Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Mar 22.
Published in final edited form as: Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2009 Mar 3;18(3):866–875. doi: 10.1158/1055-9965.EPI-07-2950

Development and Validation of a Cervical Cancer Screening Self-Efficacy Scale for Low-Income Mexican American Women

Maria E Fernández 1, Pamela M Diamond 2, William Rakowski 3, Alicia Gonzales 4, Guillermo Tortolero-Luna 5, Janet Williams 6, Daisy Y Morales-Campos 7
PMCID: PMC3062501  NIHMSID: NIHMS212647  PMID: 19258484

Abstract

While self-efficacy (SE), a construct from Social Cognitive Theory, has been shown to influence other screening behaviors, few measures currently exist for measuring Pap test SE. This paper describes the development and psychometric testing of such a measure for Mexican-American women. Data from two separate samples of Mexican-American women 50 years or older, obtained as part of a study to develop and evaluate a breast and cervical cancer screening educational program, were used in the current study. Exploratory factor analysis indicated a single factor solution and all item loadings were > .73. Confirmatory analysis confirmed a single factor structure with all standardized loadings greater than .40 as hypothesized. The eight item SE scale demonstrated high internal consistency (Cronbach's alpha = .95). As hypothesized, SE was correlated with knowledge, prior experience, and screening intention. Logistic regression supported the theoretical relationship that women with higher SE were more likely to have had a recent Pap test. Findings showed a significant increase in SE following the intervention, indicating the measure has good sensitivity to change over time.

Keywords: self-efficacy, Pap test, cancer screening, Mexican-Americans, Hispanic women, scale validation

Introduction

Hispanics have higher cervical cancer incidence and mortality rates compared to non-Hispanic Whites (incidence: 13.8 vs. 8.5 per 100,000; mortality 3.3 vs. 2.3 per 100,000, 2000-2004) (1). Data from the 2006 BRFSS survey reports an overall high percentage of women aged 18 or older who have had a Pap test within the last three years (84%) (2). Nonetheless, these trends are not visible in subgroups such as women with less than a high school education, women with the lowest incomes, uninsured women, and Hispanics (3). Hispanic women have lower rates of recent Pap, only 74% reported having had a Pap test in the preceding three years (4-6). Even greater disparities exist among older Hispanic women and among those living in certain regions of the country such as the US-Mexico border (5, 7).

Demographic factors associated with low levels of Pap test screening include having less than 12 years of education; being unemployed; not being married; recent immigration; and lower income (7-13). Psychosocial factors include embarrassment; uncomfortable examinations; low acculturation; fatalism; language barriers; physician distrust; lack of childcare; fear of the procedure; fear of the results; concern about confidentiality; lack of knowledge; perceived discrimination; and perceived partner disapproval (8, 14-21). External factors include lack of physician referral; lack of health insurance; cost; no regular place of care; restrictive work policies; rigid clinic payment policies; poor transportation; and quality of care (8, 9, 15, 19, 22-25). Cognitive-affective processes that interact with external factors also negatively impact screening (20).

According to Bandura's Social Cognitive Theory (SCT), self-efficacy (SE) is one's confidence in being able to exert personal control (26). Bandura (27) proposes that self-efficacy is a task-specific expectation. People gauge their confidence in their capacity to handle a situation through evaluating the specific activities or steps involved in the successful achievement of the task (or behavior). For example, a number of steps are involved for a woman to have a Pap test screening. First, the woman may call and make an appointment with her doctor. A self-efficacy belief corresponding to this step would be the confidence a woman has in her capability to find and call a location that offers Pap test screening and arrange an appointment. The level of a woman's self-efficacy may vary for each step. For instance, the woman may be able to set-up an appointment, but may have a harder time overcoming the fear of pain or embarrassment during the test. Thus a self-efficacy measure should assess the major steps involved in the behavior studied to have the best predictive power (28).

SCT holds that persons with high self-efficacy beliefs about a task call upon these beliefs and abilities to handle the task (26, 29). Within the context of Pap test screening, Pap test self-efficacy can be described as confidence in being able to schedule and complete screening (30).

Self-efficacy varies across different behaviors (e.g., efficacy for physical activity differs from self-efficacy for smoking cessation) and across various levels of performance of a domain of function (e.g., walking laps compared to running a marathon) (29). Since the construct of self-efficacy is a function of both the behavior in question and the situational contexts in which the behavior takes place and may differ from one population to another, there are no standard sets of self-efficacy measures that can be utilized for all individuals in all circumstances (31-33). Therefore, self-efficacy scales need to be developed for specific domains of functioning (33) and for different populations because their situational contexts may differ.

Measures of self-efficacy have been developed for other types of cancer screening behaviors, such as mammography screening (28), breast self-examination (BSE) (34), and testicular self-examination (35). Self-efficacy measures for other health behaviors have also been developed specifically for Hispanic populations: exercise (36), HIV risk behaviors (37, 38), perimenopausal health (39), chronic disease self-management (40), mammography (41), and arthritis self-management (42).

No measure of self-efficacy for Pap test screening for Hispanic women has been published in the literature and, at the time of the study, no Pap test screening self efficacy measure existed at all. Currently, the only related scale for any group is a recently published 20-item instrument for sheltered homeless inner city women (43). The scale contains population-specific items concerning confidence in overcoming Pap screening barriers in the context of homelessness, such as living without permanent housing, drug treatment, and alcohol use. According to criteria set for by Hui and Triandis (1985) that describe dimensions of equivalence when attempting to measure constructs across culture, the scale developed for homeless women is unlikely to function adequately in other groups (44). Our paper describes the development and psychometric testing of a Pap test screening instrument for Mexican American women that includes behavior and barrier-specific items that reflect their cultural and situational context. It is the first to develop a Pap test self-efficacy scale for Hispanics and thus fills an important gap in cervical cancer-related measures.

Our definition of self-efficacy, based on Bandura's social cognitive theory, was perceived confidence in one's personal ability to obtain a Pap test.

Hypotheses were as follows:

  1. A confirmatory factor analysis will support a single factor model in which all hypothesized paths have a standardized magnitude of at least .40.

  2. Cronbach's alpha for the Pap self-efficacy scale will be above .70.

  3. Perceived self-efficacy will be more highly correlated with screening intention, knowledge, and prior Pap experience, than with perceived risk and subjective norms.

  4. Perceived self-efficacy will be independently associated with Pap test screening adherence.

  5. The scale will detect expected changes in self-efficacy.

Materials and Methods

Study Population

Data from two separate samples were used to inform the development and validation of the instrument. Both datasets were obtained as part of a study to develop and evaluate the effectiveness of a breast and cervical cancer screening educational program for Hispanic women living in farmworker communities, called Cultivando la Salud (CLS) (Cultivating Health) (45). The first dataset, used for the exploratory factor analysis (EFA), was obtained using a convenience sample of 200 female Hispanic women living in neighborhoods in Cameron and Hidalgo counties, located in the Lower Rio Grande Valley (LRGV) of Texas. The data were collected as part of the CLS pilot study to identify factors associated with mammography and Pap test screening and to gather data that would be used for instrument refinement. Women were recruited from LRGV neighborhoods known to have high proportions of farm worker families. Female bilingual Hispanic interviewers approached women in their homes, determined eligibility, and invited women to participate in a survey. Eligibility criteria included women 50 years or older, no prior or current cancer diagnosis and farmworker status (defined as personal or family participation in farm work for at least 5 years during their lifetime). Women gave written consent before completing the interview and received a $20 incentive.

The second dataset, used for the all other analyses, consisted of data collected from women participating in the CLS intervention trial. Recruitment occurred along the TX-Mexico border and the central valley in California in the following cities: Anthony, NM; Eagle Pass, TX; Merced, CA; and Watsonville, CA. We selected neighborhoods or colonias in these areas based on two criteria: 1) high percentages of farmworker families residing within them and 2) within 20 miles of health care facilities that offered NBCCEDP funded cancer screening services. We randomized selected communities to either the intervention condition (Merced and Eagle Pass) or the comparison condition (Watsonville and Anthony).

To obtain the sample of study participants, the EPI Sampling Quadrants Scheme was used (46). Colonias were divided into four quadrants, data collectors randomly selected a starting point in each quadrant and walked the neighborhood. Administering the screening questionnaire door-to-door, each data collector continued to screen for eligibility and conduct interviews until she had screened all the households in the quadrant. Eligibility criteria were identical to the pilot study. Only one woman per household was invited to participate. If more than one woman was eligible, the woman with the most recent birth date was selected. Eligible women interested in participating gave written consent before the interview and received a $20 incentive upon completion.

Consistent with principles of community-based participatory research methods described by Israel (47), we recruited data collectors and data collection supervisors from the communities at each site. We made the decision to use community members for data collection because, although more intensive training is typically needed, their use often results in higher consent rates as well as more accurate and honest responses (48). Interviews were conducted in Spanish and lasted approximately two hours. All interviewers were female, bilingual, and attended a two-day training. During the training, data collectors became facile with the study protocol and instrument and participated in several practice sessions. The second day of training included actual data collection in a test community (LRGV) and project staff and investigators observed all data collectors. The practice was followed by a debriefing session during which data collectors clarified the answers to any of their questions and receive comments about their performance.

A total of 713 women were interviewed, which included 578 women who satisfied the eligibility criteria above, plus an additional 135 women who were over-sampled based on their nonadherence to breast and cervical cancer screening recommendations (i.e. no mammography in the past year and/or no Pap test in the past 3 years). There were a total 243 women who were non-adherent to recommended Pap test screening guidelines.

Lay health workers delivered the CLS intervention (video, flipchart, and resource list) to all women in the intervention communities who had completed the baseline survey. The intervention was designed to address factors influencing Pap test screening such as knowledge about guidelines, barriers to screening, perceived risk, and self efficacy. Various methods were used in intervention materials to influence self efficacy such as modeling, vicarious learning and reinforcement (video and graphic portrayal of women overcoming barriers to screening, talking with their doctor about the Pap test, etc.) (27, 31), and verbal persuasion. We expected that exposure to the intervention materials would increase perceived Pap test self-efficacy.

Six months following program implementation, data collectors conducted follow-up face-to-face interviews. The overall follow-up rate was 66.9% with no statistically significant differences on demographic variables or acculturation between women contacted for followed up and those lost to follow-up. We also detected no statistically significant demographic or acculturation differences by follow-up status across study conditions.

Measures

The baseline survey instrument consisted of 276 items including demographic, general health, knowledge, attitudinal, and cancer screening questions. The items and scales relevant for the current study were those used to assess: Pap test screening behavior, Pap test knowledge, perceived susceptibility (or risk) to cervical cancer, prior experience with Pap test screening, subjective norms, and Pap test screening intention.

We measured Pap test screening behavior by asking participants the exact month and year of her last Pap test. Those unable to remember the date were asked to estimate the number of years elapsed. We measured acculturation using the Bidimensional Acculturation Scale (49) that includes 60 items assessing English and Spanish language proficiency and frequency. The Pap test knowledge scale consisted of 15 items with a yes/no/don't know response format. Example items include: “Women who have gone through menopause do not need a Pap test” and a “Pap test can detect problems before they become cancer.” All of the following psychosocial constructs were assessed with 5-point Likert-type scales. Prior experience, a construct defined as previous performance of a task or experience (27), included 5 items (e.g. “Your Pap test was reassuring”). Subjective norms, the belief concerning the desires (related to Pap test screening) of important people or groups and the value that an individual places on those desires (50), was assessed using 6 items including phrases such as: “Your family thinks you should get a Pap test”; and “You want to do what your family thinks you should do about a Pap test.” Screening intention, an indication of an individual's readiness to perform a behavior (50), was assessed with two items: “Do you plan on having a Pap test in the future?” and “When do you plan on having your next Pap test?” Perceived susceptibility, an individual's perception that she is at risk for a particular condition (cervical cancer in this case) (51), was assessed with 4 items, and included items such as: “I believe I have a high chance of developing cervical cancer in the next 10 years.” Scales such as subjective norms and perceived susceptibility, included items from existing scales (52, 53) and new items generated from our focus groups and other findings, as described elsewhere §.

We assessed the internal consistency using baseline data. The Chronbach's alphas of scales with more than three items were: perceived susceptibility to cervical cancer (.93), prior experience (.50), pap test subjective norms (.82), and acculturation (.90).

Bandura suggests guidelines and that scale items should include: 1) the major steps associated with the process of obtaining a Pap test; 2) efficacy beliefs despite certain barriers or difficulties in obtaining the screening; and 3) the strength of the belief using a Likert-type scale (very sure to very unsure) (27). The combination of a woman's confidence in her ability to accomplish the major behavioral subcomponents and her belief in her ability to overcome obstacles to the behavior are what constitute overall self-efficacy (27).

Community members participating in the formative phase of our study contributed to the development and review of the items used in the self-efficacy scale. Prior to the development of items, we carefully delineated both the steps involved in obtaining a Pap test, and the barriers and difficulties that low income Mexican American women might encounter. We conducted four focus groups with women who had obtained a recent Pap test and with those who had never had a Pap test or had not had one in over three years. Both adherent and nonadherent women generated barriers to Pap test screening, including having to pay for the test, fear of pain, no transportation, no provider recommendation, and discouragement from others . The focus group participants also confirmed the three major steps involved in obtaining a Pap test: discussing Pap test screening with a healthcare provider, scheduling the appointment, and completing the screening ,. These findings, as well as a literature review on test-specific barriers to screening were used to develop the scale items.

Two experts (William Rakowski, Ph.D. and Alfred McAlister, Ph.D.) evaluated the instrument for content validity. Dr. Rakowski, Professor at Brown University, Medical Sciences, Department of Community Health is an expert in extending the Transtheoretical Model to cancer screening through instrument design and testing and intervention development and evaluation research. Dr. McAlister, Professor at the University of Texas, School of Public Health, studied under Albert Bandura and is an expert in Social Cognitive Theory and self-efficacy. These experts reviewed scale items and participated in a phone interview. They provided comments about the relevance of each item to the construct. They also made suggestions about slight revisions in item wording.

The self-efficacy scale was translated into Spanish using “universal broadcast Spanish,” a style of Spanish that avoids subgroup-specific expressions or colloquialisms and is often used in international broadcast media. Spanish speakers from various countries of origin (Columbia, Mexico, Honduras, Spain, Puerto Rico, and Cuba) then reviewed the instrument to ensure that the Spanish was comprehensible across subgroups. The resulting Spanish language instrument was then back translated to English, and the two English versions were compared to judge the quality and equivalence of translation and to resolve any inconsistencies, disagreements, or changes in meaning (54). Rather than considering the original English language version of the instrument the “gold standard,” we used a modified decentering technique as described by Vinokurov (2007)(55). Using this technique, both the original language and translated versions are considered equally important (Beck et al., 2003). Therefore, the original instrument may be revised to incorporate modifications made in the Spanish language version instrument to reflect linguistic and cultural norms of the target audience (54). Decentering, then, permits modifications based on the nuances of each language and culture to contribute to the final version of the instrument (55, 56).

The instrument was then pre-tested with a group of 50 female Hispanic migrant farm workers to examine response format and question clarity. Based on pretest findings, the response format was modified to a two-level Likert scale, first women were asked if they were “sure, undecided, or unsure” and then, depending on the response, women were asked about the strength of their confidence.

Statistical Analysis

Data from both samples (Pilot and Baseline) were screened prior to use for evidence of outliers, random responding and missing value patterns. Cases that were found to be missing all items postulated to be on the Self-Efficacy Scale were deleted. SPSS Missing Value Analysis (MVA) was then used to determine if the remaining missing data patterns were consistent with data that were missing at random (MAR) and to impute values for missing data using the expectation-maximization (EM) procedure (57).

Data from the Pilot study were used to develop and refine the Self-Efficacy Scale which would then be used in the Baseline survey. EFA was used to determine the factor structure for the items written to reflect self-efficacy. The Principal Axis Factor (PAF) method was used to identify underlying latent constructs (58). The Scree plot and factor solutions were inspected to identify the most interpretable solution. Varimax rotation was requested for solutions that identified more than one factor. In general, items with factor loadings over .35 were retained and oblique (Oblimin) rotations were considered when the solutions obtained using the Varimax procedure failed to achieve simple structure.

Items that were found to measure self-efficacy reasonably well in the Pilot study were retained for use in the Baseline study. Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) was used to assess the fit of the hypothesized model to the data obtained from the Baseline sample using AMOS 6.1. Model estimation was done using maximum likelihood procedures and model fit was assessed using a variety of common indices including the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) (59), the goodness of fit index (GFI) (60), the comparative fit index (CFI) (61), and the non-normed fit index (NNFI) (62). Values of the RMSEA of ≤ .05 indicate adequate fit, and values of up to .08 are often considered acceptable (63). Confidence intervals are available for the RMSEA and are reported in the results. For the GFI, NNFI and CFI, values ≥ .95 are considered reasonable (64).

We computed scale scores by summing the items. To further evaluate the measurement qualities of the final Self-Efficacy Scale, we computed Cronbach's alpha to assess internal consistency reliability. Discriminant and convergent validity was assessed by computing correlations between the self-efficacy (SE) score and the measures of other constructs included in the survey. Conceptually, self-efficacy should correlate highly and positively with knowledge (KNOW), screening intention (INT), and prior experience (EXP). According to Bandura, self-efficacy is positively associated with people's knowledge and skills (31). Additionally, both one's intention to engage in a certain health behavior and the actual health behavior are positively associated with beliefs in self efficacy (31, 65, 66). Personal experience is also associated with self efficacy. Bandura and others have proposed that self-efficacy is acquired through a) direct or mastery experience b) indirect or vicarious experience and c) verbal persuasion or symbolic experience (27, 65, 67, 68). To assess discriminant validity, we computed correlations between self-efficacy and two other scales with which it should have lower correlations: perceived risk (RISK), and subjective norms (NORMS). These constructs were chosen because they are not included in social cognitive theory and there is no evidence that they would be associated with self-efficacy. Even in Fishbien's Integrated model (69) in which both self-efficacy and subjective norms are included, no relation between the constructs is described. Perceived susceptibility, a construct of the Health Belief Model (70, 71), is not expected to be highly correlated with self-efficacy. Although the most recent version of the health belief model does include self efficacy as a construct that predicts behavior it does not propose an association between self-efficacy and perceived susceptibility (72). To test whether the correlations between SE and INT, KNOW and EXP were higher than the correlations between SE and SURVIVE, RISK and NORMS a series of dependent samples t-tests for correlations were run. In order to control for Type I error in this series of nine tests, an alpha of .005 was selected to maintain the experiment wise alpha at less than .05. Two-tailed tests were run to allow us to detect differences that may be in the opposite direction than expected.

To test the hypothesized theoretical relationship between self-efficacy and adherence to Pap testing, logistic regression analysis was used. The intent of this analysis was to test the independent association between self efficacy and Pap test screening while controlling for other potential influences on screening behavior. Social Cognitive Theory guided the hypothesis that self efficacy would be associated with Pap test screening. The selection of co-variates of screening was determined by both theory and empirical evidence. Bandura's concept of reciprocal determinism - a reciprocal causation among environmental, personal and behavioral factors which when interrelated impact one another, suggests that social and behavioral factors will influence self efficacy (27). In the model, we selected the variables age, education, marital status, birth status, income and insurance. The inclusion of these factors was further backed by empirical evidence of their association with Pap screening among Hispanics (10-13, 21). Analysis of the data in among our sample, led to the final decision about what specific variables would be included. We identified demographic variables that were significantly associated with the outcome (Pap test screening) or with self-efficacy. First, marital status, birth status, and income were collapsed into a smaller number of categories to remove small cell sizes. Then, to determine which variables would be entered into the logistic regression analysis, we computed a Chi-square analyses of demographic variables with adherence to Pap test screening, and conducted t-tests of demographic variables with self-efficacy. Significant demographic variables (p<.05) were entered as a block of variables, and self-efficacy was then included as a separate predictor variable.

Another measure of scale validity is its ability to detect expected changes in the construct over time (sensitivity to change) (28, 73). We would not expect self-efficacy for Pap test screening to change without exposure to an intervention or other event (such as practice of the behavior). Therefore, to assess whether the measure detects expected change in the construct, we compared changes in the self-efficacy measure in a situation where change was expected (under the intervention condition) to the changes in a situation in which no (or less) change was expected (comparison condition).

Sensitivity to change analysis was conducted by calculating an effect size reflecting the magnitude of change from baseline to follow-up in the self-efficacy scores of both the intervention and control groups. We hypothesized that the intervention group would show the largest magnitude of change in self-efficacy over time. The effect size associated with the pre-post change in self-efficacy for the intervention group reflects the ability of the self-efficacy measure to detect actual change over time. An effect size formula (d=t2(1r)/N) was used to appropriately measure effect size for non-independent samples to provide a standardized measure of change in self-efficacy (74, 75). We then computed Cohen's d for the difference between change scores for the intervention and control conditions as well as the confidence intervals for the effect size measure and assessed the statistical significance of this difference (p<.00)

Results

Exploratory and Confirmatory Factor Analyses on Pilot Data

MVA found that there were 8 cases which were missing responses on all self-efficacy variables and they were removed from the sample. Another three cases had some missing data which were imputed. The final sample included 192 women (Table 1). EFA identified 1 factor that explained 67.32% of the variance among the items. The first three columns of Table 2 provide the mean, standard deviations, and factor loadings for the items.

Table 1. Demographics.

Variable Pilot Study N=200 Baseline N=678
N (%) N (%)
Birth Status and Years in the US
Born in US 44 (22.0) 148 (20.8)
Born in Mexico, <5 years in US 7 (3.5) 26 (3.6)
Born in Mexico, 5-10 years in US 15 (7.5) 59 (8.3)
Born in Mexico, 11-19 years in US 28 (14.0) 57 (8.0)
Born in Mexico, > 20 years in US 102 (51.0) 399 (56.0)
Education (years)
None 18 (9.0) 63 (8.8)
1-5 106 (53.0) 329 (46.1)
6-11 57 (28.5) 237 (33.2)
12 and over 13 (6.5) 63 (8.8)
Age
50-59 115 (57.5) 349 (48.9)
60-69 47 (23.5) 199 (27.9)
70 and over 38 (19.0) 160 (22.4)
Income
None --- ---- 48 (6.7)
Less than $5,000 --- ---- 112 (15.7)
$5,000 - $9,999 ---- --- 204 (28.6)
<$10,000 147 (73.5) --- ---
$10,000 - $19,999 39 (19.5) 175 (24.5)
$20,000 or more 1 (0.5) 55 (7.7)
Insurance
Any 75 (37.5) 416 (58.3)
None 124 (62.0) 297 (41.7)
Marital Status
Never Married 1 (0.5) 21 (2.9)
Married 129 (64.5) 472 (66.2)
Separated 12 (6.0) 34 (4.8)
Divorced 18 (9.0) 24 (3.4)
Widowed 40 (20.0) 151 (21.2)
Living Together --- ----- 10 (1.4)
Pap Test Ever
Yes 140 (70.0) 589 (82.6)
No 56 (28.0) 90 (12.6)
Adherent to Pap Testing (Within 3 years)
Yes 118 (59.0) 437 (61.3)
No 77 (38.5) 243 (34.1)

Table 2. Sample Means, Standard Deviations and Factor Loadings.

Question Pilot Data
N=193
Baseline Data
N=678


Mean Standard deviation Factor loadings Mean Standard deviation Factor loadings

Q1 How sure are you that you can discuss having a pap test with your health care provider even if (s)he does not bring it up 4.22 1.215 .73 4.08 1.05 .81
Q2 How sure are you that you can schedule a pap test appointment and keep it 4.22 1.231 .80 4.16 1.02 .87
Q3 How sure are you that you can keep having a pap tests, even if you had to go to a new office in order to get one 4.01 1.318 .76 4.07 1.02 .83
Q4 How sure are you that you can ask your primary care physician for a referral to get a pap test 4.13 1.326 .89 4.16 .99 .89
Q5 How sure are you that you can go to get your next pap test 4.20 1.219 .90 4.17 .97 .90
Q6 How sure are you that you can get a pap test even if you are worried that it will be painful 4.09 1.323 .90 4.19 .98 .88
Q7 How sure are you that you can get a pap test even if a friend discouraged you from having one 4.16 1.318 .86 4.21 .97 .82
Q8 How sure are you that you can get a pap test even if you had to pay for it 3.94 1.406 .71 3.96 1.15 .71

Note: The following correlated residuals were included in the final model: Q1 and Q2 (r = .29); Q1 and Q4 (r = .28); Q6 and Q7 (r = .27).

Exploratory and Confirmatory Factor Analyses on Baseline Data

In the Baseline dataset 35 cases were eliminated because they were missing all items included on the hypothesized Self-Efficacy Scale. Item values were imputed for another 5 cases using the EM procedure resulting in a final sample of 678 women (Table 1). The eight items found to measure self-efficacy in the Pilot study were retained for the Baseline study and hypothesized to reflect a single factor. The initial CFA resulted in a Chi Square value of 194.653 with 20 degrees of freedom and fit the data fairly well based on all but one of the selected fit indices (GFI = .93; CFI = .97; NNFI = .95; RMSEA = .11: CI = .10 to .13). Since the RMSEA indicated less than adequate fit, the pattern of residuals and the Modification Indices were inspected in order to ascertain whether the addition of some correlated error terms might improve the fit of the model to the data. Three correlated residuals were sequentially added to the model, each improving the fit of the model significantly as assessed by the difference in Chi-Square test. Correlated residuals are found frequently in measures using a self report format where common extraneous sources of variation can influence the respondent's answers on multiple related items (76). The final model had a Chi Square value of 66.34 with 17 degrees of freedom. Other fit indices indicated adequate fit (GFI = .98; CFI = .99; NNFI = .99; RMSEA = .06: CI = .04 to .08). The last 3 columns of Table 2 provide the means, standard deviations, and the standardized regression weights (factor loadings) for this final model. The correlated residuals in the model were between Q1 and Q2, Q1 and Q4 and Q6 and Q7. These ranged from .27 to .29 and are shown in Table 2 as well.

