Skip to main content
Springer logoLink to Springer
. 2010 Dec 16;214(4):911–921. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-2102-6

The effects of NMDA receptor antagonists on attentional set-shifting task performance in mice

Tomasz Kos 1, Agnieszka Nikiforuk 1, Dominik Rafa 1, Piotr Popik 1,2,
PMCID: PMC3063548  PMID: 21161188

Abstract

Rationale and objectives

Cognitive deficits, including an impaired ability to shift perceptual attentional set, belong to the core features of schizophrenia, are associated with prefrontal cortical dysfunctions, and may involve glutamate NMDA receptors. Although phencyclidine disturbs cognitive flexibility, little is known about the effects of ketamine and other NMDA antagonists that differ in receptor subunit selectivity, particularly in the mouse species.

Methods

At different times following the administration of ketamine, the NMDA NR2B-subtype specific antagonist Ro 25-6981, or the atypical antipsychotic sertindole, male C57Bl/6J mice were investigated in a modified version of attentional set-shifting task (ASST).

Results

Specific extra-dimensional shift (EDS) deficit was observed in all control mice. As revealed by the increased number of trials, time and errors to reach criterion, ketamine at 10 or 20 mg/kg given 50 min prior to sessions, but not at 10 mg/kg given 3 or 24 h prior to sessions, further worsened the EDS performance. Sertindole (2.5 mg/kg) prevented ketamine-induced cognitive inflexibility, although it did not affect ASST performance when given alone. In contrast to ketamine, Ro 25-6981 at 10 but not 3 mg/kg, reduced the number of trials and errors to criterion, suggesting a facilitation of cognitive flexibility. Finally, as revealed by the number of trials and time to criterion measures, Ro 25-6981 (10 mg/kg) administration to ketamine (10 mg/kg)-pretreated mice inhibited ketamine-induced cognitive inflexibility.

Conclusion

The present study provides an improved and reliable mouse ASST protocol and confirms and extends previous findings demonstrating that NR2B subunit-selective antagonists improve cognitive processes.

Keywords: Ketamine, NMDA receptor antagonist, NR2B-selective antagonist, Mouse, Cognition, Schizophrenia, Animal model

Introduction

Schizophrenia is characterized by well-known “positive” and “negative” symptoms and by less recognized cognitive deficits in executive functions, working memory, and attention (Bowie and Harvey 2006). These cognitive deficits are present at the onset of illness, persist without remission throughout most of patients’ lives, and may precede the development of “positive” symptoms (Brewer et al. 2006). Flexibility in response to a change in attentional set toward stimuli is controlled by the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and is commonly assessed in patients using the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST; Grant and Berg 1948; Elliott et al. 1998). A poor WCST performance is observed not only in schizophrenics but also in patients with frontal lobe damage (Pantelis et al. 1999).

The impairments in executive functions observed in WCST in humans can be investigated using a rodent version of the attentional set-shifting task (ASST) developed by Birrell and Brown (2000). The ASST requires animals to initially learn a rule and form “an attentional set” within the same stimulus dimension, by discriminating between, e.g., the odors associated with food reward. During the extra-dimensional (ED) shift, the crucial and most difficult part of the test, the subjects have to rapidly switch their attention to a new and previously irrelevant stimulus dimension and to discriminate between, e.g., the media covering the food reward. Although an increasing number of laboratories have successfully used the ASST in rats, most of the attempts to adapt this procedure for use in mice have failed to demonstrate an ED shift deficit (Colacicco et al. 2002; Laurent and Podhorna 2004; Zhuo et al. 2007; Levi et al. 2008). Only one procedure, described by Garner et al. (2006) and replicated with slight modification by Papaleo et al. (2008), has shown a significant extra-dimensional shift (EDS) phase deficit in this species. However, our laboratory was unable to demonstrate an ED shift impairment even with these modifications, and therefore, the first goal of the present study was to establish the conditions allowing for the measurement of reliable set-shifting in mice.

Converging lines of evidence indicate the involvement of glutamate NMDA transmission in schizophrenia. A single dose of an NMDA receptor antagonist, such as phencyclidine (PCP) or ketamine, induces symptoms of acute psychosis in healthy volunteers (Luby 1959; Javitt and Zukin 1991). Because these symptoms are indistinguishable from some symptoms of schizophrenia (Krystal et al. 1994), antagonists of NMDA receptors are routinely used in preclinical research as a pharmacological model of this psychosis (Olney and Farber 1995; Jentsch and Roth 1999). The acute administration of ketamine impairs attentional set-shifting as measured by the WCST in healthy humans (Krystal et al. 2000) and in the rat (Nikiforuk et al. 2010). Therefore, the second goal of the present study was to investigate the ketamine-induced impairment of ASST in mice.

Compared to conventional antipsychotics, relatively novel atypical compounds, such as sertindole, display an advantageous profile of pro-cognitive actions in schizophrenia. A recent multi-center, randomized, double-blinded clinical trial indicated a beneficial effect of sertindole on executive functions in patients with schizophrenia (Gallhofer et al. 2007); this medication also reduced the debilitating effects of ketamine in the Stroop task in healthy volunteers (Vollenweider et al. 1999). Several preclinical studies have shown that sertindole reduces or reverses the impairing effects of subchronic PCP treatment (Rodefer et al. 2008; Broberg et al. 2009; Goetghebeur and Dias 2009) and of an acute dose of ketamine (Nikiforuk et al. 2010) in the rat ASST. An obligatory step in the validation of a given procedure relies on the demonstration of a positive effect of the medication previously found to be effective in similar conditions. The third goal of the present study was to investigate whether sertindole could reverse ketamine-induced deficits in the mouse version of the ASST.

NMDA receptors are heteromers composed of an obligatory NR1 (mouse zeta 1) subunit and of at least one type of the NR2 (NR2A–NR2D; mouse epsilon 1–4) subunits (Laube et al. 1998; Schorge and Colquhoun 2003). The type of NR2 subunit constituting the NMDA receptor affects its physiological and pharmacological properties (Monaghan and Larsen 1997; Loftis and Janowsky 2003). In addition, the brain distribution of NR2 subunits is not uniform (Monyer et al. 1994; Wenzel et al. 1995); although NR2A messenger RNA (mRNA) is distributed particularly in the cerebral cortex, hippocampus, and cerebellum, the NR2B transcript is selectively present in the forebrain with a high level of expression in the cerebral cortex, hippocampus, septum, caudate–putamen, and olfactory bulb. The NR2C mRNA is expressed predominantly in the cerebellum, and the NR2D transcript is detected in the thalamus, brain stem, and olfactory bulb. The NR2C and NR2D transcripts are found in a subset of hippocampal neurons, which are most likely interneurons (Ozawa et al. 1998).

This pattern of distribution suggests that subunit-specific NMDA antagonists may have differential effects under various physiological and pathological conditions (Skolnick et al. 2009). Although ketamine is regarded as an NR2-unspecific antagonist (Dravid et al. 2007), recent data indicate that it mainly affects NR1/2C and NR1/2D NMDA receptors (Kotermanski and Johnson 2009). Due to its antagonistic action at hippocampal NMDA receptors, ketamine may cause cortical disinhibition, which is relevant to some symptoms of schizophrenia, including cognitive inflexibility (Greene 2001; Homayoun and Moghaddam 2007; Lisman et al. 2008). In contrast, the NR2B-selective antagonist, CP-101,606 (traxoprodil), under certain test conditions, improves cognitive flexibility as assessed by the five-choice serial reaction time task (5-CSRT; Higgins et al. 2005). Therefore, the fourth goal of the present study was to investigate the effects of an NR2B-selective antagonist alone and in combination with ketamine in the ASST procedure. We used the Ro 25-6981 compound [(aR,bS)-a-(4-hydroxyphenyl)-b-methyl-4-(phenylmethyl)-1-piperidinepropanol hydrochloride] that is characterized by a >5,000-fold selectivity for NR2C/NR2B over NR2C/NR2A subunits of the NMDA receptor, use-dependent binding properties and neuroprotective effects against glutamate toxicity (Fischer et al. 1997).

Materials and methods

Ethics

The experiments were conducted in accordance with the NIH Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and were approved by the Ethics Committee for Animal Experiments, Institute of Pharmacology.

Animals

The studies were performed on male C57Bl/6J mice (Institute of Pharmacology breeding facility, originating from the Jackson Laboratories and purchased from Charles-River, Germany) weighing approximately 24 g and being approximately 7 to 8 weeks old at the start of the experiment. The mice were housed in standard laboratory cages under standard colony A/C-controlled conditions: room temperature of 21 ± 2°C, humidity of 40% to 50%, and a 12-h light/dark cycle (lights on at 06:00). The mice were individually housed and mildly food deprived (2.9 g of food pellets per day), with ad libitum access to water for 1 week prior to testing. Mice were food restricted throughout the experiment to maintain approximately 85% of their initially body weight. Behavioral testing was performed during the light phase of the light/dark cycle. Different groups of animals were used in different drug experiments.

Attentional set-shifting task procedure

The experiments were performed according to the procedure for mice described by Garner et al. (2006) and Papaleo et al. (2008) with some modifications. The mice were tested in a black acrylic U-shaped maze apparatus (W × L, 39 × 39 cm) with a wire grid floor. The starting compartment provided an entrance via guillotine doors into two identical testing compartments. A bowl with water was placed in the starting compartment to allow mice free access to water throughout the whole training procedure. The food reward (1/8 of Honey Nut Cheerio, Nestle®) was presented in containers (glass dishes, 5 cm in diameter, 3 cm in depth). The containers were defined by a pair of cues with two stimulus dimensions: different kinds of digging media and distinct odors (2 μl of a flavoring essence, Dr. Oetker®, Poland, on a piece of the blotting paper fixed to the external wall of a glass dish prior to use). Different containers were used for each combination of the digging medium and odor. Stimulus dimensions are shown in Table 1.

Table 1.

Pairs of different stimuli used in the mouse version of the ASST

Dimension Exemplar pairs
Odor Orange Lemon Spicy Cream
Arrack Almond Vanilla Rum
Medium Fine gravel Crumpled tissue Cardboard Bamboo sticks
Beads Aluminum balls Plastic Polystyrene

The entire study consisted of several experiments. Each experiment was performed on three consecutive days with training sessions 1 and 2 on the 2nd and 3rd days (Table 2).

Table 2.

Detailed illustration of the present ASST procedure

Phase Dimension Discrimination 1 Discrimination 2
Session 1
SD Media Fine gravel vs. beads
 
CD Media Orange Arrack Orange Arrack
Fine gravel Beads Beads Fine gravel
 
CDR Media Orange Arrack Orange Arrack
Fine gravel Beads Beads Fine gravel
 
IDS Media Lemon Almond Lemon Almond
Crumpled tissue Aluminum balls Aluminum balls Crumpled tissue
 
IDSR Media Lemon Almond Lemon Almond
Crumpled tissue Aluminum balls Aluminum balls Crumpled tissue
 
Session 2
IDS2 Media Spicy Vanilla Spicy Vanilla
Cardboard Plastic Plastic Cardboard
 
IDSR2 Media Spicy Vanilla Spicy Vanilla
Cardboard Plastic Plastic Cardboard
 
EDS Odors Cream Rum Cream Rum
Bamboo sticks Polystyrene Polystyrene Bamboo sticks
 
EDSR Odors Cream Rum Cream Rum
Bamboo sticks Polystyrene Polystyrene Bamboo sticks

For each trial, mice were presented with either discrimination 1 or 2. In this example at the SD phase, all digging dishes had no odor, and the subjects had to choose the beads as the correct digging medium (in italics). Note that the stimuli are the same for CD and CDR, for IDS and IDR, and for EDS and EDSR

Habituation (day 1)

On the first day of testing, mice were habituated to the apparatus, containers, and food reward for 5 min. The four phases of shaping were then initiated; the first three phases with two trials each and the fourth phase with three trials were performed sequentially. During each trial, the mouse was placed in the starting compartment, and the guillotine door was raised to allow entry to the test compartment with the containers filled with the food reward. During the first phase, the food reward was placed at the bottom of an empty container. For the next phase, the containers were filled with 0.5 cm of wood shavings, and the food reward was placed on top of the filling. In the third phase, the reward was placed under a thin layer of wood shavings. In the final phase, the food reward was buried under 2 cm of wood shavings. Once the mice retrieved the food reward, they were allowed to consume it before being returned to the starting compartment.

Session 1 (2nd day) and session 2 (3rd day)

Each mouse was trained to discriminate between pairs of containers that differed in odor and/or the media covering the food reward. This training was performed during two sessions, which consisted of several phases with several trials. During each trial, the animal was placed in the starting compartment, and the guillotine door was raised to give the mouse access to two containers, only one of which contained the food reward. The food reward was buried under the surface of distinct digging media. The first three trials of each session were “free”, meaning that even after an incorrect choice, the door remained open, and the mouse was allowed to investigate and collect the food reward from the opposite container. On subsequent trials, the door was closed upon the animal’s entry to the compartment. If the mouse started to dig in the un-baited container (i.e., the container without the food reward), an error was recorded, the trial was terminated, and the mouse was returned to the starting compartment. Each mouse had to reach a criterion of eight correct discriminations out of ten consecutive trials to complete each phase. The inter-trial interval was approximately 5 s, and the interval between consecutive phases in a given session was approximately 45 s.

We followed the protocol of Papaleo et al. (2008) by inserting additional phases (IDS2 and IDSR2). However, in contrast to the protocols of Papaleo et al. (2008) and Garner et al. (2006), we divided the whole ASST procedure into two sessions. The reason for using a two-session procedure was that the mice appeared satiated when we attempted to complete all of the phases of testing in 1 day. Therefore, session 1 consisted of a warm-up habituation trial (identical to the last trial of the habituation session) and five phases: (1) simple discrimination (SD), (2) compound discrimination (CD), (3) compound discrimination reversal (CDR), (4) intra-dimensional shift (IDS), and (5) intra-dimensional shift reversal (IDSR). The overall testing time for one animal during this session was approximately 60 to 70 min. Session 2 also consisted of a warm-up habituation trial and four phases: (1) intra-dimensional shift 2 (IDS2), (2) intra-dimensional shift reversal (IDSR2), (3) EDS, and (4) extra-dimensional shift reversal (EDSR). The overall testing time for one animal during this session was approximately 40 to 50 min.

Thus, the mice were exposed to a series of increasingly difficult discriminations during which they discriminated within a dimension (the media covering the food reward) that was relevant from the SD phase until the EDS phase. At the EDS phase, the mouse had to start discriminating within a new stimulus dimension (odors), and the previously relevant dimension (media) became irrelevant. This schedule was used for all of the subjects and was based on preliminary results that demonstrated a tendency for better performance when the sequence of dimension change was from the media to odors (data not shown). An example of the entire task is shown in Table 2.

Drugs

Ketamine (10% aqueous solution of 115.34 mg/ml, Biowet, Poland) and Ro 25-6981 compound [(aR,bS)-a-(4-hydroxyphenyl)-b-methyl-4-(phenylmethyl)-1-piperidinepropanol hydrochloride] (Sequoia, UK) were dissolved in distilled water. Sertindole (Sigma-Aldrich, Germany) was dissolved in a minimum amount of 0.1 M hydrochloric acid and diluted with water. Drugs were administered before each of the two sessions. Therefore, the same animal received drug treatment twice: ketamine at 50 min, 3 h or 24 h (IP), the Ro 25-6981 compound at 30 min (IP), and sertindole at 120 min (PO). The ketamine “24-h” animals received the drug approximately 1 h after the last trial of habituation and approximately 1 h after session 1. All of the compounds were administered in a volume of 10 ml/kg, and the doses were chosen based on in-house data and published literature (Higgins et al. 2005; Rodefer et al. 2008).

Statistics

The following parameters were measured for each mouse: the number of trials to criterion, the number of errors to criterion, and the time to reach criterion, which was defined as eight correct choices out of ten consecutive trials. Statistical analyses were performed with Statistica 8.0 for Windows. The data from sessions 1 and 2 were calculated separately using repeated measures ANOVAs. Whenever significant interactions between the treatment conditions and the phase of a session were found, the data were analyzed with Newman–Keuls post hoc test; P < 0.05 was deemed significant.

Results

Modification of the ASST procedure: general remarks

The modification of the protocols of Garner et al. (2006) and Papaleo et al. (2008) produced reliable set-shifting in mice because in all replications, the subjects required significantly more trials to reach the criterion during the EDS than during the IDS2 phase of the task. The interaction factor between treatment conditions and the session phase for all of the experiments’ session 1 measures, including trials, errors, and time to criterion, yielded insignificant results.

Time-related effects of ketamine on ASST performance

As assessed by the interaction factors between treatment condition and the phase of a given session, ketamine (10 mg/kg, 3 or 24 h before the test) changed neither the number of trials and errors nor the time to reach the criterion at any of the discrimination phases (Fig. 1a–c). In contrast, when administered 50 min before testing, ketamine significantly increased the number of trials (F(9, 84) = 2.72, P < 0.01; Fig. 1a) and errors (F(9, 84) = 2.86, P < 0.01; Fig. 1c) to criterion during the EDS phase. Although the time to criterion in this group was increased (Fig. 1b), the interaction (treatment × phase) for session 2 yielded insignificant results.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

ac Relationship between the time of ketamine administration and attentional set-shifting performance in mice. Ketamine (10 mg/kg) was administered 50 min and 3 or 24 h before both sessions. N = 8 mice per treatment condition. Data are presented as the mean ± SEM. Symbols: *P < 0.05 vs. vehicle, i P < 0.05 vs. IDS2 phase, k P < 0.05 vs. ketamine 10 mg/kg group at 50 min (the standard ketamine treatment); Newman–Keuls post hoc test

Dose-related effects of ketamine on ASST performance

Ketamine (10 and 20 mg/kg, administered 50 min before the test) dose-dependently, significantly, and specifically increased the number of trials (F(6, 66) = 17.49, P < 0.001; Fig. 2a), errors (F(6, 66) = 8.22, P < 0.001; Fig. 2b), and the time to criterion (F(6, 66) = 9.04, P < 0.001; Fig. 2c) during the EDS phase. In all three measures, the effects of 20 mg/kg ketamine were significantly stronger than the effects of 10 mg/kg.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

ac Dose-dependent effects of ketamine on the attentional set-shifting task in mice. Ketamine (10 or 20 mg/kg) was administered 50 min before both sessions. N = 8–9 mice per treatment. Data are presented as the mean ± SEM. Symbols: *P < 0.05 vs. vehicle, i P < 0.05 vs. IDS2 phase, k P < 0.05 vs. ketamine 10 mg/kg group; Newman–Keuls post hoc test

Effects of sertindole on ketamine-induced deficits in the ASST

Similar to the two earlier experiments, 10 mg/kg of ketamine administered 50 min before the sessions increased both the number of trials (F(3, 84) = 2.62, P < 0.05; Fig. 3a) and errors (F(3, 84) = 2.67, P < 0.05; Fig. 3c) to criterion during the EDS phase. Sertindole + ketamine treatment decreased these measures to the levels of the control group, and they were significantly lower compared to the ketamine alone treatment. Sertindole (2.5 mg/kg) did not change the number of trials or errors to criterion during the EDS phase compared with the vehicle-treated and sertindole + ketamine-treated groups (Fig. 3a, c). In this set of data, the time to criterion measure (Fig. 3b) was not affected by any of the treatments.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Effects of sertindole on ketamine-induced deficits in the attentional set-shifting task in mice. Sertindole (2.5 mg/kg) and ketamine (10 mg/kg) were administered 2 h and 50 min before both sessions, respectively (N = 7–9 mice per treatment). Data are presented as the mean ± SEM. Symbols: *P < 0.05 vs. vehicle, i P < 0.05 vs. IDS2 phase, k P < 0.05 vs. ketamine 10 mg/kg group; Newman–Keuls post hoc test

Dose-related effects of Ro 25-6981 on ASST performance

Ro 25-6981, administered 30 min before both sessions at 10 mg/kg, but not at 3 mg/kg, significantly and specifically facilitated performance during the EDS phase. The higher dose decreased the number of trials (F(6, 63) = 4.97, P < 0.001; Fig. 4a) and errors (F(6, 63) = 4.27, P < 0.01; Fig. 4c) to criterion compared to the vehicle-treated group and compared to the group treated with the lower dose. The time to criterion measure (Fig. 4b) was not affected by these treatments.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

ac Effects of Ro 25-6981 on attentional set-shifting task performance in mice. Ro 25-6981 (3 or 10 mg/kg) was administered 30 min before both sessions (N = 8 mice per treatment). Data are presented as the mean ± SEM. Symbols: *P < 0.05 vs. vehicle, i P < 0.05 vs. IDS2 phase, # P < 0.05 vs. Ro 25-6981 at 3 mg/kg; Newman–Keuls post hoc test

Effects of Ro 25-6981 on ketamine-induced deficits in the ASST

Ketamine (10 mg/kg) increased the number of trials to criterion during the EDS phase, but the Ro 25-6981 treatment alone (10 mg/kg) decreased this measure compared to the vehicle-treated group (F(3, 84) = 8.72, P < 0.001; Fig. 5a), confirming our earlier findings. The administration of Ro 25-6981 to ketamine-pretreated mice decreased the number of trials to criterion during the EDS phase compared to the vehicle- and ketamine-treated groups (Fig. 5a). A similar pattern of effects was observed for the errors to criterion measure; however, this statistical analysis yielded insignificant results (P = 0.10; Fig. 5c). Ketamine (10 mg/kg) prolonged the time to criterion during the EDS phase (see also Fig. 2), and the ketamine + Ro 25-6981 treatment decreased this measure compared to the ketamine alone group (F(3, 84) = 4.09, P < 0.01; Fig. 5b). Similar to the earlier experiment, the Ro 25-6981 treatment shortened the time to criterion compared to the vehicle-treated group, but statistical analysis yielded insignificant differences.

Fig. 5.

Fig. 5

ac Effects of Ro 25-6981 on ketamine-induced deficits in the attentional set-shifting task in mice. Ro 25-6981 (10 mg/kg) and ketamine (10 mg/kg) were administered 30 and 50 min before both sessions, respectively (N = 8 mice per treatment). Data are presented as the mean ± SEM. Symbols: *P < 0.05 vs. vehicle, i P < 0.05 vs. IDS2 phase, k P < 0.05 vs. ketamine 10 mg/kg group; Newman–Keuls post hoc test

Discussion

The present study demonstrated a procedure of attentional set-shifting in mice capable of revealing a deficit during the EDS phase, which was further impaired by ketamine administration. We confirmed that the atypical antipsychotic sertindole alleviated ketamine-induced cognitive deficits. In addition, the present data suggest that the NR2B subunit-selective NMDA antagonist Ro 25-6981 not only facilitated ASST performance during the EDS phase but also reversed ketamine-induced cognitive inflexibility.

Only two published modifications of the primary ASST procedure for rats (Birrell and Brown 2000) have successfully demonstrated significant EDS phase impairment in mice (Garner et al. 2006; Papaleo et al. 2008). Because mice were unable to complete the test in 1 day under our experimental conditions, we divided the procedure into two sessions and included additional phases (IDS2 and IDSR2). As pointed out by Garner et al. (2006), like reversal phases (CDR and IDSR), these additional phases serve to strengthen the cognitive set formation. This outcome was manifested by a general improvement in performance during the phases ranging from CD to IDSR2 followed by a poorer EDS phase performance, which is regarded as the primary measure of set-shifting. Under these conditions, we demonstrated reliable set-shifting in mice because, in all replications (Figs. 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5), the subjects required significantly more trials to reach the criterion during the EDS phase than during the IDS2 phase of the task. A drawback of the 2-day protocol is that the investigated compound has to be administered twice, but the relatively short duration of the test session could be considered as a potential advantage for the investigation of compounds characterized by a short half-life.

The present work confirms the results of Nikiforuk et al. (2010) demonstrating that, in the rat ASST, ketamine produces specific deficits in cognitive flexibility after a single dose given 50 min before the test and extends these findings to the mouse species. Although these studies cannot be compared directly, the observed effect was dose dependent; the higher dose of ketamine (20 mg/kg) more severely impaired the task performance compared to the lower dose (10 mg/kg). It was also time dependent; the ED shift deficit was demonstrated at 50 min, but not 3 or 24 h, following drug administration. The latter observation somewhat contrasts with the findings of Egerton et al. (2005), demonstrating that a single dose of PCP tested 24 h after administration produced an ED shift impairment. However, different NMDA receptor antagonists were used, and the studies were performed in a different species. Notably, in clinical experiments, WCST impairment is observed 40 min after ketamine infusion (Krystal et al. 2000), and ketamine-induced psychotomimetic effects, as measured by the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale, disappeared within 2 h after infusion (Berman et al. 2000; Zarate et al. 2006). Therefore, the current protocol appears to be a promising pharmacological tool for modeling mouse cognitive inflexibility. Its predictive validity is based on the inhibitory effects of sertindole (Fig. 3), which also reduced the impairing effects of PCP-like compounds in the rat ASST in former studies (Rodefer et al. 2008; Broberg et al. 2009; Goetghebeur and Dias 2009; Nikiforuk et al. 2010) and alleviated the cognitive impairments associated with schizophrenia (Gallhofer et al. 2007) and ketamine administration in healthy volunteers (Vollenweider et al. 1999).

The major finding of this set of experiments concerns the pro-cognitive effects of the NMDA NR2B subunit-selective antagonist Ro 25-6981 in the mouse ASST. These effects were shown by the reversal of the ketamine-induced cognitive deficit (Fig. 5) and by cognitive improvement in mice treated with Ro 25-6981 only (Fig. 4). The pro-cognitive effects of the NR2B-selective antagonist are in sharp contrast to the PCP-like antagonists, and to our knowledge, this is the first demonstration of an NMDA antagonist-induced pro-cognitive action in the ASST. The effects of NR2B-selective antagonists on cognitive functions are complicated. The Ro 63-1908 does not affect performance in the Morris water maze and delayed matching to position task (DMTP) performance, though in 5-CSRT Ro 63-1908 produces an increase in premature responses, and in differential reinforcement of low-rate 24 s (DRL24) task, it reduces the inter-response time, suggesting an impairment of response inhibition (Higgins et al. 2003). Although traxoprodil does not affect spatial learning (Guscott et al. 2003), Ro 25-6981 disrupts spatial reversal learning and enhances perseveration in the Morris water maze (Duffy et al. 2008). Consistent with the latter report, NMDA 2B subunit over-expression results in better performance in learning and memory tasks, like novel object recognition, the Morris water maze, and the t-maze, in mice (Tang et al. 1999; Cao et al. 2007) and rats (Wang et al. 2009).

Nonetheless, in tasks specifically assessing cognitive flexibility, like the 5-CSRT, both traxoprodil and Ro 25-6981 (3–30 mg/kg) increase premature responding and response speed with no error trade-off similar to Ro 63-1908. At a short inter-trial interval, Ro 63-1908 reliably improved performance, both in terms of response speed and accuracy, and traxoprodil improves accuracy and increases response speed in the DMTP task (Higgins et al. 2005). These authors conclude that selective NR2B NMDA antagonists promote impulsive-type responding that—under certain test conditions, such as a higher event rate of stimulus presentation—results in an improvement of task performance. Somewhat similar results have been reported by Gilmour et al. (2009), who show that NR2B-specific antagonists increase instrumental output in a series of variable interval schedules of reinforcement in an operant task. Our data appear to confirm these findings because the treatment with Ro 25-6981 not only reduced the number of trials to criterion (Fig. 4a) but also reduced, albeit insignificantly, the time required to complete the EDS phase of the set-shifting task (Figs. 4b and 5b).

The present data cannot offer a convincing explanation of the mechanism for the pro-cognitive action of Ro 25-6981. The ASST does not allow for the measurement of all of the parameters that are typically recorded in 5-CSRT and is a relatively simple procedure with a remarkably lower workload. Nonetheless, we may speculate that the Ro 25-6981-induced reduction in the number of trials to criterion and in the time to complete the EDS phase resembles a similar improvement in performance (response speed and accuracy) as the improvements found in the 5-CSRT (Higgins et al. 2005). The ketamine-induced inhibition of hippocampal NMDA receptors may enhance the firing rate of glutamatergic PFC neurons and impair PFC-dependent functions, including cognitive flexibility (Greene 2001; Homayoun and Moghaddam 2007; Lisman et al. 2008). A high density of the NR2B subunit within the PFC, including infra-limbic regions (Wenzel et al. 1995), confirmed by autoradiography with [3H]-Ro 25-6981 (Mutel et al. 1998), suggests that the compound exerted its actions in the PFC. It is likely that the unique localization of the NR2B-containing NMDA receptors allows for the reversal of the ketamine-induced cognitive deficit in ASST via blockade of glutamatergic cortical disinhibition. Future studies will reveal the precise mechanism of the pro-cognitive action of this NR2B-selective NMDA antagonist.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by statutory activity funds from the Institute of Pharmacology, Polish Academy of Sciences (Kraków, Poland) and by the project UDA-POIG.01.03.01-12-063/09-00 Prokog co-financed by the European Union from the European Fund of Regional Development (EFRD). The financial support of the Foundation for Polish Science to T. Kos is highly appreciated.

Open Access

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Noncommercial License which permits any noncommercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and source are credited.

References

  1. Berman RM, Cappiello A, Anand A, Oren DA, Heninger GR, Charney DS, Krystal JH. Antidepressant effects of ketamine in depressed patients. Biol Psychiatry. 2000;47:351–354. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00230-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Birrell JM, Brown VJ. Medial frontal cortex mediates perceptual attentional set shifting in the rat. J Neurosci. 2000;20:4320–4324. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-11-04320.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bowie CR, Harvey PD. Treatment of cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Curr Opin Investig Drugs. 2006;7:608–613. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Brewer WJ, Wood SJ, Phillips LJ, Francey SM, Pantelis C, Yung AR, Cornblatt B, McGorry PD. Generalized and specific cognitive performance in clinical high-risk cohorts: a review highlighting potential vulnerability markers for psychosis. Schizophr Bull. 2006;32:538–555. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbj077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Broberg BV, Glenthoj BY, Dias R, Larsen DB, Olsen CK. Reversal of cognitive deficits by an ampakine (CX516) and sertindole in two animal models of schizophrenia—sub-chronic and early postnatal PCP treatment in attentional set-shifting. Psychopharmacol Berl. 2009;206:631–640. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1540-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Cao X, Cui Z, Feng R, Tang YP, Qin Z, Mei B, Tsien JZ. Maintenance of superior learning and memory function in NR2B transgenic mice during ageing. Eur J Neurosci. 2007;25:1815–1822. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05431.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Colacicco G, Welzl H, Lipp HP, Wurbel H. Attentional set-shifting in mice: modification of a rat paradigm, and evidence for strain-dependent variation. Behav Brain Res. 2002;132:95–102. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00391-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Dravid SM, Erreger K, Yuan H, Nicholson K, Le P, Lyuboslavsky P, Almonte A, Murray E, Mosely C, Barber J, French A, Balster R, Murray TF, Traynelis SF. Subunit-specific mechanisms and proton sensitivity of NMDA receptor channel block. J Physiol. 2007;581:107–128. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.124958. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Duffy S, Labrie V, Roder JC. D-serine augments NMDA-NR2B receptor-dependent hippocampal long-term depression and spatial reversal learning. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:1004–1018. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Egerton A, Reid L, McKerchar CE, Morris BJ, Pratt JA. Impairment in perceptual attentional set-shifting following PCP administration: a rodent model of set-shifting deficits in schizophrenia. Psychopharmacol Berl. 2005;179:77–84. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-2109-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Elliott R, McKenna PJ, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. Specific neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenic patients with preserved intellectual function. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 1998;3:45–70. doi: 10.1080/135468098396242. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  12. Fischer G, Mutel V, Trube G, Malherbe P, Kew JN, Mohacsi E, Heitz MP, Kemp JA. Ro 25-6981, a highly potent and selective blocker of N-methyl-d-aspartate receptors containing the NR2B subunit. Characterization in vitro. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1997;283:1285–1292. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Gallhofer B, Jaanson P, Mittoux A, Tanghoj P, Lis S, Krieger S. Course of recovery of cognitive impairment in patients with schizophrenia: a randomised double-blind study comparing sertindole and haloperidol. Pharmacopsychiatry. 2007;40:275–286. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-990291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Garner JP, Thogerson CM, Wurbel H, Murray JD, Mench JA. Animal neuropsychology: validation of the intra-dimensional extra-dimensional set shifting task for mice. Behav Brain Res. 2006;173:53–61. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Gilmour G, Pioli EY, Dix SL, Smith JW, Conway MW, Jones WT, Loomis S, Mason R, Shahabi S, Tricklebank MD. Diverse and often opposite behavioural effects of NMDA receptor antagonists in rats: implications for “NMDA antagonist modelling” of schizophrenia. Psychopharmacol Berl. 2009;205:203–216. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1530-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Goetghebeur P, Dias R. Comparison of haloperidol, risperidone, sertindole, and modafinil to reverse an attentional set-shifting impairment following subchronic PCP administration in the rat—a back translational study. Psychopharmacol Berl. 2009;202:287–293. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1132-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Grant DA, Berg EA. A behavioral analysis of degree of reinforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in a Weigl-type card-sorting problem. J Exp Psychol. 1948;38:404–411. doi: 10.1037/h0059831. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Greene R. Circuit analysis of NMDAR hypofunction in the hippocampus, in vitro, and psychosis of schizophrenia. Hippocampus. 2001;11:569–577. doi: 10.1002/hipo.1072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Guscott MR, Clarke HF, Murray F, Grimwood S, Bristow LJ, Hutson PH. The effect of (±)-CP-101, 606, an NMDA receptor NR2B subunit selective antagonist, in the Morris water maze. Eur J Pharmacol. 2003;476:193–199. doi: 10.1016/S0014-2999(03)02182-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Higgins GA, Ballard TM, Huwyler J, Kemp JA, Gill R. Evaluation of the NR2B-selective NMDA receptor antagonist Ro 63-1908 on rodent behaviour: evidence for an involvement of NR2B NMDA receptors in response inhibition. Neuropharmacology. 2003;44:324–341. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3908(02)00402-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Higgins GA, Ballard TM, Enderlin M, Haman M, Kemp JA. Evidence for improved performance in cognitive tasks following selective NR2B NMDA receptor antagonist pre-treatment in the rat. Psychopharmacol Berl. 2005;179:85–98. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-2203-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Homayoun H, Moghaddam B. NMDA receptor hypofunction produces opposite effects on prefrontal cortex interneurons and pyramidal neurons. J Neurosci. 2007;27:11496–11500. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2213-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Javitt DC, Zukin SR. Recent advances in the phencyclidine model of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 1991;148:1301–1308. doi: 10.1176/ajp.148.10.1301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Jentsch JD, Roth RH. The neuropsychopharmacology of phencyclidine: from NMDA receptor hypofunction to the dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1999;20:201–225. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(98)00060-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kotermanski SE, Johnson JW. Mg2+ imparts NMDA receptor subtype selectivity to the Alzheimer's drug memantine. J Neurosci. 2009;29:2774–2779. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3703-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Krystal JH, Karper LP, Seibyl JP, Freeman GK, Delaney R, Bremner JD, Heninger GR, Bowers MB, Charney DS. Subanesthetic effects of the noncompetitive NMDA antagonist, ketamine, in humans. Psychotomimetic, perceptual, cognitive, and neuroendocrine responses. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;51:199–214. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950030035004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Krystal JH, Bennett A, Abisaab D, Belger A, Karper LP, Dsouza DC, Lipschitz D, Abidargham A, Charney DS. Dissociation of ketamine effects on rule acquisition and rule implementation: possible relevance to NMDA receptor contributions to executive cognitive functions. Biol Psychiatry. 2000;47:137–143. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00097-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Laube B, Kuhse J, Betz H. Evidence for a tetrameric structure of recombinant NMDA receptors. J Neurosci. 1998;18:2954–2961. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-08-02954.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Laurent V, Podhorna J. Subchronic phencyclidine treatment impairs performance of C57BL/6 mice in the attentional set-shifting task. Behav Pharmacol. 2004;15:141–148. doi: 10.1097/00008877-200403000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Levi Y, Kofman O, Schwebel M, Shaldubina A. Discrimination and avoidance learning in adult mice following developmental exposure to diisopropylfluorophosphate. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2008;88:438–445. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2007.09.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Lisman JE, Coyle JT, Green RW, Javitt DC, Benes FM, Heckers S, Grace AA. Circuit-based framework for understanding neurotransmitter and risk gene interactions in schizophrenia. Trends Neurosci. 2008;31:234–242. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Loftis JM, Janowsky A. The N-methyl-d-aspartate receptor subunit NR2B: localization, functional properties, regulation, and clinical implications. Pharmacol Ther. 2003;97:55–85. doi: 10.1016/S0163-7258(02)00302-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Luby ED. Study of a new schizophrenomimetic drug—Sernyl. A M A Arch Neurol Psychiatry. 1959;81:363–369. doi: 10.1001/archneurpsyc.1959.02340150095011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Monaghan DT, Larsen H. NR1 and NR2 subunit contributions to N-methyl-d-aspartate receptor channel blocker pharmacology. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1997;280:614–620. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Monyer H, Burnashev N, Laurie DJ, Sakmann B, Seeburg PH. Developmental and regional expression in the rat brain and functional properties of four NMDA receptors. Neuron. 1994;12:529–540. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(94)90210-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Mutel V, Buchy D, Klingelschmidt A, Messer J, Bleuel Z, Kemp JA, Richards JG. In vitro binding properties in rat brain of [3H]Ro 25-6981, a potent and selective antagonist of NMDA receptors containing NR2B subunits. J Neurochem. 1998;70:2147–2155. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.1998.70052147.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Nikiforuk A, Golembiowska K, Popik P. Mazindol attenuates ketamine-induced cognitive deficit in the attentional set shifting task in rats. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2010;20:37–48. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2009.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Olney JW, Farber NB. NMDA antagonists as neurotherapeutic drugs, psychotogens, neurotoxins, and research tools for studying schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1995;13:335–345. doi: 10.1016/0893-133X(95)00079-S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Ozawa S, Kamiya H, Tsuzuki K. Glutamate receptors in the mammalian central nervous system. Prog Neurobiol. 1998;54:581–618. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0082(97)00085-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Pantelis C, Barber FZ, Barnes TR, Nelson HE, Owen AM, Robbins TW. Comparison of set-shifting ability in patients with chronic schizophrenia and frontal lobe damage. Schizophr Res. 1999;37:251–270. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(98)00156-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Papaleo F, Crawley JN, Song J, Lipska BK, Pickel J, Weinberger DR, Chen J. Genetic dissection of the role of catechol-O-methyltransferase in cognition and stress reactivity in mice. J Neurosci. 2008;28:8709–8723. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2077-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Rodefer JS, Nguyen TN, Karlsson JJ, Arnt J. Reversal of subchronic PCP-induced deficits in attentional set shifting in rats by sertindole and a 5-HT(6) receptor antagonist: comparison among antipsychotics. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:2657–2666. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Schorge S, Colquhoun D. Studies of NMDA receptor function and stoichiometry with truncated and tandem subunits. J Neurosci. 2003;23:1151–1158. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-04-01151.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Skolnick P, Popik P, Trullas R. Glutamate-based antidepressants: 20 years on. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 2009;30:563–569. doi: 10.1016/j.tips.2009.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Tang YP, Shimizu E, Dube GR, Rampon C, Kerchner GA, Zhuo M, Liu GS, Tsien JZ. Genetic enhancement of learning and memory in mice. Nature. 1999;401:63–69. doi: 10.1038/43432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Vollenweider FX, Bachle D, Umbricht D, Geyer M, Hell D. Sertindole reduces S-ketamine induced attentional deficits in healthy volunteers. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;45:100S. [Google Scholar]
  47. Wang D, Cui Z, Zeng Q, Kuang H, Wang LP, Tsien JZ, Cao X. Genetic enhancement of memory and long-term potentiation but not CA1 long-term depression in NR2B transgenic rats. PLoS ONE. 2009;4:e7486. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0007486. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Wenzel A, Scheurer L, Kunzi R, Fritschy JM, Mohler H, Benke D. Distribution of NMDA receptor subunit proteins NR2A, 2B, 2C and 2D in rat brain. NeuroReport. 1995;7:45–48. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Zarate CA, Jr, Singh JB, Carlson PJ, Brutsche NE, Ameli R, Luckenbaugh DA, Charney DS, Manji HK. A randomized trial of an N-methyl-d-aspartate antagonist in treatment-resistant major depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63:856–864. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.8.856. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Zhuo JM, Prescott SL, Murray ME, Zhang HY, Baxter MG, Nicolle MM. Early discrimination reversal learning impairment and preserved spatial learning in a longitudinal study of Tg2576 APPsw mice. Neurobiol Aging. 2007;28:1248–1257. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.05.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Psychopharmacology are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES