Abstract
Neighborhood socioeconomic status (SES) is related to a wide range of health outcomes, but existing research is dominated by cross-sectional study designs, which are particularly vulnerable to bias by unmeasured characteristics related to both residential location decisions and health-related outcomes. Further, little is known about the mechanisms by which neighborhood SES might influence health. Therefore, we estimated longitudinal relationships between neighborhood SES and physical activity (PA), a theorized mediator of the neighborhood SES-health association. We used data from four years of the Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) study (n=5,115, 18–30 years at baseline, 1985–86), a cohort of U.S. young adults followed over 15 years, and a time-varying geographic information system. Using two longitudinal modeling strategies, this is the first study to explicitly examine how the estimated association between neighborhood SES (deprivation) and PA is biased by (a) measured characteristics theorized to influence residential decisions (e.g., controlling for individual SES, marriage, and children in random effects models), and (b) time-invariant, unmeasured characteristics (e.g., controlling for unmeasured motivation to exercise that is constant over time using repeated measures regression modeling, conditioned on the individual). After controlling for sociodemographics (age, sex, race) and individual SES, associations between higher neighborhood deprivation and lower PA were strong and incremental in blacks, but less consistent in whites. Furthermore, adjustment for measured characteristics beyond sociodemographics and individual SES had little influence on the estimated associations; adjustment for unmeasured characteristics attenuated negative associations more strongly in whites than in blacks.
Keywords: Environment design, socioeconomic status, physical activity, epidemiologic methods, confounding factors, USA, race
INTRODUCTION
Neighborhood socioeconomic status (SES) such as census-tract level poverty or composite measures are consistently associated with numerous health outcomes, including mortality (Subramanian, Chen, Rehkopf, Waterman, & Krieger, 2005), general health (Do, 2009), and cardiovascular disease (Diez Roux, Merkin, Arnett, Chambless, Massing, Nieto et al., 2001). Theorized mechanisms by which neighborhood SES influences health (Diez Roux, 2007; R.J. Sampson, Morenoff, & Gannon-Rowley, 2002) include mediation by health behaviors through inequitable access to physical activity (PA) opportunities, healthy foods, or health care (structural perspective) or through establishment of social norms (contagion perspective) (Ross, 2000), or direct, cumulative biological effects of chronic stress (Cox, Boyle, Davey, Feng, & Morris, 2007; Merkin, Basurto-Davila, Karlamangla, Bird, Lurie, Escarce et al., 2009). While there is an international literature on this topic (e.g, (Boyle, Norman, & Rees, 2002; Curtis, Setia, & Quesnel-Vallee, 2009)), we focus on the U.S., given our study population and the nature of the research question in a U.S. context.
Existing research largely focuses on the influence of neighborhood exposures on broader health outcomes (e.g., neighborhood poverty as a predictor of mortality), rather than on health behaviors (e.g., neighborhood poverty as a predictor of physical activity [PA]). Physical inactivity and obesity are key outcomes related to neighborhood SES (Do, Dubowitz, Bird, Lurie, Escarce, & Finch, 2007; Lee, Cubbin, & Winkleby, 2007; Wen & Zhang, 2009) and in countries like the U.S. exhibit dramatic racial and socioeconomic disparities (Gordon-Larsen, McMurray, & Popkin, 1999; Ogden, Carroll, Curtin, McDowell, Tabak, & Flegal, 2006) which may result in part from differences in structural (e.g., built environment) (Gordon-Larsen, Nelson, Page, & Popkin, 2006; Moore, Diez Roux, Evenson, McGinn, & Brines, 2008), contagion, or stress-related factors. While neighborhood SES and physical fitness at a single time point has been examined using the U.S.-based Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) Study (Shishehbor, Gordon-Larsen, Kiefe, & Litaker, 2008), PA is a modifiable behavior that is amenable to intervention, whereas fitness is influenced by physiological factors.
Additionally, a major limitation of existing research examining neighborhood influences on health and related behaviors is potential bias resulting from self-selection into neighborhoods (Boone-Heinonen, Gordon-Larsen, Guilkey, Jacobs, & Popkin; Diez Roux, 2004Oakes, 2004; van Lenthe, Martikainen, & Mackenbach, 2007). Briefly, factors such as financial resources and household structure (e.g., marital status, children) influence not only where people are able (through affordability or other constraints) or prefer to live (Clark & Ledwith, 2007; Geist & McManus, 2008; Lund, 2006), but also health behaviors (Bell & Lee, 2005; Yannakoulia, Panagiotakos, Pitsavos, Skoumas, & Stafanadis, 2008) and outcomes (Gordon-Larsen, Adair, & Popkin, 2003; Sobal, Rauschenbach, & Frongillo, 2003). Without accounting for factors which influence residential mobility or location decisions, neighborhood SES-health associations could be biased and incorrectly interpreted as neighborhood influence on health. Yet, studies investigating neighborhood influences on PA – which generally stem from the built environment literature (Papas, Alberg, Ewing, Helzlsouer, Gary, & Klassen, 2007), as opposed to demographic and geographic studies (e.g., (Curtis et al., 2009)) -- generally control for individual SES but not other observed characteristics related to residential selection such as marriage and children.
Furthermore, key drivers of residential self-selection may be difficult or impossible to measure. For example, unmeasured characteristics of individuals who are more likely to select a neighborhood with high quality schools (in the U.S., generally in high SES areas) may also influence adoption of physically active lifestyles. Therefore, unmeasured characteristics may bias traditional, covariate-adjusted estimates of how neighborhood SES influences healthy behaviors. In contrast, with longitudinal data, unmeasured characteristics that are stable over time (time-invariant) can be addressed with within-person estimators (e.g., first difference and fixed effects models), which condition on the individual, thereby exploiting variation observed within person, over time (Boone-Heinonen et al.; Do & Finch, 2008; Eid, Overman, Puga, & Turner, 2008). While within-person estimators do not address dynamic feedback processes in which health may influence subsequent residential selection (Boyle et al., 2002; Curtis et al., 2009), they are uniquely suited to control for unmeasured confounders.
However, few neighborhood health studies have the longitudinal exposure and outcome data necessary to estimate within-person effects. The vast majority of neighborhood health research in the U.S. and elsewhere is cross-sectional, and the few existing longitudinal studies examine general health measures (Do & Finch, 2008) rather than behaviors that might mediate general health, such as PA. In addition, they do not always take advantage of the potentialities of a repeated measures design for addressing unmeasured confounders. Controlling for measured confounders related to residential self-selection and within-person estimation can provide insights into possible causal processes linking neighborhoods and health and into the sensitivity of studies to omission of these unmeasured confounders.
Therefore, we capitalized on longitudinal neighborhood and behavior data from four CARDIA study examinations to investigate how the estimated association between neighborhood SES and PA is influenced by controlling for the confounding effects of (a) measured characteristics related to residential selection in a large body of mobility research (e.g., individual SES, marriage, and children), and (b) unmeasured characteristics which are constant over time.
METHODS
Study Population and Data Sources
The CARDIA Study is a population-based prospective epidemiologic study of the determinants and evolution of cardiovascular risk factors among black and white young adults. At baseline (1985–6), 5,115 eligible subjects, aged 18–30 years, were enrolled with balance according to race (black, white), gender, education (≤ and >high school) and age (18–24 and 25–30 years) from four U.S. communities: Birmingham, Alabama; Chicago, Illinois; Minneapolis, Minnesota; and Oakland, California. Specific recruitment procedures were described elsewhere (Hughes, Cutter, Donahue, Friedman, Hulley, Hunkeler et al., 1987). Study data were collected under protocols approved by Institutional Review Boards at each study center and the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill. Follow-up examinations conducted in 1987–1998 (Year 2), 1990–1991 (Year 5), 1992–1993 (year 7), 1995–1996 (year 10), and 2000–2001 (year 15) had retention rates of 90%, 86%, 81%, 79%, and 74% of the surviving cohort, respectively.
Using a Geographic Information System, we linked time-varying, community-level, U.S. census data to CARDIA respondent residential locations in exam years 0, 7, 10, and 15 from geocoded home addresses. 48.2, 68.8, and 33.0% of participants moved residential locations between years 0 and 7, 7 and 10, and 10 and 15, respectively.
Of the possible 20,460 observations for 5115 participants at baseline across 4 examinations, 4,400 observations were missing due to loss to follow-up (including mortality): 80, 77, and 72% of the initial participants were observed at years 7, 10, and 15, respectively. Of remaining observations, we excluded observations for women who were pregnant at the time of examination (n=114 observations), and with missing PA (n=126 observations), neighborhood SES variables (n=86 observations) or covariate data (n=274 additional observations). Those lost to follow-up or missing data were generally more likely black, male, younger, and of lower baseline education (p<0.05); however, attrition (except for year 7, p=0.02) and missing data were unrelated to baseline PA and, to the extent that attrition and missing data are related to unobserved fixed characteristics of the individuals, our fixed effects models may mitigate selection bias. The final analytical sample totaled 15,460 observations for 4,179 individuals.
Neighborhood socioeconomic measures
Several commonly used neighborhood socioeconomic measures were approximately time-matched to each examination period (CARDIA year, Census: Year 0, 1980; Years 7 and 10, 1990; Year 15, 2000). Census tracts were used to define neighborhoods because they are consistent with prior research, block groups were not universally implemented until the 1990 census, and we theorized counties as too large to capture the neighborhood environment.
Measures of socioeconomic disadvantage included percent of persons with income less than 150% of federal poverty level [1.5 times federal poverty level (Krieger, Zierler, Hogan, Waterman, Chen, Lemieux et al., 2003; U.S. Census Bureau, 2009)] and percent of persons aged 25 years and over with less than high school level of education. Because nearly the entire study population (>96%) resided in metropolitan areas, we used the 150% poverty cutpoint to account for higher cost of living in urban areas; results using the 100% poverty cutpoint were similar but slightly weaker. Other SES measures included percent of persons ≥25 years with college degrees and median household income, which, for comparability across exam periods, was inflated to reflect the value of U.S. dollars in the year 2000, based on the Consumer Price Index.
Exploratory factor analysis was used to summarize neighborhood SES exposures and indicated that the four measures represented a single construct (factor) with similar factor loadings across exam years (electronic appendix 1, Table A1.1, available with the online version of paper). Therefore, factor analysis was performed in data pooled across years. Higher factor scores represent higher neighborhood deprivation, indicated by higher neighborhood poverty and proportion with <high school education and lower median household income and proportion with a college education. Each respondent’s score on this factor was used as a composite neighborhood deprivation measure.
Outcome: physical activity index
At each examination, frequency of participation in 13 categories of moderate and vigorous recreational sports, exercise, leisure, and occupational activities (electronic appendix 1, Table A1.2, available with the online version of paper) over the previous 12 months was ascertained by an interviewer-administered questionnaire designed for CARDIA. As described elsewhere (Jacobs, Hahn, Haskell, Pirie, & Sidney, 1989), PA scores were calculated in exercise units based on frequency and intensity of each activity. Reliability and validity of the instrument is comparable to other activity questionnaires (Jacobs Jr., Ainsworth, Hartman, & Leon, 1993; Jacobs Jr., Hahn, Haskell, Pirie, & Sidney, 1989). We excluded occupational and household PA from our PA score because they were not theorized to be influenced by the neighborhood environment.
Individual-level covariates
Individual-level baseline characteristics included age (mean centered), race (white, black), and study center (Birmingham, Chicago, Minneapolis, Oakland). Education at Year 7, after most individuals attained their highest education level, was examined as a time invariant variable; Year 0 education was used if Year 7 education was missing. Time-varying individual-level characteristics included income, marital status (married, not married), and children or stepchildren 18 years or younger living in the household (any, none). Income was examined as a categorical variable (approximate tertiles: <$25,000, $25,000–49,900, >$49,900) because of a non-linear relationship with PA. Income was not collected in year 0 or 2, so the closest measurement (year 5) was analyzed. To avoid over-adjustment and induction of selection bias, we did not control for BMI, a theorized outcome of PA.
Statistical analysis
Effects of neighborhood deprivation on PA score throughout young to middle adulthood were estimated in a series of longitudinal random and fixed effects linear models. Conditioned on the individual, fixed effect models account for time-invariant unmeasured variables (e.g., motivation to exercise that remains constant over time) which may be related to both PA and neighborhood deprivation (Boone-Heinonen et al.). By analyzing within-person variation observed in repeated measures over time, each individual in essence serves as his/her own control. In contrast, random effects models (random person-level intercept) (Rabe-Hesketh & Skrondal, 2008) analyze variation both within and between individuals; they do not control for possible correlation between observed and unmeasured characteristics and are therefore most comparable to cross-sectional associations reported in prior research. Random slopes were not estimated in order to maintain comparability with fixed effects estimates. See electronic appendix 2 (available with the online version of paper) for a detailed discussion of random and fixed effects models. All models were fit using the Stata 10.1 xtreg function, using the “fe” option for fixed effects models (StataCorp, 2005). The Hausman specification test formally compared fixed and random effects estimates.
In race-stratified random and fixed effect models, PA scores were modeled as a function of three cumulative sets of confounders which were theorized to influence residential selection: Model 1 included concurrent neighborhood deprivation, age and study center; Model 2 added individual-level education and time-varying income; and Model 3 further added time-varying marital status and children in household.
The built environment – such as PA facilities (Diez Roux, Evenson, McGinn, Brown, Moore, Brines et al., 2007; Gordon-Larsen et al., 2006), parks (Bedimo-Rung, Mowen, & Cohen, 2005), and pedestrian infrastructure (Krizek & Johnson, 2006) – and crime (Foster & Giles-Corti, 2008) were theorized to mediate the relationship between neighborhood deprivation and PA and thus not included in our models. Concurrent (versus time lagged) neighborhood deprivation was examined because the neighborhood socioeconomic environment was theorized to represent relatively immediate environmental PA influences. Coefficients for time-invariant individual-level variables (study center, education, sex) are estimated in random effects models but not fixed effects models.
PA scores were natural log-transformed to address skewness, so model coefficients were interpreted as the percent change in PA score expected from a 1-unit change in the corresponding independent variable; analysis of the continuous outcome variable allowed examination of changes across the full distribution of PA. To examine non-linear relationships between neighborhood deprivation and PA (assessed graphically and through testing of higher order terms), neighborhood deprivation was modeled as quartiles (in observations pooled over time). Quartiles were race-specific because, due to residential patterning by race in the U.S. (R. J. Sampson & Sharkey, 2008), there was limited overlap in neighborhood deprivation between whites and blacks. Sex interactions with each independent variable were tested using backward elimination; for comparability, interaction terms were retained if significant (likelihood ratio test, p<0.10) in Models 1, 2, or 3 in random or fixed effects models. Time interactions with neighborhood deprivation in Model 1 (using baseline instead of time varying age to avoid collinearity) were not significant and thus excluded; time main effects were not included because the sequencing of observations were not important for our study objective.
Because CARDIA respondents lived in different census tracts over time, our data were not nested across three levels (e.g., multiple people per census tract, multiple time periods per person) as required for multi-level models. Therefore, neighborhood deprivation was treated as an individual-level exposure. Our data were sparse (few individuals on average within census tracts) and unbalanced (variable numbers of individuals), with the following number of individuals per census tract, by study center [mean (range)]: Birmingham [10.1 (1, 46)], Chicago [6.3 (1, 47)], Minneapolis [6.3 (1, 173)], Oakland [5.1 (1, 28)] in year 0, declining to [2.7 (1, 17)], [1.4 (1, 13)], [1.8 (1,27)] and [1.6 (1, 11)] by Year 15, respectively. Intraclass correlations were relatively small (0.08 in pooled data).
RESULTS
Compared to blacks, whites exhibited substantially higher PA and individual- and neighborhood-level education and income, were more likely to be married, and, except at Year 15, less likely to have children (Tables 1 and 2). Electronic appendix 1, Table A1.3, available with the online version of the paper, reports neighborhood characteristics within each race-specific neighborhood deprivation quartile.
Table 1.
Individual-level sample characteristics by exam yeara and raceb [mean/% (standard error)]: the Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) Study, 1985–2000
Blacks | Whites | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Year 0 | Year 7 | Year 10 | Year 15 | Year 0 | Year 7 | Year 10 | Year 15 | |
Count | 2,037 | 1,861 | 1,864 | 1,665 | 2,142 | 2,022 | 1,975 | 1,894 |
Female (%) | 57.4 | 53.0 | ||||||
Education (%) | ||||||||
>=College degree | 12.1 | 20.6 | 22.5 | 25.6 | 41.4 | 55.8 | 57.6 | 60.6 |
Some college | 7.5 | 14.2 | 15.1 | 20.6 | 5.5 | 6.6 | 9.3 | 11.0 |
≤High School | 80.5 | 65.2 | 62.4 | 53.8 | 53.1 | 37.5 | 33.1 | 28.4 |
Income (%) | ||||||||
>$50k | 15.3 | 18.2 | 24.1 | 42.7 | 32.2 | 40.8 | 51.6 | 72.4 |
$25–49.9k | 34.1 | 36.3 | 36.5 | 31.4 | 40.8 | 37.1 | 32.7 | 20.0 |
<$25k | 50.6 | 45.5 | 39.4 | 25.9 | 27.0 | 22.1 | 15.7 | 7.6 |
Married (%) | 20.4 | 34.6 | 38.2 | 43.2 | 25.7 | 51.9 | 58.2 | 62.4 |
Childrenc in HH (%) | 38.4 | 56.0 | 58.5 | 58.0 | 16.3 | 41.0 | 49.5 | 56.9 |
PA Index (mean) | 338.6 (278.9) | 261.8 (257.0) | 246.2 (248.3) | 250.5 (258.6) | 392.4 (260.1) | 290.0 (227.0) | 281.0 (228.3) | 293.6 (238.6) |
Age (mean) | 24.4 (3.8) | 31.5 (3.8) | 34.5 (3.8) | 39.6 (3.8) | 25.5 (3.4) | 32.5 (3.4) | 35.6 (3.4) | 40.7 (3.4) |
All variables significantly different by race, within year (p<0.0001) except PA index at year 7 (p=0.0003) and children in household at year 15 (p>0.05)
All variables significantly different at each year compared to year 0, within race (p<0.0001, except income between Year 7 and 0 for blacks, p=0.004)
Children or stepchildren living in household
TABLE 2.
Neighborhood socioeconomic measures, by exam yeara and raceb [median, (interquartile range)]: the Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) Study, 1985–2000
Year 0 | Year 7 | Year 10 | Year 15 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
BLACKS | |||||
%<150% poverty level | 35.1 (25.8, 46.4) | 33.9 (19.8, 45.6) | 27.1 (13.6, 41.9) | 26.7 (13.3, 39.5) | |
difference [t−(t−1)] | 0.1 (−11.1, 7.1) | 0.0 (−15.5, 0.3) | 0.0 (−5.8, 4.4) | ||
%<High School | 38.1 (27.2, 47.3) | 29.8 (17.8, 39.2) | 26.9 (15.6, 37.1) | 21.0 (12.1, 32.0) | |
difference [t−(t−1)] | −7.0 (−15.0, −0.4) | 0.0 (−9.5, 2.2) | −3.8 (−9.7, 1.5) | ||
%≥college degree | 10.0 (5.7, 20.8) | 13.8 (6.6, 23.9) | 15.1 (8.3, 25.3) | 16.5 (9.6, 32.1) | |
difference [t−(t−1)] | 1.9 (−1.0, 7.9) | 0.0 (−2.1, 5.9) | 2.2 (−1.4, 8.2) | ||
median income | 25,811 (20,771, 31,878) | 30,880 (23,050, 40,595) | 33,911 (25,150, 44,636) | 37,813 (27,930, 50,718) | |
difference [t−(t−1)] | 3,562 (−1,565, 11,479) | 0 (−1,110, 10,565) | 2,509 (−1,984, 8,770) | ||
Neighborhood deprivation factor scorec | 0.89 (0.27, 1.28) | 0.56 (−0.17, 1.06) | 0.29 (−0.36, 0.93) | 0.10 (−0.56, 0.76) | |
difference [t−(t−1)] | −0.24 (−0.68, 0.08) | 0.00 (−0.64, 0.09) | −0.16 (−0.47, 0.10) | ||
WHITES | |||||
%<150% poverty level | 17.8 (10.5, 30.9) | 12.7 (8.0, 23.0) | 10.5 (5.9, 18.8) | 10.3 (5.7, 18.2) | |
difference [t−(t−1)] | −2.1 (−11.0, 2.8) | 0.0 (−6.7, 0.0) | −0.2 (−2.3, 1.8) | ||
%<High School | 21.6 (12.0, 29.4) | 12.7 (6.5, 20.2) | 12.1 (6.4, 20.2) | 9.1 (4.3, 15.9) | |
difference [t − (t−1)] | −6.9 (−13.7, −1.4) | 0.0 (−3.8, 2.8) | −2.5 (−6.2, 0.2) | ||
%≥college degree | 26.2 (17.2, 42.8) | 34.5 (18.1, 51.5) | 32.5 (18.3, 48.6) | 39.0 (22.9, 57.1) | |
difference [t−(t−1)] | 6.3 (−0.9, 14.6) | 0.0 (−8.6, 3.7) | 6.2 (1.2, 11.5) | ||
median income | 32,039 (24,979, 42,983) | 44,865 (35,036, 58,026) | 48,336 (37,826, 63,711) | 54,232 (41,750, 71,172) | |
difference [t−(t−1)] | 9,499 (2,524, 21,579) | 0 (−1,291, 13,134) | 4,670 (−337, 11,536) | ||
Neighborhood deprivation factor scorec | −0.02 (−0.54, 0.49) | −0.53 (−1.05, −0.02) | −0.63 (−1.09, −0.09) | −0.83 (−1.37, −0.31) | |
difference [t − (t−1)] | −0.41 (−0.85, −0.12) | 0.00 (−0.40, 0.13) | −0.21 (−0.42, 0.00) |
All variables significantly different by race, within year (p<0.0001)
All differences variables significantly different from 0, within race (p<0.0001, except %≥college degree between year 7 & 10 for blacks, p=0.0034; %<High School between years 7 and 10 for whites, p=0.0118; and %<150% poverty level between years 10 and 15 for whites, p=0.0061).
Higher scores represent higher neighborhood deprivation.
Among blacks, neighborhood deprivation was related to incrementally lower PA, reaching 27% lower PA for those living in neighborhoods with the highest compared to the lowest deprivation (Table 3; random effects Model 1). This relationship did not differ by sex and was attenuated after controlling for individual income and education (Model 2), with no further attenuation after controlling for marriage and children (Model 3). After additionally controlling for time-invariant unmeasured characteristics using fixed effects models, estimates were slightly attenuated (Hausman p≤0.0003) compared to individual SES-adjusted random effects estimates (Table 3; fixed effects Model 1, random effects Model 2). There was little attenuation after controlling for additional time-varying covariates (Models 2 and 3).
TABLE 3.
Longitudinal relationship between composite neighborhood deprivation factor and ln(physical activity) [coefficient (95% CI)] – Blacks: the Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) Study, 1985–2000a
Model 1b | Model 2c | Model 3d | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Males | Females | Males | Females | ||
Random effects | |||||
Neighborhood deprivatione quartiles (vs. 1st quartile) | |||||
2 | −0.13 (−0.21, −0.04) | −0.08 (−0.17, 0.00) | −0.09 (−0.17, 0.00) | ||
3 | −0.24 (−0.33, −0.16) | −0.17 (−0.26, −0.08) | −0.18 (−0.27, −0.09) | ||
4 | −0.27 (−0.37, −0.17) | −0.18 (−0.28, −0.08) | −0.19 (−0.29, −0.09) | ||
Education (vs. ≥college degree) | |||||
some college | 0.27 (0.04, 0.50) | −0.15 (−0.33, 0.04) | −0.39 (−0.69, −0.10) | −0.12 (−0.31, 0.06) | |
≤High School | −0.08 (−0.24, 0.09) | −0.31 (−0.45, −0.17) | −0.19 (−0.41, 0.02) | −0.27 (−0.41, −0.13) | |
Income (vs.≥$50k) | |||||
$25–49.9k | −0.17 (−0.25, −0.08) | −0.22 (−0.30, −0.13) | |||
<$25k | −0.32 (−0.41, −0.22) | −0.39 (−0.49, −0.30) | |||
Married (vs. not married) | −0.21 (−0.29, −0.13) | ||||
Children <18 years (vs. none) | 0.09 (−0.02, 0.20) | −0.10 (−0.20, −0.01) | |||
Fixed effectsf | |||||
Neighborhood deprivatione quartiles (vs. 1st quartile) | |||||
2 | −0.04 (−0.14, 0.06) | −0.03 (−0.13, 0.07) | −0.04 (−0.14, 0.06) | ||
3 | −0.12 (−0.22, −0.01) | −0.10 (−0.21, 0.01) | −0.12 (−0.22, −0.01) | ||
4 | −0.16 (−0.28, −0.04) | −0.14 (−0.26, −0.02) | −0.16 (−0.28, −0.04) | ||
Income (vs. ≥$50k) | |||||
$25–49.9k | −0.14 (−0.24, −0.04) | −0.19 (−0.29, −0.09) | |||
<$25k | −0.20 (−0.32, −0.08) | −0.27 (−0.40, −0.15) | |||
Married (vs. not married) | −0.23 (−0.32, −0.13) | ||||
Children <18 years (vs. none) | −0.02 (−0.15, 0.12) | −0.15 (−0.27, −0.02) |
CARDIA Study data from years 0, 7, 10, and 15. Physical activity was natural log transformed to address skewness. A single estimate indicates no significant sex interaction in any of the three models. Bold font indicates statistical significance (p<0.05)
Model 1 adjusts for study center, age, age2, age3, sex, interaction between sex and age terms, and interaction between sex and study center
Model 2 adjusts for Model 1 variables, education, income, and interaction between sex and education
Model 3 adjusts for Model 1 and 2 variables, marital status, any children <18 living in the household, and interaction between sex and children in household
Higher scores represent higher neighborhood deprivation.
Fixed effects models do not include time invariant characteristics, including education, which showed small variability over time.
In contrast, among whites, we observed a weak association (or no association) between neighborhood deprivation and lower PA (Table 4; random effects Model 1). Neighborhood deprivation-PA relationships differed by sex in fixed effects models; for comparability, sex interactions were included in random effects models. Associations were attenuated after controlling for individual SES (Model 2), with no additional attenuation after controlling for marriage and children (Model 3). Fixed versus random effects estimates differed significantly (Hausman p<0.0001), with substantial attenuation to Model 1 estimates. Similar to blacks, controlling for time-varying covariates in fixed effects Models 2 and 3 resulted in minimal additional attenuation.
TABLE 4.
Longitudinal relationship between composite neighborhood deprivation factor and ln(physical activity) [coefficient (95% CI)] – Whites: Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) Study, 1985–2000a
Model 1b | Model 2c | Model 3d | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Males | Females | Males | Females | Males | Females | |
Random effects | ||||||
Neighborhood deprivatione quartiles (vs. 1st quartile) | ||||||
2 | −0.09 (−0.18, 0.00) | −0.05 (−0.13, 0.04) | −0.06 (−0.15, 0.03) | −0.02 (−0.11, 0.06) | −0.07 (−0.16, 0.02) | −0.05 (−0.13, 0.04) |
3 | −0.12 (−0.22, −0.03) | −0.16 (−0.25, −0.07) | −0.08 (−0.18, 0.01) | −0.12 (−0.21, −0.03) | −0.09 (−0.18, 0.01) | −0.15 (−0.24, −0.06) |
4 | −0.16 (−0.26, −0.05) | −0.09 (−0.19, 0.01) | −0.09 (−0.20, 0.02) | −0.03 (−0.13, 0.07) | −0.10 (−0.21, 0.00) | −0.04 (−0.14, 0.06) |
Education (vs. ≥college degree) | ||||||
some college | −0.28 (−0.42, −0.14) | −0.26 (−0.40, −0.12) | ||||
≤HS | −0.32 (−0.39, −0.24) | −0.29 (−0.36, −0.22) | ||||
Income (vs. ≥$50k) | ||||||
$25–49.9k | −0.07 (−0.12, −0.01) | −0.10 (−0.16, −0.05) | ||||
<$25k | −0.15 (−0.22, −0.07) | −0.19 (−0.27, −0.12) | ||||
Married (vs. not married) | −0.13 (−0.19, −0.08) | |||||
Children <18 years (vs. none) | −0.02 (−0.10, 0.07) | −0.23 (−0.30, −0.15) | ||||
−0.02 (−0.10, 0.07) | −0.23 (−0.30, −0.15) | |||||
Fixed effectsf | ||||||
Neighborhood deprivatione quartiles (vs. 1st quartile) | ||||||
2 | −0.02 (−0.12, 0.09) | 0.00 (−0.09, 0.10) | −0.02 (−0.12, 0.09) | 0.00 (−0.09, 0.10) | −0.03 (−0.13, 0.07) | −0.02 (−0.12, 0.07) |
3 | −0.02 (−0.13, 0.09) | −0.04 (−0.14, 0.07) | −0.01 (−0.13, 0.10) | −0.02 (−0.13, 0.08) | −0.03 (−0.14, 0.08) | −0.07 (−0.17, 0.03) |
4 | −0.04 (−0.17, 0.08) | 0.10 (−0.02, 0.21) | −0.04 (−0.16, 0.09) | 0.10 (−0.01, 0.22) | −0.05 (−0.18, 0.07) | 0.08 (−0.03, 0.19) |
Income (vs. ≥$50k) | ||||||
$25–49.9k | −0.01 (−0.07, 0.05) | −0.04 (−0.10, 0.02) | ||||
<$25k | −0.05 (−0.13, 0.04) | −0.07 (−0.16, 0.01) | ||||
Married (vs. not married) | −0.15 (−0.21, −0.08) | |||||
Children <18 years (vs. none) | −0.06 (−0.15, 0.04) | −0.23 (−0.33, −0.14) |
CARDIA Study data from years 0, 7, 10, and 15. Physical activity was natural log transformed to address skewness. A single estimate indicates no significant sex interaction in any of the three models. Bold font indicates statistical significance (p<0.05)
Model 1 adjusts for study center, age, age2, age3, sex, interaction between sex and age terms, and interaction between sex and study center
Model 2 adjusts for Model 1 variables, education, income, and interaction between sex and education
Model 3 adjusts for Model 1 and 2 variables, marital status, any children <18 living in the household, and interaction between sex and children in household
Higher scores represent higher neighborhood deprivation.
Fixed effects models do not include time invariant characteristics, including education, which showed small variability over time.
DISCUSSION
In one of the first studies to examine the longitudinal relationship between neighborhood SES and PA, we found that in U.S. adults, high neighborhood deprivation was incrementally associated with lower PA in blacks – reaching 16% lower PA for the most versus least deprived neighborhoods in fully adjusted models – while associations were less consistent in whites. Furthermore, by comparing various strategies to adjust for confounders related to residential selection, we found that adjustment for measured characteristics beyond sociodemographics (age, sex, race) and individual SES had little influence on the estimated associations. Adjustment for unmeasured time-invariant characteristics via fixed effects models attenuated negative associations in blacks and whites, but associations remained significant and relatively strong in blacks.
Our findings suggest that PA may be one mechanism through which neighborhood deprivation might influence health, particularly in blacks. Our work also improves understanding of residential self-selection bias, a critical limitation to existing neighborhood health research, and raises questions concerning race differences and ongoing methodological limitations (e.g., adjustment for unmeasured time-variant characteristics) of neighborhood health research.
Race differences
Prior U.S. studies examining neighborhood SES and various health outcomes have similarly found stronger associations in blacks than whites (Chichlowska, Rose, Diez-Roux, Golden, McNeill, & Heiss, 2008; Merkin et al., 2009; Subramanian et al., 2005), while race comparisons in other studies are inconsistent (e.g., (Auchincloss, Diez Roux, Brown, O’Meara, & Raghunathan, 2007; Do et al., 2007; Wen, Kandula, & Lauderdale, 2007)). However, race-specific associations in this study may not be comparable because, like others (Chichlowska et al., 2008; Diez-Roux, Kiefe, Jacobs, Haan, Jackson, Nieto et al., 2001), we examined race-specific neighborhood deprivation quantiles to address dramatic differences in neighborhood deprivation between whites and blacks. Racial disparities in neighborhood SES reflect residential segregation in the U.S. (R. J. Sampson & Sharkey, 2008) and have important implications for social justice. Furthermore, more pronounced associations for the 3rd and 4th quartiles in blacks suggest that the PA gradient with increasing deprivation may sharpen at the highest neighborhood deprivation levels more completely captured in blacks than whites.
Alternatively, stronger associations between neighborhood deprivation and lower PA in blacks than whites may result from mechanisms specific to health behaviors rather than health outcomes (e.g., cardiovascular disease) for which most race-specific estimates are reported. For example, whites may have better access to PA supports (e.g., higher quality PA facilities or stronger social norm) in employment or other nonresidential settings, thereby diluting residential neighborhood deprivation effects. Additionally, due to residential segregation by race, whites living in poor neighborhoods may be less isolated and geographically closer to PA supports in other neighborhoods. Indeed, Auchincloss and colleagues found that distance to a wealthy neighborhood was more strongly associated with insulin resistance than local neighborhood poverty (Auchincloss et al., 2007). Accounting for effects of deprivation in nearby neighborhoods (spatial lag) may be useful to further investigate this issue (Mears & Bhati, 2006).
Heterogeneity in estimated neighborhood deprivation effects by race may also reflect racial differences in unmeasured characteristics. In the U.S., knowledge, attitudes, and resources to engage in healthy lifestyles vary by race (Watters, Satia, & Galanko, 2007; Winkleby, Kraemer, Ahn, & Varady, 1998), and blacks are more likely to experience discrimination (Williams & Mohammed, 2009) and more prolonged periods of individual- and neighborhood-level poverty (Do, 2009). That is, the experience of living in any particular environment may vary by race due to cultural and historical differences not captured in this or similar studies. Such differences may also change over time, particularly throughout the time period (1985–2000) and life stages (young- to middle-adulthood) captured by this study.
Residential self-selection
We investigated whether measured and unmeasured characteristics related to residential selection – either through differences in economic or cultural constraints or in residential preferences – were important sources of bias. In blacks and whites, the association between neighborhood deprivation and PA was sharply attenuated after controlling for individual-level education and income, which are, aside from race, the strongest predictors of residential selection in U.S. mobility and migration studies (Ioannides & Zabel, 2008;R. J. Sampson & Sharkey, 2008). Marriage and children were theorized to influence residential needs but did not bias the neighborhood deprivation-PA association, perhaps because they were largely captured by traditional sociodemographic control variables.
Comparison of random and fixed effects estimates suggest that confounding by unmeasured time-invariant characteristics was minimal in blacks. In whites, confounding was stronger, but associations generally remained weak and nonsignificant. Relevant unmeasured time-invariant confounders might include preferences for neighborhoods with PA-supportive amenities or other environment characteristics which do not directly support PA, but are correlated with low neighborhood deprivation and more frequently selected by those with high propensity for PA (e.g., upscale suburban neighborhoods). These findings suggest that longitudinal data and incorporation of specific, hypothesized neighborhood attributes are important in neighborhood health studies. Weaker confounding by unmeasured time-invariant characteristics in blacks may reflect the strong role of race in neighborhood selection in the U.S. (Clark & Ledwith, 2007;R. J. Sampson & Sharkey, 2008), thereby constraining the role of residential preferences in blacks.
Importantly, in both blacks and whites, unmeasured characteristics that vary over time were not addressed by fixed effects models and may contribute additional bias. For example, proximity to work, school, or social networks may be critical drivers of residential selection which may change over time and is, in this and most other studies, unmeasured. To the extent that proximity to such resources is also related to neighborhood deprivation and PA, the neighborhood deprivation-PA association may be biased. To illustrate, one possible non-causal explanation for negative fixed effects neighborhood deprivation-PA associations might be that individuals who start new, more demanding jobs may have less leisure time for PA and move to neighborhoods closer to work (which may be closer to commercial centers and have low neighborhood SES). That is, employment factors may influence both residential selection and PA, is unmeasured, and changes over time, therefore resulting in bias.
In sum, individual SES, a typical control variable, was the most important measured confounder, but longitudinal analysis controlled for additional confounding. However, the role of time-varying unmeasured characteristics should be further investigated with instrumental variables or other simultaneous equation strategies which model residential selection factors that affect residential choice but not the outcome measure (Boone-Heinonen et al.).
Implications and future research
Our study, which examined PA – a specific modifiable outcome known to strongly influence health (Haskell, Lee, Pate, Powell, Blair, Franklin et al., 2007) – using a longitudinal modeling strategy to address bias due to time-invariant, unmeasured residential self-selection factors, provides support that the neighborhood environment influences health and may therefore be a promising intervention focus. Differences in association by race further suggest that neighborhood-level interventions may benefit from being targeted to groups particularly sensitive to neighborhood environments.
However, specific policy implications require better understanding of mechanisms through which neighborhood deprivation influences PA and through which racial differences might arise. We hypothesize that inequitable distribution of built environment features such as PA facilities (Gordon-Larsen et al., 2006) is an important pathway. In this study, we did not adjust for potentially mediating neighborhood characteristics, but mediation analysis of such variables may help to develop policy strategies related to built or other modifiable neighborhood characteristics.
Mediation analysis can correspondingly reveal the importance of pathways other than the built environment. Neighborhood deprivation may capture multiple synergistic paths through which financial and social resources of a community might influence PA including not only PA opportunities (built environment) but also social cues and social support for PA. Indeed, neighborhood deprivation-PA associations in this study were stronger than typical built environment-PA associations (Wendel-Vos, Droomers, Kremers, Brug, & van Lenthe, 2007), perhaps because alternative mechanisms (e.g., social norms) influence the wide range of activities captured by our PA measure. Research clarifying the role of alternative pathways is needed to develop effective policy strategies.
Methodological implications of our findings include the importance of stratifying by race and the use of longitudinal data. Future research might include better analytical strategies to handle residential segregation, perhaps through stratified sampling (Oakes, Forsyth, & Schmitz, 2007) to obtain adequate sample size in various race-neighborhood deprivation classifications.
While we conceptualized PA as a mediating behavior in the well documented association between neighborhood SES and health, we did not address the potential role of health status on residential sorting (Boyle et al., 2002; Curtis et al., 2009). Those with declining health status are likely to have limited PA and more often relocate to areas of lower neighborhood SES, thereby magnifying associations between neighborhood SES and PA or health. However, such complex dynamic relationships involve their own set of selection and other biases which may be addressed using marginal structural models (Robins, Hernan, & Brumback, 2000), structural equation modeling (Kline, 2005), or similar strategies. Additionally, neighborhood SES may influence individual SES, so similar modeling may be required to avoid overadjustment by individual SES (Diez Roux, 2005; Do & Finch, 2008).
Finally, we assumed relatively immediate effects of the neighborhood environment on PA. Cumulative effects of neighborhood SES (Do, 2009) on PA or other behaviors, possibly through habits and attitudes established early in life, should also be investigated. Fixed effects models may be limited for outcomes with longer development duration because the time intervals examined may be too short to capture the corresponding lag time and may exhibit temporal mismatch with the relevant etiologic periods (Glymour, 2008).
Strengths and Limitations
In addition to the methodological issues described above, limitations of our study include self-reported PA and individual characteristics. However, bias resulting from measurement error in our outcome and exposure is minimized because errors in self-reported PA and in objective neighborhood deprivation likely arise from different mechanisms and are therefore unlikely to be correlated. Additionally, the CARDIA PA score has been validated (Jacobs Jr. et al., 1993; Jacobs Jr. et al., 1989) and shown to be an important predictor of numerous health outcomes (e.g., (Carnethon, Jacobs, Sidney, Sternfeld, Gidding, Shoushtari et al., 2005; Schmitz, Jacobs, Leon, Schreiner, & Sternfeld, 2000)). Second, our analysis may have omitted important residential selection factors such as car ownership and employment type and location, which are not readily available for our sample. Third, there is slight temporal mismatch between the census and exam periods. However, neighborhood deprivation is unlikely to change rapidly enough to result in substantial exposure misclassification. Fourth, our results are vulnerable to selection bias, but fixed effects models may help to mitigate bias by controlling for time invariant unmeasured characteristics related to attrition and missing data. Fifth, our measure of neighborhood deprivation may be a poor proxy for neighborhood features relevant to PA such as social organization, but it is consistent with prior work examining obesity-related behaviors and outcomes (Auchincloss et al., 2007; Diez-Roux et al., 2001). Finally, log transformation of our outcome addressed skewness, but may have overemphasized changes for low PA levels.
However, our study is unique in using longitudinal data to estimate the effects of neighborhood deprivation on PA, a potential mediator between neighborhood SES and health. Our study population was large, sociodemographically and geographically diverse, and followed over 15 years. Lastly, we used several objective measures of neighborhood SES, which may better capture neighborhood SES than any single measure.
Conclusion
After accounting for several potential sources of residential self-selection bias, we found strong estimated longitudinal effects of neighborhood deprivation on lower PA in U.S. blacks but not whites, although racial stratification makes race comparisons problematic. Typical adjustment variables (e.g., individual SES) were the most influential confounders, but time-invariant unmeasured characteristics also appear to contribute bias. Our examination of PA is a first step toward understanding mechanisms of neighborhood influences on health, but identification of policy strategies requires more research examining the role of the built environment and other environment factors in the relationship between neighborhood SES and PA.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
Funding for this study comes from the National Institutes of Health: (R01 HL104580, R01-CA109831, R01-CA121152). Additional funding has come from NIH (R01-AA12162 & DK056350), the UNC-CH Center for Environmental Health and Susceptibility (CEHS) (NIH P30-ES10126), the UNC-CH Clinic Nutrition Research Center (NIH DK56350), and the Carolina Population Center; and from contracts with the University of Alabama at Birmingham, Coordinating Center, N01-HC-95095; University of Alabama at Birmingham, Field Center, N01-HC-48047; University of Minnesota, Field Center, N01-HC-48048; Northwestern University, Field Center, N01-HC-48049; Kaiser Foundation Research Institute, N01-HC-48050 from the National Heart, Lung and Blood Institute.
Analysis was supported by the Interdisciplinary Obesity Training postdoctoral fellowship (T32MH075854-04). The authors would like to thank Drs. Barbara Sternfeld and Joshua S. Richman for their comments on the manuscript, Brian Frizzelle, Marc Peterson, Chris Mankoff, James D. Stewart, Phil Bardsley, and Diane Kaczor of the University of North Carolina, Carolina Population Center (CPC) and the CPC Spatial Analysis Unit for creation of the environmental variables, and Ms. Frances Dancy for her helpful administrative assistance.
Footnotes
There were no potential or real conflicts of financial or personal interest with the financial sponsors of the scientific project.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Auchincloss AH, Diez Roux AV, Brown DG, O’Meara ES, Raghunathan TE. Association of insulin resistance with distance to wealthy areas: the multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis. Am J Epidemiol. 2007;165(4):389–397. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwk028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bedimo-Rung AL, Mowen AJ, Cohen DA. The significance of parks to physical activity and public health: a conceptual model. Am J Prev Med. 2005;28(2 Suppl 2):159–168. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2004.10.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bell S, Lee C. Emerging adulthood and patterns of physical activity among young Australian women. Int J Behav Med. 2005;12(4):227–235. doi: 10.1207/s15327558ijbm1204_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boone-Heinonen J, Gordon-Larsen P, Guilkey D, Jacobs DR, Popkin BM. Environment and physical activity dynamics: the role of residential self-selection. Psychology of Sport and Exercise. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2009.09.003. in press, available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2009.09.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Boyle P, Norman P, Rees P. Does migration exaggerate the relationship between deprivation and limiting long-term illness? A Scottish analysis. Soc Sci Med. 2002;55(1):21–31. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(01)00217-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carnethon MR, Jacobs DR, Jr, Sidney S, Sternfeld B, Gidding SS, Shoushtari C, et al. A longitudinal study of physical activity and heart rate recovery: CARDIA, 1987–1993. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2005;37(4):606–612. doi: 10.1249/01.mss.0000158190.56061.32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chichlowska KL, Rose KM, Diez-Roux AV, Golden SH, McNeill AM, Heiss G. Individual and Neighborhood Socioeconomic Status Characteristics and Prevalence of Metabolic Syndrome: The Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) Study. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2008;70(9):986–992. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e318183a491. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark WAV, Ledwith V. How much does income matter in neighborhood choice? Population Research and Policy Review. 2007;26(2):145–161. [Google Scholar]
- Cox M, Boyle PJ, Davey PG, Feng Z, Morris AD. Locality deprivation and Type 2 diabetes incidence: a local test of relative inequalities. Soc Sci Med. 2007;65(9):1953–1964. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.05.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Curtis S, Setia MS, Quesnel-Vallee A. Socio-geographic mobility and health status: a longitudinal analysis using the National Population Health Survey of Canada. Soc Sci Med. 2009;69(12):1845–1853. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.08.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diez-Roux AV, Kiefe CI, Jacobs DR, Jr, Haan M, Jackson SA, Nieto FJ, et al. Area characteristics and individual-level socioeconomic position indicators in three population-based epidemiologic studies. Ann Epidemiol. 2001;11(6):395–405. doi: 10.1016/s1047-2797(01)00221-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diez Roux AV. Estimating neighborhood health effects: the challenges of causal inference in a complex world. Soc Sci Med. 2004;58(10):1953–1960. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(03)00414-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diez Roux AV. Commentary: estimating and understanding area health effects. Int J Epidemiol. 2005;34(2):284–285. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyi020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diez Roux AV. Neighborhoods and health: where are we and where do we go from here? Rev Epidemiol Sante Publique. 2007;55(1):13–21. doi: 10.1016/j.respe.2006.12.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diez Roux AV, Evenson KR, McGinn AP, Brown DG, Moore L, Brines S, et al. Availability of recreational resources and physical activity in adults. Am J Public Health. 2007;97(3):493–499. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2006.087734. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diez Roux AV, Merkin SS, Arnett D, Chambless L, Massing M, Nieto FJ, et al. Neighborhood of residence and incidence of coronary heart disease. N Engl J Med. 2001;345(2):99–106. doi: 10.1056/NEJM200107123450205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Do DP. The dynamics of income and neighborhood context for population health: do long-term measures of socioeconomic status explain more of the black/white health disparity than single-point-in-time measures? Soc Sci Med. 2009;68(8):1368–1375. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.01.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Do DP, Dubowitz T, Bird CE, Lurie N, Escarce JJ, Finch BK. Neighborhood context and ethnicity differences in body mass index: a multilevel analysis using the NHANES III survey (1988–1994) Econ Hum Biol. 2007;5(2):179–203. doi: 10.1016/j.ehb.2007.03.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Do DP, Finch BK. The link between neighborhood poverty and health: Context or composition? American Journal of Epidemiology. 2008;168(6):611–619. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwn182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eid J, Overman HG, Puga D, Turner MA. Fat city: Questioning the relationship between urban sprawl and obesity. Journal of Urban Economics. 2008;63(2):385–404. [Google Scholar]
- Foster S, Giles-Corti B. The built environment, neighborhood crime and constrained physical activity: An exploration of inconsistent findings. Prev Med. 2008 doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2008.03.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geist C, McManus PA. Geographical mobility over the life course: Motivations and implications. Population Space and Place. 2008;14(4):283–303. [Google Scholar]
- Glymour MM. Sensitive periods and first difference models: integrating etiologic thinking into econometric techniques: a commentary on Clarkwest’s “Neo-materialist theory and the temporal relationship between income inequality and longevity change”. Soc Sci Med. 2008;66(9):1895–1902. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.12.035. discussion 1903–1898. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gordon-Larsen P, Adair LS, Popkin BM. The relationship of ethnicity, socioeconomic factors, and overweight in U.S. adolescents. Obes Res. 2003;11(1):121–129. doi: 10.1038/oby.2003.20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gordon-Larsen P, McMurray RG, Popkin BM. Adolescent physical activity and inactivity vary by ethnicity: The National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health. J Pediatr. 1999;135(3):301–306. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3476(99)70124-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gordon-Larsen P, Nelson MC, Page P, Popkin BM. Inequality in the built environment underlies key health disparities in physical activity and obesity. Pediatrics. 2006;117(2):417–424. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-0058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haskell WL, Lee IM, Pate RR, Powell KE, Blair SN, Franklin BA, et al. Physical activity and public health: updated recommendation for adults from the American College of Sports Medicine and the American Heart Association. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2007;39(8):1423–1434. doi: 10.1249/mss.0b013e3180616b27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hughes GH, Cutter G, Donahue R, Friedman GD, Hulley S, Hunkeler E, et al. Recruitment in the Coronary Artery Disease Risk Development in Young Adults (Cardia) Study. Control Clin Trials. 1987;8(4 Suppl):68S–73S. doi: 10.1016/0197-2456(87)90008-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ioannides YA, Zabel JE. Interactions, neighborhood selection and housing demand. Journal of Urban Economics. 2008;63(1):229–252. [Google Scholar]
- Jacobs DR, Hahn LP, Haskell WL, Pirie P, Sidney S. Validity and reliability of short physical activity history: Cardia and the Minnesota Heart Health Program. J Cardiopulmonary Rehabil. 1989;9:448–459. doi: 10.1097/00008483-198911000-00003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacobs D, Jr, Ainsworth B, Hartman T, Leon A. A simultaneous evaluation of 10 commonly used physical activity questionnaires. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1993;25(1):81–91. doi: 10.1249/00005768-199301000-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacobs D, Jr, Hahn L, Haskell W, Pirie P, Sidney S. Validity and reliability of sort physical activity history: CARDIA and the Minnesota Heart Health Program. J Cardiopulm Rehabil. 1989;9(11):448–459. doi: 10.1097/00008483-198911000-00003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kline R. Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York: The Guilford Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Krieger N, Zierler S, Hogan JW, Waterman P, Chen JT, Lemieux K, et al. Geocoding and measurement of neighborhood socioeconomic position: a U.S. perspective. In: Kawachi I, Berkman LF, editors. Neighborhood and Health. New York: Oxford University Press; 2003. pp. 147–178. [Google Scholar]
- Krizek KJ, Johnson PJ. Proximity to trails and retail: effects on urban cycling and walking. J Am Plan Assn. 2006;72(1):33–42. [Google Scholar]
- Lee RE, Cubbin C, Winkleby M. Contribution of neighbourhood socioeconomic status and physical activity resources to physical activity among women. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2007;61(10):882–890. doi: 10.1136/jech.2006.054098. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lund H. Reasons for living in a transit-oriented development, and associated transit use. J Am Plan Assn. 2006;72(3):357–366. [Google Scholar]
- Mears DP, Bhati AS. No community is an island: the effects of resource deprivation on urban violence in spatially and socially proximate communities. Criminology. 2006;44(3):509–547. [Google Scholar]
- Merkin SS, Basurto-Davila R, Karlamangla A, Bird CE, Lurie N, Escarce J, et al. Neighborhoods and Cumulative Biological Risk Profiles by Race/Ethnicity in a National Sample of US Adults: NHANES III. Elsevier Science Inc; 2009. pp. 194–201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore LV, Diez Roux AV, Evenson KR, McGinn AP, Brines SJ. Availability of recreational resources in minority and low socioeconomic status areas. Am J Prev Med. 2008;34(1):16–22. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2007.09.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oakes JM. The (mis)estimation of neighborhood effects: causal inference for a practicable social epidemiology. Soc Sci Med. 2004;58(10):1929–1952. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2003.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oakes JM, Forsyth A, Schmitz KH. The effects of neighborhood density and street connectivity on walking behavior: the Twin Cities walking study. Epidemiol Perspect Innov. 2007;4:16. doi: 10.1186/1742-5573-4-16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ogden CL, Carroll MD, Curtin LR, McDowell MA, Tabak CJ, Flegal KM. Prevalence of overweight and obesity in the United States, 1999–2004. Jama. 2006;295(13):1549–1555. doi: 10.1001/jama.295.13.1549. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Papas MA, Alberg AJ, Ewing R, Helzlsouer KJ, Gary TL, Klassen AC. The built environment and obesity. Epidemiol Rev. 2007;29:129–143. doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxm009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rabe-Hesketh S, Skrondal A. Multilevel and Longitudinal Modeling Using Stata. Stata Press; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Robins JM, Hernan MA, Brumback B. Marginal structural models and causal inference in epidemiology. Epidemiology. 2000;11(5):550–560. doi: 10.1097/00001648-200009000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ross CE. Walking, exercising, and smoking: does neighborhood matter? Soc Sci Med. 2000;51(2):265–274. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(99)00451-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sampson RJ, Morenoff JD, Gannon-Rowley T. Assessing “Neighborhood Effects”: Social processes and new directions for research. Ann Rev Sociol. 2002;28:443–478. [Google Scholar]
- Sampson RJ, Sharkey P. Neighborhood selection and the social reproduction of concentrated racial inequality. Demography. 2008;45(1):1–29. doi: 10.1353/dem.2008.0012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmitz KH, Jacobs DR, Jr, Leon AS, Schreiner PJ, Sternfeld B. Physical activity and body weight: associations over ten years in the CARDIA study. Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 2000;24(11):1475–1487. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0801415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shishehbor MH, Gordon-Larsen P, Kiefe CI, Litaker D. Association of neighborhood socioeconomic status with physical fitness in healthy young adults: the Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) study. Am Heart J. 2008;155(4):699–705. doi: 10.1016/j.ahj.2007.07.055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sobal J, Rauschenbach B, Frongillo EA. Marital status changes and body weight changes: a US longitudinal analysis. Soc Sci Med. 2003;56(7):1543–1555. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(02)00155-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- StataCorp. Longitudinal Panel Data manual, Stata Statistical Software, Release 9. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Subramanian SV, Chen JT, Rehkopf DH, Waterman PD, Krieger N. Racial disparities in context: a multilevel analysis of neighborhood variations in poverty and excess mortality among black populations in Massachusetts. Am J Public Health. 2005;95(2):260–265. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2003.034132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- U.S. Census Bureau. How the Census Bureau Measures Poverty. 2009. [Google Scholar]
- van Lenthe FJ, Martikainen P, Mackenbach JP. Neighbourhood inequalities in health and health-related behaviour: results of selective migration? Health Place. 2007;13(1):123–137. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2005.09.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watters JL, Satia JA, Galanko JA. Associations of psychosocial factors with fruit and vegetable intake among African-Americans. Public Health Nutr. 2007;10(7):701–711. doi: 10.1017/S1368980007662284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wen M, Kandula NR, Lauderdale DS. Walking for transportation or leisure: what difference does the neighborhood make? J Gen Intern Med. 2007;22(12):1674–1680. doi: 10.1007/s11606-007-0400-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wen M, Zhang X. Contextual Effects of Built and Social Environments of Urban Neighborhoods on Exercise: A Multilevel Study in Chicago. American Journal of Health Promotion. 2009;23(4):247–254. doi: 10.4278/ajhp.07052448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wendel-Vos W, Droomers M, Kremers S, Brug J, van Lenthe F. Potential environmental determinants of physical activity in adults: a systematic review. Obes Rev. 2007;8(5):425–440. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2007.00370.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams DR, Mohammed SA. Discrimination and racial disparities in health: evidence and needed research. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2009;32(1):20–47. doi: 10.1007/s10865-008-9185-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winkleby MA, Kraemer HC, Ahn DK, Varady AN. Ethnic and socioeconomic differences in cardiovascular disease risk factors: findings for women from the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey, 1988–1994. Jama. 1998;280(4):356–362. doi: 10.1001/jama.280.4.356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yannakoulia M, Panagiotakos D, Pitsavos C, Skoumas Y, Stafanadis C. Eating patterns may mediate the association between marital status, body mass index, and blood cholesterol levels in apparently healthy men and women from the ATTICA study. Soc Sci Med. 2008;66(11):2230–2239. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.01.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.