Testing of Theoretical Relationships

Table 3 shows the correlation matrix between the following scales: self-efficacy scale score that was obtained by summing the items (SE: high scores indicate high self-efficacy); prior experience with pap tests (EXP: high scores indicate positive experience); intention to obtain future pap tests (INT: high scores indicate positive intentions); knowledge (KNOW: high scores indicate more knowledge); perceived risk (RISK: high scores indicate high risk); and subjective norms (NORMS: high scores indicate agreement with norms). All correlations were significant in the predicted direction with the exception of the one between EXP and RISK. The results of the series of dependent t-tests for correlations testing the hypotheses that correlations between SE and EXP, INT, and KNOW would be higher than those between SE and RISK and NORMS are shown in Table 4. The correlations between SE and RISK and between SE and NORMS were found to be significantly different from SE and INT and SE and KNOW. An unexpected finding was that the correlation between SE and EXP was not found to be different from that between SE and NORMS.

Table 3. Correlations between Self-Efficacy and Selected Scales using Baseline Data (N=678).

SE EXP INT KNOW RISK NORMS
SE 1.00
EXP .37 1.00
INT .50 .14 1.00
KNOW .51 .33 .29 1.00
RISK -.21 -.06 -.13 -.14 1.00
NORMS .38 .24 .23 .26 -.29 1.00

SE= Self-Efficacy, EXP=Prior Experience, INT=Screening Intention, KNOW=Knowledge, RISK=Perceived Risk, NORMS=Subjective Norms

Note: All correlations except the one between EXP and RISK were significantly different in the predicted direction.

Table 4. T-tests for Dependent Samples Correlations between Self-Efficacy and Selected Scales (N=678).

Test for Equal Correlations t p
rSE,EXP
rSE,RISK 11.55 < .0001
rSE,NORM -.24 .81
rSE,INT
rSE,RISK 14.60 < .0001
rSE,NORM 3.10 .002
rSE,KNOW
rSE,RISK 14.82 < .0001
rSE,NORM 3.43 .0006

SE= Self-Efficacy, EXP=Prior Experience, INT=Screening Intention, KNOW=Knowledge, RISK=Perceived Risk,

We conducted Chi-square tests of all demographic variables with Pap test screening adherence (Table 5) and t-tests of demographic variables with self-efficacy (Table 6) to determine significant associations prior to entering variables into logistic regression analysis. All of the demographic variables except education and birth status were related to self-efficacy or Pap test adherence. Significant demographic variables were entered as a block of variables and self-efficacy was then included as a separate predictor variable. Results, shown in Table 7, show an independent effect of self-efficacy on Pap test screening.

Table 5. Univariate Analyses of Demographic Variables and Pap test Adherence (N=678).

Variable Pap Adherent (Within 3 years)
YES
Pap Adherent (Within 3 years)
NO
Chi-Square
P value


% N % N
Age (n=673) .003
50-59 years 68.3 228 31.7 106
60 -69 years 67.4 128 32.6 62
70 years and older 53.0 79 47.0 70
Education (n=657) .446
None 62.7 37 37.3 22
1-5 years 65.2 204 34.8 109
6 -11 years 62.4 138 37.6 83
12 or more years 73.4 47 26.6 17
Birth Status (n=650) .935
Born in U.S. 65.0 91 35.0 49
Born in Mexico <20 yrs in U.S. 65.2 90 34.8 47
Born in Mexico and >20 yrs in U.S. 63.6 237 36.4 136
Insurance (n=673) .162
Any 66.8 260 33.2 129
None 61.6 175 38.4 109
Marital Status (n=673) .013
Married or Living Together 67.9 309 32.1 146
Never Married, Separated, Divorced or Widowed 57.8 126 42.2 92
Income (n=672) .053
None 58.1 25 41.9 18
<$5,000 63.8 67 36.2 38
$5,000-$9,999 70.5 136 29.5 57
$10,000 or more 67.0 146 33.0 72
Don't Know 54.0 61 46.0 52

Table 6. Univariate Analyses of Demographic Variables and Self-efficacy (N=678).

Variable Mean Baseline
Self-Efficacy (SD)
t df P value
Age .635 672 .526
50-59 years 4.15 (.879)
60 and older 4.11 (.863)
Education -1.05 656 .294
0-5 years 4.10 (.850)
6 or more years 4.17 (.874)
Birth Status -.208 649 .836
Born in Mexico <20 yrs in U.S. 4.11 (.756)
Born in Mexico and >20 yrs in U.S. or Born in U.S. 4.13 (.907)
Insurance -2.478 672 .013
Yes 4.20 (.781)
No 4.03 (.975)
Marital Status 2.574 672 .010
Married or Living Together 4.19 (.839)
Never Married, Separated, Divorced or Widowed 4.01 (.922)
Income -.441 558 .659
$9,999 or Less 4.20 (.752)
$10,000 or more 4.23 (.746)

Table 7. Logistic Regression of Self-efficacy and Demographic Variables (N=678).

Variable Odds Ratio C.I.
Age Referent: 50-59 years
60 -69 years .78 .504, 1.212
70 years and older .57 .335, .959
Marital Status Referent: Married/Living Together
Never Married, Separated, Divorced, or Widowed .81 .549, 1.209
Income Referent: None
<$5,000 1.44 .665, 3.130
$5,000-$9,999 1.53 .742, 3.146
$10,000 or more 1.23 .601, 2.537
Don't Know 1.21 .557, 2.631
Insurance Referent: None
Any 1.38 .927, 2.64
Pap Self-Efficacy 2.69 2.107, 3.432

Sensitivity to change over time

Theoretically, women in the intervention group should have a greater change in self-efficacy from baseline levels than women in the control group. Among women in the intervention group (N=80) the baseline and follow-up self-efficacy scores were 3.63 and 4.28 (r=.184 and t=4.448) respectively, while scores in the comparison group (N=89) were 3.71 and 3.87 (r=.323 and t=1.312) for baseline and follow-up respectively. Using the effect size formula mention above, a moderate effect size (d=.635) was obtained for the intervention group and a small effect size of (d=.162) was obtained for the control group. The analysis estimating the effect size of the difference between change scores in the intervention and control group (d=2.45, CI=2.04, 2.84) indicated a greater change in self-efficacy over time in the intervention group (p<.000).

Discussion

Five of the six hypotheses for this study were supported and one was partially supported. The self-efficacy scale had a Cronbach alpha of .95, indicating good internal consistency. EFA indicated a single factor solution and all items loadings were > .73. CFA on an independent sample confirmed a single factor structure with all standardized loadings greater than .40 as hypothesized. In fact all loadings but one were larger than .80, indicating strong relationships between the items and the latent factor.

The hypothesized relationships with theoretical constructs were partially supported in that self-efficacy was found to be more highly correlated with knowledge, prior experience, and intention than with hypothesized unrelated constructs (perceived survivability, perceived risk, and perceived social norms). There was one exception. The correlation between self-efficacy and subjective norms was higher than expected (.38) and not significantly different from the correlation between self-efficacy and prior experience (a hypothesized related construct). This finding indicates that the perception that significant others want the woman to engage in the behavior (and the value she places on these desires) is correlated with the woman's own self-efficacy. To the extent that subjective norms represent an individual's belief that others not only want her to perform a behavior but also that they believe she can do it, may explain the correlation between self-efficacy and subjective norms. It seems likely to assume that others would not encourage a woman to complete a behavior unless they believed she was capable of doing so. This expressed confidence could impact the woman's own efficacy beliefs. Additionally, verbal persuasion and reinforcement as known methods to enhance self-efficacy (27) and it is likely that individuals endorsing perceived subjective norms have experienced this type of encouragement from their family, friends, and/or doctor concerning Pap test screening. Additionally, if subjective norms reflects actual external social influences, Bandura's concept of “reciprocal determinism” which posits a reciprocal causation among environmental, personal, and behavioral factors also supports this observed relationship(27). Studies to further examine the relationship between self-efficacy and subjective norms are warranted.

Logistic regression results supported the theoretical relationship between self-efficacy and health behavior in that women with higher self-efficacy were more likely to have had a recent Pap test than women with lower self-efficacy. Self-efficacy has been found to be an important determinant of many health behaviors (37, 39, 40, 42, 77), and these findings indicate that it is important for Pap test screening as well.

Finally, because one criterion for a good measure is its ability to detect change in a construct over time (28, 73), we hypothesized that the intervention group would show positive change in self-efficacy over time. This hypothesis was supported and the effect size associated with that change was in the high-medium range. This means that future studies using this measure of self-efficacy can plan on obtaining similar effect sizes when measuring change over time. This will allow for studies with higher levels of power at lower sample sizes than would be the case if only a small effect size had been found.

The limitations of the study include that cross sectional baseline data was used for the test of the theoretical relationship between self-efficacy and Pap test screening. This decision was made because of sample-size limitations in the longitudinal cohort of controls. Use of cross sectional data obscures the directionality of the relationship between Pap test self efficacy and actual screening behavior. It is possible that the association observed between self-efficacy and Pap test screening may reflect an increase in self-efficacy beliefs that resulted from completion of the Pap test. However, the intent of this analysis was to examine the expected association between self efficacy and Pap test screening behavior to test the validity of the construct. Whether self efficacy impacted screening behavior or vice versa, the findings still reflect an independent association between self efficacy and screening behavior, and thus contribute to the construct validity (concurrent validity) of the measure. Future studies should use longitudinal data to test the measure's predictive validity.

The lost to follow-up rate (33.1%) in this study represents another limitation. While we scheduled data collection during periods when we expected fewer women would be travelling for farm-work, migration schedules often vary and it is possible that women who were unable to be reached were migrating during that period. We also believe that the length of the interview (2 hours) may have deterred participation. Nevertheless, data indicated no demographic nor acculturation differences across study conditions between women follow-up and those lost to follow-up.

Another limitation is that the data for this study was generated from participants of Mexican-American origin, possibly limiting generalizability to other non-Mexican-origin Hispanics. Still, because the items developed for this self-efficacy scale reflect barriers relevant across Hispanic subgroups (11, 12, 78, 79) and were written in a style of Spanish easily understood by all Spanish speakers regardless of country of origin, there is potential for the portability of this measure for other Hispanic subgroups. Studies testing this instrument among English-speaking Hispanics and Hispanics of other national origins would add to the validity of this measure across Hispanic subgroups.

One of the unique features of this study is that it included the development and testing of a Spanish language self-efficacy scale for Pap test screening among Hispanics. Developing measures for Hispanic populations involves more than creating simple translations of English language scales but instead developing measures that are culturally relevant, addressing the behavioral tasks within the context and culturally-specific demands of the population. To ensure appropriate assessment of theoretical constructs, it is important that measures are both developed and tested in the language they will be used, or translated appropriately and tested to ensure that the characteristics of the measure have not changed. Since cervical cancer represents a significant problem among Hispanic women and Pap test screening continues to be underutilized it is essential to identify, assess, and address the factors influencing Pap test screening behavior.

Acknowledgments

Gratitude is given to the migrant health centers who participated in the pilot testing and development phases of this program: Brownsville Community Health Center, Nuestra Clinica Del Valle, Salud para la Gente, United Medical Centers, and La Clinica de Familia. Special thanks to the CLS Cancer Coalition members and the women who took part in this study. The authors wish to thank Karyn Popham for editorial assistance.

Funding: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention [U57/CCU614491]; National Institutes of Health [5K07CA79759-05 to M.F.]

Footnotes

*

Lecca, PJ, Greenstein, TN, McNeil, JS. A profile of Mexican American Health: Data from Hispanic Health and Nutrition Survey, 1982-1984. Arlington, TX: Health Services Research; 2001.

Cultivando La Salud: Breast and Cervical Cancer Replication and Dissemination Program, Final Report. Fernandez ME, Gonzales A, Saavedra M, Tortolero-Luna G, editors. Buda, TX: National Center for Farmworker Health; 2003. Supported by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.

Cultivando La Salud: Breast and Cervical Cancer Replication and Dissemination Program, Pilot Report. Gonzales A, Fernandez M, Saavedra M, Tortolero-Luna G, editors. Buda, TX: National Center for Farmworker Health; 2001.

§

Cultivando La Salud: Breast and Cervical Cancer Replication and Dissemination Program, Focus Group Report. Fernandez M, Gonzales A. Buda, TX: National Center for Farmworker Health; 2000.

Saavedra-Embesi M. Barriers to Breast and Cervical Cancer Screening among Migrant and Seasonal Farmworker Women in the Lower Rio Grande Valley, Texas [thesis]. University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston, School of Public Health (San Antonio campus); 2008.

