ADHD symptoms, symptoms of depression, and aggression across the 4 assessment periods (see Hoza et al., in press).
Table 1.
Study Time |
||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | |||||
ADHD | Comparison | ADHD | Comparison | ADHD | Comparison | ADHD | Comparison | |
Social Acceptance | ||||||||
Child Report | 3.15 (.71) | 3.23 (.69) | 3.27 (.66) | 3.33 (.62) | 3.22 (.66) | 3.38 (.55) | 3.22 (.67) | 3.40 (.50) |
Teacher Report | 2.30 (.84) | 3.08 (.83) | 2.41 (.81) | 3.05 (.81) | 2.50 (.76) | 2.87 (.68) | 2.57 (.68) | 2.92 (.66) |
Illusory Biases | .76 (1.01) | .14 (.95) | .82 (.87) | .28 (.86) | .71 (.85) | .51 (.69) | .64 (.78) | .50 (.77) |
Behavioral Conduct | ||||||||
Child Report | 3.13 (.69) | 3.39 (.58) | 3.16 (.63) | 3.39 (.62) | 2.86 (.58) | 3.13 (.60) | 2.86 (.60) | 3.15 (.61) |
Teacher Report | 2.40 (.95) | 3.28 (.86) | 2.60 (.91) | 3.49 (.71) | 2.81 (.82) | 3.39 (.64) | 2.93 (.79) | 3.37 (.67) |
Illusory Biases | .71 (1.09) | .13 (.91) | .55 (.99) | −.10 (.82) | .05 (.89) | −.27 (.75) | −.06 (.87) | −.22 (.72) |
Social Skills | 1.10 (.40) | 1.43 (.40) | 1.12 (.39) | 1.45 (.36) | 1.19 (.34) | 1.39 (.32) | 1.27 (.35) | 1.42 (.34) |
Rejection | 2.01 (1.22) | 1.32 (.77) | 1.87 (1.10) | 1.27 (.69) | 1.69 (.94) | 1.25 (.54) | 1.49 (.75) | 1.12 (.38) |
Aggression | 1.16 (.18) | 1.06 (.12) | 1.16 (.18) | 1.05 (.07) | 1.15 (.20) | 1.06 (.15) | 1.15 (.21) | 1.05 (.09) |
Note. Higher scores reflect higher levels of competence, biases, social skills, rejection, and aggression.
As expected, ADHD children were more likely than their peers to have a positive illusory bias in both the social and behavioral domains. Hoza et al. (2004) found that ADHD children exhibited elevated biases in the social and behavioral domains at Time 1. Analysis of variance tests indicated that ADHD children also exhibited elevated biases in the social domain at Time 2, F(1, 652) = 61.39, p < .001, Time 3, F(1, 563) = 8.43, p < .01, and Time 4, F(1, 427) = 3.90, p < .05. ADHD children also exhibited elevated biases in the behavioral domain at Time 2, F(1, 654) = 76.28, p < .001, and Time 3, F(1, 557) = 18.64, p < .001, but not at Time 4, F(1, 426) = 3.82, p = .051 (although this effect approached statistical significance). Additional details regarding developmental change in biases in the social and behavioral domain with this sample are available in Hoza et al. (in press).
Analyses were also conducted to examine whether children with ADHD who had received each of the four different MTA treatments (Medication Management; Behavior Therapy; Combined Treatment; or Community Care) differed on study variables at Time 1. A multivariate ANOVA with study variables at Time 1 serving as the dependent variable and MTA treatment group serving as the independent variable was significant, F(15, 1197) = 1.84, p < .05. Univariate tests indicated that treatment group was associated with positive illusions in the behavioral domain, F(3, 401) = 4.44, p < .05, and social skills, F(3, 403) = 3.48, p < .05.4 Tukey post-hoc tests revealed that participants in the Combined Treatment had lower behavioral biases at Time 1 than those exhibited by participants who received Behavioral Therapy. In addition, participants in the Combined Treatment exhibited higher levels of social skills at Time 1 when compared to those in the Medication Management treatment. No other effects were significant.
Social Illusory Biases
Nested model comparisons were conducted to evaluate the fit of each model including positively biased self-perceptions in the social domain. Results, presented in Table 2, indicated that Model 1 provided poor fit to the data. However, the addition of the paths in Model 2 and Model 3 both provided significant improvement in model fit. In contrast, addition of the paths on Model 4 did not result in better model fit; as a result, Model 3 was selected as the final model (CFI = .93, RMSEA = .06). These results indicate that the addition of cross-lagged paths between biases in the social domain and the other three areas of functioning improved model fit only from Time 1 to Time 2.
Table 2.
Biases Domain | Model | No. of Cross-Domain Paths | df | C | χ2 | CFI | RMSEA | Model Comparison | cd | Δdf | Δ χ2 | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Social | 1 | 0 | 96 | 1.17 | 434.23 | .875 | .066 | 2 vs. 1 | 1.226 | 18 | 132.39 | < .0001 |
2 | 18 | 78 | 1.16 | 289.78 | .922 | .058 | 3 vs. 2 | 1.139 | 4 | 30.82 | <.0001 | |
3 | 22 | 74 | 1.16 | 268.23 | .928 | .057 | 4 vs. 3 | 1.199 | 8 | 12.50 | .13 | |
4 | 30 | 66 | 1.15 | 256.38 | .930 | .059 | ||||||
Behavioral | 1 | 0 | 96 | 1.15 | 429.25 | .887 | .065 | 2 vs. 1 | 1.258 | 18 | 108.69 | < .0001 |
2 | 18 | 78 | 1.13 | 317.21 | .919 | .061 | 3 vs. 2 | .977 | 4 | 52.50 | < .0001 | |
3 | 22 | 74 | 1.13 | 269.70 | .934 | .057 | 4 vs. 3 | 1.051 | 8 | 19.73 | < .05 | |
4 | 30 | 66 | 1.14 | 249.20 | .938 | .058 |
Notes. c is the weighting constant for computing the chi-square statistic when robust estimation procedures are used. cd is the weighting constant for the difference between two chi-square values when robust estimation procedures are used.
Table 3 presents the within-time correlations between areas of functioning at each time point. At each time point, biases in the social domain were associated with lower levels of social skills and higher levels of rejection. Poor social skills were associated with rejection at all four time points whereas aggression was related to rejection only at Time 1. Finally, aggression was related to lower social skills at all time points other than Time 4.
Table 3.
Biases Domain | SocSkills | Agg | Rej | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Social | Time 1 | |||
PIB | −.36*** (.03) | .01 (.05) | .43*** (.04) | |
SocSkills | −.27*** (.03) | −.49*** (.03) | ||
Agg | .18*** (.05) | |||
Time 2 | ||||
PIB | −.29*** (.04) | .06 (.04) | .40*** (.04) | |
SocSkills | −.16*** (.04) | −.40*** (.04) | ||
Agg | .11* (.05) | |||
Time 3 | ||||
PIB | −.24*** (.04) | .09 (.05) | .35*** (.04) | |
SocSkills | −.15** (.04) | −.41*** (.04) | ||
Agg | .06 (.05) | |||
Time 4 | ||||
PIB | −.26*** (.05) | .02 (.07) | .16** (.06) | |
SocSkills | −.06 (.07) | −.41*** (.04) | ||
Agg | .05 (.08) | |||
Behavioral | Time 1 | |||
PIB | −.51*** (.03) | .20*** (.04) | .38*** (.04) | |
SocSkills | −.28*** (.03) | −.49*** (.03) | ||
Agg | .18*** (.05) | |||
Time 2 | ||||
PIB | −.42*** (.03) | .08 (.04) | .33*** (.03) | |
SocSkills | −.16** (.05) | −.39*** (.04) | ||
Agg | .11 (.06) | |||
Time 3 | ||||
PIB | −.39*** (.04) | .05 (.04) | .33*** (.04) | |
SocSkills | −.15** (.04) | −.41*** (.04) | ||
Agg | .06 (.05) | |||
Time 4 | ||||
PIB | −.43*** (.04) | .01 (.07) | .31*** (.05) | |
SocSkills | −.06 (.07) | −.41*** (.04) | ||
Agg | .04 (.08) |
p < .05
p < .01
p < .001
The longitudinal stability of functioning in each area is presented in Table 4. As expected, all within-area paths were significant across time, indicating stability of functioning in each area. Specifically, biases in the social domain, social skills, and rejection exhibited moderate levels of stability. Consistent with previous work (e.g., Burt et al., 2008), aggression was highly stable.
Table 4.
Biases Domain | Subsequent Assessment Period | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
PIB | SocSkills | Agg | Rej | ||
Social | Time 1 | ||||
PIB | .32*** (.04) | −.04 (.04) | .08* (.04) | ||
SocSkills | −.17*** (.04) | .40*** (.04) | −.04 (.04) | −.18*** (.05) | |
Agg | −.00 (.05) | −.20*** (.04) | .64*** (.05) | .08 (.05) | |
Rej | −.10* (.04) | .08 (.05) | .21*** (.05) | ||
Time 2 | |||||
PIB | .29*** (.04) | ||||
SocSkills | .33*** (.05) | −.11** (.04) | −.19*** (.04) | ||
Agg | −.16*** (.05) | .61*** (.05) | .15** (.05) | ||
Rej | −.05 (.05) | −.01 (.04) | .17*** (.05) | ||
Time 3 | |||||
PIB | .36*** (.05) | ||||
SocSkills | .31*** (.05) | −.08* (.03) | −.16** (.06) | ||
Agg | −.12* (.05) | .65*** (.05) | .07 (.05) | ||
Rej | −.12* (.05) | .04 (.05) | .20** (.07) | ||
Behavioral | Time 1 | ||||
PIB | .39*** (.04) | −.11** (.04) | .05 (.04) | ||
SocSkills | −.13** (.04) | .36*** (.05) | −.04 (.04) | −.16*** (.05) | |
Agg | .16*** (.04) | −.19*** (.04) | .63*** (.05) | .08 (.05) | |
Rej | −.10** (.04) | .10* (.05) | .23*** (.05) | ||
Time 2 | |||||
PIB | .18*** (.05) | −.04 (.05) | −.01 (.04) | ||
SocSkills | −.11* (.05) | .31*** (.05) | −.12** (.05) | −.18*** (.04) | |
Agg | .11* (.05) | −.15** (.05) | .61*** (.05) | .15* (.06) | |
Rej | −.03 (.04) | −.01 (.04) | .16** (.05) | ||
Time 3 | |||||
PIB | .32*** (.06) | −.04 (.05) | .04 (.04) | ||
SocSkills | −.06 (.06) | .29*** (.06) | −.06 (.04) | −.16** (.06) | |
Agg | .06 (.06) | −.16** (.05) | .65*** (.05) | .10 (.05) | |
Rej | −.08 (.05) | .03 (.05) | .17* (.07) |
p < .05
p < .01
p < .001
NOTE: only paths estimated in final models are included.
Estimates for the cross-lagged longitudinal paths are presented in Table 4. Significant paths are depicted in Figure 2. As expected, as compared to the comparison group, the ADHD group exhibited heightened biases in the social domain (estimate = .29, p < .0001), poor social skills (estimate = −.37, p < .001), aggression (estimate = .28, p < .0001), and rejection (estimate = .29, p < .0001) at Time 1. In addition, a vicious cycle between aggression and social skills was observed over time. Specifically, aggression at Time 1 was associated with lower social skills at Time 2. These lower social skills at Time 2 predicted heightened aggression at Time 3, which in turn predicted lower social skills at Time 4. A similar cycle was observed between social skills and rejection. Specifically, rejection at Time 1 was related to lower social skills at Time 2, which in turn predicted rejection at Time 3. This Time 3 rejection was related to lower social skills at Time 4.
Finally, biases in the social domain appeared to confer risk for rejection because of their influence on peer-based behavior. Specifically, biases in the social domain at Time 1 predicted aggression at Time 2. This Time 2 aggression, in turn, was related to rejection based on two pathways. First, Time 2 aggression predicted rejection at Time 3. We examined the significance of the indirect effect of ADHD status on peer rejection via this pathway. The test of indirect effects with the robust estimator employs the delta method for standard errors (see Muthén & Muthén, 2007, Ch. 16), and this approach has lower power than alternative tests of mediation (see Taylor, MacKinnon, & Tien, 2007). Moreover, tests of complex models such as 3-path effects exhibit lower power than traditional 2-path mediation tests (see Williams & MacKinnon, 2008); thus, indirect effects that approach conventional levels of statistical significance are reported. The test of the indirect effect of ADHD status on Time 3 rejection via this pathway (ADHD status → Time 1 social biases → Time 2 aggression → Time 3 rejection) approached conventional levels of statistical significance, p = .09. In addition, Time 2 aggression was associated with lower Time 3 social skills, which in turn predicted heightened Time 4 rejection. However, test of the indirect pathway from ADHD to Time 4 rejection via this second pathway (ADHD → Time 1 social biases → Time 2 aggression → Time 3 social skills → Time 4 rejection) was not significant, p = .12. Interestingly, poor social skills at Time 1 also predicted heightened illusory bias at Time 2, suggesting that children may develop biases for self-protective purposes when performing poorly in a particular area. Importantly, longitudinal predictions controlled for previous levels of functioning and within-time correlations, thus providing a stringent test of predictors of increases in problems over time.
Behavioral Illusory Biases
Nested model comparisons were conducted to evaluate the fit of each model in the behavioral domain. Results, presented in Table 2, indicated that Model 1 provided poor fit to the data. Moreover, the addition of the paths in Model 2, Model 3, and Model 4 all provided significant improvement in model fit. As a result, Model 4 was selected as the final model (CFI = .94, RMSEA = .06). Given the overlap with the analyses for the social domain, only new findings are highlighted below.
Table 3 presents the within-time correlations between areas of functioning at each time point. At each time point, biases in the behavioral domain were associated with lower levels of social skills and higher levels of rejection. In addition, biases in the behavioral domain were associated with aggression at Time 1. The longitudinal stability of each area of functioning is presented in Table 4. As expected, biases in the behavioral domain exhibited moderate levels of stability.
