Skip to main content
The Journal of Neuroscience logoLink to The Journal of Neuroscience
. 2002 Mar 1;22(5):1807–1815. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-05-01807.2002

Extended Plasticity of Visual Cortex in Dark-Reared Animals May Result from Prolonged Expression of cpg15-Like Genes

Wei-Chung Allen Lee 1, Elly Nedivi 1
PMCID: PMC3062906  NIHMSID: NIHMS226330  PMID: 11880509

Abstract

cpg15 is an activity-regulated gene that encodes a membrane-bound ligand that coordinately regulates growth of apposing dendritic and axonal arbors and the maturation of their synapses. These properties make it an attractive candidate for participating in plasticity of the mammalian visual system. Here we comparecpg15 expression during normal development of the rat visual system with that seen in response to dark rearing, monocular blockade of retinal action potentials, or monocular deprivation. Our results show that the onset of cpg15 expression in the visual cortex is coincident with eye opening, and it increases until the peak of the critical period at postnatal day 28 (P28). This early expression is independent of both retinal activity and visual experience. After P28, a component of cpg15 expression in the visual cortex, lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN), and superior colliculus (SC) develops a progressively stronger dependence on retinally driven action potentials. Dark rearing does not affectcpg15 mRNA expression in the LGN and SC at any age, but it does significantly affect its expression in the visual cortex from the peak of the critical period and into adulthood. In dark-reared rats, the peak level of cpg15 expression in the visual cortex at P28 is lower than in controls. Rather than showing the normal decline with maturation, these levels are maintained in dark-reared animals. We suggest that the prolonged plasticity in the visual cortex that is seen in dark-reared animals may result from failure to downregulate genes such as cpg15 that could promote structural remodeling and synaptic maturation.

Keywords: dark rearing, visual system, cpg15, critical period, cortex, plasticity, TTX, monocular deprivation


The critical period for susceptibility to environmental influences is one of the central concepts to emerge from studies of visual system development. The capacity for change in response to manipulation of patterned visual input is normally restricted to a defined age window during postnatal development (Hubel and Wiesel, 1970; Hubel et al., 1977). However, in kittens or rats dark-reared (DR) past the end of the normal critical period, manipulation of visual input results in significant plasticity of cortical binocular connectivity as measured by electrophysiological recordings (Cynader and Mitchell, 1980; Mower et al., 1981; Fagiolini et al., 1994; Guire et al., 1999). This prolonged plasticity is not accompanied by a prolonged plasticity measured by anatomical methods. When visualized by autoradiography of geniculocortical afferents, the segregation of ocular dominance columns is incomplete in DR cats and is not altered by later visual manipulations (Mower et al., 1985). The complexity of interpreting dark-rearing experiments highlights our poor understanding of the molecular mechanisms that underlie plasticity during the critical period.

Electrophysiological studies of the rodent visual system show that cortical neurons develop well-defined functional properties relating to eye preference and orientation selectivity, although they are not anatomically segregated according to these properties as is seen in the visual cortex (VC) of cats and monkeys (Parnavelas et al., 1981; Maffei et al., 1992; Fagiolini et al., 1994; Gordon and Stryker, 1996;Antonini et al., 1999). In a manner similar to that in cats and monkeys, cells in the primary visual cortex of rodents are capable of undergoing shifts in eye preference during a critical period in development (Drager, 1978; Fagiolini et al., 1994; Gordon and Stryker, 1996; Guire et al., 1999). Anatomical studies show that in mice, monocular deprivation (MD) during the critical period affects arbor growth of thalamocortical afferents (Antonini et al., 1999). These studies, in combination with new technologies for manipulating mouse genetics, argue that the rodent visual projection can be a valuable experimental system for examining the cellular and molecular mechanisms of developmental plasticity.

Candidate plasticity gene 15 (cpg15) was isolated in a screen for seizure-induced genes in the dentate gyrus of the rat hippocampus (Nedivi et al., 1993). cpg15 expression is regulated by light in the visual cortex of adult rats (Nedivi et al., 1996); its localization and regulation in the developing visual system of cats are consistent with a role in activity-dependent plasticity (Corriveau et al., 1999). cpg15 overexpression in the developing Xenopus optic tectum induces exuberant growth of dendritic arbors in tectal cells (Nedivi et al., 1998); this is accompanied by enhanced growth of retinal axon arbors and retinotectal synapse formation (Cantallops et al., 2000). The effects ofcpg15 on these different aspects of circuit formation are all non-cell autonomous, consistent with its glycosylphosphatidylinositol cell-surface attachment (Nedivi et al., 1998; Cantallops et al., 2000).

The potential role of cpg15 in dendritic and axonal arbor restructuring and synaptic maturation led us to study the effect of dark rearing on its expression and regulation. We monitoredcpg15 mRNA levels in the visual structures of the rat before, during, and after the cortical critical period for the development of eye-specific preference. We compared the effects of dark rearing with those of retinal action potential blockade and MD. Our results suggest a mechanism whereby the visual cortex of DR animals maintains a capacity for delayed plasticity through elevated levels of genes such as cpg15 that can enhance local circuit remodeling and new synaptic stabilization. These results may provide a clue to the intriguing discrepancy seen in DR animals between the capacity for change as measured by anatomical methods and that measured by electrophysiological methods.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Animal manipulations and tissue isolation. All animal work was approved by the Massachusetts Institute of Technology Committee on Animal Care; it conforms to National Institutes of Health guidelines for the use and care of vertebrate animals. Wistar–Kyoto rats (Taconic, Germantown, NY) were housed either in a room with a 12 hr light/dark cycle or in a room sealed from visible light. A 15 W safelight shielded by a number 2 Kodak dark-red filter (Eastman Kodak, Rochester, NY) was used for the daily care and maintenance of animals housed in the darkroom (∼30 min/d). At various points during the experiment, Polaroid (Cambridge, MA) photographic paper placed in the darkroom was monitored for exposure. In cats, intermittent exposure to a safelight can be sufficient to prevent the apparent extension of the critical period by dark rearing. To test whether exposure to a safelight affected the dark rearing of rats, a group of animals handled with an infrared viewing system (950 nm) was raised in the dark to 3, 4, 5, and 8 weeks of age (each group,n = 3). The levels of cpg15 expression in the rats handled with intermittent exposure to a safelight were not significantly different from those handled under infrared conditions; both were significantly different from their age-matched controls at 4 and 8 weeks (data not shown). The difference in sensitivity to exposure to a safelight may reflect the relatively poor vision of albino rats compared with cats.

