Abstract
Objective
We compared ratings of behavior and attention problems between youth-onset psychosis and ADHD, two disorders in which attentional impairments play a key role, and examined the effect of psychostimulant use on age of onset in psychosis.
Method
Parent and teacher ratings of behavioral problems and ADHD symptoms were collected using the Achenbach CBCL, TRF, and SNAP-IV Teacher Rating Scales on 42 participants with psychosis, 36 with ADHD and 57 controls (ages 8-19).
Results and Conclusions
Results suggested that academic, externalizing, and attention problems reflect symptoms shared between the disorders, whereas internalizing, social and thought problems reflect factors that differ between disorders. Furthermore, participants with psychosis who had been prescribed psychostimulants had a younger age of onset of psychotic symptoms than those who had not. This difference could reflect dissimilarities in symptom severity symptom between subgroups or potentially harmful effects of psychostimulants in individuals predisposed to develop psychosis.
Keywords: ADHD, youth-onset psychosis, behavior problems, internalizing, externalizing
Attentional impairment is a central cognitive feature in schizophrenia and in ADHD. A history of ADHD symptoms is common in individuals who develop schizophrenia (Alaghband-Rad et al., 1995; Kumra et al., 1998; Marenco & Weinberger, 2000; McKenna et al., 1994; Niemi, Suvisaari, Tuulio-Henriksson, & Lönnqvist, 2003; Ross, Heinlein, & Tregellas, 2006; Schaeffer & Ross, 2002), and ADHD is diagnosed in a high proportion of children at genetic risk for schizophrenia (Keshavan, Diwadkar, Montrose, Rajarethinam, & Sweeney, 2005). Comparisons between high-risk youth with and without ADHD (Keshavan, Sujata, Mehra, Montrose, & Sweeney, 2002; Öner & Munir, 2005), and adolescents who have schizophrenia with and without ADHD (Elman et al., 1998) show that those with ADHD fare worse on developmental, neurological, cognitive and clinical measures; have a greater probability of developing schizophrenia; and face a worse prognosis.
Yet, there have been few direct comparisons between schizophrenia, or psychoses in general, and ADHD. Almost all of these comparisons have involved youth-onset schizophrenia or “multidimensionally impaired” (MDI) children with psychotic symptoms (McKenna et al., 1994). Youth-onset schizophrenia is a rare, severe and more genetically loaded form of the disorder that does not differ qualitatively from the adult-onset form on most of the dimensions examined (e.g., Asarnow et al., 2001; Frazier et al., 2007; Jacobsen & Rapoport, 1998; Nicolson et al., 2003; Ueland, Øie, Landrø, & Rund, 2004).
Parents of offspring with schizophrenia or MDI children are more likely to have schizophrenia-spectrum disorders than parents of offspring with ADHD (Asarnow et al., 2001; Kumra et al., 1998). Parents of children with schizophrenia perform worse than parents of children with ADHD on tests of sustained attention and motor sequencing, but not selective attention (Asarnow et al., 2002). Although there is evidence of thought disorder in both schizophrenia and ADHD, the severity and extent of these deficits are greater in schizophrenia (Caplan, Guthrie, Tang, Nuechterlein, & Asarnow, 2001). Participants with psychosis perform worse than participants with ADHD on certain cognitive tasks, but not all (Karatekin, 2007; Karatekin & Asarnow, 1998a, 1998b, 1999; Karatekin, White, & Bingham; 2008, 2009; Kumra et al., 1998; Olincy et al., 2000; Øie, Sundet & Rund, 1999; Ross, Harris, Olincy, & Radant, 2000; Rund, Øie, & Sundet, 1996).
What is the nature of the relationship between psychosis and ADHD? One possibility is that those with ADHD constitute a distinct and severe subgroup within psychosis (Elman et al., 1998). Alternatively, the ADHD in psychosis may not be “true” ADHD but simply an index of severity. Individuals who are cognitively and clinically impaired could also have difficulties with attention, disorganization, impulsivity, and feel tense and agitated. These symptoms could make them look like they have ADHD. However, there is evidence pointing to similarities between psychosis and ADHD in cognitive impairments as well as abnormalities in similar neurotransmitter systems and brain regions (Barr, 2001; Karatekin, 2001). If the ADHD symptoms in psychosis are reflecting this overlap, it might be useful to consider ADHD and psychosis under the broader category of attentional disorders. Thus, direct comparisons between these disorders can elucidate the nature of their relationship, determine the extent of specificity at different levels of analysis, delineate their boundaries, and make decisions about diagnosis and treatment.
Goals of the Current Study
We had two goals for the current study. Our first goal was to examine the relationship between youth-onset psychosis and ADHD on behavioral measures. In the only previous comparison of behavioral problems between youth-onset psychosis and ADHD (Øie et al., 1999), average ratings for Total Behavior Problems on the Achenbach Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001) were similar between schizophrenia and ADHD. However, subscale scores were not reported. Thus, our goal was to compare in greater detail parent and teacher ratings of behavior problems and ADHD symptoms between participants with youth-onset psychosis and ADHD (Combined subtype) on.
Our second aim was to examine the effect of ADHD on youth-onset psychosis. We first investigated the effects of psychostimulant use for ADHD symptoms. A history of treatment with psychostimulants is widespread in psychosis samples. For example, in a review of case histories of 17 youth with psychoses, Schaeffer and Ross (2002) found that 77% had been exposed to psychostimulants. Although some individuals with psychosis benefit from psychostimulants, the majority show a worsening of symptoms (Barch & Carter, 2005; Barr, 2001; Curran, Byrappa, & McBride, 2004). Furthermore, the FDA (2007) has issued a warning that psychostimulants can cause hallucinations and delusions in ADHD. Although these psychotic episodes tend to be brief, there are reports of children suffering for longer periods and being rediagnosed with bipolar disorder or schizophrenia (Ross et al., 2006). Psychostimulants might have especially severe adverse effects for children with a genetic predisposition to psychosis (Ross et al., 2006; Schmidt & Freidson, 1990). In adolescents with bipolar disorder, psychostimulants worsen symptoms and are associated with a younger age of onset (DelBello et al., 2001). Thus, we predicted that age of onset of psychosis in the current study would be lower for participants who had been exposed to psychostimulants than those who had not. Finally, we examined the effect of ADHD symptoms on behavioral ratings by comparing participants with and without ADHD symptoms within the psychosis sample.
Method
Participants
Table 1 lists participants’ demographic and clinical characteristics. Participants with psychosis were recruited from inpatient and outpatient clinics at the University of Minnesota, mental health professionals in the community, and flyers distributed at regional mental health conferences. One participant was an inpatient at the time of the study, three were in day treatment, and one was in residential treatment. The rest were living at home. Participants in the control and ADHD groups were recruited from advertisements in the local community, and friends of families who had participated in the study. Participants in the ADHD group were also recruited from parent support groups for ADHD.
Table 1.
Control | Psychosis | ADHD | Results of Statistical Tests | |
---|---|---|---|---|
N | 57 | 42 | 36 | |
M:F, N (%) | 26:31 (46%:54%) | 26:16 (62%:38%) | 28:8 (78%:22%) | , p = .008; C ≠ ADHD |
Age in months (SD) | 152 (30) | 167 (41) | 152 (31) | ns |
Age range | 105-226 | 101-238 | 106-224 | |
Socioeconomic | 53 (9) | 40 (14) | 50 (10) |
F(2, 126) = 15.75, p < .001; (C = ADHD) > Psychosis |
status | ||||
(Hollingshead, 1975) | ||||
Estimated IQ (SD) | 114 (13) | 99 (15) | 108 (13) |
F(2, 122) = 14.87, p < .001; (C = ADHD) > Psychosis |
SANS/SAPS | ||||
Negative symptoms | 2.7 (1.0) | |||
Psychotic symptoms | 1.9 (0.9) | |||
Disorganized symptoms |
2.3 (1.3) | |||
Medications, N (%) | ||||
Antipsychotic | 0 | 28 (67%) | 0 | |
Psychostimulant | 0 | 10 (24%) | 23 (64%) | |
Antidepressant | 0 | 10 (24%) | 1 (3%) | |
Mood stabilizer | 0 | 9 (21%) | 0 | |
Benzodiazepine | 0 | 5 (12%) | 1 (3%) | |
Anti-histamine | 0 | 4 (10%) | 0 | |
Alpha-adrenergic | 0 | 3 (7%) | 2 (6%) | |
Ethnicity, N (%) |
, p = .044, pairwise comparisons between groups ns |
|||
Caucasian | 50 (88%) | 28 (67%) | 31 (86%) | |
African American | 1 (2%) | 4 (10%) | 4 (11%) | |
Asian | 1 (2%) | 3 (7%) | 0 | |
Hispanic | 0 | 0 | 0 | |
Mixed/Other | 5 (9%) | 7 (17%) | 1 (3%) |
Note: ns = not significant. Medications taken by fewer than 5% of participants in the ADHD and psychosis groups are not reported.
Potential participants were excluded if they were not fluent in English or were color blind, if they had been premature by more than 4 weeks, had a history of significant neurological conditions (e.g., seizures, severe concussions), or an IQ of lower than 70. Potential participants were excluded from the ADHD and control groups if they had been adopted, or had first-degree biological relatives with schizophrenia. Potential participants were excluded from the ADHD group if they were taking psychoactive medications other than psychostimulants, if their parents were not willing to discontinue psychostimulants for 24 hours prior to cognitive testing, if they had been diagnosed with or suspected of having a pervasive developmental disorder, or if they had never met criteria for the Combined subtype. However, we included two adolescents who currently met criteria for the Inattentive subtype, but who had previously met criteria for the Combined subtype and scored above 60 on the Attention Problems scale of the Achenbach Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). Potential controls were excluded if they had ever taken psychoactive medications, been diagnosed with a major psychiatric disorder or met criteria for a current disorder, had attention problems for which they had sought help, or had first-degree biological relatives with ADHD.
Diagnoses were made using DSM-IV criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 1994), and were based on semistructured interviews (Kiddie-Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia-Present and Lifetime Version, K-SADS-PL; Kaufman, Birmaher, Brent, Rao, & Ryan, 1996) conducted separately with participants and at least one parent/guardian. The interviews were conducted by child and adolescent psychiatrists, a PhD level clinical psychologist, advanced PhD students in counseling psychology, and a trained BA level researcher. A licensed psychologist with a master’s degree in counseling psychology supervised the diagnostic assessments of 42 of the participants in the control and all but four of the participants in the ADHD group. The assessments of the remaining participants in the control and ADHD groups and all of the participants in the psychosis group were supervised by one of the coauthors, a child, and adolescent psychiatrist (TW).
In addition, parents filled out questionnaires on their children’s developmental and medical history. The questionnaires included items from the Yale Children’s Inventory (Shaywitz, Schnell, Shaywitz, & Towle, 1986) that focus on early development, social and academic functioning, and the module on pregnancy, delivery and infancy complications from the Diagnostic Interview for Children and Adolescents-Parent version (DICA-P; Herjanic & Campbell, 1977; Herjanic & Reich, 1982). Parents also rated their children on behavioral symptoms (CBCL; Achenbach, 1991a; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). Questionnaires (Achenbach Teacher Report Form; Achenbach, 1991b, Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001; SNAP-IV; Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham Teacher and Parent Rating Scale; Swanson, 1992) were sent to teachers to ensure that diagnoses were based on reports from multiple informants familiar with participants’ behavior in different settings. Participants with psychosis were also administered the Scales for the assessment of negative and positive symptoms (SANS/SAPS; Andreasen, 1983, 1984).
To obtain estimates of intellectual and academic functioning, we administered the Vocabulary and Block Design subtests from the Wechsler Intelligence Scale, 3rd ed. (WISC-III; Wechsler, 1991), the WISC-IV (Wechsler, 2003), or the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 3rd ed. (Wechsler, 1997), and the Reading and Spelling subtests from the Wechsler Individual Achievement Test (1992). An IQ estimate was obtained from 56 participants in the control, 32 in the ADHD, and 37 in the psychosis group. Estimated IQs are reported in Table 1.
Participants’ diagnoses are listed in Table 2. In the psychosis group, average age of onset of psychotic symptoms was 12.2 years (SD = 3.2, range = 6-18). Of the 13 participants with psychosis not otherwise specified (NOS), 6 could have been in the prodromal phase, and 6 had psychotic symptoms in the context of a complex clinical picture (these subgroups were not mutually exclusive). Two participants might have met criteria for “multidimensionally impaired disorder” (McKenna et al., 1994). Two had psychotic and affective symptoms but did not meet criteria for a disorder. Most of these 13 participants reported hallucinations, delusions, or showed evidence of formal thought disorder.
Table 2.
Control | Psychosis | ADHD | |
---|---|---|---|
Schizophrenia | 22 (52%) | ||
Schizophreniform | 3 (7%) | ||
Schizoaffective | 4 (10%) | ||
Psychosis not otherwise specified |
13 (31%) | ||
ADHD | |||
Combined | 34 (94%) | ||
Inattentive (with history of combined) |
2 (6%) | ||
Mood disorders | 2 (4%) | 7 (17%) | 7 (20%) |
Anxiety disorders | 2 (4%) | 6 (14%) | 5 (14%) |
Oppositional defiant disorder/conduct disorder |
0 | 8 (19%) | 14 (39%) |
Substance use/abuse | 0 | 3 (7%) | 2 (6%) |
Tic disorder | 0 | 1 (2%) | 2 (6%) |
Note: All diagnoses in controls refer to past diagnoses.
The DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) requires a diagnosis of ADHD to be made only if “the symptoms do not occur exclusively during the course of … Schizophrenia or other psychotic disorder and are not better accounted for by another mental disorder” (p. 85). In the case of youth-onset psychosis, it is often difficult to determine when the course of the psychotic disorder began and its relationship to ADHD symptoms. Therefore, we categorized participants as having “ADHD symptoms” if they had a past diagnosis of ADHD on the K-SADS-PL, if there was any mention in the chart of their having been suspected of or diagnosed with ADHD, treated with psychostimulants, or if they met ADHD criteria during the current evaluation when the hierarchical rule was ignored. By this definition, 27 participants had ADHD symptoms, 14 did not, and there was not enough information about one.
Interrater reliability was assessed by having a PhD level psychologist, blind to participants’ final diagnosis, review the videotaped interviews, questionnaires, and IQ and achievement scores of 11 participants in the control and 11 in the ADHD group randomly selected from the larger sample. Cohen’s kappa was .91 for ADHD (reliability was not measured in the psychosis group due to practical constraints). Consensus diagnoses were assigned in cases of disagreement.
Most participants were administered cognitive tasks over two sessions, and some underwent neuroimaging. Results of these analyses are reported in other manuscripts (53 control participants were included in Karatekin, Marcus, & White, 2007; 11 control and 9 ADHD participants were included in Karatekin, 2006; 10 control and 9 psychosis participants were included in White et al., 2007; 33 control, 15 ADHD, and 11 psychosis participants were included in Karatekin et al., 2008; and 34 control, 14 ADHD, and 11 psychosis participants were included in Karatekin et al., 2009).
Families were provided with monetary compensation for participation. Most families were also provided with a diagnostic report.
The study was approved by the University of Minnesota Institutional Review Board, and informed consent and assent were obtained.
Measures
CBCL and TRF
The CBCL and TRF (Achenbach, 1991a, 1991b; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001) are measures of general behavioral problems for 6- to 18-year-olds. Items are rated on a 3-point scale (0 = not true, 1 = somewhat or sometimes true, 2 = very true or often true). The CBCL and TRF include 113 items on behavioral problems.
Sixteen parents and 11 teachers in the psychosis group, and 10 parents and nine teachers in the control group were administered the 1991 version of these questionnaires. Because correlations between the 1991 and 2001 versions range from .87 to .99 for scales used in the current study (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001), data were combined across versions.
To compare groups, we used the Total Competence scale (rated by parents), the Adaptive Functioning scale (rated by teachers), the Internalizing and Externalizing scales, and the Social, Thought and Attention Problems subscales (rated by both). These subscales were analyzed because they are not included in the Internalizing or Externalizing scales.
The Total Competence scale is based on the number and quality of activities, sports, and chores the child performs, his or her social behavior, and a general index of academic problems. The Adaptive Functioning scale assesses the degree to which the child is perceived as behaving appropriately, learning, hard working, and happy compared to his or her peers. The Internalizing scale is based on the Anxious/Depressed, Withdrawn/Depressed, and Somatic Complaints subscales. The Externalizing scale is based on the Rule-Breaking and Aggressive Behavior subscales.
On both questionnaires, participants were compared to the normative sample for the CBCL and TRF, and analyses were conducted on T-scores. Two 19-year-olds in the psychosis group were compared to 18-year-olds.
The SNAP-IV Teacher Rating scale
The SNAP-IV (Swanson, 1992) assesses symptoms of ADHD on items based directly on the DSM-IV. Items are rated on a 4-point scale (0 = not at all, 1 = just a little, 2 = quite a bit, and 3 = very much). Inattention was assessed by calculating average scores for Items 1 to 9 and Hyperactivity/Impulsivity was assessed by calculating average scores for Items 11 to 19.
In a study of factors influencing teachers’ perceptions of children’s behaviors (Stevens, Quittner, & Abikoff, 1998), internal consistencies for the SNAP-IV Inattention, Hyperactivity/Impulsivity and Oppositional Defiant Disorder (ODD) scales were between .84 and .95. In the current study, internal consistencies (calculated with Cronbach’s alpha) for the Inattention and Hyperactivity/Impulsivity scales were .75, and .88, respectively in the controls; .89, and .93 in the ADHD group, and .90 and .92 in the psychosis group.
History of treatment with psychostimulants
History of psychostimulant use in the psychosis group was obtained from the interviews conducted with the parents, questionnaires completed by the parents prior to their visit, and medical or psychiatric records of the participants when available.
Statistical Analyses
Statistical analyses were conducted with SPSS 14.0 and MacAnova 5.06 (an open-source cross-platform statistics program available for Windows, Macintosh, and Linux at http://www.stat.umn.edu/macanova/).
Appropriate transforms of responses to achieve normality and constant variance were sought among the Box-Cox family of distributions. These are equivalent to power transformations y → yp, except that y → log(y) when p = 1. The power was selected to be close to the maximum likelihood estimate of p using a graphical procedure (Box & Cox, 1964). The control data were extremely skewed on CBCL and TRF Social, Thought, and Attention Problems, and SNAP-IV Inattention and Hyperactivity/Impulsivity. Data transformations did not improve the distributions adequately across groups. Because our main goal was to compare the clinical groups to each other, and because data transformations involving categorical scales would reduce the statistical power of these comparisons, we compared only the psychosis and ADHD groups on these scales. On the TRF, the question asking teachers how well they knew the participant was treated as a categorical variable, with 1 and 2 as not well, and above 2 as well.
Continuous demographic variables (age, SES, IQ) were analyzed with univariate ANOVAs, and significant findings were followed up with Tukey tests. Categorical demographic variables (gender, ethnicity) were analyzed with X2 tests, followed up with 2 × 2 X2 tests. Correlations between parent and teacher ratings were calculated using Pearson productmoment correlation coefficients. Differences in age and age of onset between subgroups of participants were calculated with independent-samples t tests.
Repeated-measures Type III ANCOVAs, with age as the covariate, were used to examine the effects of diagnosis and age on the questionnaire ratings. Type III SS were used because they test the same hypotheses as post hoc tests computed from the same ANOVA model. The covariate was modified by subtracting the mean age of all participants from each participant’s age. Each ANCOVA tested linear and quadratic trends for age. We examined quadratic trends in addition to linear trends because age trends were clearly nonlinear in some cases, and a quadratic trend is among the simpler alternatives to a linear trend. Models were selected by backward elimination of nonsignificant terms involving age, starting with the highest order interactions. When the quadratic trend on age was significant, the linear trend was not reported. IQ and SES were not used as covariates because controlling for these variables would have reduced variance due to the disorders. Huynh-Feldt adjustments to dfs were used to compute F-statistic p values, and Huynh-Feldt-adjusted dfs were reported where applicable.
Post hoc analyses of ANCOVA results were conducted using custom macros for MacAnova. Main effects or interactions were generally not followed up when there were higher-order interactions involving the same variables. Between-subjects contrasts involved pairwise comparisons between groups. Within-subjects contrasts involved comparison of parent and teacher ratings within each group. Tests of between- and within-subject contrasts and slopes were based on appropriate t statistics. To protect against multiple testing, p values were Bonferroni corrected, that is, multiplied by the appropriate number of simultaneous tests. When the contrast involved a between-subjects contrast, Tukey-Kramer p values based on the Studentized range were computed and then, where appropriate, Bonferroni corrected by the number of intrasubject contrasts being considered simultaneously.
To calculate effect size, we used a measure similar to Cohen’s d but that took into account the age differences among groups. Specifically, we divided the difference of the group means (age adjusted as appropriate) by the square root of the MSe term for the between-subjects analysis section of the ANCOVA. In cases where there was an interaction between group and age, the value reflects the size of the effect at the average age for the whole sample.
Findings are reported as significant if α ≤ .05.
Results
CBCLs were obtained for all but one participant in the psychosis group. At least one TRF was obtained for 44 participants in the control, 27 in the psychosis, and 28 in the ADHD group. TRFs were obtained from at least two teachers for 15 participants in the control, 4 in the psychosis, and 8 in the ADHD group. SNAP-IVs were obtained from at least two teachers for 5 participants in the psychosis and 9 in the ADHD group. In these cases, data were averaged across teachers.
Information on Teachers
On the TRF, all the teachers in the control, 26 in the ADHD, and 15 in the psychosis group indicated that they taught regular or advanced courses. The remaining teachers taught special education classes. Average duration of time they had known the participants ranged from 10 to 18 months across groups. An ANCOVA on number of months did not yield a group effect. In contrast, a X2 test on how well the teachers knew the participant, on a scale of 1 to 3, showed a diagnosis effect, X2 (2) = 7.43, p = .024. Post hoc 2 × 2 X2 tests indicated that teachers in the psychosis group rated their knowledge of the participants higher than teachers in the ADHD group. The difference is likely to be due to the fact that more participants in the ADHD group were in regular, and larger, classes.
Correlations Between Parent and Teacher Ratings
In all groups, parent and teacher ratings were correlated more highly for externalizing than for internalizing problems (Table 3). Furthermore, parent–teacher correlations were as high, or higher, in the psychosis than in the ADHD group. It should be noted that the low correlations in the ADHD group for Social and Thought Problems reflect range restriction. Fisher’s r-to-z transformations were used to test if the magnitude of the correlations differed between groups. None of the differences reached significance.
Table 3.
Controls |
Psychosis |
ADHD |
|
---|---|---|---|
(N = 44) | (N = 26) | (N = 28) | |
Internalizing problems | .02 (.906) | .27 (.180) | .03 (.865) |
Externalizing problems |
.40 (.007) | .56 (.003) | .44 (.018) |
Social problems | .59 (.001) | .21 (.274) | |
Thought problems | .29 (.146) | .13 (.526) | |
Attention problems | .36 (.071) | .37 (.055) |
Note: Numbers in parentheses refer to p values.
Group Effects on Parent and Teacher Ratings
Table 4 lists parent and teacher ratings on the CBCL and TRF. As shown in this table, there was only one interaction between diagnosis and respondent: In controls, teacher ratings of externalizing problems were higher than parent ratings, whereas parent and teacher ratings did not differ in the clinical groups. Thus, parent and teacher ratings pointed to identical conclusions for the two clinical groups.
Table 4.
Control M (SD) |
Psychosis M (SD) |
ADHD M (SD) |
Effect Size |
ANCOVA Results | Post-hoc Tests/ Group difference |
|
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
CBCL Total Competence |
57 (8) | 32 (7) | 44 (8) | 1.68 | Diagnosis: 111.612,120 p < .001 | C > ADHD > Psychosis |
TRF Adaptive Functioning |
56 (6) | 44 (9) | 42 (7) | 0.20 | Diagnosis: 41.762,91 p < .001 | C > (ADHD = Psychosis) |
Linear increase with age: 6.611,91 p = .012 |
||||||
Total behavior problems | ||||||
CBCL | 41 (8) | 69 (9) | 61 (7) | 0.95 | ||
TRF | 43 (7) | 65 (10) | 59 (7) | 0.65 | ||
Diagnosis: 146.102,94 p < .001 | C < ADHD < Psychosis | |||||
Linear decrease with age : 6.271,94 p = .014 |
||||||
aAge (linear) × respondent: 4.061,94 p = .020 |
||||||
Internalizing problems | ||||||
CBCL | 45 (7) | 69 (10) | 55 (10) | 1.34 | ||
TRF | 45 (7) | 67 (10) | 53 (7) | 1.62 | ||
Diagnosis: 108.882,94 p < .001 | C < ADHD < Psychosis | |||||
bAge (linear) × respondent: 6.841,94 p = .010 |
||||||
Externalizing problems | ||||||
CBCL | 42 (7) | 62 (12) | 60 (9) | 0.46 | ||
TRF | 46 (5) | 58 (11) | 58 (8) | 0.31 | ||
Diagnosis: 65.212,92 p < .001 | C < (ADHD = Psychosis) | |||||
Linear decrease with age: 10.891,92 p = .001 |
||||||
cDiagnosis × age (linear): 6.092,92 p = .003 |
||||||
dRespondent × diagnosis: 7.952,92 p < .001 |
||||||
Social problems | ||||||
CBCL | 51 (2) | 67 (10) | 59 (9) | 0.82 | ||
TRF | 52 (3) | 63 (8) | 57 (6) | 0.96 | ||
Diagnosis: 18.641,51 p < .001 | ADHD < Psychosis | |||||
Linear decrease with age: 5.961,51 p = .018 |
||||||
Respondent: 4.221,51 p = .045 | Parents > Teachers | |||||
Thought problems | ||||||
CBCL | 51 (2) | 74 (10) | 60 (8) | 1.57 | ||
TRF | 50 (2) | 69 (13) | 57 (7) | 1.10 | ||
Diagnosis: 39.471,52 p < .001 | ADHD < Psychosis | |||||
Respondent: 6.541,52 p = .014 | Parents > Teachers | |||||
Attention problems | ||||||
CBCL | 51 (3) | 72 (13) | 68 (9) | 0.46 | ||
TRF | 51 (2) | 61 (7) | 62 (9) | 0.18 | ||
Respondent: 28.421,52 p < .001 | Parents > Teachers | |||||
SNAP-IV Inattention | 0.20 (0.4) | 1.29 (0.7) | 1.65 (0.7) | 0.33 | Linear decrease with age: 7.461,47 p = .009 |
|
SNAP-IV H/I | 0.12 (0.2) | 0.59 (0.7) | 1.08 (0.8) | 0.48 | Linear decrease with age: 6.581,48 p = .013 |
Values refer to T-scores for CBCL and TRF. On the SNAP, items are rated on a 4-point scale (0 = not at all, 1 = just a little, 2 = quite a bit, and 3 = very much). On CBCL Competence and TRF Adaptive Functioning, higher scores indicate better functioning. On all other scales, higher scores indicate worse functioning. Effect sizes refer to the psychosis-ADHD difference. ADHD = Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Calculation of effect sizes was based on ANCOVAs involving only the psychosis and ADHD groups. C = Control. H/I = Hyperactivity/Impulsivity. ns = not significant.
The difference between parent and teacher ratings was greater in the psychosis than in the control group.
Parent ratings of internalizing problems decreased linearly with age, whereas teacher ratings did not.
Externalizing problems decreased with age in the psychosis group, but not in the other two groups.
Teacher ratings of externalizing problems were higher than parent ratings in the control group, but there were no differences between respondents in the clinical groups.
Compared to the ADHD group, the psychosis group was rated as functioning more poorly on CBCL Total Competence, and Internalizing, Social, and Thought Problems. In contrast, the ADHD and Psychosis groups did not differ on Adaptive Functioning, Externalizing, and Attention Problems. Although the ADHD group was rated higher on SNAP-IV Inattention and Hyperactivity/Impulsivity than the psychosis group, the difference did not reach significance for either scale.
Effect of ADHD Symptoms Within the Youth-Onset Psychosis Sample
As expected, participants in the psychosis group who had been exposed to psychostimulants had a younger age of onset of psychotic symptoms (M = 11.2 years, SD = 3.0) than those who had not (M = 13.7, SD = 2.8), t (31) = 2.48, p = .019, Cohen’s d = 0.81.
Next, we compared behavioral ratings of participants with ADHD to participants in the psychosis group with or without ADHD symptoms (see Table 5). The two psychosis subgroups did not differ on negative, disorganized or psychotic symptoms on the SANS/SAPS. As shown in Table 5, participants in the psychosis group with and without ADHD symptoms did not differ from each other on any other variable. Participants with both psychosis and ADHD symptoms were rated lower than participants with ADHD on Total Competence, and higher on Internalizing, Social, and Thought Problems. The two subgroups did not differ on attention problems on either questionnaire.
Table 5.
P | P-ADHD | ADHD | ANCOVA Results | Group Differences | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
CBCL total competence | 35 (9) | 30 (5) | 44 (8) | Diagnosis: 25.362,64 p < .001 | ADHD > (P-ADHD = P) |
TRF adaptive functioning | 47 (12) | 44 (8) | 42 (7) | ||
Total problems | |||||
CBCL | 66 (8) | 71 (10) | 61 (7) | ||
TRF | 62 (13) | 66 (8) | 59 (7) | ||
Diagnosis: 10.722,48 p < .001 Quadratic decrease with age: 4.031,48 p =.050 |
ADHD < P-ADHD | ||||
Internalizing problems | |||||
CBCL | 67 (8) | 69 (11) | 55 (10) | ||
TRF | 65 (13) | 68 (8) | 53 (7) | Diagnosis: 22.002, 50 p < .001 | ADHD < (P-ADHD = P) |
Externalizing problems | |||||
CBCL | 60 (12) | 63 (13) | 60 (9) | ||
TRF | 56 (13) | 59 (10) | 58 (8) | ||
Social problems | |||||
CBCL | 63 (8) | 69 (10) | 59 (9) | ||
TRF | 61 (11) | 63 (7) | 57 (6) | Diagnosis: 6.612, 50 p = .003 | ADHD < P-ADHD |
Thought problems | |||||
CBCL | 71 (9) | 76 (10) | 60 (7) | ||
TRF | 70 (15) | 68 (13) | 57 (7) | Diagnosis: 18.222, 50 p < .001 | ADHD < (P = P-ADHD) |
Attention problems | |||||
CBCL | 63 (9) | 76 (13) | 68 (9) | ||
TRF | 59 (9) | 61 (6) | 62 (9) | ||
SNAP-IV Inattention | 0.94 (1.0) | 1.41 (0.5) | 1.65 (0.7) | ||
SNAP-IV H/I | 0.24 (0.5) | 0.71 (0.7) | 1.08 (0.8) |
Note: Only the significant main effects and interactions involving.
Discussion
Comparison of Behavior Ratings and ADHD Symptoms Between the Clinical Groups
Compared to participants with ADHD (Combined subtype), participants with youth-onset psychosis were rated by both parents and teachers as functioning more poorly on CBCL Total Competence and on Internalizing, Social and Thought Problems scales. It is not surprising that youth with psychosis are more impaired than youth with ADHD on these domains. What is more surprising is the similarity between disorders in other domains despite differences in clinical presentation and severity. That is, the groups were not reliably different on TRF Adaptive Functioning, CBCL and TRF Externalizing and Attention Problems scales. Effect sizes for these scales were small to medium, so our failure to find significant differences may have been due, in part, to lack of statistical power. It is important to note, however, that on the SNAP-IV Inattention and Hyperactivity/Impulsivity Scales, ratings for the ADHD group were higher than those for the psychosis group. Although the groups did not differ on either scale, results nevertheless suggest that the SNAP-IV may have some specificity to ADHD and that it is not simply tapping general behavioral problems or the clinical severity of a disorder.
The elevated scores of both clinical groups on Thought Problems and the higher scores of the psychosis compared to the ADHD group are consistent with Caplan et al. (2001), who found more severe and extensive thought disorder in childhood-onset schizophrenia than in ADHD. However, unlike a previous study that found no difference in CBCL Total Behavior Problems between adolescents with schizophrenia or ADHD (Øie et al., 1999), we did find a difference. Although scores of the ADHD groups were similar between that study (M = 60, SD = 18) and the current study (M = 61, SD = 7), scores of the psychosis group were higher in the current study (M = 69, SD = 9) compared to the prior study (M = 62, SD = 33). The discrepancy between these results could be due to the fact that the previous psychosis group was on average 3 years older and had extremely high variability in their scores.
A possible reason for the lack of specificity of the Adaptive Functioning, Externalizing and Attention Problems scales is that they may not be sensitive enough to detect small group differences. However, results are also consistent with research showing no differences on cognitive measures between youth with psychosis and ADHD or between their parents, as listed in the Introduction. This overlap could reflect common characteristics between the disorders, such as environmental risk factors and neurobiological substrates. Conversely, the greater impairment of the psychosis group on Total Competence, Internalizing and Thought Problems might reflect factors that differ between the disorders.
Effect of ADHD Symptoms Within Youth-Onset Psychosis
Consistent with other studies (Alabaghand-Rad et al., 1995; Kumra et al., 1998; Marenco & Weinberger, 2000; McKenna et al., 1994; Niemi et al., 2003; Ross et al., 2006; Schaeffer & Ross, 2002), a majority (66%) of the participants with psychosis had clinically significant ADHD symptoms. Also consistent with the high rates of psychostimulant use in other youth-onset psychosis samples (e.g., Schaeffer & Ross, 2002), 59% of the psychosis participants in the current study had been prescribed psychostimulants. As expected, psychosis participants who had been prescribed psychostimulants had a younger age of onset of psychotic symptoms than those who had not. Unfortunately, there are only a handful of studies on the effects of psychostimulants on the course and prognosis of individuals at risk for, or with, youth-onset psychosis (Ross et al., 2006). Thus, although psychostimulants are more likely to be prescribed to children with severe symptoms, clinicians should nevertheless be careful about prescribing psychostimulants to youth with psychoses or at genetic risk for psychosis.
When psychosis participants with and without ADHD symptoms were compared on behavioral ratings, results yielded no significant differences. This result does not support the hypothesis that individuals with psychosis and ADHD symptoms form a distinct and severe subgroup (Elman et al., 1998). Nevertheless, it is possible that although the two subgroups start out on different paths, they may converge after the illness takes effect.
Developmental Trends
We found only one interaction between diagnosis and age for parent and teacher ratings of behavior problems: externalizing problems decreased linearly with age in the psychosis but not in the other groups. This differential decrease is probably due to the fact that the severity of externalizing problems in younger participants with psychosis was particularly high. With the caveat that this was not a longitudinal study, results indicate that the groups did not differ in terms of age-related changes in the other behaviors assessed in this study. In future research, it would be informative to compare developmental trajectories of ADHD symptoms in youth-onset psychosis (or at risk for psychosis) and ADHD on more sensitive measures and to determine if there are qualitative differences in symptom presentation at different ages.
Credibility of Parent Reports in the Psychosis Group
Achenbach and Rescorla (2001) reported that parent–teacher correlations ranged from .18 to .44 for scales reported in Table 4 for children in mental health and special education settings and in a national survey. The correlations we observed in the psychosis group are higher than those in the manual for all scales, perhaps because severe behavior disturbance leads to increased agreement in the behaviors observed by parents and teachers. In any case, these findings suggested that parent reports of behavior problems of children with psychosis are consistent with those of teacher reports.
Limitations
Limitations of the study include a relatively small sample size and limited power to test differences between psychosis subgroups, recruitment of the psychosis and ADHD groups from different sources, and differences in gender composition. In addition, floor effects on the CBCL and TRF, and ceiling and floor effects on the SNAP-IV could have prevented us from detecting differences.
Although teachers were instructed to rate participants’ behavior off medications, some teachers’ ratings for both the ADHD and psychosis groups were based on medicated behavior only, as they did not have the opportunity observe participants off medications. This constraint on the ratings may have reduced group differences.
As shown in Table 1, IQs were about 0.5 to 1 SD above average in both the ADHD and control groups, further limiting the generalizability of the results. It should be noted, however, that the control and ADHD groups did not differ significantly on IQ. In addition, average IQ in the psychosis group, although lower than that in the other groups, was well within the normal range.
The current study was part of a larger study that included two cognitive testing sessions for all and brain imaging for some participants. This procedure likely excluded severely impaired participants and chaotic, dysfunctional or low-SES families who were unable or unwilling to invest the necessary time to participate in the study, limiting generalizability of the results.
Youth with internalizing problems may have been under-represented in the ADHD group. Potential participants with ADHD were excluded if they were taking psychoactive medications that could not be discontinued. Of the 49 participants excluded from the ADHD group based on phone screening, 6 (12%) were excluded because they were taking atomoxetine, and 17 (35%) were excluded because they were taking antidepressant or antianxiety medications. However, as shown in Table 2, a sizable minority of participants in the ADHD group had histories of mood and anxiety disorders. In addition, T-scores of participants with ADHD on the CBCL Internalizing scale and its subscales range from the mid-50s to the low 60s in other studies that did not exclude participants on medications (Biederman et al., 2001; Connor et al., 2003) and scores of the ADHD group in the current study are in the mid-50s. As shown in Table 4, the scores in the psychosis group were in the high 60s. Therefore, even if we had a more representative ADHD sample, group differences on Internalizing Problems might still have been significant.
Bios
Canan Karatekin, PhD, is an associate professor at the Institute of Child Development, University of Minnesota.
Tonya White, MD, is Wetenschappelijk onderzoeker at the Dept. of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry at Erasmus Medical Center, Rotterdam, the Netherlands.
Christopher Bingham, PhD, is Professor Emeritus at the School of Statistics, University of Minnesota.
References
- Achenbach TM. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4-18 and 1991 Profile. University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry; Burlington, VT: 1991a. [Google Scholar]
- Achenbach TM. Manual for the Teacher’s Report Form and 1991 Profile. University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry; Burlington, VT: 1991b. [Google Scholar]
- Achenbach TM, Rescorla L. Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles: An integrated system of multi-informant assessment. ASEBA; Burlington, VT: 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Alaghband-Rad A, McKenna K, Gordon CT, Albus KE, Hamburger SD, Rumsey JM, et al. Childhood-onset schizophrenia: The severity of premorbid course. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1995;34:1273–1283. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199510000-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed. Author; Washington, DC: 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Andreasen NC. The scale for the assessment of negative symptoms (SANS) University of Iowa; Iowa City, IA: 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Andreasen NC. The scale for the assessment of positive symptoms (SAPS) University of Iowa; Iowa City, IA: 1984. [Google Scholar]
- Asarnow RF, Nuechterlein KH, Fogelson D, Subotnik KL, Payne DA, Russell AT, et al. Schizophrenia and schizophrenia-spectrum personality disorders in the first-degree relatives of children with schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2001;58:581–588. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.6.581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Asarnow RF, Nuechterlein KH, Subotnik KL, Fogelson DL, Torquato RD, Payne DL, et al. Neurocognitive impairments in nonpsychotic parents of children with schizophrenia and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2002;59:1053–1060. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.11.1053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barch DM, Carter CS. Amphetamine improves cognitive function in medicated individuals with schizophrenia and in healthy volunteers. Schizophrenia Research. 2005;77:43–58. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.12.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barr WB. Schizophrenia and attention deficit disorder: Two complex disorders of attention. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2001;931:239–250. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05782.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biederman J, Monuteaux MC, Greene RW, Braaten E, Doyle AE, Faraone SV. Long-term stability of the child behavior checklist in a clinical sample of youth with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 2001;30:492–502. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3004_06. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Box GEP, Cox DR. An analysis of transformations. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B. 1964;26:211–252. [Google Scholar]
- Caplan R, Guthrie D, Tang B, Nuechterlein KH, Asarnow RF. Thought disorder in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2001;40:965–972. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200108000-00019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Connor DF, Edwards G, Fletcher KE, Baird J, Barkley RA, Steingard RJ. Correlates of comorbid psychopathology in children with ADHD. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2003;42:193–200. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200302000-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Curran C, Byrappa N, McBride A. Stimulant psychosis: A systematic review. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2004;185:196–204. doi: 10.1192/bjp.185.3.196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DelBello MP, Soutullo CA, Hendricks W, Niemeier RT, McElroy SL, Strakowski SM. Prior stimulant treatment in adolescents with bipolar disorder: Association with age at onset. Bipolar Disorders. 2001;3:53–57. doi: 10.1034/j.1399-5618.2001.030201.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elman E, Sigler M, Kronenberg J, Lindenmayer J-P, Doron A, Mendlovic S, et al. Characteristics of patients with schizophrenia successive to childhood attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) Israeli Journal of Psychiatry and Related Sciences. 1998;35:280–286. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- FDA News Food and Drug Administration Web site. 2007 Retrieved June 15, 2007, from http://www.fda.gov/bbs/topics/NEWS/2007/NEW01568.html.
- Frazier JA, McClellan J, Findling RL, Vitiello B, Anderson R, Zablotsky B, et al. Treatment of early-onset schizophrenia spectrum disorders (TEOSS): Demographic and clinical characteristics. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2007;46:979–988. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e31807083fd. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herjanic B, Campbell W. Differentiating psychiatrically disturbed children on the basis of a structured interview. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1977;5:127–134. doi: 10.1007/BF00913088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herjanic B, Reich W. Development of a structured psychiatric interview for children: Agreement between child and parent on individual symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1982;10:307–324. doi: 10.1007/BF00912324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hollingshead AB. Four factor index of social status. Yale University; New Haven, CT: 1975. [Google Scholar]
- Jacobsen LK, Rapoport JL. Childhood-onset schizophrenia: Implications of clinical and neurobiological research. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1998;39:101–113. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karatekin C. Developmental disorders of attention. In: Nelson CA, Luciana M, editors. Handbook of Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. MIT Press; Cambridge, MA: 2001. pp. 561–576. [Google Scholar]
- Karatekin C. Improving antisaccade performance in adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) Experimental Brain Research. 2006;174:324–341. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0467-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karatekin C. Eye tracking studies of normative and atypical development. Developmental Review. 2007;27:283–348. [Google Scholar]
- Karatekin C, Asarnow RF. Components of visual search in childhood-onset schizophrenia and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1998a;26:367–380. doi: 10.1023/a:1021903923120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karatekin C, Asarnow RF. Working memory in childhood-onset schizophrenia and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) Psychiatry Research. 1998b;80:165–176. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(98)00061-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karatekin C, Asarnow RF. Exploratory eye movements to pictures in childhood-onset schizophrenia and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1999;27:35–49. doi: 10.1023/a:1022662323823. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karatekin C, Marcus DJ, White TJ. Oculomotor and manual indices of incidental and intentional spatial sequence learning in middle childhood and adolescence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2007;96:107–130. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2006.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karatekin C, White T, Bingham C. Divided attention in youth-onset psychosis and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2008;117:881–895. doi: 10.1037/a0013446. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karatekin C, White T, Bingham C. Incidental and intentional sequence learning in youth-onset psychosis and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) Neuropsychology. 2009;23:445–459. doi: 10.1037/a0015562. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D, Rao U, Ryan N. Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia—Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL) University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine; Pittsburgh, PA: 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Keshavan MS, Diwadkar VA, Montrose DM, Rajarethinam R, Sweeney JA. Premorbid indicators and risk for schizophrenia: A selective review and update. Schizophrenia Research. 2005;79:45–47. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keshavan MS, Sujata M, Mehra A, Montrose DM, Sweeney JA. Psychosis proneness and ADHD in young relatives of schizophrenia patients. Schizophrenia Research. 2002;59:85–92. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(01)00400-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumra S, Jacobsen LK, Lenane M, Zahn TP, Wiggs E, Alaghband-Rad J, et al. “Multidimensionally impaired disorder”: Is it a variant of very early-onset schizophrenia? Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1998;37:91–99. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199801000-00022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marenco S, Weinberger DR. The neurodevelopmental hypothesis of schizophrenia: Following a train of evidence form cradle to grave. Development and Psychopathology. 2000;12:501–527. doi: 10.1017/s0954579400003138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKenna K, Gordon C, Lenane M, Kaysen D, Fahey K, Rapaport J. Looking for childhood-onset schizophrenia: The first 71 cases screened. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1994;33:636–644. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199406000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nicolson R, Brookner FB, Lenane M, Gochman P, Ingraham LJ, Egan MF, et al. Parental schizophrenia spectrum disorders in childhood-onset and adult-onset schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160:490–495. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.3.490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Niemi LT, Suvisaari JM, Tuulio-Henriksson A, Lönnqvist JK. Childhood developmental abnormalities in schizophrenia: Evidence from high-risk studies. Schizophrenia Research. 2003;60:239–258. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(02)00234-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olincy A, Ross RG, Harris JG, Young DA, McAndrews MA, Cawthra E, et al. The P50 auditory event-evoked potential in adult attention-deficit disorder: Comparison with schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry. 2000;47:969–977. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)00239-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Øie M, Sundet K, Rund BR. Contrasts in memory functions between adolescents with schizophrenia or ADHD. Neuropscyhologia. 1999;37:1351–1358. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(99)00043-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Öner Ö , Munir K. Attentional and neurocognitive characteristics of high-risk offspring of parents with schizophrenia compared with DSM-IV attention deficit hyperactivity disorder children. Schizophrenia Research. 2005;76:293–299. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.01.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ross RG, Harris JG, Olincy A, Radant A. Eye movement task measures inhibition and spatial working memory in adults with schizophrenia, ADHD, and a normal comparison group. Psychiatry Research. 2000;95:35–42. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(00)00153-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ross RG, Heinlein S, Tregellas H. High rates of comorbidity are found in childhood-onset schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research. 2006;88:90–95. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rund B, Øie M, Sundet K. Backward-masking deficit in adolescents with schizophrenic disorders or attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1996;153:1154–1157. doi: 10.1176/ajp.153.9.1154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schaeffer JL, Ross RG. Childhood-onset schizophrenia: Premorbid and prodromal diagnostic and treatment histories. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2002;41:538–545. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200205000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt K, Freidson S. Atypical outcome in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1990;29:566–570. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199007000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaywitz SE, Schnell C, Shaywitz BA, Towle VR. Yale Children’s Inventory (YCI): An instrument to assess children with attentional deficits and learning disabilities: I. Scale development and psychometric properties. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1986;14:347–364. doi: 10.1007/BF00915431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stevens J, Quittner AL, Abikoff H. Factors influencing elementary school teachers’ ratings of ADHD and ODD behaviors. Journal of Clinical and Child Psychology. 1998;27:406–414. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp2704_4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swanson J. School based assessments and interventions for ADD students. K.C. Publishing; Irvine, CA: 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Ueland T, Øie M, Landrø NI, Rund BR. Cognitive functioning in adolescents with schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Psychiatry Research. 2004;126:229–239. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2004.02.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. 3rd ed. Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. 3rd ed. Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. 4th ed Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler Individual Achievement Test (WIAT) Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1992. [Google Scholar]
- White T, Kendi ATK, Lehericy S, Kendi M, Karatekin C, Guimaraes A, et al. Disruption of hippocampal connectivity in children and adolescents with schizophrenia—a voxel-based diffusion tensor imaging study. Schizophrenia Research. 2007;90:302–307. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.09.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]