Estimates for the cross-lagged longitudinal paths are presented in Table 4. Significant paths are depicted in Figure 3. As expected, relative to the comparison group, children in the ADHD group exhibited heightened biases in the behavioral domain (estimate = .27, p < .0001). In addition, the cascade effects between aggression, social skills, and rejection from the social biases model were replicated. As expected, biases in the behavioral domain appeared to confer risk for rejection due to their influence on peer-based behavior. Specifically, biases in the behavioral domain at Time 1 predicted poor social skills at Time 2. Time 2 social skills, in turn, were related to rejection at Time 3. Tests of the indirect effect of ADHD status on Time 3 rejection via this pathway (ADHD → Time 1 behavioral biases → Time 2 social skills → Time 3 rejection) was significant, p < .05. Time 2 social skills also predicted Time 3 social skills, which in turn were associated with Time 4 rejection. Tests of the indirect effect of ADHD status on Time 4 rejection via this pathway (ADHD → Time 1 behavioral biases → Time 2 social skills → Time 3 social skills → Time 4 rejection) approached conventional levels of statistical significance, p = .08.
In addition, there was evidence that social behavior was related to subsequent biases in the behavioral domain. Specifically, both poor social skills and aggression at Time 1 predicted heightened illusory bias at Time 2. In addition, these social behaviors at Time 2 were associated with behavioral biases at Time 3. These results are consistent with the notion that children may develop biases for self-protective purposes when performing poorly in a particular area.
Additional Models: Age, Sex, ADHD Status, and MTA Treatment Group
Given the large age range of participants at each assessment period, we ran a Multiple Indicators Multiple Causes model (MIMIC; Muthén, 1989) for our two final models controlling for participant age. In both models, all significant cross-lagged paths remained significant when age was controlled.
A second set of follow-up analyses examined whether sex moderated the longitudinal associations between areas of functioning. Given evidence that the chi-square difference test for nested model comparisons for invariance tests may be too sensitive with relatively large samples (Cheung & Rensvold, 2002), we also examined whether the change in CFI for this nested model comparison exceeded the recommended .01 difference threshold (i.e., thought to reflect a meaningful difference in model fit; Cheung & Rensvold, 2002). For the model with biases in the social domain, findings indicated that within-time correlations, Δχ2(24) = 35.15, p = .07, ΔCFI = .003, the stability of the four factors, Δχ2(12) = 16.55, p = .17, ΔCFI = .000., the associations between ADHD status (ADHD versus comparison group) and the four factors at Time 1, Δχ2(4) = 3.40, p = .49, ΔCFI = .000, and the significant cross-lagged paths in our final social illusory biases model were invariant across gender, Δχ2(13) = 16.54, p = .22, ΔCFI = .001. A parallel set of analyses examined sex invariance of the model with illusory biases in the behavioral domain. Results indicated that within-time correlations, Δχ2(24) = 35.67, p = .06, ΔCFI = .003, stability estimates, Δχ2(12) = 15.86, p = .20, ΔCFI = .001, associations between ADHD status and study variables at Time 1, Δχ2(4) = 5.37, p = .25, ΔCFI = .001, and cross-lagged paths, Δχ2(14) = 21.31, p = .09, ΔCFI = .003, were also invariant across sex. The ΔCFI for the addition of the stability constraint actually reflected an improvement in the model fit for the constrained model.
Next, we examined whether the strength of the significant cross-domain paths differed for ADHD participants and the comparison group. The final models were re-run with ADHD status as a moderator rather than as a predictor of study variables at Time 1. The results for the social illusory biases model indicated that within-time correlations, Δχ2(24) = 55.81, p < .001, ΔCFI = .017, and stability estimates, Δχ2(12) = 34.91, p < .001, ΔCFI = .003 were not invariant across ADHD and the comparison groups. The ΔCFI for the stability estimate actually reflected an improvement for model fit in the constrained model. Constraining the estimated cross-domain paths to be equal across groups did not result in a significant decrease in model fit, Δχ2(13) = 13.94, p = .38, and this constraint actually improved the CFI, ΔCFI = .015, suggesting similar vicious cycles and cascading across groups. Similarly, in the model with behavioral biases, within-time correlations, Δχ2(24) = 44.35, p < .01, ΔCFI = .007, and stability estimates, Δχ2(12) = 33.85, p < .001, ΔCFI = .005, were not invariant across ADHD and the comparison groups although the change in CFI suggested that these differences may not be meaningful (Cheung & Rensvold, 2002). Constraining the estimated cross-domain paths to be equal across groups did not result in a significant decrease in model fit, Δχ2(14) = 15.09, p = .37, and actually resulted in an improvement in CFI, ΔCFI = .013, once again suggesting similar vicious cycles and cascading effects across groups.
Finally, a set of follow-up analyses were conducted to examine whether MTA treatment group moderated the vicious cycles and developmental cascades across areas of functioning. To address this question, analyses were re-run excluding participants in the comparison group. For the final model including biases in the social domain, within-time correlations were invariant across treatment groups, Δχ2(72) = 54.80, p = .93 and this constraint resulted in an improvement in CFI, ΔCFI = .017. Next, the addition of the stability constraint did not result in a significant decrease in model fit, Δχ2(36) = 39.73, p = .31, and resulted in an improvement in CFI, ΔCFI = .001. Finally, we tested whether the significant cross-lagged paths in our final social illusory biases model differed across treatments. Results indicated that constraining the significant cross-lagged paths to be equal across treatment groups did not result in a decrease in model fit, Δχ2(39) = 42.31, p = .33, ΔCFI = .002. A parallel set of analyses for the final model including biases in the behavioral domain indicated that within-time correlations, Δχ2(72) = 67.60, p = .63, ΔCFI = .004 (with the more constrained model exhibiting a higher CFI), stability estimates, Δχ2(36) = 33.65, p = .58, ΔCFI = .006 (with the more constrained model exhibiting a higher CFI), and cross-lagged paths, Δχ2(42) = 46.87, p = .28, ΔCFI = .003, were invariant across treatment groups.
Discussion
The goal of this study was to explore developmental processes involved in the severe and persistent peer problems exhibited among children diagnosed with ADHD. We hypothesized that the peer problems such as rejection among these children emerge, at least in part, from failures to successfully negotiate salient developmental tasks. Consistent with this perspective, at the first assessment, children diagnosed with ADHD exhibited impairment in their social skills, aggressive and antisocial behaviors, and abilities to accurately gauge their social and behavioral competence. Moreover, these failures were related to vicious cycles such that problems in one area of functioning “spilled over” into other areas of competence and then “splashed back” to the original area over the course of development. Cascades across areas of functioning were also found, such that competence in one area had indirect effects on competence in other areas. Importantly, these results held even when controlling for age and were similar for males and females, for children with initial assignments to both the ADHD and comparison groups, and for children with ADHD who had received different MTA treatments.
One developmental task identified by Masten and Coatsworth (1998) is engaging in socially appropriate conduct, including the inhibition of aggressive tendencies and the development of socially skilled behaviors. As expected, children in the ADHD group exhibited problems in both of these areas at Time 1 compared to their peers, suggesting that these children have difficulties negotiating these important developmental tasks. Moreover, problems in each of these areas predicted increases in problems in the other areas across development. That is, Time 1 aggression predicted lower Time 2 social skills, lower Time 2 social skills predicted heightened aggression at Time 3, and heightened aggression at Time 3 predicted lower social skills at Time 4. These results are consistent with the notion that the ability to inhibit aggressive behavior may depend in part on having socially skilled alternatives for dealing with peer conflict (see Crick & Dodge, 1994). Moreover, once aggressive strategies are adopted, the availability of aggressive repertoires may interfere with children’s opportunities to learn skillful alternatives for coping with conflict.
In addition to vicious cycles between social skills and aggression, failures in both of these areas had implications for emerging peer problems across development. In fact, low social skills were associated with subsequent peer rejection at each assessment period. These results are consistent with previous studies demonstrating that poor social skills are associated with peer rejection (e.g., Harrist et al., 1997; Newcomb et al., 1993) and suggest that the high levels of peer rejection among children with ADHD may in part reflect their inability to generate socially skilled behaviors when interacting with peers. Importantly, rejection was also associated with subsequent impairment in social skills. These findings are consistent with the perspective that social interactions with peers are essential for the development of social skills (Hartup & Sicilio, 1996; Newcomb & Bagwell, 1995). Insofar as rejected children have fewer opportunities for social interaction (e.g., because they are excluded; see Cullerton-Sen & Crick, 2005), they do not have opportunities to learn and practice socially skilled behaviors with peers, leading to decreases in their skills across time.
Aggressive behavior also had implications for rejection by peers across development. For example, there was a direct effect of aggression on rejection, such that aggression at Time 2 predicted heightened rejection at Time 3. These results are consistent with previous longitudinal research indicating that aggression is associated with later rejection (e.g., Hay et al., 2004; Little & Garber, 1995; Ostrov & Crick, 2007; Pope & Bierman, 1999). Moreover, consistent with the previous research indicating that rejection leads to aggression over time (e.g., Dodge et al., 2003; Hay et al., 2004), the results of one of the two models (i.e., those with behavioral illusory biases) indicated that rejection at Time 1 was associated with aggression at Time 2.
A second important developmental task is the formation of a cohesive sense of self (Masten & Coatsworth, 1998), including increasing accuracy regarding one’s strengths and weaknesses (Harter, 1982; Marsh et al., 1998). As expected, children in the ADHD group exhibited overly positive self-perceptions in both the social and behavioral domains when compared to their peers at Time 1. These findings are consistent with a number of previous studies demonstrating that children with ADHD tend to report fairly positive self-perceptions despite poor performance in a number of domains (e.g., Hoza et al., 2004; Hoza, Pelham, Dobbs, Owens, & Pillow, 2002). Moreover, as expected, these positive illusory biases were associated with heightened aggressive and antisocial behaviors, and lower social skills. Specifically, positive illusions in the social domain at Time 1 predicted aggression at Time 2. In addition, positive illusions in the behavioral domain at Time 1 predicted lower social skills at Time 2. The aggression findings are consistent with the notion that inflated self-perceptions may lead to aggressive and antisocial behavior when peers challenge these positive self-views (Baumeister et al., 1996). In addition, both findings are consistent with the hypothesis that the failure to acknowledge poor competence in a given domain may be related to low motivation to improve one’s abilities (Hoza et al., 2009), resulting in low competence over time. Interestingly, our hypotheses that biases in the social domain would be more strongly associated with poor social skills whereas biases in the behavioral domain would be more strongly associated with aggression were not confirmed. In fact, biases in the social domain predicted later aggression whereas biases in the behavioral domain predicted impaired social skills. It is important to note, however, that Masten and Coatsworth (1998) identify aggression and social skills as components of the same developmental task: developing socially appropriate conduct. Thus, these findings may reflect the fact that learning positive social skills and inhibiting aggressive behavior are highly related abilities. Future research should include measures of functioning across distinct developmental tasks; for example, it would be informative to explore whether biases in the academic domain predicted later academic but not social functioning to further explore this important point.
Importantly, the results also provided support for the hypothesis that positive illusory biases would have indirect effects on peer functioning. In fact, biases in both domains were indirectly related to peer rejection via their influence on aggression (for social illusory biases) and poor social skills (for behavioral illusory biases). However, it is important to note that the statistical tests for indirect effects from ADHD to later peer rejection via these pathways were significant for behavioral biases to Time 3 rejection only. Nonetheless, two of the three remaining indirect pathways of ADHD status to rejection approached conventional levels of statistical significance (p < .10) despite including 3-path and 4-path effects (see Williams & MacKinnon, 2008) and using a mediation test (the delta standard error method, Muthén & Muthén, 2007, Ch. 16) that may be relatively underpowered relative to bootstrapping alternatives (see Taylor, MacKinnon, & Tein, 2007). Overall, then, these findings do provide some evidence to suggest that positive illusions may serve as an important mediator of the association between ADHD and rejection by peers, although some of these pathways should be interpreted with caution as they did not reach conventional levels of statistical significance.
This study is one of the first to examine the developmental implications of positive illusory biases for adjustment, and the findings suggest that, despite some advantages (see Taylor & Brown, 1988), there may be maladaptive consequences to overly positive self-perceptions. In fact, overly positive self-perceptions were associated with impaired functioning in the peer group over time, and these negative behaviors were in turn related to peer rejection. Interestingly, these overly positive self-perceptions (as indicated by the bias scores) appeared to reflect relatively “normal” self-reports of competence despite low levels of actual competence. Thus, it is possible that attempts to feel good about oneself, when reality does not warrant it, can cause further problems for struggling children. That is, in the face of impaired functioning, these children attempted normative self-perceptions, resulting in an overestimation of competence. This overestimation, in turn, predicted later problem behaviors.
Interestingly, negative peer behavior was related to later positive illusory biases. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that positive illusory biases serve a self-protective function for at-risk children (for a review of theoretical accounts of the positive illusory bias among children with ADHD, see Owens, Goldfine, Evangelista, Hoza, & Kaiser, 2007). In effect, if at-risk children adopt relatively positive self-views in an effort to cope with failures or limited competence, then impaired functioning should lead to increases in biases in self-perceptions over time. Consistent with this perspective, results indicated that poor social skills at Time 1 were associated with positive illusory biases in the social domain at Time 2. Moreover, poor social skills and aggressive behavior at Time 1 predicted positive illusory biases in the behavioral domain at Time 2, and poor social skills at Time 2 predicted positive illusions in the behavioral domain at Time 3. These findings are also consistent with previous findings suggesting that children with ADHD tend to inflate their self-perceptions in the domains of greatest deficit (Hoza et al., 2004; Hoza et al., 2002).
Two additional interesting patterns also emerged in the results. First, there appear to be multiple vicious cycles and cascading effects among areas of functioning across development. These findings suggest that there are a number of indirect effects among overly-positive self-perceptions, social skills, aggression, and peer rejection. As a result, failure in one area may have both direct and indirect effects on functioning in other areas across development. Second, these effects appear to be more prominent in late childhood and early adolescence than in later adolescence. For example, in the model with positive illusions in the social domain, the addition of cross-lagged paths between these biases and peer-based behavior from Time 2 to Time 3 and from Time 3 to Time 4 did not improve model fit (although it did in the model with behavioral illusory biases). These findings are consistent with the hypothesis that the effects of self-perceptions on behavior are especially likely during early adolescence, when a focus on others’ views of the self becomes especially salient (see Harter, 1999). In fact, there were a relatively high number of significant cross-lagged paths between Time 1 and Time 2 (as compared to the other time lags). Given the relatively high importance placed on successful peer functioning during middle childhood and early adolescence (e.g., Berndt, 1996; Bukowski & Kramer, 1986; Hartup, 1992), it is possible that impairment in these areas is especially problematic during these developmental periods. However, this possibility is speculative and awaits further empirical investigation, especially since the addition of these cross-lag paths did improve model fit in the model for behavioral illusory biases. Moreover, it is also possible that these effects simply reflect the shorter time lag earlier in the study relative to later in the study. It is likely that cross-lag effects will be more difficult to detect across longer periods of time. Future studies should explore cross-lag paths across middle childhood and adolescence with uniform spacing of assessments to further address this important point.
The findings from this study have a number of implications. First, the results suggest that the peer problems typical of children with ADHD may reflect the failure of these children to successfully negotiate important developmental tasks (e.g., to develop accurate self-perceptions), a perspective which is in line with the developmental psychopathology perspective (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 2002; Sroufe & Rutter, 1984). As a result, researchers and clinicians interested in understanding impairment among children with ADHD may benefit from increased attention to normative developmental processes and the ways in which children with ADHD struggle with these typical developmental challenges. In fact, the cross-domain paths among areas of functioning were the same for ADHD children and the comparison group. These findings suggest that the same processes may be involved in peer problems in both groups, with ADHD children simply exhibiting more difficulties in these areas.
Second, our findings have important implications for interventions aimed at improving peer problems among children with ADHD. There are two different “levels” at which our findings might be interpreted. At the more conservative level, we might conclude that intervention programs may benefit from intervening at multiple levels and areas of functioning (e.g., developing more accurate self-perceptions, lowering aggressive behavior, and improving social skills). In fact, given the findings of negative cascades and vicious cycles across time, it is also possible that interventions might lead to positive cascading effects (e.g., improvements in social skills leading to reductions in rejection and aggression), although impediments such as negative reputations with peers (see Hoza, 2007, for a discussion) may prevent such cascading effects. Also, these interventions may be most effective during specific developmental periods. For example, early social skills interventions might be especially helpful in reducing peer rejection. Given the many cross-domain paths associated with social skills, this level might be especially important. Nonetheless, it is hard to ignore prevailing work that documents that social skills training is of limited effectiveness with externalizing children (Bierman, 2004), suggesting that more drastic measures may be needed.
Hence, at a less conservative level, an argument can be made that the cascading effects of impaired functioning in these interrelated social and behavioral functioning domains may warrant a more comprehensive approach to intervention than has heretofore been considered. Given that poor social and behavioral functioning are potent predictors of later maladaptation that persists into adulthood (e.g., Burt et al., 2008; Masten et al., 2005), and that we have shown here that processes involved appear to be similar for children with ADHD and comparison children, an argument can be made that a comprehensive social/behavioral curriculum that addresses skill development in these areas might be a critical piece of educational curriculum to be administered in schools across childhood and adolescence. Although such a position is likely to be unpopular with school administrators who are under grave pressure to improve academic outcomes, our results suggest that social and behavioral maladaptation also has devastating effects on developmental outcomes. Nonetheless, such a shift may be beyond what our educational milieu can accept at the present time.
Regardless of whether the more or less conservative approach is taken, however, the results of this study contribute to an emerging literature documenting the utility of a developmental cascades perspective (Burt et al., 2008; Masten et al., 2005), in which impaired functioning in one area “spills over” into other areas. Although the idea of transactional processes across areas of functioning is not new, we agree with Masten and colleagues (2005) that more systematic studies of these associations over time greatly enhance our understanding of adaptation across development.
Given the interesting implications of the present study, it is important to consider the findings in the context of study strengths and limitations. This study has a number of strengths, including the large sample size, the longitudinal design, the inclusion of children with ADHD and a comparison group, and the inclusion of multiple reporters (i.e., self, teacher, and parent) to assess study constructs. In addition, the statistical methods employed are able to accommodate missing data and allow for appropriate treatment of skewed variables. Finally, some researchers have criticized the use of difference scores (e.g., self-report minus teacher report) when measuring positive illusory biases because resulting correlations may simply be an artifact of actual competence. However, it is important to note that, in the present study, effects of positively biased self-perceptions on later peer-based behavior persisted even when controlling for earlier functioning (e.g., aggressive conduct and social skills). These results suggest that it is unlikely that the associations between positive illusory biases and adjustment are simply an artifact of the low levels of actual adjustment typical among children with positive illusory biases.
Despite these many strengths, however, a number of limitations must also be acknowledged. One important limitation of the present study is the use of teacher reports to assess peer rejection. Although previous research has demonstrated the validity of teacher reports using this measure (Dishion, 1990) and studies have successfully used this measure to assess social functioning (e.g., Owens et al., 2009), a preferable method to assess peer rejection is sociometric procedures (Coie & Dodge, 1983). In addition, although our analyses are able to accommodate missing data, a number of participants had missing data by the end of the study, and participants at especially high risk (e.g., ADHD and highly aggressive) were more likely to drop out of the study than their peers. Interestingly, MTA treatment group was associated with attrition among the MTA sample. The finding that participants in the Combined Treatment and Behavioral Therapy groups were more likely to remain in the study at Time 4 than those in Medication Management and Community Care may reflect greater engagement in the study among participants who received psychosocial treatment through the MTA. A further limitation of this study is that assessments started when participants were already around 10 years of age. As a result, it is not possible to examine the cascading effects of study variables across earlier developmental periods. It is possible, for example, that aggressive behavior is more strongly associated with rejection in early and middle childhood than during the adolescent period examined in this study (see Cillessen & Mayeux, 2004). In a similar vein, other cross-lagged paths may be stronger or weaker across other developmental periods. It is also likely that early difficulties (e.g., failure to develop self-control skills in early childhood) might make successful negotiation of later tasks in the same area of functioning (e.g., abiding by societal rules regarding moral behavior in middle childhood) especially challenging. Thus, longitudinal studies that examine functioning earlier in development are sorely needed. Moreover, although a number of significant cross-lagged paths emerged, it is important to note that the effect sizes for these paths were often small. These relatively small effects may reflect the very stringent statistical tests of cascade models and the relatively focused measures in the present study (e.g., previous cascade studies have focused on functioning in broad areas of competence; e.g., Masten et al., 2005). It is also possible that the indirect effects of ADHD on peer rejection would be larger if the comparison group had been limited to participants without ADHD symptoms or other psychiatric problems. Finally, it is important to note that there were some differences in relations among variables that were included in the two models (e.g., social skills at Time 3 predicted aggression at Time 4 in the positive social bias but not behavioral bias model). Comparison of models revealed relatively similar estimates for these pathways (i.e., the standardized path estimates for two of the three paths that differed across models were within .02) and the estimates of effect sizes in both models tended to be small. Thus, these differences across models likely reflect statistical differences resulting from the inclusion of different variables in each model. Nonetheless, given these differences, we believe that readers should have greater confidence in pathways that were replicated across the two models.
Despite these limitations, the findings provide a rigorous test of the developmental processes that may be involved in peer problems among children with ADHD. As expected, children with ADHD exhibited failures in a variety of developmental tasks at the first assessment period, including engaging in heightened levels of aggression, exhibiting poor social skills, adopting overly positive self-perceptions, and being rejected by peers. Moreover, problems in each of these areas had cascading effects into other areas over a 6-year period. Importantly, the cross-lagged paths did not differ for ADHD and the comparison group, suggesting that negative cascade effects are not unique to ADHD but are instead a more ubiquitous developmental phenomenon. Nevertheless, since children with ADHD exhibit greater difficulties successfully negotiating salient developmental tasks than their peers, the findings help clarify some of the developmental processes that may place children with ADHD at risk for peer problems. The results also suggest that interventions with children with ADHD may benefit from focusing on how problems in one area of functioning may “spill over” into other areas of functioning.
Acknowledgments
Data analysis and preparation of this article was supported by grant number MH65899 from the National Institute of Mental Health to the second author. The data used in the current paper were drawn from a larger study conducted by the MTA Cooperative Group and supported by the National Institute of Mental Health, the National Institute on Drug Abuse, the Department of Justice, and the Department of Education. (Grant numbers: U01MH50440, U01MH50447, U01MH50453, U01MH50454, U01MH50461, U01MH50467; and contract numbers: N01MH12004, N01MH12007, N01MH12008, N01MH12009, N01MH12010, N01MH12011, N01MH12012). The views expressed in this paper are solely those of the authors, and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Institute of Mental Health, the National Institute on Drug Abuse, the Department of Justice, or the Department of Education.
Footnotes
The Multimodal Treatment Study of Children with ADHD (MTA) was a National Institute of Mental health (NIMH) cooperative agreement randomized clinical trial, then continued under an NIMH contract as a follow-up study. Collaborators from the National Institute of Mental Health: Benedetto Vitiello, M.D. (Child & Adolescent Treatment and Preventive Interventions Research Branch), Joanne B. Severe, M.S. (Clinical Trials Operations and Biostatistics Unit, Division of Services and Intervention Research), Peter S. Jensen, M.D. (currently at the REACH Institute and the Mayo Clinic, Rochester), L. Eugene Arnold, M.D., M.Ed. (currently at Ohio State University), Kimberly Hoagwood, Ph.D. (currently at Columbia); previous contributors from NIMH to the early phases: John Richters, Ph.D. (currently at National Institute of Nursing Research); Donald Vereen, M.D. (currently at National Institute on Drug Abuse). Principal investigators and co-investigators from the sites are: University of California, Berkeley/San Francisco: Stephen P. Hinshaw, Ph.D. (Berkeley), Glen R. Elliott, Ph.D., M.D. (San Francisco); Duke University: Karen C. Wells, Ph.D., Jeffery Epstein, Ph.D.; previous Duke contributors to early phases: C. Keith Conners, Ph.D. (former PI); John March, M.D., M.P.H.; University of California, Irvine: James Swanson, Ph.D., Timothy Wigal, Ph.D.; previous contributor from UCLA to the early phases: Dennis P. Cantwell, M.D. (deceased); Long Island Jewish Medical Center/New York University: Howard B. Abikoff, Ph.D.; Montreal Children’s Hospital/McGill University: Lily Hechtman, M.D.; New York State Psychiatric Institute/Columbia University/Mount Sinai Medical Center: Laurence L. Greenhill, M.D. (Columbia), Jeffrey H. Newcorn, M.D. (Mount Sinai School of Medicine). University of Pittsburgh: Brooke Molina, Ph.D., Betsy Hoza, Ph.D. (currently at University of Vermont), William E. Pelham, Ph.D. (PI for early phases, currently at State University of New York, Buffalo). Follow-up phase statistical collaborators: Robert D. Gibbons, Ph.D. (University of Illinois, Chicago); Sue Marcus, Ph.D (Mt. Sinai College of Medicine) Kwan Hur, Ph.D. (University of Illinois, Chicago). Original study statistical and design consultant: Helena C. Kraemer, Ph.D. (Stanford University). Collaborator from the Office of Special Education Programs/US Department of Education: Thomas Hanley, Ed.D. Collaborator from Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention/Department of Justice: Karen Stern, Ph.D.
One potential concern regarding the use of teacher reports to assess social and behavioral competence, social skills, and rejection at Time 4 is that high school teachers may not know their students well enough to provide valid reports. Evidence regarding the validity of teacher reports of social and behavioral competence at Time 4 in this sample are reported elsewhere (Hoza et al., in press). At each of the four time points, some students had teacher reports on each measure available from more than one teacher. Thus, to address the validity of teacher reports of social skills and rejection at Time 4, we compared the strength of the correlations between the two teacher reports at Time 4 to the strength of the associations among these variables at the other three time points. Results indicated that the correlations between the two teachers’ ratings at Times 1, 2, 3, and 4 for social skills were r = .44, p < .001, r = .49, p < .001, r = .46, p < .001, and r = .48, p < .001, respectively. Correlations between the two teachers’ ratings at Times 1, 2, 3, and 4 for peer rejection were r = .23, p = .06 (there were only 71 participants who had reports from two teachers at 24 months; as a result, this correlation was relatively underpowered), r = .38, p < .001, r = .27, p < .001, and r = .24, p < .001, respectively. These results do not suggest a decrease in agreement among teachers at Time 4 as compared with the other three assessment periods. Overall, these results suggest that teachers are able to provide reports of participants’ competence in the later years of the study.
Previous research with the MTA study did not find treatment effects on attrition at Time 4 (Molina et al., 2009). However, our analyses differed from those presented by Molina and colleagues in two ways. First, the analyses for this paper are based on a subsample of the MTA study; specifically, only participants who had data on relevant study variables for this study at one of the four time points were included in our attrition analyses. In contrast, Molina et al. examined attrition for the entire sample. Second, in contrast to the analyses conducted by Molina and colleagues, participants who did not have data available on relevant study variables at Time 4 were included in the attrition group, even if they had data available on other MTA study variables. Importantly, although Molina and colleagues did not find a significant effect of treatment group on attrition at Time 4, the findings approached conventional levels of statistical significance (p = .06) and the findings were in the same direction as those reported in this paper.
Hoza et al. (2004) did not find treatment effects on a multivariate test of positive illusions in the scholastic, social, athletic, appearance, and behavioral domains at Time 1. Since the overall multivariate test was not significant, the authors did not examine specific univariate tests. However, our multivariate test with different outcome variables was significant; as a result, we examined the follow-up analyses, and there was a significant effect of treatment group on positive illusions in the behavioral domain.
Contributor Information
Dianna Murray-Close, University of Vermont.
Betsy Hoza, University of Vermont.
Stephen P. Hinshaw, University of California, Berkeley
L Eugene Arnold, Ohio State University.
James Swanson, University of California, Irvine.
Peter S. Jensen, Mayo Clinic & The Reach Institute
Lily Hechtman, Montreal Children’s Hospital.
Karen Wells, Duke University.
References
- Bagwell CL, Molina BSG, Pelham WE, Hoza B. Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and problems in peer relations: Predictions from childhood to adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2001;40:1285–1292. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200111000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bagwell CL, Newcomb AF, Bukowski WM. Preadolescent friendship and peer rejection as predictors of adult adjustment. Child Development. 1998;69:140–153. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baumeister RF, Smart L, Boden JM. Relation of threatened egotism to violence and aggression: The dark side of high self-esteem. Psychological Review. 1996;103:5–33. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.103.1.5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berndt TJ. Transitions in friendship and friends’ influence. In: Graber JA, Brooks-Gunn J, editors. Transitions Through Adolescence: Interpersonal Domains and Contexts. Hillsdale: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates; 1996. pp. 57–84. [Google Scholar]
- Bierman KL. The Guilford series on social and emotional development. New York, NY, US: Guilford Press; 2004. Peer rejection: Developmental processes and intervention strategies. [Google Scholar]
- Blachman DR, Hinshaw SP. Patterns of friendship among girls with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2002;30:625–640. doi: 10.1023/a:1020815814973. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bukowski WM, Kramer TL. Judgments of the features of friendships among early adolescent girls and boys. Journal of Early Adolescence. 1986;6:331–338. [Google Scholar]
- Burt KB, Obradović J, Long JD, Masten AS. The interplay of social competence and psychopathology over 20 years: Testing transactional and cascade models. Child Development. 2008;79:359–374. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01130.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheung GW, Rensvold RB. Evaluating goodness-of-fit indices for testing measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling. 2002;9:233–255. [Google Scholar]
- Cicchetti D, Cohen DJ. Risk, disorder, and adaptation. 2. Vol. 3. Hoboken, NJ, US: John Wiley & Sons Inc; 2006. Developmental psychopathology. [Google Scholar]
- Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA. A developmental psychopathology perspective on adolescence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2002;70:6–20. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.70.1.6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cillessen AHN, Mayeux L. From censure to reinforcement: Developmental changes in the association between aggression and social status. Child Development. 2004;75:147–163. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00660.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coie, Dodge Continuities and changes in children’s social status: A five-year longitudinal study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 1983;29:261–281. [Google Scholar]
- Coie JD, Dodge KA. Aggression and antisocial behavior. In: Damon W, Eisenberg N, editors. Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development. 5. New York: Wiley; 1998. pp. 779–862. [Google Scholar]
- Colvin CR, Block J, Funder DC. Psychometric truths in the absence of psychological meaning: A reply to Zuckerman and Knee. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1996;70:1252–1255. [Google Scholar]
- Crick NR, Dodge KA. A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin. 1994;115:74–101. [Google Scholar]
- Diamantopoulou S, Rydell AM, Henricsson L. Can both low and high self-esteem be related to aggression in children? Social Development. 2008;17:682–698. [Google Scholar]
- Diener MB, Milich R. Effects of positive feedback on the social interactions of boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A test of the self-protective hypothesis. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 1997;26:256–265. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp2603_4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dishion T. The peer context of troublesome child and adolescent behavior. In: Leone P, editor. Understanding troubled and troubling youth. Vol. 116. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 1990. pp. 128–153. [Google Scholar]
- Dodge KA, Lansford JE, Burks VS, Bates JE, Pettit GS, Fontaine R, Price JM. Peer rejection and social information-processing factors in the development of aggressive behavior problems in children. Child Development. 2003;74:374–393. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.7402004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erhardt D, Hinshaw SP. Initial sociometric impressions of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and comparison boys: Predictions from social behaviors and from nonbehavioral variables. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1994;62:833–842. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.62.4.833. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gresham FM, Elliott SN. Social Skills Rating System Manual. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Harrist AW, Zaia AF, Bates JE, Dodge KA, Pettit GS. Subtypes of social withdrawal in early childhood: Sociometric status and social-cognitive differences across four years. Child Development. 1997;68:278–294. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harter S. The perceived competence scale for children. Child Development. 1982;53:87–97. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harter S. Unpublished manuscript. University of Denver; 1985. Manual for the Self-Perception Profile for Children. [Google Scholar]
- Harter S. Unpublished manuscript. University of Denver; 1988. The Self-Perception Profile for Adolescents. [Google Scholar]
- Harter S. The cognitive and social construction of the developing self. New York: Guilford; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Harter S, Pike R. The pictorial scale of perceived competence and social acceptance for young children. Child Development. 1984;55:1969–1982. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hartup WW. Friendships and their developmental significance. In: McGurk H, editor. Childhood and Social Development: Contemporary Perspectives. London: Erlbaum; 1992. pp. 175–205. [Google Scholar]
- Hartup WW, Sancilio MF. Children’s friendships. In: Schopler E, Mesibov GB, editors. Social behavior in autism. New York: Plenum Press; 1986. pp. 61–79. [Google Scholar]
- Hay DF, Payne A, Chadwick A. Peer relations in childhood. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry. 2004;45:84–108. doi: 10.1046/j.0021-9630.2003.00308.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hinshaw SP. Is ADHD an impairing condition in childhood and adolescence? In: Jensen PS, Cooper JR, editors. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: State of the science; best practices. Kingston, NJ: Civic Research Institute; 2002. pp. 5-1–5-21. [Google Scholar]
- Hinshaw SP, March JS, Abikoff H, Arnold LE, Cantwell DP, Conners CK, et al. Comprehensive assessment of childhood attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in the context of a multisite, multimodal clinical trial. Journal of Attention Disorders. 1997;1:217–234. [Google Scholar]
- Hoza B. Peer functioning in children with ADHD. Ambulatory Pediatrics. 2007;7:101–106. doi: 10.1016/j.ambp.2006.04.011. By special arrangement, published jointly in. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 32:719–727. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsn012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoza B, Gerdes AC, Hinshaw SP, Arnold LE, Pelham WE, Molina BSG, et al. Self-perceptions of competence in children with ADHD and comparison children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2004;72:382–391. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.3.382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoza B, Gerdes AC, Mrug S, Hinshaw SP, Bukowski WM, Gold JA, et al. Peer-assessed outcomes in the Multimodal Treatment Study of Children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2005;34:74–86. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3401_7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoza B, Mrug S, Gerdes AC, Hinshaw SP, Bukowski WM, Gold JA, et al. What aspects of peer relationships are impaired in children with ADHD? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2005;73:411–423. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.73.3.411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoza B, Murray-Close D, Arnold LE, Hinshaw SP The MTA Cooperative Group. Time-dependent changes in positive illusory self-perceptions of children with ADHD: A developmental psychopathology perspective. (in press) [Google Scholar]
- Hoza B, Owens JS, Pelham WE. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. In: Ammerman RT, Hersen M, Last CG, editors. Handbook of prescriptive treatments for children and adolescents. 2. Boston: Allyn & Bacon; 1999. pp. 63–83. [Google Scholar]
- Hoza B, Pelham WE, Dobbs J, Owens JS, Pillow DR. Do boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder have positive illusory self-concepts? Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2002;111:268–278. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.111.2.268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoza B, Tomb M, Kaiser NM, Waschbusch DA, Murray-Close A. Explaining social deficits in ADHD. 2009. Manuscript in preparation. [Google Scholar]
- Hu L, Benter PM. Cutoff criteria for indexes in convariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling. 1999;6:1–55. [Google Scholar]
- Hughes C, Dunn J. Understanding mind and emotion: Longitudinal associations with mental-state talk between young friends. Developmental Psychology. 1998;34:1026–1037. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.34.5.1026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hymel S, Bowker A, Woody E. Aggressive versus withdrawn unpopular children: Variations in peer and self-perceptions in multiple domains. Child Development. 1993;64:879–896. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1993.tb02949.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ialongo NS, Vaden-Kiernan N, Kellam S. Early peer rejection and aggression: Longitudinal relations with adolescent behavior. Journal of Developmental & Physical Disabilities. 1998;10:199–213. [Google Scholar]
- Kaiser NM, Hoza B, Pelham WE, Gnagy E, Greiner AR. ADHD status and degree of positive illusions: Moderational and mediational relations with actual behavior. Journal of Attention Disorders. 2008;12:227–238. doi: 10.1177/1087054707311661. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kline RB. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling. 2. New York: Guilford Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Kupersmidt JB, Burchinal M, Patterson CJ. Developmental patterns of childhood peer relations as predictors of externalizing behavior problems. Development & Psychopathology. 1995;7:825–843. [Google Scholar]
- Little SA, Garber J. Aggression, depression, and stressful life events predicting peer rejection in children. Development & Psychopathology. 1995;7:845–856. [Google Scholar]
- Little TD, Jones SM, Henrich CC, Hawley PH. Disentangling the “whys” from the “whats” of aggressive behavior. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2003;27:122–133. [Google Scholar]
- Maguire MC, Dunn J. Friendships in early childhood and social understanding. International Journal of Behavioural Development. 1997;21:669–686. [Google Scholar]
- Marsh HW, Craven R, Debus R. Structure, stability, and development of children’s self-concepts: A multicohort-multioccasion study. Child Development. 1998;69:1030–1053. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Masten AS, Coatsworth JD. The development of competence in favorable and unfavorable environments: Lessons from research on successful children. American Psychologist. 1998;53:205–220. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.53.2.205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Masten AS, Roisman GI, Long JD, Burt KB, Obradović J, Riley JR, Boelcke-Stennes K, Tellegen A. Developmental cascades: Linking academic achievement and externalizing and internalizing symptoms over 20 years. Developmental Psychology. 2005;41:733–746. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.41.5.733. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Messer B, Harter S. Unpublished manuscript. University of Denver; 1989. The Self-Perception Profile for Adults. [Google Scholar]
- Molina BSG, Hinshaw SP, Swanson JM, Arnold LE, Vitiello B, Jensen PS, Epstein JM, Hoza B, et al. MTA at 8 years: Follow-up of children treated for combined-type ADHD in a multisite study. Journal of American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2009;48:484–500. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e31819c23d0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mrug S, Hoza B, Pelham WE, Gnagy EM, Greiner AR. Behavior and peer status in children with ADHD: Continuity and change. Journal of Attention Disorders. 2007;10:359–371. doi: 10.1177/1087054706288117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MTA Cooperative Group. A 14-month randomized clinical trial of treatment strategies for Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1999;56:1073–1086. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.12.1073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muthén BO. Latent variable modeling in heterogeneous populations. Psychometrika. 1989;54:557–585. [Google Scholar]
- Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus user’s guide. 5. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; 1998–2007. [Google Scholar]
- Newcomb AF, Bagwell CL. Children’s friendship relations: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin. 1995;117:306–347. [Google Scholar]
- Newcomb AF, Bukowski WM, Pattee L. Children’s peer relations: A meta-analytic review of popular, rejected, neglected, controversial, and average sociometric status. Psychological Bulletin. 1993;113:99–128. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.113.1.99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ostrov JM, Crick NR. Forms and functions of aggression during early childhood: A short-term longitudinal study. School Psychology Review. 2007;36:22–43. [Google Scholar]
- Owens EB, Hinshaw SP, Lee SS, Lahey BB. Few girls with childhood Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder show positive adjustment during adolescence. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 2009;38:132–143. doi: 10.1080/15374410802575313. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Owens JS, Goldfine ME, Evangelista NM, Hoza B, Kaiser NM. A critical review of self-perceptions and the positive illusory bias in children with ADHD. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 2007;10:335–351. doi: 10.1007/s10567-007-0027-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pelham WE, Bender ME. Peer relationships in hyperactive children: Description and treatment. In: Gadow KD, Bialer I, editors. Advances in Learning and Behavioral Disabilities. Vol. 1. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press; 1982. pp. 365–436. [Google Scholar]
- Pettit GS, Clawson MA, Dodge KA, Bates JE. Stability and change in peer-rejected status: The role of child behavior, parenting, and family ecology. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 1996;42:267–294. [Google Scholar]
- Pope AW, Bierman Karen L. Predicting adolescent peer problems and antisocial activities: The relative roles of aggression and dysregulation. Developmental Psychology. 1999;35:335–346. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.35.2.335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rubin KH, Bukowski WM, Parker JG. Peer interactions, relationships, and groups. In: Damon W, Lerner R, Eisenberg N, editors. Handbook of child psychology. Vol. 3: Social, emotional, and personality development. New York: Wiley; 2006. pp. 571–645. [Google Scholar]
- Rudolph KD, Clark AG. Conceptions of relationships in children with depressive and aggressive symptoms: Social-cognitive distortions or reality? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2001;29:41–56. doi: 10.1023/a:1005299429060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Satorra A. Scaled and adjusted restricted tests in multi-sample analysis of moment structures. In: Heijmans RDH, Pollock DSG, Satorra A, editors. Innovations in multivariate statistical analysis: A festschrift for Heinz Neudecker. London: Kluwer Academic; 2000. pp. 233–247. [Google Scholar]
- Sroufe LA. Psychopathology as an outcome of development. Development and Psychopathology. 1997;9:251–268. doi: 10.1017/s0954579497002046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sroufe LA, Rutter M. The domain of developmental psychopathology. Child Development. 1984;55:17–29. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor SE, Brown JD. Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin. 1988;103:193–210. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tisak J, Smith CS. Defending and extending difference score methods. Journal of Management. 1994a;20:675–682. [Google Scholar]
- Tisak J, Smith CS. Rejoinder to Edwards’ comments. Journal of Management. 1994b;20:691–694. [Google Scholar]
- Vaillencourt T, Hymel S. Aggression and social status: The moderating roles of sex and peer-valued characteristics. Aggressive Behavior. 2006;32:396–408. [Google Scholar]
- Warden D, Mackinnon S. Prosocial children, bullies and victims: An investigation of their sociometric status, empathy and social problem-solving strategies. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 2003;21:367–385. [Google Scholar]
- Zakriski AL, Coie JD. A comparison of aggressive-rejected and Nonaggressive rejected children’s interpretations of self-directed and other-directed rejection. Child Development. 1996;67:1048–1070. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]