Rats were reared under normal conditions (12 hr light/dark cycle), dark-reared from birth, or dark-adapted at different developmental time points. DR and age-matched control animals were killed postnatally at progressive times, starting at postnatal day 10 (P10) and at 1 week intervals from P14 (the approximate day of eye opening) to 8 weeks (P10, each group, n = 2; P14, P21, 5 weeks, 6 weeks, each group, n = 4; 4 weeks, DR, n = 4; controls, n = 3; 8 weeks DR, n = 3; controls, n = 5). A group of DR 8-week-old rats was exposed to light for 6, 12, or 24 hr or for 7 or 14 d (n = 2–3 for each time point). Dark-adapted adult rats were placed in the dark for 2 weeks and killed in the dark, with a parallel set killed after re-exposure to 24 hr of light (each group,n = 3). Rats that were raised as dark-adapted until adulthood were raised normally to 2, 3, 4, or 5 weeks; placed in the dark until 8 weeks; and killed in the dark or after re-exposure to light for 24 hr (n = 3 per group per time point). Two additional groups of rats were allowed a 1 week window of visual experience [between weeks 3 and 4 (Lt3–4) or between weeks 4 and 5 (Lt4–5)] during dark rearing. At 8 weeks, these animals were killed in the dark (each group, n = 3) or after 24 hr of exposure to light (Lt3–4, n = 3; Lt4–5,n = 2).

Monocular blockades of retinal action potentials were done essentially as described previously (Prusky and Ramoa, 1999). A strip of Elvax containing either tetrodotoxin (TTX) or citrate buffer was surgically implanted into the vitreous of the left eye of each animal for 3 d of sustained release, starting at P11, P18, P25, P32, P39, and P53 (TTX, n = 3; citrate, n = 2). Briefly, 200 mg of washed Elvax beads (DuPont NEN, Wilmington, DE) were dissolved in methylene chloride. The dissolved Elvax was mixed with 20 μl of 1% Fast Green in dimethylsulfoxide and either 20 μl of 0.3m TTX in citrate buffer (Calbiochem, La Jolla, CA) or 20 μl of 18.6 mm citrate buffer (Sigma, St. Louis, MO). The methylene chloride was slowly evaporated over the course of 1 d at −70°C and for 5 d subsequently at −20°C. The Elvax was then sectioned into 180-μm-thick disks by cryostat and stored at −80°C. Before surgeries, the Elvax was washed in 70% ethanol for 30 min and twice in sterile PBS for 30 min. For younger animals, the Elvax was cut into ∼2 × 0.75 mm strips. For older animals, the Elvax strips were ∼4 × 1 mm. The strips were carefully inserted into the vitreous after making a small incision at the edge of the sclera. One eye in each animal was treated with either TTX or citrate Elvax; the other eye remained intact. Anesthesia was maintained by halothane/O2via mask. After Elvax implantation, the rats recovered from anesthesia under observation. The effectiveness of activity blockade was monitored daily by assaying for consensual pupil response to bright white illumination under light halothane/O2anesthesia.

MD by eyelid suture was initiated at P18, P25, P32, P39, and P53 (for each time point, n = 3). Under ketamine/xylazine (80/10 mg/kg) anesthesia, the area surrounding the left eye was cleaned with povidone-iodine and isopropyl alcohol. The lid margins were trimmed and the eye was flushed with sterile PBS. Two to three horizontal mattress sutures using 6.0 Ethilon (Johnson & Johnson, Somerville, NJ) closed the length of the apposed lids. Ophthalmic ointment (Fougera, Melville, NY) was applied and the animals were monitored for recovery. For the P14 time point (n = 3), rather than suture the unopened eye, tissue adhesive (Vetbond; 3M, St. Paul, MN) was applied at P11 to prevent possible eye opening. After 3 d of TTX blockade or MD, animals were decapitated by guillotine; the brains were removed immediately and trimmed and positioned for coronal sectioning before being frozen on powdered dry ice and stored at −80°C.

In situ hybridization. Coronal sections (10 μm) through the anterior visual cortex were sectioned by cryostat, thaw-mounted on Superfrost/plus microscope slides (VWR Scientific, West Chester, PA), dried, fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde, washed in PBS, dehydrated in ethanol, air-dried, and stored desiccated at −80°C. Before hybridization, slides were pretreated (at room temperature, unless otherwise stated) with 0.2m HCl (20 min), double distilled water (DDW) (5 min), 2× SSC (30 min at 70°C), and DDW (5 min). The next prehybridization treatments, from pronase (type XIV) (Sigma) to air-drying slides for 1 hr, were conducted as described previously (Hogan et al., 1994). RNA probes were synthesized with an RNA transcription kit (Stratagene, La Jolla, CA) and35S-UTP (800 Ci/mmol; Amersham Biosciences, Piscataway, NJ), using linearized cpg15 cDNA as a template. Hybridizations were done as described previously (Nedivi et al., 1996). Posthybridization wash conditions were as follows: 3 hr at 50°C in 50% formamide and 1× salt solution (Hogan et al., 1994) with 10 mm DTT; 15 min at 37°C in TNE (10 mm Tris, pH 7.5, 0.5 mNaCl, 1 mm EDTA); 30 min at 37°C in TNE containing RNase A (20 μg/ml; Sigma); 30 min at 37°C in TNE; and finally overnight at 50°C in 50% formamide and 1× salt solution. Slides were dehydrated with 0.3 mNH4Ac in ethanol, air-dried, and processed for autoradiography as described previously (Hogan et al., 1994), using autoradiographic emulsion type NTB-2 (Eastman Kodak) diluted 1:1 with 2% glycerol, and exposed for 3–5 d at 4°C.

Quantitative data analysis. Dark-field images of two to four sections from each brain were imported into Adobe Photoshop 5.0 (Adobe Systems, San Jose, CA) with a Diagnostic Instruments (Sterling Heights, MI) Spot2 digital camera mounted on a Nikon (Tokyo, Japan) Eclipse E600 using a 1×/0.04 Plan ultrawide objective. Images were saved as gray-scale TIFFs and imported into NIH Image (version 1.62). In addition to the VC and the superior colliculus (SC), each section contained the medial geniculate body (MGB) of the thalamus, an auditory sensory area (Fig. 1). Areas were defined by Nissl staining of alternate sections. Mean pixel density measurements were taken from four areas on each section: the VC, the SC, the background, and the MGB. Pixel density was measured on a 0–255 scale, on which 255 is white. The background served as a zero-labeling negative control, whereas the MGB served as a positive control with a high level of labeling that is unaffected by visual manipulations. VC and SC measurements were normalized on a scale of 0–1 interpolating between the background (0) and MGB (1) values. The background mean pixel density was first subtracted from the mean pixel densities in the VC, the SC, and the MGB, yielding the net mean pixel densities for each area. The net mean pixel density in the VC or SC was then divided by the net mean pixel density from the MGB in the same section. Statistical significance was determined by unpaired Student's t test. To confirm that measurements were unbiased, image files from four groups (4 week and 8 week controls, and 4 week and 8 week DR) were coded, mixed, and remeasured by technicians who were unaware of the experimental treatment. All data sets were not significantly different between blind and nonblind measurements.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

In situ hybridization on a coronal section through rat visual cortex using a cpg15 probe. The regions relevant for quantification are outlined. Mean pixel densities were measured in four areas on each section: the VC, the SC, the MGB, and an area of the section with zero labeling for background (BG). The background is used as the zero point, whereas the MGB served as a positive control with a high level of labeling that is unaffected by visual manipulations (see Materials and Methods).

RESULTS

Developmental profile of cpg15 expression

To investigate a role for cpg15 in the activity-dependent phase of the development of the rat visual system,in situ hybridizations with a cpg15 probe were conducted on sections through visual structures starting at P10. The lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN), SC, and VC were monitored before and after eye opening, throughout the critical period for development of eye-specific preference in the binocular zone of primary VC, and in the adult. At birth and up to P10, expression of cpg15 is undetectable in the neocortex (Nedivi et al., 1996). At P10, 4 d before eye opening, low levels of cpg15 mRNA were visible at the apex of the cortical hemispheres (Fig.2). General onset of cpg15expression in the VC occurred coincident with eye opening at 2 weeks after birth, with the highest expression in layers 2/3, 4, and 6. Subsequently, cpg15 mRNA expression levels gradually increased, peaking at P28, the height of the physiologically characterized critical period for the development of eye-specific preference in the VC. cpg15 mRNA levels then declined to a lower basal adult level. These results show that cpg15levels in the rat VC correspond well with the electrophysiologically mapped critical period.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

Developmental time course of cpg15expression in the VC of normal and DR rats. Representative dark-field photomicrographs of in situ hybridizations forcpg15 mRNA are shown. Coronal sections through the VC of normal animals (left) and dark-reared animals (right) are shown at the designated postnatal ages. The onset of cpg15 expression in the neocortex for both normal and DR animals is coincident with eye opening at 2 weeks of age. Normally, cpg15 expression peaks at 4 weeks and then declines to a lower basal adult level. In DR animals,cpg15 levels remain elevated past 5 weeks.Arrowheads point to the earliestcpg15 expression at the medial apex of the neocortex. Some individual panels (untreated, 6 weeks; DR, 6 weeks; untreated, 4 weeks; DR, 4 weeks; DR, 3 weeks; and DR, 2 weeks) were normalized with respect to the MGB for assembly of the montage. Scale bar, 1 mm.

Effect of dark rearing on cpg15 expression

To test whether regulation of cpg15 expression was sensitive to visual input, rats were deprived of visual experience by dark rearing to various developmental ages. cpg15 mRNA levels were compared in DR animals and age-matched controls. Dark rearing did not alter the time of onset or the early increase in levels of cpg15 expression in the VC (Fig. 2). At P28,cpg15 mRNA levels in DR animals plateaued, but at a significantly lower level of expression than in age-matched controls (Figs. 2 and 3a). This level of expression persisted in older DR animals, so that after 8 weeks of dark rearing, cpg15 expression levels were significantly higher than in their age-matched controls, in which cpg15expression normally declined after P28 (Figs. 2 and 3a). Differences in cpg15 mRNA levels between normal and DR animals were not seen in the LGN or SC (data not shown). The enhanced expression levels of cpg15 in the VC, seen with dark rearing, may be a molecular indicator of the prolonged plasticity specific to this region.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3.

Quantification of cpg15 expression in the VC of control (normal) rats, DR rats, rats after blockade of retinal activity, or rats after MD. In all cases, after background subtraction, the net average pixel value in the VC was normalized by the net average pixel value of the MGB (see Materials and Methods for details). a, Effect of dark rearing oncpg15 expression in the VC. Data from four in situ experiments are shown, of which Figure 2 is representative.Solid circles, DR animals; open circles, untreated control animals. Error bars indicate the SEM. Points marked with asterisks are significantly different between normal and DR animals (at 4 weeks, p = 0.001; at 8 weeks, p = 0.016; unpaired Student'st test). b, Effect oncpg15 expression in the VC of monocular TTX blockade for 3 d initiated at different developmental times. Data from threein situ experiments are shown, of which Figure 5 is representative. Solid circles, VC contralateral to TTX blockade; open circles, VC of control animals implanted with a citrate control in the contralateral eye. Error bars indicate the SEM. Points marked with asterisks are significantly different between the VC of control and TTX-treated rats (at 5 weeks,p = 0.0012; at 6 weeks, p = 0.032; at 8 weeks, p = 0.0008; unpaired Student'st test). c, Effect oncpg15 expression in the VC of MD for 3 d by eyelid suture initiated at different developmental time points. Solid circles, VC contralateral to the sutured eye; open circles, VC of untreated control animals. Error bars indicate the SEM. Points marked with asterisks are significantly different between the VC of control and MD rats (at 4 weeks,p = 0.003; at 5 weeks, p = 0.006; at 6 weeks, p = 0.007; unpaired Student'st test).

Reportedly, a short exposure to light can trigger the end of the delayed plasticity seen in DR animals (Mower et al., 1983; Philpot et al., 2001). To test whether a late onset of visual experience affectscpg15 expression, animals were dark-reared to 8 weeks and then exposed to light for 6, 12, or 24 hr or for 7 or 14 d.In situ hybridizations show that with exposure to light of up to 12 hr, cpg15 expression remains significantly higher than normal (Fig. 4). After 24 hr of exposure to light, DR animals showed cpg15 mRNA expression levels that were comparable with those found in normally raised adults (Fig. 4). These experiments indicate that brief visual experience in DR adults can reset cpg15 expression to normal levels. This downregulation of expression may be a molecular representation of the light-induced end of the delayed plasticity in DR animals.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4.

Light triggers downregulation ofcpg15 expression in DR rats. Quantification ofcpg15 expression in the VC of animals DR to 8 weeks and subsequently exposed to light (LT) for designated times. Data from two in situ experiments are shown. In each section, the net average pixel value in the VC is normalized by the net average pixel value of the MGB. Abnormally high levels ofcpg15 expression in animals dark-reared to 8 weeks decline to normal adult levels within 24 hr of exposure to light. Error bars indicate the SEM. Asterisks mark significant differences compared with normal animals at 8 weeks (DR, 8 weeks,p = 0.016; DR, 8 weeks plus 6 hr of light,p = 0.009; DR, 8 weeks plus 12 hr of light,p = 0.0086; unpaired Student's ttest).

Effect of blockade of retinal action potentials oncpg15 expression

To investigate whether cpg15 mRNA expression in the VC is dependent on retinally driven action potential activity, the Na+ channel blocker TTX was applied to the left retina of normally raised rats for 3 d periods, starting at different developmental times. Age-matched control animals were treated with citrate buffer applied to the left retina. cpg15 mRNA levels in the VC contralateral to the TTX-treated eye were compared with those in the VC contralateral to the citrate-treated eye in control animals. There was no change in the onset of cpg15expression in response to monocular retinal activity blockade, and there was no significant influence on cortical levels ofcpg15 mRNA during early postnatal development (Fig.5). Mean levels of cpg15expression at 2, 3, and 4 weeks after birth were not significantly different between the VC contralateral to the blocked eye and the VC contralateral to the citrate-treated controls (Fig.3b). In contrast, after the peak of the critical period and into adulthood, monocular retinal action potential blockade decreasedcpg15 expression in the VC contralateral to the blocked eye (Figs. 3b and 5).

Fig. 5.

Fig. 5.

cpg15 expression in the VC after 3 d of monocular TTX blockade initiated at different developmental times. Dark-field photomicrographs of in situhybridizations for cpg15 mRNA in coronal sections through the VC contralateral to the treated eye of either control animals (left) implanted with the citrate vehicle or TTX-treated animals (right), at the designated ages. Starting at 5 weeks after birth, retinal TTX blockade decreases levels of cpg15 expression in the VC (see cortical area betweenarrowheads). Starting at 4 weeks after birth, retinal TTX blockade causes a downregulation of cpg15 expression in the SC (SC indicated by two arrows). Some individual panels (citrate, 4 weeks; TTX, 4 weeks; TTX, 3 weeks) in this figure were normalized with respect to the MGB for assembly of the montage. Scale bar, 1 mm.

Monocular TTX blockade also decreased cpg15 expression in the contralateral LGN and SC starting 3 weeks after birth. For both the LGN and superficial layers of the SC contralateral to the treated eye, the effect of TTX blockade became more pronounced with age (LGN not shown; for SC, see Figs. 5 and 6). These experiments indicate that in all of the visual structures tested, the developmental regulation of cpg15 expression is divided into two phases. Early cpg15 expression is independent of retinally driven action potentials. During the critical period for development of eye-specific preference in the VC, an activity-dependent component of cpg15 expression emerges, and the effect of retinal action potential blockade becomes progressively more pronounced with age.

Fig. 6.

Fig. 6.

Quantification of cpg15 expression in the SC after 3 d of monocular TTX blockade initiated at different developmental times. Data from three in situexperiments are shown, of which Figure 5 is representative. After background subtraction, the net average pixel value in superficial layers of the SC was normalized by the net average pixel value of the MGB. cpg15 expression in the SC is depressed by retinal TTX blockade soon after eye opening. Solid circles, SC contralateral to TTX blockade; open circles, SC contralateral to citrate control. Error bars indicate SEM. Points marked with asterisks are significantly different between the SC of control and TTX-treated rats (at 4 weeks,p = 0.026; at 5 weeks, p = 0.0003; at 6 weeks, p = 0.027; at 8 weeks,p = 0.011; unpaired Student's ttest).

Effect of monocular deprivation oncpg15 expression

It has been shown that total blockade of retinal activity produces a smaller shift in ocular dominance plasticity than that produced by MD (Rittenhouse et al., 1999). To investigate whether the effect of monocular activity blockade on cpg15 expression is less extreme than that of MD, we monocularly deprived normally raised rats for 3 d periods, starting at different developmental times.cpg15 levels in the VC and the SC contralateral to the sutured eye were compared with the VC and the SC in normal animals. As in both DR and TTX-treated animals, there was no change in the onset ofcpg15 expression in the VC of MD rats (Fig. 3c). During the next 4 weeks of development, cpg15 expression in the VC contralateral to the MD eye was significantly less than in normal animals. Unlike animals whose retinally driven action potentials are blocked by TTX, at the peak of the critical period (P28) MD animals exhibit levels of cpg15 expression that are significantly less than those of controls. With maturation, the effect of MD becomes less significant, and at 8 weeks after birth, cpg15expression in the deprived VC is no longer different from controls. This trend is opposite to the effect of TTX, which becomes more significant as the animals mature. As in DR animals and in contrast to animals with retinal activity blockade, monocular deprivation did not affect cpg15 levels in the contralateral SC.

Effect of early visual experience on adultcpg15 expression

Previous studies suggest that early periods of visual experience are sufficient to trigger closure of the critical period despite later visual deprivation (Mower et al., 1983; Mower and Christen, 1985). To determine the window of early visual experience required for the development of normal adult regulation ofcpg15 expression in the VC, animals were raised in a normal 12 hr light/dark cycle to 2, 3, 4, or 5 weeks after birth and then transferred to a dark environment until adulthood at 8 weeks. Animals were killed without seeing any additional light or after 24 hr of re-exposure to light, to assess their ability to regulatecpg15 expression. In normally raised adult rats,cpg15 mRNA expression in the VC is downregulated after a 2 week period in the dark. After re-exposure to light for 24 hr, such dark-adapted rats will upregulatecpg15 levels (Nedivi et al., 1996) (Figs. 7h and8h). In contrast, rats raised in darkness to 8 weeks show abnormally high levels of cpg15expression in the VC; after exposure to 24 hr of light, the levels of expression are decreased (Figs. 4, 7a, and 8a). These two extremes were compared with animals exposed to visual experience at restricted windows during development. Animals exposed to visual experience for 2 or 3 weeks before being placed in the dark until they had reached adulthood displayed a regulation ofcpg15 expression that was essentially the same as that seen in DR animals (Figs. 7b,c, and 8b,c). Animals reared with normal visual experience for the first 4 or 5 weeks after birth and then placed in darkness until 8 weeks of age regulatecpg15 expression in the VC similarly to normal animals (Figs. 7d,e and 8d,e). These results suggest that 1 week of visual experience at the outset of the critical period is sufficient to confer normal patterns of adult cpg15regulation. To test this prediction, animals were raised to 8 weeks in darkness except for 1 week of light either between weeks 3 and 4 or between weeks 4 and 5. cpg15 expression in the VC was assayed at 8 weeks. cpg15 mRNA levels in animals allowed just 1 week of visual experience during the critical period are strikingly similar to those seen in normally raised animals and are dramatically induced on re-exposure to light for 24 hr (Fig.8f,g). Our results show that a restricted 1 week window of visual experience during the critical period is sufficient to determine adult patterns of cpg15 regulation in the VC.

Fig. 7.

Fig. 7.

Early visual experience confers normal adult patterns of cpg15 regulation. Representative dark-field photomicrographs of in situ hybridizations forcpg15 mRNA on coronal sections through the VC are shown. Animals were raised to different ages with normal visual experience and then transferred into a dark environment until 8 weeks of age (left), followed by a 24 hr re-exposure to light (right). Animals raised normally to 4 or 5 weeks of age (d, e) and then dark-reared to 8 weeks show the normal decline in cpg15 levels with maturation as well as the normal adult regulation (h) ofcpg15 expression by light (arrowheadsdelineate the VC). Similar to DR animals from birth (a), animals raised normally to 2 and 3 weeks of age (b, c) and then dark-reared to 8 weeks fail to downregulate cpg15 as adults and do not reinducecpg15 in response to light. Panelswere not normalized for montage assembly. Scale bar, 1 mm.

Fig. 8.

Fig. 8.

One week of visual experience during the critical period is sufficient to confer the normal adult patterns ofcpg15 regulation. Quantification of cpg15expression in the VC of rats with early visual experience. Measurements and calculations were done as described in Figure 3. The treatment legend is identical to Figure 7. A 1 week window of light during the critical period, either between weeks 3 and 4 (f) or weeks 4 and 5 (g), confers normal adult patterns of cpg15 regulation. Gray bars,cpg15 expression levels without re-exposure to light;black bars, levels after 24 hr of light exposure. Error bars indicate the SEM. Points marked with asterisks are significantly different between animals killed in the dark or after 24 hr of light exposure (e, h,p < 0.0001; f,p = 0.0049; g, p= 0.0022; d, p = 0.009; unpaired Student's t test).

DISCUSSION

The experiments described here address the temporal and spatial expression patterns of cpg15 in the developing visual system of the normal rat and in response to visual manipulations.cpg15 expression was monitored in relation to the critical period for development of eye-specific preference in the VC. We compared expression during normal development with that seen in response to dark rearing, monocular retinal action potential blockade, or MD. The regulation of cpg15 expression patterns suggests that it may serve as a molecular indicator of the potential for visual system plasticity.

Activity-independent and activity-dependent phases ofcpg15 expression

We found that in the visual system of rats, the onset ofcpg15 mRNA expression in the VC occurs 2 weeks after birth.cpg15 expression levels gradually rise and peak 2 weeks later, in the midst of the electrophysiologically mapped cortical critical period for shifts in eye preference. After its peak expression, cpg15 mRNA levels in the VC decline to a lower basal level in adults, concomitant with critical period closure at ∼6 weeks of age (Fagiolini et al., 1994; Gordon and Stryker, 1996). Although the onset of cpg15 expression is coincident with eye opening, it is unaffected by dark rearing, blockade of retinally driven action potentials, or MD. This is consistent with previous studies of cpg15 expression in the visual system of cats, which demonstrated that early cpg15 expression in the VC is activity independent (Corriveau et al., 1999). Therefore, the initial timing of cpg15 expression is likely to be controlled by a developmentally regulated activity-independent mechanism. This fits with electrophysiological and optical imaging studies that demonstrate that the basic structure of cortical maps is innate and develops in the absence of visual experience (Crair et al., 1998).

These studies also show that experience is necessary at a later stage of development for the refinement of ocular selectivity and to maintain responsiveness (Crair et al., 1998). We find that after the peak of the critical period, as the levels of cpg15 in the VC begin to decline, a component of cpg15 expression that is dependent on retinally driven action potentials becomes evident and is progressively more pronounced with age. Activity-dependent regulation of cpg15 expression in the SC and the LGN can be detected 1 week earlier than in the VC, perhaps because of the earlier maturation of these visual structures. Our results indicate that the regulation ofcpg15 expression is biphasic in all visual structures. Earlycpg15 expression is independent of retinally driven action potentials. With maturation during the critical period, an activity-dependent component of cpg15 expression emerges, and the effect of the blockade of retinally driven action potentials becomes progressively more pronounced with age. Activity-dependent regulation of cpg15 arises relatively late in development and may represent an adult feature of visual system plasticity.

Effect of visual experience oncpg15 expression

The aspects of cpg15 regulation that we found to be dependent on normal visual experience were its peak levels in the VC at P28 and its subsequent downregulation after the end of the critical period. Although cpg15 levels in the VC of DR rats are indistinguishable from those in control rats within the first 3 weeks after birth, the expression at P28 is lower than in controls. Rats DR beyond P28 maintain the same peak level of cpg15 expression through adulthood, levels that are significantly higher than in their control counterparts. There is a crossover of the cpg15expression profiles in normal and DR rats, such that at the peak of the critical period expression is higher in normal animals, whereas after the critical period cpg15 is higher in DR animals. This crossover can also be seen in the developmental profiles of susceptibility to MD in normal and DR cats (Mower, 1991). In rodents, there have been no studies that examine the effects of dark rearing on susceptibility to MD during the height of the critical period. Studies at later ages show that similar to cats, DR rats also retain a prolonged capability to respond to MD, even at P90 (Guire et al., 1999).

With respect to dark rearing, we show here two additional cases in which cpg15 regulation closely parallels plasticity as measured by susceptibility to MD. Electrophysiological studies have demonstrated that a short exposure to light can trigger the end of the delayed plasticity that results from dark rearing (Mower et al., 1983;Philpot et al., 2001). Similarly, early visual experience in DR kittens attenuates the effects of later dark rearing so that there is no delayed plasticity (Cynader, 1983). These results show that the effect of dark rearing can be negated by a short period of light exposure in DR adults, or with sufficient early visual experience. We find that a 24 hr exposure to light returns cpg15 levels in DR rats to those found in normally raised adults, and that 1 week of visual experience during the critical period is sufficient to confer normal adult patterns of cpg15 regulation.

Our results show that even 1 week of visual experience during the critical period is sufficient for establishing adult patterns ofcpg15 regulation. This suggests that visual experience is not persistently required during development for normal functional maturation of the visual system to occur. Rather, exposure to patterned vision for at least 1 week during the critical period can irreversibly trigger the molecular machinery that is required for maturation; this will likely result in normal adult responses to visual manipulations. Because cpg15 expression is abnormally high in the adult in the absence of visual experience, the molecular trigger for maturation may involve a general downregulation of plasticity genes such ascpg15.

Differences in the effect of DR on cpg15from effects of activity blockade and MD

The effect on cpg15 expression of binocular elimination of patterned vision during the critical period is profoundly different from the effect of retinal action potential blockade or MD. Dark rearing causes a prolonged upregulation of cpg15 expression starting at the peak of the critical period and continuing into adulthood, whereas retinal action potential blockade at the same developmental times causes a decrease in cpg15 expression. The effect of dark rearing is exclusive to the VC, whereas blockade of retinally driven action potentials affects cpg15 expression in the LGN and the SC as well as in the VC. This result corresponds well with the observation that the delayed plasticity seen in DR cats is not manifested in the LGN (Mower et al., 1985; Mower and Christen, 1985).

During the critical period, downregulation of cpg15expression in the VC by MD is more severe than that caused by retinal activity blockade. This is consistent with electrophysiological studies that show that MD during the critical period produces a greater shift of ocular dominance in the VC than monocular blockade of retinal activity (Rittenhouse et al., 1999). A possible explanation is that during MD the residual activity from the retina actively depresses the efficacy of synaptic connections driven by the deprived eye (Bear and Rittenhouse, 1999; Rittenhouse et al., 1999). Despite the fact that an MD eye is generating some activity as opposed to the total loss of activity caused by the blockade of retinally driven action potentials, during the critical period cpg15 levels are lower in the VC of MD animals than in animals after retinal TTX blockade. This could reflect the depression of cortical synaptic activity driven by the deprived eye.

Taken together, all of this indicates that regulation ofcpg15 expression does not correspond directly to levels of activity. Rather, it seems to reflect a propensity for functional plasticity.

Effect of dark rearing on expression of visually responsive genes

It has been proposed that mechanisms that underlie adult plasticity during learning and memory, or long-term potentiation and long-term depression, also play a key role in developmental plasticity (Kandel and O'Dell, 1992; Goodman and Shatz, 1993;Constantine-Paton and Cline, 1998; Nedivi, 1999). For this reason, many genes isolated or characterized on the basis of their response to activity in the adult have been investigated in the context of developmental plasticity in the visual system. These include both regulatory genes that encode transcription factors as well as effector genes that can directly affect neuronal morphology and function (for review, see Nedivi, 1999). Multiple genes show a transcriptional response to dark rearing, although the type of response varies. Whereas growth-associated protein 43 (GAP43), calcium calmodulin-dependent kinase II (CaMKII), and glutamic acid decarboxylase (GAD) all show the same increase in expression shown by cpg15 in response to dark rearing past the critical period (Neve and Bear, 1989), junB,zif/268, and BDNF show the opposite response and are downregulated (Rosen et al., 1992; Lein and Shatz, 2000). In contrast to the light-independent onset of cpg15expression, dark rearing prevents the normal onset and transcriptional increase of Homer, zif/268, andBDNF in the VC (Worley et al., 1990; Brakeman et al., 1997;Capsoni et al., 1999; Lein and Shatz, 2000). Subsequent exposure to light causes their rapid induction (Worley et al., 1990; Brakeman et al., 1997; Capsoni et al., 1999; Lein and Shatz, 2000). The transcriptional regulation of this latter group provides an accurate “molecular readout” of activity, whereas regulation ofcpg15 together with GAP43, CaMKII, andGAD corresponds more closely with the capacity for plasticity.

Summary

During the development of the Xenopus visual system,cpg15 concurrently regulates multiple aspects of retinotectal circuit formation (Cantallops et al., 2000). It promotes tectal cell dendritic arbor growth, stabilizes retinal axon arbors, and promotes maturation of retinotectal synapses (Nedivi et al., 1998;Cantallops et al., 2000). Our finding that DR rats fail to downregulatecpg15 raises the possibility that perhaps the residual plasticity measured electrophysiologically in these animals reflects an extended capacity for local synaptic remodeling. The prolonged plasticity seen in DR animals may result from failure to downregulate genes such as cpg15, which could promote structural remodeling and synaptic maturation.

Footnotes

This work was supported by grants from the National Eye Institute, the Ellison Medical Foundation, and the Alfred P. Sloan Foundation (E.N.). We are grateful to members of the Nedivi laboratory and to Drs. Hollis T. Cline, Martha Constantine-Paton, and Rachel Wong for critical review of this manuscript. We thank Matthew Colonnese for help with Elvax surgeries.

Correspondence should be addressed to Elly Nedivi, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, E18-670, 50 Ames Street, Cambridge, MA 02139. E-mail: nedivi@mit.edu.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Antonini A, Fagiolini M, Stryker MP. Anatomical correlates of functional plasticity in mouse visual cortex. J Neurosci. 1999;19:4388–4406. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-11-04388.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Bear MF, Rittenhouse CD. Molecular basis for induction of ocular dominance plasticity. J Neurobiol. 1999;41:83–91. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1097-4695(199910)41:1<83::aid-neu11>3.0.co;2-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Brakeman PR, Lanahan AA, O'Brien R, Roche K, Barnes CA, Huganir RL, Worley PF. Homer: a protein that selectively binds metabotropic glutamate receptors. Nature. 1997;386:284–288. doi: 10.1038/386284a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Cantallops I, Haas K, Cline HT. Postsynaptic CPG15 promotes synaptic maturation and presynaptic axon arbor elaboration in vivo. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:1004–1011. doi: 10.1038/79823. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Capsoni S, Tongiorgi E, Cattaneo A, Domenici L. Dark rearing blocks the developmental down-regulation of brain-derived neurotrophic factor messenger RNA expression in layers IV and V of the rat visual cortex. Neuroscience. 1999;88:393–403. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(98)00250-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Constantine-Paton M, Cline HT. LTP and activity-dependent synaptogenesis: the more alike they are, the more different they become. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1998;8:139–148. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(98)80017-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Corriveau R, Shatz CJ, Nedivi E. Dynamic regulation of cpg15 during activity-dependent development in the mammalian visual system. J Neurosci. 1999;19:7999–8008. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-18-07999.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Crair MC, Gillespie DC, Stryker MP. The role of visual experience in the development of columns in cat visual cortex. Science. 1998;279:566–570. doi: 10.1126/science.279.5350.566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Cynader M. Prolonged sensitivity to monocular deprivation in dark-reared cats: effects of age and visual exposure. Brain Res Dev Brain Res. 1983;8:155–164. doi: 10.1016/0165-3806(83)90002-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Cynader M, Mitchell DE. Prolonged sensitivity to monocular deprivation in dark-reared cats. J Neurophysiol. 1980;43:1026–1040. doi: 10.1152/jn.1980.43.4.1026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Drager UC. Observations on monocular deprivation in mice. J Neurophysiol. 1978;41:28–42. doi: 10.1152/jn.1978.41.1.28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Fagiolini M, Pizzorusso T, Berardi N, Domenici L, Maffei L. Functional postnatal development of the rat primary visual cortex and the role of visual experience: dark rearing and monocular deprivation. Vision Res. 1994;34:709–720. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(94)90210-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Goodman CS, Shatz CJ. Developmental mechanisms that generate precise patterns of neuronal connectivity. Cell. 1993;10:77–98. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(05)80030-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Gordon JA, Stryker MP. Experience-dependent plasticity of binocular responses in the primary visual cortex of the mouse. J Neurosci. 1996;16:3274–3286. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-10-03274.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Guire ES, Lickey ME, Gordon B. Critical period for the monocular deprivation effect in rats: assessment with sweep visually evoked potentials. J Neurophysiol. 1999;81:121–128. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.81.1.121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Hogan B, Beddington R, Constantini F, Lacy E. A laboratory manual, Ed 2. Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory; New York: 1994. Manipulating the mouse embryo. pp. 344–351. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Hubel DH, Wiesel TN. The period of susceptibility to the physiological effects of unilateral eye closure in kittens. J Physiol (Lond) 1970;206:419–436. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1970.sp009022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Hubel DH, Wiesel TN, LeVay S. Plasticity of ocular dominance columns in monkey striate cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1977;278:377–409. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1977.0050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Kandel ER, O'Dell TJ. Are adult learning mechanisms also used for development? Science. 1992;258:243–245. doi: 10.1126/science.1411522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Lein ES, Shatz CJ. Rapid regulation of brain-derived neurotrophic factor mRNA within eye-specific circuits during ocular dominance column formation. J Neurosci. 2000;20:1470–1483. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-04-01470.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Maffei L, Berardi N, Domenici L, Parisi V, Pizzorusso T. Nerve growth factor (NGF) prevents the shift in ocular dominance distribution of visual cortical neurons in monocularly deprived rats. J Neurosci. 1992;12:4651–4662. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-12-04651.1992. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Mower GD. The effect of dark rearing on the time course of the critical period in cat visual cortex. Brain Res Dev Brain Res. 1991;58:151–158. doi: 10.1016/0165-3806(91)90001-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Mower GD, Christen WG. Role of visual experience in activating critical period in cat visual cortex. J Neurophysiol. 1985;53:572–589. doi: 10.1152/jn.1985.53.2.572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Mower GD, Berry D, Burchfiel JL, Duffy FH. Comparison of the effects of dark rearing and binocular suture on development and plasticity of cat visual cortex. Brain Res. 1981;220:255–267. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(81)91216-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Mower GD, Christen WG, Caplan CJ. Very brief visual experience eliminates plasticity in the cat visual cortex. Science. 1983;221:178–180. doi: 10.1126/science.6857278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Mower GD, Caplan CJ, Christen WG, Duffy FH. Dark rearing prolongs physiological but not anatomical plasticity of the cat visual cortex. J Comp Neurol. 1985;235:448–466. doi: 10.1002/cne.902350404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Nedivi E. Molecular analysis of developmental plasticity in neocortex. J Neurobiol. 1999;41:135–147. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Nedivi E, Hevroni D, Naot D, Israeli D, Citri Y. Numerous candidate plasticity-related genes revealed by differential cDNA cloning. Nature. 1993;363:718–722. doi: 10.1038/363718a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Nedivi E, Fieldust S, Theill L, Hevroni D. A set of genes expressed in response to light in the adult cerebral cortex and regulated during development. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1996;93:2048–2053. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.5.2048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Nedivi E, Wu GY, Cline H. Promotion of dendritic growth by CPG15, an activity-induced signaling molecule. Science. 1998;281:1863–1866. doi: 10.1126/science.281.5384.1863. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Neve RL, Bear MF. Visual experience regulates gene expression in the developing striate cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1989;86:4781–4784. doi: 10.1073/pnas.86.12.4781. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Parnavelas JG, Burne RA, Lin CS. Receptive field properties of neurons in the visual cortex of the rat. Neurosci Lett. 1981;27:291–296. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(81)90445-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Philpot BD, Sekhar AK, Shouval HZ, Bear MF. Visual experience and deprivation bidirectionally modify the composition and function of NMDA receptors in visual cortex. Neuron. 2001;29:157–169. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00187-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Prusky GT, Ramoa AS. Novel method of chronically blocking retinal activity. J Neurosci Methods. 1999;87:105–110. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0270(98)00172-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Rittenhouse CD, Shouval HZ, Paradiso MA, Bear MF. Monocular deprivation induces homosynaptic long-term depression in visual cortex. Nature. 1999;397:347–350. doi: 10.1038/16922. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Rosen KM, McCormack MA, Villa-Komaroff L, Mower GD. Brief visual experience induces immediate early gene expression in the cat visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1992;89:5437–5441. doi: 10.1073/pnas.89.12.5437. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Worley PF, Cole AJ, Murphy TH, Christy BA, Nakabeppu Y, Baraban JM. Synaptic regulation of immediate-early genes in brain. Cold Spring Harb Symp Quant Biol. 1990;55:213–222. doi: 10.1101/sqb.1990.055.01.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The Journal of Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Society for Neuroscience

RESOURCES