Reference List

  • 1.Reis LAG, Melbert D, Krapcho M, et al. SEER Cancer Statistics Review, 1975-2004. 2007 Available from: http://seer.cancer.gov/csr/1975_2004/
  • 2.Centers for Disease Control and Prevention: National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion . Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion; 2007. Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System. Cited 2007 May 1. Available from: http://www.cdc.gov/brfss/index.htm. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Blackman DK, Bennett EM, Miller DS. Trends in self-reported use of mammograms (1989-1997) and Papanicolaou tests (1991-1997)--Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System. MMWR CDC Surveill Summ. 1999;48:1–22. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.U.S. Department of Health and Human Services . U S Department of Health and Human Services; 2001. Healthy People 2010. Cited 2008 Aug. 18. Available from: http://www.health.gov/healthypeople/document/tableofcontents.htm. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Coughlin SS, Uhler RJ, Richards T, Wilson KM. Breast and cervical cancer screening practices among Hispanic and non-Hispanic women residing near the United States-Mexico border, 1999-2000. Fam Community Health. 2003;26:130–139. doi: 10.1097/00003727-200304000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.American Cancer Society Cancer Facts and Figures for Hispanics 2006-2008. 2008 Available from: http://www.cancer.org/docroot/STT/content/STT_1x_Cancer_Fact__Figures_for_Hispanics_2006-2008.asp.
  • 7.Wu ZH, Black SA, Markides KS. Prevalence and associated factors of cancer screening: Why are so many older Mexican American women never screened? Prev Med. 2001;33:268–273. doi: 10.1006/pmed.2001.0880. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Otero-Sabogal R, Stewart S, Sabogal F, Brown BA, Perez-Stable EJ. Access and attitudinal factors related to breast and cervical cancer rescreening: Why are Latinas still underscreened? Health Educ Behav. 2003;30:337–359. doi: 10.1177/1090198103030003008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Fernandez ME, Tortolero-Luna G, Gold RS. Mammography and Pap test screening among low-income foreign-born Hispanic women in USA. Cad Saude Publica. 1998;14:133–147. doi: 10.1590/s0102-311x1998000700014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Selvin E, Brett KM. Breast and cervical cancer screening: Sociodemographic predictors among White, Black, and Hispanic women. Am J Public Health. 2003;93:618–623. doi: 10.2105/ajph.93.4.618. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Zambrana RE, Breen N, Fox SA, Gutierrez-Mohamed ML. Use of cancer screening practices by Hispanic women: analyses by subgroup. Prev Med. 1999;29:466–477. doi: 10.1006/pmed.1999.0566. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Suarez L, Ramirez AG, Villarreal R, et al. Social networks and cancer screening in four U.S. Hispanic groups. Am J Prev Med. 2000;19:47–52. doi: 10.1016/s0749-3797(00)00155-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Bazargan M, Bazargan SH, Farooq M, Baker RS. Correlates of cervical cancer screening among underserved Hispanic and African-American women. Prev Med. 2004;39:465–473. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2004.05.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Harmon MP, Castro FG, Coe K. Acculturation and cervical cancer: knowledge, beliefs, and behaviors of Hispanic women. Women Health. 1996;24:37–57. doi: 10.1300/j013v24n03_03. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Somkin CP, McPhee SJ, Nguyen T, et al. The effect of access and satisfaction on regular mammogram and Papanicolaou test screening in a multiethnic population. Med Care. 2004;42:914–926. doi: 10.1097/01.mlr.0000135832.28672.61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.McMullin JM, De A I, Chavez LR, Hubbell FA. Influence of beliefs about cervical cancer etiology on Pap smear use among Latina immigrants. Ethn Health. 2005;10:3–18. doi: 10.1080/1355785052000323001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Austin LT, Ahmad F, McNally MJ, Stewart DE. Breast and cervical cancer screening in Hispanic women: a literature review using the health belief model. Womens Health Issues. 2002;12:122–128. doi: 10.1016/s1049-3867(02)00132-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Chavez LR, McMullin JM, Mishra SI, Hubbell FA. Beliefs matter: Cultural beliefs and the use of cervical cancer-screening tests. Am Anthropol. 2001;103:1114–1129. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Coughlin SS, Wilson KM. Breast and cervical cancer screening among migrant and seasonal farmworkers: a review. Cancer Detect Prev. 2002;26:203–209. doi: 10.1016/s0361-090x(02)00058-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Miller SM, Mischel W, O'Leary A, Mills M. From human papillomavirus (HPV) to cervical cancer: Psychosocial processes in infection, detection, and control. Ann Behav Med. 1996;18:219–228. doi: 10.1007/BF02895283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Fernandez LE, Morales A. Language and use of cancer screening services among border and non-border Hispanic Texas women. Ethn Health. 2007;12:245–263. doi: 10.1080/13557850701235150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Hayward RA, Shapiro MF, Freeman HE, Corey CR. Who gets screened for cervical and breast cancer? Results from a new national survey. Arch Intern Med. 1988;148:1177–1181. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Abraido-Lanza AF, Chao MT, Gammon MD. Breast and cervical cancer screening among Latinas and non-Latina whites. Am J Public Health. 2004;94:1393–1398. doi: 10.2105/ajph.94.8.1393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Hewitt M, Devesa SS, Breen N. Cervical cancer screening among U.S. women: analyses of the 2000 National Health Interview Survey. Prev Med. 2004;39:270–278. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2004.03.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Borrayo EA, Thomas JJ, Lawsin C. Cervical cancer screening among Latinas: the importance of referral and participation in parallel cancer screening behaviors. Women Health. 2004;39:13–29. doi: 10.1300/J013v39n02_02. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Bandura A, Adams NE. Analysis of self-efficacy theory of behavior change. Cognit Ther Res. 1977;1:287–308. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Bandura A. Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control. New York: W.H. Freeman; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Champion V, Skinner CS, Menon U. Development of a self-efficacy scale for mammography. Res Nurs Health. 2005;28:329–336. doi: 10.1002/nur.20088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Manne SL, Ostroff JS, Norton TR, et al. Cancer-specific self-efficacy and psychosocial and functional adaptation to early stage breast cancer. Ann Behav Med. 2006;31:145–154. doi: 10.1207/s15324796abm3102_6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Hogenmiller JR. Measures and predictors of Pap smear screening participation among inner-city sheltered women [dissertation] Omaha, NB: University of Nebraska Medical Center; 2003. p. 323. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Bandura A. Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychol Rev. 1977;84:191–215. doi: 10.1037//0033-295x.84.2.191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Strecher VJ, DeVellis BM, Becker MH, Rosenstock IM. The role of self-efficacy in achieving health behavior change. Health Educ Q. 1986;13:73–92. doi: 10.1177/109019818601300108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Maibach E, Murphy DA. Self-efficacy in health promotion research and practice: conceptualization and measurement. Health Educ Res. 1995;10:37–50. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Gonzalez JT, Gonzalez VM. Initial Validation of A Scale Measuring Self-Efficacy of Breast Self-Examination Among Low-Income Mexican-American Women. Hisp J Behav Sci. 1990;12:277–291. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Rew L, McDougall G, Riesch L, Parker C. Development of the self-efficacy for testicular self-examination scale. Journal of Men's Health and Gender. 2005;2:59–63. doi: 10.1016/j.jmhg.2004.12.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Resnick B, Luisi D, Vogel A, Junaleepa P. Reliability and validity of the self-efficacy for exercise and outcome expectations for exercise scales with minority older adults. J Nurs Meas. 2004;12:235–247. doi: 10.1891/jnum.12.3.235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Kang SY, Deren S, Andia J, Colon HM, Robles R. Effects of changes in perceived self-efficacy on HIV risk behaviors over time. Addict Behav. 2004;29:567–574. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2003.08.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Smith KW, McGraw SA, Costa LA, McKinlay JB. A self-efficacy scale for HIV risk behaviors: development and evaluation. AIDS Educ Prev. 1996;8:97–105. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Reece SM, Harkless GE. Perimenopausal health self-efficacy among Hispanic Caribbean and non-Hispanic white women. Health Care Women Int. 2006;27:223–237. doi: 10.1080/07399330500506519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Lorig KR, Ritter PL, Gonzalez VM. Hispanic chronic disease self-management: a randomized community-based outcome trial. Nurs Res. 2003;52:361–369. doi: 10.1097/00006199-200311000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Palmer RC, Fernandez ME, Tortolero-Luna G, Gonzales A, Mullen PD. Correlates of mammography screening among Hispanic women living in lower Rio Grande Valley farmworker communities. Health Educ Behav. 2005;32:488–503. doi: 10.1177/1090198105276213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Lorig K, Gonzalez VM, Ritter P. Community-based Spanish language arthritis education program: a randomized trial. Med Care. 1999;37:957–963. doi: 10.1097/00005650-199909000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Hogenmiller JR, Atwood JR, Lindsey AM, et al. Self-efficacy scale for Pap smear screening participation in sheltered women. Nurs Res. 2007;56:369–377. doi: 10.1097/01.NNR.0000299848.21935.8d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Hui C, Triandis HC. Measurement in cross-cultural psychology: A review and comparison of strategies. Cross Cultural Psychology. 1985;16:131–151. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Fernandez ME, Gonzales A, Tortolero-Luna G, et al. Effectiveness of Cultivando La Salud: A breast and cervical cancer screening education program for low-income Hispanic women living in farmworker communities. Am J Public Health. 2009 doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2008.136713. In press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Bennett DS. Depression among children with chronic medical problems: a meta-analysis. J Pediatr Psychol. 1994;19:149–169. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/19.2.149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Israel BA, Eng E, Schulz AJ, Parker EA, editors. Methods in Community-Based Participatory Research for Health. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Krieger J, Allen CA, Roberts JW, Ross LC, Takaro TK. What's with the wheezing? Methods used by the Seattle-King County Healthy Homes Project to assess exposure to indoor asthma triggers. In: Israel BA, Eng E, Schulz AJ, Parker EA, editors. Methods in Community-Based Participatory Research for Health. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass; 2005. pp. 230–250. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Marin G, Gamba RJ. A new measurement of acculturation for Hispanics: The Bidimensional Acculturation Scale for Hispanics (BAS) Hisp J Behav Sci. 1996;18:297–316. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Ajzen I. The theory of planned behavior. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process. 1991;50:179–211. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Janz NK, Becker MH. The Health Belief Model: A decade later. Health Educ Q. 1984;11:1–47. doi: 10.1177/109019818401100101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Vernon SW, Myers RE, Tilley BC. Development and validation of an instrument to measure factors related to colorectal cancer screening adherence. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 1997;6:825–832. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Carpenter V, Colwell B. Cancer knowledge, self-efficacy, and cancer screening behaviors among Mexican-American women. J Cancer Educ. 1995;10:217–222. doi: 10.1080/08858199509528377. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.McGorry SY. Measurement in cross-cultural environment: Survey translation issues. Qualitative Market Research: An International Journal. 2000;2:74–81. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Vinokurov A, Geller D, Martin TL. Translation as an ecological tool for instrument development. International Journal of Qualitative Methods. 2007;6:3. [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Brislin RW. Comparative research methodology: cross-cultural studies. Int J Psychol. 1976;11:215–229. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.SPSS for Windows. Chicago: SPSS, Inc; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Floyd FJ, Widaman KF. Factor analysis in the development and refinement of clinical assessment instruments. Psychol Assess. 1995;7:299. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Steiger JH. Tests for comparing elements of a correlation matrix. Psychol Bull. 1980;87:245–251. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Jöreskog KG, Sörbom D, SPSS Inc . LISREL 8 user's reference guide. Chicago, IL: Scientific Software International; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Bentler PM. Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychol Bull. 1990;107:238–246. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Tucker LR, Lewis C. A reliability coefficient for maximum likelihood factor analysis. Psychometrika. 1973;38:10. [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Browne MW, Cudeck R. Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In: Bollen KA, Long JS, editors. Testing structural equation models. Newbury Park: Sage Publications; 1993. pp. 136–162. [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Hu L, Bentler PM. Evaluating model fit. In: Hoyle RH, editor. Structural equation modeling: Concepts, issues, and applications. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications; 1995. pp. 76–99. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Schwarzer R, editor. Self-efficacy: Thought control of action. Washington, DC: Hemisphere; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Bralock AR, Koniak-Griffin D. Relationship, power, and other influences on self-protective sexual behaviors of African American female adolescents. Health Care Women Int. 2007;28:247–267. doi: 10.1080/07399330601180123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Bandura A. Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall; 1986. [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Bandura A. The explanatory and predictive scope of self-efficacy theory. J Soc Clin Psychol. 1986;4:359–373. [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Institute of Medicine, Board on Neuroscience and Behavioral Health, Committee on Communication for Behavior Change in the 21st Century: Improving the Health of Diverse Populations Speaking of Health: Assessing Health Communication Strategies for Diverse Populations. Washington, DC: National Academies Press; 2002. Theory [Chapter 2] pp. 28–75. [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Rosenstock IM. Historical origins of the Health Belief Model. Health Educ Monogr. 1974;2:328–335. [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Becker MH. The health belief model and personal health behavior. Health Educ Monogr. 1974;2:324–508. [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Janz NK, Champion VL, Strecher VJ. The health belief model. In: Glanz K, Rimer BK, Lewis FM, editors. Health Behavior and Health Education: Theory, Research, and Practice. 3rd. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass; 2002. pp. 45–66. [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Stewart BJ, Archbold PG. Nursing intervention studies require outcome measures that are sensitive to change: Part One. Res Nurs Health. 1992;15:477–481. doi: 10.1002/nur.4770150610. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Dunst CJ, Hamby DW, Trivette CM. Guidelines for Calculating Effect Sizes for Practice-Based Research Syntheses. Centerscope. 2004;3:1–10. [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd. Hillsdatle, NH: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Byrne BM. Structural equation modeling with AMOS: Basic concepts, applications, and programming. Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Palmer RC, Fernandez ME, Tortolero-Luna G, Gonzales A, Dolan Mullen P. Acculturation and mammography screening among Hispanic women living in farmworker communities. Cancer Control. 2005;12:21–27. doi: 10.1177/1073274805012004S04. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Goldman RE, Risica PM. Perceptions of breast and cervical cancer risk and screening among Dominicans and Puerto Ricans in Rhode Island. Ethn Dis. 2004;14:32–42. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Gorin SS, Heck JE. Cancer screening among Latino subgroups in the United States. Prev Med. 2005;40:515–526. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2004.09.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES