Abstract
Recent discoveries of single-gene influences on social behaviour have generated a great deal of interest in the proximate mechanisms underlying the expression of complex behaviours. Length polymorphism in a microsatellite in the regulatory region of the gene encoding the vasopressin 1a receptor (avpr1a) has been associated with both inter- and intra-specific variation in socially monogamous behaviour in voles (genus Microtus) under laboratory conditions. Here, we evaluate the relationship between avpr1a length polymorphism and social associations, genetic monogamy, and reproductive success in free-living prairie vole (M. ochrogaster) populations. We found no evidence of a relationship between avpr1a microsatellite length and any of our correlates of either social or genetic monogamy in the field. Our results, especially when taken in conjunction with those of recent experimental studies in semi-natural enclosures, suggest that avpr1a polymorphism is unlikely to have been a major influence in the evolution or maintenance of social monogamy in prairie voles under natural conditions.
Keywords: avpr1a, mating system, Microtus ochrogaster, monogamy, vasopressin
Recent interest in the proximate mechanisms underlying complex behaviour has led to the discovery of single genes with large effects on both neurobiological and behavioural variation (Robinson et al. 2008). Both genetic and neuroendocrine influences on a particular type of behaviour may be remarkably conserved across taxa; for example, the peptides oxytocin and vasopressin (and their non-mammalian homologues) affect similar social behaviours in vertebrate and invertebrate species (Donaldson & Young 2008; Goodson & Bass 2001). The gene that codes for the vasopressin 1a receptor (V1aR) in the mammalian brain, avpr1a, has been suggested to be a regulator of complex behaviour, and it is of particular interest due to its apparent effects on socially monogamous behaviour in mammals. Although most research has taken place with voles in the genus Microtus, particularly the socially monogamous prairie vole (M. ochrogaster, Hammock et al. 2005; Hammock & Young 2005), influences of avpr1a length on social behavior are not limited to rodents. A recent study found correlations between avpr1a polymorphism and social behavior in humans (Walum et al. 2008).
There is extensive evidence that variation in neural V1aR expression influences social behaviour both within and among species (Hammock & Young 2005, Ophir et al. 2008, Pitkow et al. 2001, Young et al. 2001). In particular, variation in V1aR expression in areas of the brain making up the ‘pair-bonding circuit’ has been associated with variation in socially monogamous behavior (Pitkow et al. 2001; Young et al. 2001). For example, experimental modification of neural expression of V1aR generates meadow voles (M. pennsylvanicus) and house mice (Mus musculus) that exhibit social behaviour typical of socially monogamous prairie voles, rather than the non-monogamous behaviour that is typical of their species (Lim et al. 2004, Young 1999). These and other studies demonstrate that variation in neural V1aR expression results in variation in social behaviour.
It has been suggested that both within- and among-species variation in neural V1aR is associated with a length polymorphism in the regulatory region of the avpr1a gene, which codes for V1aR (Hammock & Young 2005, Hammock et al. 2005, Pitkow et al. 2001). In particular, longer avpr1a microsatellites appear to be associated with increased V1aR expression in the pair-bonding circuit and ‘more monogamous’ behaviour (i.e., increased social contact) by male prairie voles in the laboratory (Hammock & Young 2005). Thus, it seems that the avpr1a length polymorphism influences neural V1aR expression, which in turn influences social behaviour.
However, the evidence for a direct relationship between avpr1a microsatellite length per se and social behaviour remains less clear than the evidence for a relationship between V1aR expression and social behaviour. Although laboratory studies have shown a positive correlation between avpr1a microsatellite length and the strength of partner preferences (Hammock & Young 2005), a recent phylogenetic study indicated that the presence of the expanded microsatellite alone does not predict the occurrence of social monogamy; most Microtus species possess an expanded avpr1a microsatellite region, but not all Microtus species that possess the expanded region are socially monogamous (Fink et al. 2006). A similar lack of an effect of avpr1a length polymorphism has been observed in a phylogenetic study in another rodent genus, Peromyscus (Turner et al. 2010). In light of these phylogenetic studies, the role of avpr1a microsatellite variation in regulating variation in socially monogamous behaviour appears to be more complex than initially hypothesized.
The results of previous studies of the effect of avpr1a length on social behaviour in the laboratory are intriguing, and these results lead to the question of how avpr1a might affect behaviour under natural conditions. The question of how avpr1a affects behaviour in nature is especially crucial for understanding how this gene may have been involved in the evolution of social monogamy, as has been suggested by Young & Hammock (2007). To have evolved initially, socially monogamous behaviour should convey some fitness benefit. If avpr1a variation is correlated with behavioural variation, we might also expect avpr1a variation to be related to reproductive success, with individuals possessing some variant of the gene producing more offspring. To date, two studies have examined the relationship between avpr1a length, neural V1aR expression, and social and reproductive behaviour in the field, using experiments conducted in semi-natural enclosures (Ophir et al. 2008; Solomon et al. 2009). In neither case did avpr1a length have a detectable effect on behavior indicative of social monogamy in males. However, Solomon et al. (2009) found that males with shorter avpr1a microsatellites mated with more females and sired more offspring than males with longer microsatellites, suggesting an effect of avpr1a on mating behaviour and genetic monogamy. Ophir et al. (2008) found correlations between V1aR expression and avpr1a length, but not between avpr1a length and either social monogamy or reproductive behaviour. Based on the results of these two semi-natural enclosure studies, there is currently little or no support for the existence of a relationship between avpr1a length and behaviors involved in social monogamy in male M. ochrogaster outside of the laboratory.
The objective of this study was to determine whether avpr1a length polymorphism is related to social or reproductive behaviour of male prairie voles in unmanipulated populations. If avpr1a is influential in the evolution of social monogamy, we expected a relationship between microsatellite length variation, social behaviour, and mating patterns in the field under conditions similar to those under which prairie voles evolved.
Methods
Study Sites and Animals
Our study sites were located at the University of Kansas Nelson Environmental Study Area (northeast of Lawrence, KS, USA; 39°03′07″N, 95°11′27″W), and the Indiana University Bayles Road Preserve (north of Bloomington, IN, USA; 39°13′00″N, 86°32′27″W). Both sites were old fields dominated by grasses and forbs with scattered tree seedlings, and were maintained by yearly mowing to prevent the encroachment of woody plants. Field work was conducted in KS during May-June 2005, 2006, and 2008, and in IN during July-August 2006 -2008, for a total of three field seasons at each site. We began field work earlier in KS because the peak prairie vole breeding season begins about one month earlier in KS than in IN (Myers & Krebs 1971; Rose & Gaines 1978), and because the KS population experiences a breeding lull in midsummer (Rose & Gaines 1978). The size of the study area varied among sites and years, from 1 ha (KS 2005) to 2.2 ha (IN 2007). However, because we used individual animals, rather than study sites, as the unit of replication in statistical analyses, differences in the area of the study sites should not affect our results.
In the field, prairie voles are typically considered to be socially but not necessarily genetically monogamous (Getz & Hofmann 1986; Ophir et al. 2008; Solomon et al. 2004; Solomon et al. 2009). Due to a recent population decline in Urbana-Champaign, IL, where much previous work on the social behavior of free-living prairie voles has taken place, we were unable to include IL voles. Social structure in the KS and IN populations is not identical to that in IL: 50% of social units in IL are male-female pairs (Getz et al. 1993) while 29% and 37% of social units are pairs in KS and IN, respectively (Streatfeild et al. unpublished data). However, all types of social units (pairs, multi-adult groups, and singletons) are present in the IL, KS, and IN populations.
Social behaviour
We characterised the social behaviour of prairie voles using live-trapping at known prairie vole nests to determine social associations. We began each field season with either one (2005-2007) or two (2008) weeks of trapping a 10 × 10 m grid laid out across the entire study site. Grid trapping allowed us to capture and individually mark animals, and obtain females to track to their nests. During grid trapping, a single Ugglan multiple capture trap (Grahnab, Hillerstorp, Sweden) was placed in a vole runway ≤ 1 m from each grid marker. All traps were set in the late afternoon and checked that evening and the following morning for five (2005-2007) or four (2008) days each week. At all other times traps were left in place but unset. We baited traps with cracked corn, covered traps with either an aluminum shield or a wooden cover board to protect trapped animals from heat and rain, and provided cotton batting as bedding material when overnight temperatures were ≤ 10° C.
We identified nest sites by either radio-tracking or fluorescent-powder tracking of live-trapped females back to their nests (see Lucia et al. 2008 for a complete description of nest location methods). After locating nests and recording the coordinates with a hand-held global positioning system unit (eTrex Legend; Garmin, Olathe, KS, USA), we placed four live-traps within 30 cm of the entrances of each nest. Following the initial grid trapping weeks, we conducted intensive live-trapping at nests for either three (2005-2007) or two (2008) consecutive weeks to identify which adults were captured together at nests, and to capture newly emerged offspring. During nest trapping weeks, we checked traps in the mornings and evenings from Sunday evening until Tuesday evening, and again from Wednesday evening until Friday evening, for a total of 10 nest trap checks/week. During 2008, we conducted an additional four weeks (two grid and two nest) of live-trapping at both sites after the initial assessment of social behaviour. Because of trap disturbance by mammalian carnivores (raccoons, Procyon lotor, and domestic dogs, Canis lupus) during the second nest trapping period in KS, we include only trapping data from the first nest trapping period at each site during 2008. However, we did include animals trapped during the entire eight-week study period at each site in genetic analyses.
At every capture event, the individual identification number, capture location, sex, age class, reproductive condition (males: scrotal or non-scrotal testes; females: pregnant and/or lactating), and mass (g) of the captured animal were recorded. Individuals were permanently marked using unique toe-clip combinations, and tissue was preserved for later genetic analysis (see Ethical note, below). Animals ≥ 30 g in mass were classified as adults, while animals 21-29 g were considered subadults, and animals < 21 g were classified as juveniles (Gaines et al. 1979; Getz et al. 1993).
We used data from live-trapping at nests to determine the number of unique adult females with which an adult male was associated, and to quantify the strength of the associations between adult males and females. Individuals that were captured at the same nest during the same trap check were considered ‘associated’ during that trap check; there were 20-30 nest trap checks, and thus, 20-30 opportunities for individuals to be trapped together each year. We calculated the number of unique females with which a male was associated during nest trapping and the pairwise half-weight Association Index (hereafter AI; Cairns & Schwager 1987) between each male and every female with which he was associated. To quantify the relative strength of each male's association with the female with which he was most often associated, we calculated a Relative Association Index (Relative AI). Relative AI was calculated by dividing a male's AI with his ‘most associated’ female by the sum of all AIs with all females with which he associated. Relative AI is conceptually very similar to the Maximum Relative Encounter Rate (RERMAX) used by Ophir et al. (2008) to quantify the relative overlap of male and female prairie vole home ranges. Values of both Relative AI and RERMAX ≥ 0.5 indicate that a male associated with a particular female more than twice as often as with all other females combined; a Relative AI of 1 indicates that the male associated with only one female. To reduce the chances of including animals that were not local residents in analyses of social behaviour, we included only adults captured > 3 times during nest trapping.
Genetic parentage analysis
To determine which adult prairie voles produced offspring together, we genotyped all live-trapped voles at six polymorphic microsatellite loci (Keane et al. 2007). We used either standard phenol/chloroform extraction techniques (Sambrook et al. 1989) or DNeasy extraction kits (Qiagen, Valencia, CA, USA) to extract genomic DNA from tissue samples and conducted polymerase chain reactions (PCR) to amplify microsatellites (see Keane et al. 2007 and Solomon et al. 2009 for further details on PCR conditions). PCR products were diluted, combined with an internal size standard (LIZ 500, Applied Biosystems, Foster City, CA, USA) and detected using an ABI 3130xl or 3730 DNA sequencer (Applied Biosystems, Foster City, CA, USA). Base-pair (bp) lengths of the fluorescently labeled DNA fragments were determined with GeneMapper 3.7 software (Applied Biosystems, Foster City, CA, USA), and alleles were binned into discreet size classes using FlexiBin (Amos et al. 2006).
We conducted genetic parentage assignment in Cervus 3.0, which calculates a likelihood ratio score for each candidate parent to identify the male and female that are most likely to be the true parents of a particular offspring (Kalinowski et al. 2007; see Keane et al. 2007 for details on genetic parentage assignment). Before conducting parentage analysis, we calculated observed and expected heterozygosities at each microsatellite locus for all adult voles at each site during each year, and assessed deviations from Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium using Cervus 3.0 (Table 1). We then conducted separate parentage analyses for juveniles captured at each site during each year. Following Winters & Waser (2003), we conducted a multi-stage genetic parentage analysis, in which we first used the parent-pair option and considered all adults trapped ≤ 20 m from a juvenile's site of origin (natal nest or first grid trap site) to be candidate parents. We chose the 20 m criterion because it is approximately equal to the diameter of an average adult home range for our study populations (unpublished data). If both parents were not assigned with ≥ 95% confidence in the first iteration of the parentage analysis, we then expanded the set of candidate parents to include all adults captured ≤ 40 m from the juvenile's site of origin. We restricted the set of candidate parents to those trapped within 40 m of a juvenile's natal location to minimize the possibility of spurious assignments of parentage due to chance. Finally, if we were able to assign a mother, but not a father, with ≥ 95% confidence, we used the ‘known mother’ option in Cervus 3.0, with all adult males captured ≤ 40 m from the juvenile's site of origin as candidate fathers. We used live-trapping data, particularly captures of adult females with nursing young, to confirm the genetic assignments of juveniles to a ‘known’ mother. Adult females that Cervus identified as mothers at 95% confidence were always captured at least once in the same trap as the juvenile to which they were assigned, bolstering our confidence in the genetic parentage assignments.
Table 1.
Observed and expected heterozygosities for 6 microsatellite loci used in genetic parentage analysis.
locus | Kansas | Indiana | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
2005 | 2006 | 2008 | 2006 | 2007 | 2008 | |||||||
Ho | He | Ho | He | Ho | He | Ho | He | Ho | He | Ho | He | |
AV13 | 0.737 | 0.911‡ | 0.807 | 0.922‡ | 0.832 | 0.873 | 0.881 | 0.862‡ | 0.848 | 0.864 | 0.847 | 0.874 |
MOE2 | 0.909 | 0.883‡ | 0.931 | 0.918‡ | 0.874 | 0.888 | 0.756 | 0.857 | 0.823 | 0.854 | 0.854 | 0.843 |
MSMM2 | 0.963 | 0.921‡ | 0.966 | 0.938‡ | 0.897 | 0.915‡ | 0.875 | 0.877 | 0.848 | 0.872 | 0.893 | 0.888 |
MSMM3 | 0.875 | 0.892‡ | 0.933 | 0.886‡ | 0.786 | 0.867 | 0.872 | 0.854 | 0.815 | 0.857 | 0.809 | 0.849* |
MSMM5 | 0.772 | 0.929‡ | 0.613 | 0.900‡ | 0.870 | 0.934‡ | 0.814 | 0.903‡ | 0.820 | 0.895 | 0.859 | 0.887 |
MSMM6 | 0.456 | 0.458‡ | 0.400 | 0.501‡ | 0.458 | 0.423 | 0.142 | 0.174‡ | 0.077 | 0.096‡ | 0.091 | 0.094‡ |
locus not in Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (HWE)
HWE test not done by Cervus in cases where the expected number of alleles is < 5
Genetic parentage data were used to determine the total number of offspring each male sired, and the number of different females with which a male sired offspring. We considered males that sired two or more offspring with a single female genetically monogamous, and males that sired two or more offspring by two or more females were considered not genetically monogamous.
avpr1a genotyping
The avpr1a genotypes of adult males were determined by PCR amplification of the microsatellite region of the V1aR gene (using primers reported in Hammock & Young 2005). Our avpr1a genotyping procedure was identical to that of Solomon et al. (2009), who confirmed that these primers amplified the functional copy of the avpr1a gene, rather than the avpr1a pseudogene. Because avpr1a contains a complex microsatellite with multiple types of repeat units (Hammock & Young 2005; Ophir et al. 2008; Young 1999), the locus does not easily lend itself to binning into distinct allele size classes. Therefore, we quantified allele length using the raw length measurements (bp) exported from GeneMapper and treated avpr1a length as a continuous variable. Although there is some error in estimating microsatellite length this way, we were most interested in relative avpr1a length among individuals, rather than absolute microsatellite length.
Statistical analyses
Because avpr1a has been suggested to influence inter-population variation in social behaviour (Cushing et al. 2001), we first determined whether the distribution of the summed avpr1a lengths of adult males differed between KS and IN with a non-parametric Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, pooling data among years within sites. We also determined whether the distribution of microsatellite lengths at each site was statistically distinguishable from a normal distribution.
We analyzed all data on patterns of social associations and genetic parentage using parametric regressions; sample sizes were large enough to justify the use of parametric tests (N = 40-138 individuals), and they were at least as large as sample sizes in other field tests of effects of avpr1a on behaviour (Ophir et al. 2008; Solomon et al. 2009). In all analyses, the independent variable was avpr1a microsatellite length. We conducted separate tests of the effects of the summed avpr1a length, as well as the long and the short alleles individually, on behaviour. In the initial analysis, we included both avpr1a length and site (KS or IN) as predictors. Because we found no statistically significant effects of site on behaviour, we dropped the “site” term. However, for the sake of thoroughness, we also conducted regression analyses for each site independently (results were not qualitatively different from those of the pooled analysis). Because results were similar whether we used summed or individual allele lengths, we report only the effect of summed avpr1a microsatellite length on behaviour. Because we were unable to collect data on avpr1a length, social behaviour, and genetic mating patterns from every adult male, sample sizes vary among analyses. For example, some males were captured too infrequently for the social behaviour analysis, but sired offspring. In the case of binary responses (i.e., whether a male was genetically monogamous), we used logistic regression. Analysis of reproductive success includes all males that sired offspring, while analysis of genetic monogamy includes only the subset of successful males that sired at least two offspring. All statistical tests were conducted in PASW 18 (SPSS, Chicago, IL, USA), and were two-tailed with α=0.05; means are presented ± 1 SE.
Ethical note
All vole trapping, handling, and marking procedures were approved by the animal care and use committees of Miami University (protocol 689), the University of Kansas (protocol 185-01), and Indiana University (protocol 10-020), and were conducted under a state of Kansas scientific permit (SC-135-2007). Collecting permits were not required by the state of Indiana.
There are multiple methods that may be used for individually marking small mammals (Gannon et al. 2007). Our study required unique, permanent marks for each individual. Non-invasive marks such as hair dyes and hair clips were inappropriate because of their temporary nature (marks are lost as hair grows or is molted). More permanent marking procedures include freeze brands, ear notches, ear tags, tattoos, passive integrated transponders (PIT tags), and toe-clips. Each of these procedures likely causes some amount of stress to the animal. Freeze brands, ear notches, and tattoos were inappropriate for this study because of the limited number of available unique marks in relation to the large number of individual animals that we trapped at each site. Ear tags are widely used to mark small mammals, but are less useful for prairie voles, which have very small external ear pinnae. The rate of ear tag loss in prairie voles may be as high as 10-16% (Wood & Slade 1990; Harper & Batzli 1996). Ear tags are lost when they become snagged on vegetation and are ripped from the animal's ear. In addition to causing pain to the animal when a tag is lost, lost tags also compromise the integrity of the data collected because individuals trapped over long time periods cannot be positively identified after tag loss. Subcutaneously implanted PIT tags allow researchers to identify individually a large number of animals. However, the implantation procedure is invasive, and implantation wounds may become infected (Harper & Batzli 1996). PIT tags were also inappropriate for this study because many animals were initially trapped as very small juveniles, and PIT tags are quite large (about the size of a grain of rice) in relation to newborn prairie voles. Toe-clipping is recommended by the American Society of Mammalogists (ASM) Animal Care and Use Committee in cases where other marking methods are inappropriate (Gannon et al. 2007). No other marking method provided the combination of permanence, a large number of unique marks, and the ability to mark extremely small animals necessary for our study. Additionally, toe-clipping is suggested by the ASM as being especially appropriate for studies such as ours that require a small amount of tissue for genetic analysis. Finally, in a previous study, prairie voles that were toe clipped-maintained weight and survived as well as those that were ear-tagged (Wood & Slade 1990).
We individually marked animals in the field using uniquely-coded combinations of toe clips. We used a clean, sharp pair of scissors to remove toes at the second joint. Most often, 0-1 toes were clipped per foot. In rare cases, such as when an animal had already lost a toe naturally, we removed a second toe from a single foot. Approximately 3% of animals had two toes removed from a single foot. The ASM guidelines generally recommend removing only one toe per foot, but removal of two toes is permitted in unusual circumstances (Gannon et al. 2007). The ASM guidelines do not recommend the use of anesthetics or analgesics during toe-clipping because of the prolonged period of restraint that is necessary to apply them, and because consumption of analgesic substances by licking may cause additional stress to the animal (Gannon et al. 2007).
Results
avpr1a genotyping
We successfully genotyped 470/589 (79.8%) male prairie voles at the avpr1a microsatellite locus. Summed avpr1a lengths ranged from 1398-1574 bp in KS and 1392-1579 bp in IN. The distributions of avpr1a lengths from adult males differed between the KS and IN populations (Kolmogorov-Smirnov test: Z = 1.736, N = 470, P = 0.005). There were proportionally more males from KS with summed avpr1a lengths of 1430-1450 bp, and proportionally more males from IN with avpr1a lengths of 1450-1470 bp. (Fig. 1). In neither population was the distribution of avpr1a lengths statistically different from a normal distribution (Kolmogorov Smirnov test (KS): Z = 0.81, N = 146, P = 0.52; Kolmogorov Smirnov test (IN): Z = 0.60, N = 324, P = 0.91).
Fig. 1.
Frequency distribution of adult male avpr1a microsatellite lengths in KS and IN. The x-axis is the summed length of the two avpr1a microsatellite alleles for each male.
Social behaviour
The number of females with which a male associated was not predicted by avpr1a genotype (Fig. 2a; Regression: T = 0.13, N = 138, P = 0.89). Most males associated with 1-3 different females. The relative strength of a male's association with a particular female (Relative AI) was not predicted by avpr1a length (Fig. 2b; Regression: T = 1.22, N = 138, P = 0.22). Relative AI assesses the strength of a male's association with the female with which he was most frequently captured, and ranges from 0-1. Relative AI of 0 indicates that a male associated with no females, and Relative AI of 1 indicates that he associated with just 1 female. Relative AI values ≥0.5 and <1 indicate that a male associated with more than 1 female, but was captured with a particular female more than twice as often as he was captured with all other females combined.
Fig. 2.
Relationship between the summed length of a male's two avpr1a alleles and: a) Number of females with which the male associated, and b) Relative Association Index (Relative AI) between the male and the female with which he was most-strongly associated. Black dots represent males from KS, and white dots represent males from IN.
Genetic parentage assignment and genetic monogamy
The 6 microsatellite loci used for parentage analysis had 4-23 unique alleles in a population, with polymorphic information content ranging from 0.093-0.916 (Tables 2, 3). Overall, we were able to assign parent-pairs at the 95% confidence level for 346/601 (57.6%) of individuals first caught as juveniles.
Table 2.
Summary of microsatellite data from adult prairie voles in KS.
locus | N | # alleles | polymorphic information content | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
2005 | 2006 | 2008 | 2005 | 2006 | 2008 | 2005 | 2006 | 2008 | |
AV13 | 57 | 31 | 143 | 17 | 16 | 14 | 0.895 | 0.900 | 0.856 |
MOE2 | 55 | 29 | 143 | 15 | 15 | 16 | 0.864 | 0.895 | 0.874 |
MSMM2 | 54 | 29 | 136 | 17 | 17 | 17 | 0.906 | 0.916 | 0.905 |
MSMM3 | 56 | 30 | 140 | 13 | 16 | 15 | 0.874 | 0.862 | 0.853 |
MSMM5 | 57 | 31 | 138 | 20 | 19 | 23 | 0.916 | 0.876 | 0.926 |
MSMM6 | 57 | 30 | 144 | 9 | 10 | 10 | 0.437 | 0.466 | 0.409 |
mean | 15.17 ± 1.56 | 15.50 ± 1.23 | 15.83 ± 1.74 | 0.82 ± 0.08 | 0.82 ± 0.07 | 0.80 ± 0.08 |
Table 3.
Summary of microsatellite data from adult prairie voles in IN.
locus | N | # alleles | polymorphic information content | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
2006 | 2007 | 2008 | 2006 | 2007 | 2008 | 2006 | 2007 | 2008 | |
AV13 | 109 | 283 | 326 | 14 | 14 | 13 | 0.842 | 0.847 | 0.860 |
MOE2 | 119 | 282 | 328 | 16 | 17 | 15 | 0.841 | 0.840 | 0.829 |
MSMM2 | 120 | 276 | 328 | 16 | 14 | 14 | 0.861 | 0.857 | 0.876 |
MSMM3 | 117 | 259 | 299 | 11 | 12 | 10 | 0.834 | 0.841 | 0.831 |
MSMM5 | 102 | 272 | 327 | 21 | 20 | 21 | 0.890 | 0.885 | 0.877 |
MSMM6 | 120 | 285 | 328 | 8 | 10 | 4 | 0.170 | 0.095 | 0.093 |
mean | 14.33 ± 1.84 | 14.50 ± 1.45 | 12.83 ± 2.30 | 0.74 ± 0.11 | 0.73 ± 0.13 | 0.72 ± 0.13 |
avpr1a length was not associated with any measure of genetic monogamy or reproductive success of males. Neither the number of genetic mates, nor the number of offspring, was predicted by avpr1a length (Fig. 3a,b; Regression (mates): T = -0.59, N = 65, P = 0.56; Regression (offspring): T = -0.61, N = 65, P = 0.55). Finally, avpr1a length did not predict whether a male that sired at least two offspring was genetically monogamous (Logistic regression: χ2 = 0.39, N = 40, P = 0.53). Overall, 48.9% (23/47) males with at least two offspring were genetically monogamous.
Fig. 3.
Relationship between the summed length of a male's two avpr1a alleles and: a) Number of genetic mates, and b) Number of offspring sired. Black dots represent males from KS, and white dots represent males from IN.
Discussion
Although male prairie voles displayed substantial variation in behaviour indicative of social monogamy, genetic monogamy and reproductive success, we found no relationship between the avpr1a genotype of males and any of these variables in free-living prairie voles in two geographically-distinct natural populations. Our results from unmanipulated populations, especially when taken in conjunction with the results of field experiments by Ophir et al. (2008) and Solomon et al. (2009), suggest that avpr1a length polymorphism is not strongly associated with behaviour indicative of social monogamy in prairie voles under ecologically-relevant conditions. To date, the only evidence consistent with an effect of avpr1a genotype per se on any aspect of behaviour of male prairie voles in the field is Solomon et al.'s (2009) finding that males with shorter avpr1a microsatellites mated with more females and sired more offspring than males with longer microsatellites in semi-natural enclosures. It is possible that the effects of avpr1a on mating behaviour only become evident when males sire multiple litters (∼3 or more) over an extended breeding season, as in Solomon et al.'s (2009) 14-week study. It is also possible that Solomon et al.'s (2009) results from semi-natural populations were influenced by the absence of potential predators, which likely led to higher adult survival rates and extended lifespans over which males might sire more litters with more females. However, it should be noted that individual males sired multiple litters in the current study as well, with at least 24 males siring 2+ litters by different females (this estimate is conservative, because those males that sired multiple litters with the same female were not included).
What factors might be responsible for the general lack of agreement between laboratory and field studies of the effect of avpr1a polymorphism on social behaviour in male prairie voles? One possibility is that field and laboratory researchers are measuring different aspects of behaviour. For example, in the laboratory partner preferences are assessed by comparing the amount of time a male spends in contact with two different females, with which he has equal access (Cushing et al. 2001, Hammock & Young 2005). In the field, however, it is unlikely that a male has equal access to two females simultaneously (Getz et al. 2004) and interacts with only those females without interference from other males. Field researchers typically quantify indirect measures that are expected to correlate with partner preferences, such as home range overlap (Ophir et al. 2008; Solomon et al. 2009), or the frequency with which two individuals are trapped simultaneously at the same nest, as we have done here. Finally, in the laboratory studies, investigators examined behaviour during pair-bond formation, while in the field we may be more likely to see behaviour involved in the maintenance of pair bonds (see Bales et al. 2007). Thus, field data are fundamentally different from data collected under controlled experimental conditions in the laboratory, and results from field and lab studies may not be directly comparable (Wolff 2003).
The behavioural challenges facing free-living animals are also fundamentally different from those posed in the laboratory. In the laboratory, animals are presented with behavioural situations and choices that are likely to be much simpler than those faced in nature. In laboratory preference tests, a focal male is presented with a choice between two female conspecifics, each of which is tethered and unable to interact physically with the other ‘competitor.’ The behaviour exhibited by the focal animal under these conditions is unlikely to represent what the same animal might do in nature where there are multiple potential mates, potential mates are distributed over a larger area, and multiple individuals of both sexes are able to interact over a considerably longer time (weeks, rather than hours). Further complicating matters, under ecologically-relevant conditions animals must also forage, defend their nest, and avoid predators, in addition to choosing a mate and helping to rear offspring. These competing demands may mean that in the field, potential effects of genetic variation on social behaviour may be overwhelmed by other factors.
In natural habitats, both ecological and social conditions are inherently variable through space and time. For example, many Microtus species experience dramatic population cycles, fluctuating from < 10 to > 600 individuals/ha (Getz et al. 2001). The social behaviour and mating choices exhibited by a male might be quite different if radically different numbers of female partners are available to him, regardless of his avpr1a genotype. Consistent with the possibility of a demographic effect on behaviour, Solomon et al. (2009) found that population density, but not avpr1a genotype, influenced space use and behaviour indicative of social monogamy by male prairie voles in semi-natural enclosures. In their study, males in low-density populations had larger home ranges and physically contacted fewer females than did males in high-density populations (Solomon et al. 2009). We were unable to test explicitly for effects of population density on behaviour, because naturally-occurring densities during our study were much lower than those reported for “high density” prairie vole populations (> 600/ha in nature; Getz et al. 1993, and up to 770/ha in semi-natural enclosures; Lucia et al. 2008). However, our data did suggest a possible effect of population density on behaviour. The apparent tendency for IN males to associate with more females than KS males may be due to higher population densities and a correspondingly higher probability of encountering more females in IN than in KS (Streatfeild et al., unpublished data).
Finally, our inability to detect an effect of avpr1a length on measures of either social or genetic monogamy in natural populations may result from extensive individual behavioural variation present under natural conditions. That is, a correlation between avpr1a length and behaviour may be present, but the pattern is obscured by a great deal of naturally-occurring behavioural variation among individuals. We cannot rule out this possibility, but sample sizes were large enough (N = 40-138) that biologically significant effects of avpr1a should have been evident, particularly as our sample sizes are comparable to those in other studies that detected statistically significant effects of avpr1a (on neural V1aR expression, Ophir et al. 2008; and genetic monogamy, Solomon et al. 2009).
Other researchers have suggested that rather than variation in avpr1a microsatellite length per se, sequence variation within the microsatellite might be important in regulating social behaviour in prairie voles (Ophir et al. 2008; Young & Hammock 2007). This suggestion is consistent with earlier observations of a positive relationship between avpr1a length and degree of socially monogamous behaviour if longer microsatellites are more likely to include functionally significant sequences (Hammock & Young 2005). Our data did not allow us to test the hypothesis that avpr1a sequence variation influences behaviour, but given the lack of agreement among recent studies of the effects of microsatellite length, both between laboratory and field studies, and among different lab studies (Hammock & Young 2005 compared to Hammock et al., 2005), sequence variation deserves further investigation. The possibility of functional sequence variation within the microsatellite control region of avpr1a is particularly intriguing in light of recent documentation of functional variation within the coding region of avpr1a in Microtus (Fink et al. 2007).
Recent excitement about the role of avpr1a in inter- and intra-specific mating system variation is primarily based on evidence from laboratory studies of voles in the genus Microtus, with several studies suggesting a link between avpr1a variation and the occurrence of social monogamy (Hammock & Young 2005; Young 1999; Young & Wang 2004), a highly unusual mating system in mammals (Kleiman 1977). However, the data we present here indicate that avpr1a length polymorphism is correlated with neither social nor genetic monogamy under unmanipulated field conditions, and the presence of a normal distribution of avpr1a lengths in both KS and IN populations suggests that there is not a great advantage to possessing either short or long alleles (Fig. 1). Our data are consistent with other studies of social and mating behaviour under semi-natural conditions; Ophir et al. (2008) found no relationship between avpr1a and social or mating behaviour, while Solomon et al. (2009) found a relationship between avpr1a, the genetic mating system and reproductive success, but not between avpr1a length and behaviour indicative of social monogamy. The results of these field studies, in conjunction with a recent phylogenetic analysis of avpr1a polymorphism across Microtus (Fink et al. 2006), lead us to suggest that avpr1a length polymorphism is unlikely to be an important factor in the evolution or maintenance of social, and possibly genetic monogamy under natural conditions.
Acknowledgments
We thank Galen Pittman and Keith Clay for logistical assistance at KU and IU field sites, respectively, and Chris Wood for his help at the Miami University Center for Bioinformatics and Functional Genomics. Tom Crist kindly wrote the R script used to calculate the association index. We also thank Frank Castelli, Adrian Chesh, and Kristen Lucia, as well as the many undergraduate students and field assistants who participated in field and laboratory research. Comments from Sarah Karlen, Kendra Sewall, and two anonymous reviewers on a previous version greatly improved the manuscript. Funding was provided by the National Science Foundation (IOS-0614015) to BK, Paul A. Harding, and NGS, and the National Institutes of Health (NIGMS GM 06409-01) to NGS.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Amos W, Hoffman JI, Frodsham A, Zhang L, Best S, Hill AVS. Automated binning of microsatellite alleles: problems and solutions. Molecular Ecology Notes. 2006;7:10–14. [Google Scholar]
- Bales KL, Mason WA, Catana C, Cherry SR, Mendoza SP. Neural correlates of pair-bonding in a monogamous primate. Brain Research. 2007;1184:245–253. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.09.087. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cairns SJ, Schwager SJ. A comparison of association indexes. Animal Behaviour. 1987;35:1454–1469. [Google Scholar]
- Cushing BS, Martin JO, Young LJ, Carter CS. The effects of peptides on partner preference formation are predicted by habitat in prairie voles. Hormones and Behavior. 2001;39:48–58. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.2000.1633. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donaldson ZR, Young LJ. Oxytocin, vasopressin, and the neurogenetics of sociality. Science. 2008;322:900–904. doi: 10.1126/science.1158668. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fink S, Excoffier L, Heckel G. Mammalian monogamy is not controlled by a single gene. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2006;103:10956–10960. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0602380103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fink S, Excoffier L, Heckel G. High variability and non-neutral evolution of the mammalian avpr1a gene. BMC Evolutionary Biology. 2007;7 doi: 10.1186/1471-2148-7-176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaines MS, Vivas AM, Baker CL. An experimental analysis of dispersal in fluctuating vole populations: demographic parameters. Ecology. 1979;60:814–828. [Google Scholar]
- Gannon WL, Sikes RS, Animal Care and Use Committee of the American Society 454 of Mammalogists Guidelines of the American Society of Mammalogists for the use of wild mammals in research. Journal of Mammalogy. 2007;88:809–832. doi: 10.1093/jmammal/gyw078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Getz LL, Hofmann JE. Social organization in free-living prairie voles, Microtus ochrogaster. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1986;18:275–282. [Google Scholar]
- Getz LL, McGuire B, Pizzuto T, Hofmann JE, Frase B. Social organization of the prairie vole (Microtus ochrogaster) Journal of Mammalogy. 1993;74:44–58. [Google Scholar]
- Getz LL, Hofmann JE, McGuire B, Dolan TW. Twenty-five years of population fluctuations of Microtus ochrogaster and M. pennsylvanicus in three habitats in east-central Illinois. Journal of Mammalogy. 2001;82:22–34. [Google Scholar]
- Getz LL, McGuire B, Pizzuto T. Does mate choice take place in free-living prairie voles, Microtus ochrogaster?Evidence from field data. Acta Zoologica Sinica. 2004;50:527–534. [Google Scholar]
- Goodson JL, Bass AH. Social behavior functions and related anatomical characteristics of vasotocin/vasopressin systems in vertebrates. Brain Research Reviews. 2001;35:246–265. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(01)00043-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hammock EAD, Lim MM, Nair HP, Young LJ. Association of vasopressin 1a receptor levels with a regulatory microsatellite and behavior. Genes Brain and Behavior. 2005;4:289–301. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2005.00119.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hammock EAD, Young LJ. Microsatellite instability generates diversity in brain and sociobehavioral traits. Science. 2005;308:1630–1634. doi: 10.1126/science.1111427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harper SJ, Batzli GO. Monitoring use of runways by voles with passive integrated transponders. Journal of Mammalogy. 1996;77:364–369. [Google Scholar]
- Kalinowski ST, Taper ML, Marshall TC. Revising how the computer program CERVUS accommodates genotyping error increases success in paternity assignment. Molecular Ecology. 2007;16:1099–1106. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2007.03089.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keane B, Bryant L, Goyal U, Williams S, Kortering SL, Lucia KE, Richmond AR, Solomon NG. No effect of body condition at weaning on survival and reproduction in prairie voles. Canadian Journal of Zoology-Revue Canadienne De Zoologie. 2007;85:718–727. [Google Scholar]
- Kleiman DG. Monogamy in mammals. Quarterly Review of Biology. 1977;52:39–69. doi: 10.1086/409721. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lim MM, Wang Z, Olazabal DE, Ren X, Terwilliger EF, Young LJ. Enhanced parter preference in a promiscuous species by manipulating the expression of a single gene. Nature. 2004;429:754–757. doi: 10.1038/nature02539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lucia KE, Keane B, Hayes LD, Lin YK, Schaefer RL, Solomon NG. Philopatry in prairie voles: An evaluation of the habitat saturation hypothesis. Behavioral Ecology. 2008;19:774–783. [Google Scholar]
- Myers JH, Krebs CJ. Genetic, behavioral, and reproductive attributes of dispersing field voles Microtus pennsylvanicus and Microtus ochrogaster. Ecological Monographs. 1971;41:53–78. [Google Scholar]
- Ophir AG, Campbell P, Hanna K, Phelps SM. Field tests of cis-regulatory variation at the prairie vole avpr1a locus: Association with v1ar abundance but not sexual or social fidelity. Hormones and Behavior. 2008;54:694–702. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.07.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pitkow LJ, Sharer CA, Ren XL, Insel TR, Terwilliger EF, Young LJ. Facilitation of affiliation and pair-bond formation by vasopressin receptor gene transfer into the ventral forebrain of a monogamous vole. Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;21:7392–7396. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-18-07392.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson GE, Fernald RD, Clayton DF. Genes and social behavior. Science. 2008;322:896–900. doi: 10.1126/science.1159277. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rose RK, Gaines MS. The reproductive cycle of Microtus ochrogaster in eastern Kansas. Ecological Monographs. 1978;48:21–42. [Google Scholar]
- Sambrook J, Fritch EF, Maniatus T. Molecular cloning: A laboratory manual. 2. Cold Spring Harbor, NY: Cold Spring Harbor Press; 1989. [Google Scholar]
- Solomon NG, Keane B, Knoch LR, Hogan PJ. Multiple paternity in socially monogamous prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster) Canadian Journal of Zoology-Revue Canadienne De Zoologie. 2004;82:1667–1671. [Google Scholar]
- Solomon NG, Richmond AR, Harding PA, Fries A, Jacquemin S, Schaefer RL, Lucia KE, Keane B. Polymorphism at the avpr1a locus in male prairie voles correlated with genetic but not social monogamy in field populations. Molecular Ecology. 2009;18:4680–4695. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2009.04361.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turner LM, Young AR, Rompler H, Schoneberg T, Phelps SM, Hoekstra HE. Monogamy evolves through multiple mechanisms: Evidence from V1aR in deer mice. Molecular Biology and Evolution. 2010;27:1269–1278. doi: 10.1093/molbev/msq013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walum H, Westberg L, Henningsson S, Neiderhiser JM, Reiss D, Igl W, Ganiban JM, Spotts EL, Pedersen NL, Eriksson E, Lichtenstein P. Genetic variation in the vasopressin receptor 1a gene (avpr1a) associates with pair-bonding behavior in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2008;105:14153–14156. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0803081105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winters JB, Waser PM. Gene dispersal and outbreeding in a philopatric mammal. Molecular Ecology. 2003;12:2251–2259. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-294x.2003.01896.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolff JO. Laboratory studies with rodents: Facts or artifacts? Bioscience. 2003;53:421–427. [Google Scholar]
- Wood MD, Slade NA. Comparison of ear-tagging and toe clipping in prairie voles, Microtus ochrogaster. Journal of Mammalogy. 1990;71:252–255. [Google Scholar]
- Young LJ. Oxcytocin and vasopressin receptors and species-typical social behaviors. Hormones and Behavior. 1999;36:212–221. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1999.1548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young LJ, Lim MM, Gingrich B, Insel TR. Cellular mechanisms of social attachment. Hormones and Behavior. 2001;40:133–138. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.2001.1691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young LJ, Wang ZX. The neurobiology of pair bonding. Nature Neuroscience. 2004;7:1048–1054. doi: 10.1038/nn1327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young LJ, Hammock EAD. On switches and knobs, microsatellites and monogamy. Trends in Genetics. 2007;23:209–212. doi: 10.1016/j.tig.2007.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]