Summary
Synchronized firing of mitral cells (MCs) in the olfactory bulb (OB) has been hypothesized to help bind information together in olfactory cortex (OC). In this first survey of synchronized firing by suspected MCs in awake-behaving vertebrates we find the surprising result that synchronized firing conveys information on odor value (is it rewarded?) rather than odor identity (what is the odor?). We observed that as mice learned to discriminate between odors synchronous firing responses to the rewarded and unrewarded odors became divergent. Further, adrenergic blockage decreases the magnitude of odor divergence of synchronous trains suggesting that MCs contribute to decision-making through adrenergic-modulated synchronized firing. Thus, in the olfactory system information on stimulus reward is found in MCs one synapse away from the sensory neuron.
Introduction
In invertebrates associative learning resulting in adequate responses to stimuli is mediated partially by plasticity in the synapse that the sensory neuron makes with a second order neuron (Bailey and Kandel, 2008; Roberts and Glanzman, 2003). However, in vertebrates synaptic changes that encode for the value associated with a stimulus take place several synapses downstream from the sensory neuron (Gold and Shadlen, 2007; Komura et al., 2001; Pantoja et al., 2007). Potential exceptions are in V1 cortex in the visual system (Shuler and Bear, 2006), the brainstem in the gustatory system (Chang and Scott, 1984) and in the olfactory system where learning-induced changes occur within the OB one or two synapses away from the sensory neuron (Friedrich et al., 2004; Gao and Strowbridge, 2009; Gray et al., 1986; Kay and Laurent, 1999; Nissant et al., 2009; Wilson and Leon, 1988). However, it is not clear whether learning-related plasticity in these early circuits represents a modulation in the circuitry to enhance discrimination or whether it plays a more dynamic role and actively contributes to the encoding of stimulus value (Kay and Laurent, 1999). Please note that when we refer to odor value we do not exclude that the circuit may carry information on a related reward signal (Wallis and Kennerley, 2010).
Olfactory sensory neurons transform information about the chemical structure of an odor into neuronal activity and transmit information synaptically to second order cells including the MCs (Shepherd et al., 2004; Tan et al., 2010). Interneuron circuits within the OB modulate MC firing and likely provide contrast enhancement (Aungst et al., 2003; Mori et al., 1999; Shepherd et al., 2004) and learning modifies activity of MCs through plasticity that is likely caused by feedback from neuromodulatory systems and centrifugal input from the OC back into the OB (Doucette and Restrepo, 2008; Gao and Strowbridge, 2009; Mandairon and Linster, 2009; Restrepo et al., 2009; Wilson and Mainen, 2006). Interestingly, studies of odor-induced oscillatory field potentials in olfactory discrimination tasks suggest the involvement of changes in synchronous firing between neurons in the OB circuit in learning in vertebrates (Gray et al., 1986; Kay and Beshel, 2010; Martin et al., 2006). In addition MCs are hypothesized to aid in synthesis of simultaneously detected odor features through synchronized firing and convergence on neurons in OC (Kashiwadani et al., 1999; Mori et al., 1999) supported by experiments in invertebrates (Stopfer et al., 1997). Studies in vertebrates are consistent with the claim that synchronous firing of MCs increases the probability of driving target OC neurons (Franks and Isaacson, 2006; Luna and Schoppa, 2008). However, direct evidence for synchronized firing of MCs in vertebrates is limited to a measurement of synchrony in anesthetized animals (Kashiwadani et al., 1999) that was not replicated (Egana et al., 2005). Thus, the precise role for involvement of synchronized MC firing in transfer of olfactory information and/or in learning of olfactory stimulus/reward association is not well understood.
Here we measure synchronized spiking in suspected MCs (SMCs, see Methods) in awake-behaving mice engaged in a go-no go behavioral task where they learn to recognize a new odor as rewarded. We ask the question whether synchronized firing conveys information on odor identity (what is the odor?), or alternatively, whether value (is it rewarded?) is encoded by synchronized firing. In addition, noradrenergic (NA) modulation is known to play a role in new olfactory stimulus/reward association (Bouret and Sara, 2004; Doucette et al., 2007), and we ask whether NA antagonist application in the OB affects synchronized spike odor responses of SMCs to rewarded and unrewarded odors in the go-no go behavioral task. We find that responses of synchronized SMC spikes to odors convey information on odor value (or a related reward signal), and that the differential synchronized spike response to rewarded and unrewarded odor is not as robust in the presence of inhibitors of noradrenergic modulation of the OB. Thus, the olfactory system stands out from other sensory systems in that information on stimulus value is found in the MC that is one synapse away from the sensory neuron, in the same place in the circuit as a bipolar cell in the visual system or a spiral ganglion cell in the auditory system.
Results
Go-No Go Task
Mice were implanted with two eight-microelectrode arrays targeted to the MC layer (Figure 1A). During each trial in the go-no go task, thirsty mice were asked to respond to a rewarded odor by licking on a tube and received a water reward if they licked at least once in the last four 0.5 sec periods of the trial (the response area, see Figure 1Bi, no reward for the unrewarded odor). The sniffing behavior of animals during this task is illustrated in Figure 1Bii. Consistent with previous reports (Wesson et al., 2008), animals showed an increase in sniffing frequency in anticipation of odor presentation. Sniffing frequency started differing between successful rewarded and unrewarded odor trials at ∼1.7 sec in the middle of the decision making period when mice steadily reduced their breathing rate to a final frequency of 2-3 Hz after the water reward. Figure 1C shows an example of how a mouse learns to respond in a session where the animal is presented with a new pair of odors. Mice stop responding to the unrewarded odor because the licking entails considerable effort not rewarded with water. Mice learned to respond reliably (better than 80% correct) within 3-6 blocks of 20 trials (10 rewarded and 10 unrewarded) (Slotnick and Restrepo, 2005).
Precise Synchronization Between Spikes from Different SMCs
We recorded from 345 single- and 820 multi-units in the MC layer of 8 animals in 67 separate sessions (39 first day and 28 reversals). In recordings from mice performing odor discrimination, we find precise synchronization between a subset of spikes (Figure 2). Figure 2A shows precise spiking for three SMCs and Figures 2 B1 and B2 show the histograms of inter-spike lags. As in Doucette and Restrepo (2008), spikes were identified as voltage deviations larger than three times the standard deviation of the noise in extracellular voltage recording. Single-unit spikes were separated from all other spikes using unsupervised spike sorting (see Methods). Multi-units were all spikes left after identification of single-unit spikes and spike time was defined as the time of the peak of the voltage deviation. As shown in the histograms in Figures 2 B1-3 synchronization is precise, with spikes from different units firing within <250 μsec (in the Supplemental Text we rule out artifactual spike pairing). Precise synchronous firing was also found when a single-unit was compared to a multi-unit (Figure 2 B3) and when multi-units were compared to each other (not shown). Hereafter we define synchronized spikes as spikes that happen within less than 250 μsec.
The average fraction of synchronized spikes was significantly different from the fraction of synchronized spikes arising by chance (compare red line to histograms in Figures 2B1-3 and see Methods) and ranged from 0.9% for single-unit pairs (SU×SU, n=138) to 6.0% for multi-unit (MU×MU, n=2,578) pairs (see Table 1). As shown in Figure 2A, synchronized spikes were sparse in single-unit pairs. Sparseness in these SU×SU synchronized trains made it difficult to calculate statistics for changes in firing rate elicited by odors. Therefore when evaluating odor-induced changes we used synchronized trains estimated from multi-unit pairs. Importantly, in the supplemental text and Figure S1 we show that the percent of synchronized spikes in MU×MU pairs is consistent with makeup of the multi-unit spikes by single-units, and in Figure S2 we show that the waveforms of the synchronized multi-unit spikes do not differ from the rest of the spikes in the multi-unit. Finally, an autocorrelogram of the synchronized spike trains in the RA shows a weak oscillatory pattern (at ∼5 Hz, Figure 2B4) consistent with changes in simultaneous synchronized firing associated with breathing.
Table 1. Percent of Spikes Synchronized in a Unit Pair.
Unit Pairs Reference unit × Partner unit |
Percent of spikes synchronized mean±SD(n) |
---|---|
SU×SU | 0.9±1.1(138) |
MU×SU | 0.7±1.1(566) |
SU×MU | 3.7±6.7(566) |
MU×MU | 6.0±6.1(2,578) |
Unit pairs are classified depending on whether the units were single (SU) or multi-units (MU). The table shows the average percent of spikes within the reference unit that are within 250 μsec of one of the spikes in the partner unit (synchronized spikes). The percent synchronized spikes (%Synch) in the response area was calculated from the number of spikes in the reference unit (nref), the number of synchronized spikes (nsynch) and the number of synchronized spikes in the reference unit after ISI shuffling (nShsynch):
%Synch = 100(nsynch - nShsynch)/nref
This value is calculated for those unit pairs that are significantly synchronized in 39 sessions.
Synchronized Spike Trains Develop a Divergent Odor Response
Figure 3Ai shows the development of differential responsiveness to new odors by synchronized spike trains through a go-no go session. As shown in an earlier study for spikes from individual units (Doucette and Restrepo, 2008), in the first twenty-trial block the synchronized spike trains do not respond differentially to the two odors (Figure 3Ai, B) and the mouse does not respond differentially to the odors (Figure 3C). In contrast, after 60-100 trials (3 to 5 blocks) the animal develops a differential behavioral response and the synchronized spike trains respond with excitation to the rewarded odor and inhibition to the unrewarded odor. Responses were classified as divergent using a t-test corrected for multiple comparisons through false discovery rate (FDR) with a significant p-value in at least two blocks in a session (see Methods). The most divergent block (best block) varied from blocks 2 to 9 for synchronous firing in different multi-unit pairs (mean is block 6 with SD of 2.4, n=48 pairs). These data show that divergent responses of synchronized spike trains develop during learning.
Importantly, in the first two blocks where the animal is performing at chance (Figure 3C) when the animal licks correctly to the rewarded odor (a trial denoted as a “hit”) there is little change in synchronized firing over time (Figure 3Aii). In contrast, in later blocks (i.e. blocks 7 and 8), where the animal responds correctly in over 80% of the trials, there is a robust excitatory response to the odor in the hit trials (Figure 3Aii). Although the animal is performing the same action in hit trials for blocks 1&2 vs. blocks 7&8 the odor only induces synchronized train reposes in the later blocks. Lack of responses in hit trials in blocks 1 & 2 indicates that the odor-induced increases in synchronized firing rate are not a result of common source noise caused by stereotyped movement during licking in the hit trials (see also Supplemental Text).
Responses of Synchronized Trains are either Excitatory or Inhibitory depending on whether the Odor is Rewarded
Do synchronous spikes carry information unavailable in spike trains from individual units considered in isolation? Figure 4Aii shows the average z-score defined as the average odor-induced change in firing rate in a block of twenty trials divided by the standard deviation before odor application. A z-score greater than zero indicates an increase in firing rate, while a z-score less than zero indicates a decrease in firing rate. The z-scores were derived from the block of trials that showed the largest odor-induced divergence in synchronous firing (solid lines) or in spike firing rates of each unit considered in isolation (broken lines). The average z-score curves for the units (all spikes, not just synchronous spikes, broken lines) show that when all spikes are counted without regard to synchrony, rewarded odor responses (red) could be either increases or decreases in firing rate and that unrewarded odor responses (blue) had some increases but were mostly decreases. In contrast, when only synchronous spikes were considered (solid lines), the odor responses were much more informative, as they were ‘divergent’ in that the rewarded odor (red) always yielded an increase in synchronous firing and the unrewarded odor (blue) always elicited a decrease in synchronous firing (Figure 4Aii, solid lines).
Synchronized Spikes Carry Information on Odor Reward
Do the synchronized spikes carry information on odor identity, or odor reward? We addressed this question by reversing the value of the odor. Comparing z-score cumulative histograms for the first session where odor A was rewarded and AB unrewarded (Figure Aii) with those in the reversal where AB was rewarded while A was unrewarded (Figure 4Bii) shows a remarkable effect of value reversal. Regardless of the identity of the odor, synchronized spike trains (solid lines in Figures 4Aii and Bii) displayed an increase in firing to the rewarded odor and a decrease to the unrewarded odor. Thus the information conveyed by synchronous spikes is related to the odor's value rather than the odor's identity.
Synchronized Responses to the Two Odors Diverge before the Animal Makes a Decision
At this point we asked whether the information on the value of the odor conveyed by the synchronized firing trains diverged between the two odors at a time in the trial before the animal made a decision. We performed principal component (PC) analysis of divergent synchronized pair responses to the odors. Figure S3A shows for the first and best blocks the time course for the responses to odors in two dimensional PC space and Figure S3B shows the time course of the Euclidean distance in PC space between the points for the rewarded and unrewarded odors. There is clear divergence of the responses to the odors in the best block, but not in the first block. Figure 4C shows the p-value for a ranksum test of divergence of the Euclidean distance between rewarded and unrewarded odors. Divergence of synchronized unit firing becomes significant at ∼1 sec (0.7 sec after addition of the odor), which is ∼0.25 sec earlier than the time when the animals make a decision to stop licking to the unrewarded odor (1.25 sec, estimated with a ranksum test on licks). A fraction of a second afterwards at ∼1.7 sec the mice change their sniff frequency (Figure 1Bii). Thus, the divergence between rewarded and unrewarded odors for synchronized trains carries information that the animal can use for odor discrimination.
Synchronized Train Odor Responses in Trials where the Animal made a Mistake
We next asked whether analysis of trials where the animals made mistakes shows that synchrony reflected responses to odor, and not responses that mirrored the behavioral action. In other words, when the animal makes a mistake and licks on the water tube to obtain a reward when exposed to the unrewarded odor (false alarm) are the synchronized spike trains more like the synchronized firing that takes place when the animal correctly licks for a water reward to a rewarded odor (hit) or more like the synchronized responses when the animal correctly does not lick for the unrewarded odor (correct rejection)? As shown by the z-score cumulative histograms in Figure 5 the synchronized spiking decreased (Δz<0) in response to the unrewarded odor, regardless of whether the animal licked during this odor (false alarm, green) or not (correct rejection, black). Similarly, for the majority of the trials synchronized firing increased (Δz>0) in response to the rewarded odor whether the animal licked during this odor (hit, blue) or refrained from licking (miss, red). Thus, the odor-induced changes in synchronized firing are responses to the odor as opposed to responses that follow the animal's behavior or licking. In addition, because the responses follow the odor presented rather than the movement the animal made, the data in this figure indicate that the synchronized spike trains are not brought about by noise caused by the animal's movements.
The Circuit Mediating Divergent Synchronized Spike Responses is Limited in Spatial Extent
The percent of unit pairs whose synchronized spike trains respond differentially to the odors decreased as a function of distance between electrodes (Figure 6A, blue). Importantly, this decrease in the percent of unit pairs exhibiting divergent responses for synchronized spikes as a function of distance is in sharp contrast with the absence of a decrease in the percent of synchronized spike trains responsive to odors as a function of distance (Figure 6A, red) and the absence of a change in the percent of synchronized spikes for each unit pair as a function of distance (Figure 6B). These data show that divergent responsiveness for synchronized firing is effective within a limited distance (<1.5 mm). In addition, the difference in dependence on electrode-to-electrode distance between divergence and responsiveness of synchronized firing of unit pairs indicates that synchronized firing is not due to synchronous electrode noise (see also Supplemental Text).
Noradrenergic Modulation Participates in Conveying Information on Odor Reward through Synchronized Spikes
The data presented above show that synchronized spike SMC output is modified in a manner dependent on behavioral context (is the new odor rewarded?). This context-dependent modification is likely mediated by centrifugal innervation into the OB from olfactory cortical networks and/or neuromodulatory centers (Mandairon and Linster, 2009; Restrepo et al., 2009). Interestingly, blockade of adrenergic receptors in the OB prevents mice from discriminating closely related novel odors in the go-no go task (Doucette et al., 2007) and adrenergic activation results in enhanced synchronized oscillations of the local field potential in the bulb (Gire and Schoppa, 2008). These studies motivated us to ask whether blocking of the adrenergic receptors in the OB affects differential synchronized spike odor responsiveness to rewarded and unrewarded odors. For adrenergic drug delivery animals received bilateral restricted injection into the OB of a solution with α and β adrenergic blockers under isoflourane anesthesia (Doucette et al., 2007) 10 minutes prior to the go-no go task. Application of the drugs resulted in delay of discrimination between odors in the go-no go task (Figure S4C).
Application of α and β adrenergic blockers diminished the magnitude of divergent synchronized spike train responses to odors in the go-no go task. Figure 7A shows the average z-scores for responses to odors (red: rewarded, blue: unrewarded). While the unit average z-score cumulative histograms are similar in the presence/absence of adrenergic block (compare broken lines in Figures 7A and 4Aii) the responses of synchronized spike trains appeared different compared to controls. Specifically, rewarded odors elicited some inhibitory responses in the presence of adrenergic blockers but not in controls (compare where solid red lines cross zero –vertical black line- in Figures 7A and 4Aii).
To quantify the magnitude of the difference in average z-scores between rewarded and unrewarded odor trials we calculated the d', the difference in z-score between the response to the rewarded and unrewarded odors (see inset in Figure 7C). Figure 7C shows that d' for units did not differ between control (red) and adrenergic antagonist (green). In sharp contrast, adrenergic blockade elicited a clear left shift in d' for synchronized spike trains as would be expected for loss of magnitude of the divergence in z-scores (leftward shift in green –adrenergic- line compared to red –control- line in Figure 7D). Interestingly, and consistent with the left shift in d', for odor-divergent pairs there was a sharp reduction in the odor-induced change in percent of synchronized spikes between adrenergic block and control (Figure 7B, also see Figure S4). Thus the odor-induced changes in synchronized firing in the presence of adrenergic block are entirely due to changes in firing rate of the reference units, not changes in the percent of synchronized spikes.
Discussion
Our findings indicate that firing of synchronized spikes between groups of SMCs, the second order neurons in the olfactory circuit, carries information on odor value, or on other reward signals such as attention and vigilance (Wallis and Kennerley, 2010). An observer can make a decision on odor value based on whether the number of synchronized spikes fired by SMCs increases or decreases in response to an odor. Thus, placing a vertical line at Δz=0 in Figure 4Aii allows successful discrimination between rewarded (Δz>0) and unrewarded odor (Δz<0) based on synchronized firing responses to odors (solid lines). In contrast, there is no vertical line that ensures successful determination of odor value based on the odor responses of the units that make up the synchronized firing pair (Figure 4Aii, broken lines). Interestingly, odors, like tastants, vary in whether they are naturally perceived as attractive or repulsive. Based upon this observation, we would predict that naturally repulsive odors would yield decreases in synchronized firing while attractive odors would yield increases, with reversals as the animal is trained otherwise. The observed learning-induced plasticity in the OB that provides information on odor value could contribute to downstream plasticity, decision-making, or in the estimation of expected outcomes used in prediction error calculations.
The precise timing for synchronization of spikes in different SMCs (spikes that lag by < 250 μsec, Figure 2) raises the question whether this is due to common source noise from a biological action (e.g. grinding of teeth or licking). An advantage of using the go-no go task is that behavior is stereotyped for hit trials where the animal must lick during the response area. We asked whether biological actions during this stereotyped behavior in hit trials could have yielded the increase in synchronized firing observed during responses to the rewarded odor. After all, if all hit trials display high levels of synchrony (due, perhaps, to movements the animals make during licking), an increase in synchronized firing to the rewarded odor could appear as the session progresses simply because more hit trials are associated with the rewarded odor. In order to account for this possible confound, we compared the odor responsiveness of hit trial synchronized spiking during the first set of trials in the session (while the animal was responding randomly to the rewarded odor with many hits and misses) to hit trials later in the session when the animal was responding to the rewarded odor almost exclusively with hits (Figure 3Aii). There was no odor-induced increase in synchronized spike firing in the hit trials at the beginning of the session. This demonstrates that the observed increase in synchronized firing was not due to biological common noise occurring consistently during hit trials. In addition, common noise artifacts tend to affect voltage recorded by multiple electrodes. The fact that synchronized spikes occur in different unit pairs exclusively (Figures 2A and S1, and supplemental text) is evidence that these are not due to common noise. Further, since divergence in synchronized firing is clearly dependent upon the distance between electrodes (Figure 6B, blue points), it is not plausible that biological common source noise is the source of this synchronization, as biological common noise occurring across units should not depend on distance between electrodes. Finally, if the synchronized spikes were common noise their shape would be expected to differ from that of the unsynchronized spikes, and this is not the case (Figure S2). These observations and other findings (see results and supplemental text) show that the precisely synchronized spikes are not due to common noise.
The precise timing for synchronization of spikes in different SMCs (spikes that lag by < 250 μsec) is not consistent with the temporal dynamics of MC synchrony previously recorded in OB slices and anesthetized animals that show correlogram peak width of ∼10 ms (Galan et al., 2006; Kashiwadani et al., 1999; Schoppa, 2006). Current OB network theory postulates that synchrony between MCs could occur as the result of interaction with the large inhibitory granule cell network (Mori et al., 1999). Consistent with theory, OB slice and anesthetized animal work has shown that granule cells can induce synchrony with ∼10 millisecond temporal dynamics within distances as far as 500 μm (Galan et al., 2006; Kashiwadani et al., 1999; Schoppa, 2006). However, Figure 6 illustrates that the sub-millisecond synchrony observed in awake and behaving animals does not decay with distance even between SMCs recorded up to 1.5 mm apart.
Our observations raise the question whether the synchrony measured between SMCs in awake-behaving animals is the exclusive result of the bulb's inhibitory interneuron network. In fact to our knowledge, the only examples of sub-millisecond synchrony that have been observed in other systems were when excitatory output from a single neuron diverges onto multiple target neurons (Alonso et al., 1996) or when the cells are coupled electrotonically (Takahashi and Sakurai, 2009; Wang et al., 2010). Because most synchronized SMCs are located many microns apart, it is unlikely that precise synchronization is caused by somatic gap junctions. MC lateral dendrite gap junctions could play a role, but if this were the case ultrafast spike synchrony should be observed in the OB slices because in these slices dendrodendritic circuits are intact.
We favor the view that our data showing precise synchronization is most likely due to coincident excitatory input to MCs through centrifugal input from anterior olfactory nucleus (AON) or OC (Matsutani, 2010; Restrepo et al., 2009). Cells responsible for centrifugal input from OC or AON would not be included in regular OB slices and are likely to be affected by anesthetics (urethane is thought to affect NMDA receptors (Dalo and Larson, 1990)) explaining why ultrafast synchronization is not found in these preparations. Interestingly, if excitatory centrifugal input is involved then these fibers would have to make excitatory synapses on MCs. Such synapses have not been demonstrated, but Cajal suggested they occur (Ramón y Cajal, 1904), and recent studies by Matsutani (2010) provide support for synaptic boutons from centrifugal fibers in the MC layer; this issue needs future studies. Importantly, Figure 6 shows that while SMC synchronization does not decrease as a function of distance, the differential response of synchronized spike trains to the rewarded and unrewarded odors is steeply dependent on distance disappearing for > 1.5 mm (Figure 6A, blue circles). The two circuits of limited spatial extent that could be involved in regulating divergent odorant responses in synchronized firing by MCs would be either the extensive MC lateral dendrite/granule cell circuit (Shepherd, 2004), or interactions through short axon cells extending long axons that reach subsets of glomeruli (Kiyokage et al., 2010).
NA modulation is involved in association of stimulus and reward in what has been called a “network reset” that takes place when the occurrence of task-relevant stimuli cannot be predicted and the animal must learn a new association (Bouret and Sara, 2005). Indeed, neurons in the locus coreuleus that release NA in the OB are known to respond in rewarded trials during the go-no go task (Bouret and Sara, 2004, 2005). In addition, NA modulation of the OB circuit is known to be necessary to ensure odor discrimination for closely related odors in the go-no go task (Doucette et al., 2007). Our data suggest that part of this learning in the odor discrimination task involves developing large differential responses of synchronized firing trains from presumed MCs to the rewarded and unrewarded odors (Figure 7). The cellular mechanisms underlying this development of synchrony are not currently understood, but could involve an alteration of transmitter release (Pandipati et al., 2010).
If the SMCs we record from carry information on odor value as opposed to odor identity, a question that arises is how odor identity information is passed to higher order centers. Tufted cells, a cell type that we did not target in the current study, are more abundant than MCs (Shepherd et al., 2004), and could carry information on odor identity. Middle tufted cells respond to odors and local processing of the odorant signal in the middle tufted cells differs from that in MCs (Griff et al., 2008; Nagayama et al., 2004). In addition, external tufted cells whose cell bodies lie adjacent to glomeruli could transmit information on odor identity (Wachowiak and Shipley, 2006), although whether these cells can carry information to higher order centers has not been fully explored (Schoenfeld and Macrides, 1984; Schoenfeld et al., 1985). It is also possible that different subsets of MCs engage different networks in the piriform cortex. Indeed, in a previous publication we showed that a small percent (∼2%) of the odor divergent MCs did not change the z-score throughout a discrimination session or when odors changed between the rewarded and unrewarded state (Doucette and Restrepo, 2008). Thus, it is possible that a subset of MCs does carry information on odor identity, and the odor responsiveness of MCs within this subset may be minimally affected by behavioral context. Finally, our findings do not exclude the same MCs that carry information on odor value from also carrying information on odor identity through another coding mechanism in either a simultaneous or sequential fashion as found in taste cortical neurons (Miller and Katz, 2010). Indeed, regarding sequential transfer of information it is known that SMCs respond differentially to odors within the first sniff after odor exposure (Cury and Uchida, 2010). These issues deserve future studies.
In summary, we find that SMCs separated by large distances (up to 1.5 mm) and therefore innervating different glomeruli fire synchronously and that synchronized firing conveys information on odor value, not odor identity. This is particularly relevant because the output from MCs innervating different glomeruli converges on OC pyramidal cells (Apicella et al., 2010), and synchronized firing of MCs is effective at eliciting excitation of OC pyramidal cells (Franks and Isaacson, 2006; Luna and Schoppa, 2008). Thus, our findings suggest that the circuit encompassing the MCs and the OC pyramidal cells is involved in evaluating information on odor value.
Experimental Procedures
Microarray implantation
Eight 8-10 week old animals were implanted bilaterally with 2×4 electrode arrays (Figure 1A). Animals were anesthetized with an intraperitoneal ketamine-xylazine injection (100 μg/g–20 μg/g). The electrode arrays were manufactured by Micro Probes Inc. of platinum iridium wire etched to a 2-μm tip and coated with parylene C (3-4 MΩ at 1 kHz). The arrays were organized in a 2×4 pattern with 200-μm spacing with lengths of 4.2 to 4.8-mm angled at 45 ° along the long axis to ensure targeting of the MC layer (Figure 1A).
In this study, as reported previously by Kay and Laurent (1999) and Rinberg and co-workers (2006), no spikes were detected while the electrodes traversed the granule cell layer. Once the electrode reached the ventral MC layer spikes with amplitudes ranging from 100 to 2000 μV were detected with spontaneous firing frequency characteristic of MCs (Figure S1A, Figure S5, MCL). As shown in Figure S5 recording from the granule cell layer yielded significantly smaller voltage deviations. Recordings from electrodes displaying only such small voltage deviations were infrequent and were not analyzed to avoid contamination by granule-cell generated multi-unit activity. Since granule cell signals were too small to be detected when thresholding based upon recordings in the mitral cell layer, these cells almost certainly do not contribute to the multi-unit activity detected in the mitral cell layer. Once the MC layer was reached, the arrays were fixed in place with titanium skull screws and nail acrylic with one of the titanium screws serving as the ground. While the electrodes do not record spikes from the granule cells we term the recorded units “Suspected MCs” (SMCs) because our measurements may include some internal tufted cells. All animal procedures were performed under a protocol approved by the institutional animal care and use committee.
Surgery for implantation of sniff cannulae
Surgical procedures for cannula implantation were based upon the work of Wesson et al., (2008). Briefly, animals were anesthetized as described above, and lidocaine was injected into the epidermis above the frontal nasal bone as a local anesthetic. An incision was made down the midline and the skull cleaned with 3% H202. Next, a hole was drilled 1mm anterior to the frontal/nasal fissure and 1mm lateral from the midline. A hollow cannula was then lowered into the hole and fixed in place with nail acrylic.
Imaging of the OBs using MRI
Mice were anesthetized with nembutal (100 mg/kg) and perfused with 4% paraformaldehyde. Fixed heads were placed in PBS containing 5% Prohance (Bracco Diagnostics Inc, Princeton, NJ), 1% dH2O for 2 weeks prior to imaging. Imaging experiments were conducted on a Bruker Biospec 7-T horizontal-bore system (Bruker Inc, Billerica, MA) controlled with Paravision 4.0 software. The brain specimens were placed inside a sealed container filled with Fomblin liquid (Solvay Slexis, West Deptford, NJ) to minimize artifacts arising from air-tissue interface. A standard 3D Fast Spin Echo sequence was used to acquire the 256 images for each head (repetition time 500 ms, echo time 8.6 ms, echo train length 4, number of averages 4, scan time 11h 22min). The imaging resolution was 78 microns isotropic. Volumes were constructed using ImageJ 1.42q software and final images were contrast enhanced using Photoshop 6.0.
Initial training in the go-no go task
The mice were trained using water reinforcement and underwent testing in go-no go trials (Figure 1Bi and 1C) (Doucette et al., 2007; Doucette and Restrepo, 2008; Slotnick and Restrepo, 2005). All mice were first trained to distinguish 1% isoamyl acetate versus 1% cumin aldehyde (v/v in mineral oil). The animal's performance was evaluated in blocks of 20 trials (ten rewarded and ten unrewarded presented at random). Each block's percent correct value represents the percent of trials in which the odors were correctly discriminated and associated with the appropriate behavioral action. Each session included 6-10 blocks of 20 trials. Once the animal learned to discriminate between isoamyl acetate and cumin aldehyde they were ready for the novel odor discrimination task described below.
Screening for responsive novel odor pairs
As described in the supplemental text we screened novel odors that presumably would stimulate glomeruli in the ventral surface of the OB (the electrodes were targeted to this area of the bulb). Choice of odors is described in the supplemental text. In order to screen these odors in a behaviorally neutral setting, an 8×8×13-cm chamber was constructed where the mouse was exposed passively to odors. Odors were introduced on a constant background odor stream for 2 sec with an intertrial interval of 60 sec. Odors were screened in groups of 12 or 15 per session. After a session the data were analyzed overnight and the best two odors (odors A and B) were used in the subsequent odor discrimination task. The odors shown in italics in Table S1 were found to elicit responses more often than the others.
Once we identified responsive novel odors A and B we proceeded the next day with a novel odor pair discrimination task. As in previous studies, in order to make the odor discrimination task difficult, we asked mice to discriminate between odor mixtures (Doucette et al., 2007; Doucette and Restrepo, 2008). Odor mixtures have been employed in several studies of the speed of olfactory processing (Abraham et al., 2004; Uchida and Mainen, 2003) and odor similarity determinations (Doucette et al., 2007; Kay et al., 2006). In our behavioral paradigm the animals learned to discriminate between odor A and a 1:1 mixture of odor A:odor B at an overall concentration of 1% by volume in mineral oil. Measurements using a photoionization detector indicated that odors arrived at the chamber at ∼0.3 sec after routing of the odor into the port (mini-PID; Aurora Scientific Inc., Aurora, ON, Canada).
Delivery of Adrenergic Receptor Antagonists into the OB
Six animals were implanted bilaterally with multielectrode arrays containing a central cannula for adrenergic drug delivery. Multielectrode arrays with cannulae were constructed in a similar 2×4 pattern as described above with the addition of a 23 gauge stainless steel tube in the center of the array terminating 2 mm above the electrode tips so that it would sit above the bulb while the electrodes were implanted within the bulb as described above.
For adrenergic drug delivery we used the same procedure as in a previous publication (Doucette et al., 2007). Briefly, immediately prior to behavioral testing animals received bilateral injection of a test solution under isoflourane anesthesia. The infusions consisted of bilateral 2 μl injections of the desired drug(s) dissolved in HEPES-buffered saline over 10 minutes. We took advantage of the fact that there are well characterized subtype-specific adrenergic antagonists with known specificity for the different receptor subtypes (Pupo and Minneman, 2001) and that addition of a mixture of β and α adrenergic inhibitors affects discrimination of closely related odors in our go-no go task (Doucette et al., 2007). As in our previous study we used a mixture of alprenolol (general β blocker, 28 nmols), and phentolamine (general α blocker, 28 nmols). Five minutes following drug delivery the injection needle was replaced with the cannula-sealing stylet. Animals then required 5-10 minutes to recover fully from isoflourane anesthesia. In our previous study we showed that this procedure resulted in drug infusion that was limited to the OB (Doucette et al., 2007).
Monitoring sniffing
We monitored sniffing by recording intranasal pressure via implanted nasal cannulae connected to a pressure sensor (Model No 24PCEFA6G(EA), 0-0.5 psi, Honeywell, Canada) via polyethylene tubing. The sensor was mounted on a commutator (TDT: Tucker Davis Technologies, Alachua, FL) to allow for the animal's free rotation during the task. Pressure transients were digitized and sampled at 24 kHz. Sniff data was analyzed for instantaneous frequency as in Wesson, et al., (2008).
Recording setup
The output of the two electrode arrays was directed to a 16-channel TDT 1× gain headstage connected to a TDT motorized commutator that was connected to a CWE 16-channel amplifier and bandpass filter (CWE, Ardmore, PA). The signal from 14 electrodes was amplified 2,000 times and filtered at 300–3,000 Hz before outputting to a Data Translation Inc (Marlboro, MA) DT3010 A/D card in a PC. Data were acquired at 24 kHz with custom software written in MATLAB (Mathworks, Inc. Natick, MA). Digitized behavioral events from the Slotnick olfactometer (licks, nose pokes, and odor on) were also acquired in real time.
Offline spike clustering
Offline spike clustering was performed as in a previous publication (Doucette and Restrepo, 2008). Briefly, custom software written in MATLAB was used to threshold each channel at 3× RMS of the baseline noise. Every thresholded spike (24 points at 24 kHz) was saved from each channel and imported into a second program where we clustered the waveforms of similar shape by performing wavelet decomposition and superparamagnetic clustering using the method and MATLAB software developed by Quiroga and coworkers (2004). In addition to determining 18 wavelet coefficients used in the Quiroga program, our modified program also determined the first three coefficients of a principal component analysis of the spikes and calculated the peak to valley ratio. As explained in Quiroga et al (2004), the program then proceeded to determine which of these descriptors showed a multimodal distribution and used the ten best descriptors to separate the spikes into well defined clusters using superparamagnetic clustering. We defined a single-unit using the criterion of finding <3% of the spikes in the refractory period of 2 ms in the interspike interval (ISI) histogram. On average, we obtained 12 multi-units and five single-units per experiment. We examined the stability of the classification method over time to ensure that single-units were not misclassified. Spikes that occurred in every channel at 3-8 Hz when the animal was licking (likely an electrical event elicited by licking) were easily identified and excluded from the analysis.
Analysis of Synchronized Spikes
In a preliminary survey of correlograms such as those shown in Figure 2, we found a large number of pairs of single and multi-units that exhibited peaks different from correlograms calculated after the original spike trains had been shuffled by a random time ranging ± one mean interspike interval (ISI) (ISI shuffle, red lines in Figure 2B1-3). In order to tally the number of unit pairs that exhibited significant synchronized firing we wrote a MATLAB program that tested for all trials in a session in the response area (0.5 to 2.5 sec) whether the number of synchronized spikes, defined as spikes in the two units that were within 250 μsec of each other, was significantly different in a t-test from the number of synchronized spikes after ISI shuffling. The choice of the 250 μsec window was not arbitrary. We performed a thorough survey of the data by surveying cross correlograms such as those shown in Figure 2B 1-3, and we found a robust cross correlation different from that of the shuffled spike trains that fell within the 250 μsec lag window. The p-value for the t-test was corrected for multiple comparisons within each session using a FDR method (Curran-Everett, 2000). For those unit pairs that exhibited significant synchronization, a synchronized spike train was generated that included all spikes in the first (reference) unit that were within 250 μsec of the second (partner) unit.
Data analysis
Analysis was performed using custom written MATLAB programs tested using simulated data (see Figure S6). A t-test was used to classify unit firing rates or synchronized spike train firing rates as odor “divergent” when the responses to the rewarded and unrewarded odors were statistically different. Within each block of 20 trials differences between firing rates in response to the different odors (ten rewarded and ten unrewarded odor trials) in the odor response area (0.5 to 2.5 sec) were assessed using the t-test. Within each experiment, the calculated p-values were corrected for multiple comparisons using the FDR method (Curran-Everett, 2000). In our previous publication (Doucette and Restrepo, 2008) we had found that occasionally a single block was found to be significantly different between rewarded and unrewarded trials in the reference interval. Accordingly, we adopted the conservative measure of classifying a unit as divergent only when the p-value for the t-test of significant differences was below the FDR-corrected p-value in two or more blocks. As a control, differences in firing rate in the same block were compared between rewarded and unrewarded trials using the same procedure in the interval from -1.5 to 0.5 sec in the absence of odor (the pre-stimulus interval) to assess the effectiveness of the correction for multiple comparisons. Odors did not elicit divergent responses in this control time range (data not shown).
A t-test was also used to classify units as “responsive.” The rate of firing in the response area (0.5 to 2.5 sec) was compared to the firing rate during the reference interval (-1.5 to 0.5 sec). The FDR was used to correct for multiple comparisons, and a unit was classified as responsive only if p-values fell below FDR in at least two or more blocks.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank Drs. Gidon Felsen, Nathan Schoppa and Dan Tollin for discussions; Dr. Ed Hsu, Osama Abdulla and the Univ. of Utah Small Animal MRI Facility. Funded by NIH grants DC00566 (DR), DC04657 (DR), DC008855 (DR), DC008066 (WD), and DC002994 (ML).
References
- Abraham NM, Spors H, Carleton A, Margrie TW, Kuner T, Schaefer AT. Maintaining accuracy at the expense of speed: stimulus similarity defines odor discrimination time in mice. Neuron. 2004;44:865–876. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.11.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alonso JM, Usrey WM, Reid RC. Precisely correlated firing in cells of the lateral geniculate nucleus. Nature. 1996;383:815–819. doi: 10.1038/383815a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Apicella A, Yuan Q, Scanziani M, Isaacson JS. Pyramidal cells in piriform cortex receive convergent input from distinct olfactory bulb glomeruli. J Neurosci. 2010;30:14255–14260. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2747-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aungst JL, Heyward PM, Puche AC, Karnup SV, Hayar A, Szabo G, Shipley MT. Centre-surround inhibition among olfactory bulb glomeruli. Nature. 2003;426:623–629. doi: 10.1038/nature02185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bailey CH, Kandel ER. Synaptic remodeling, synaptic growth and the storage of long-term memory in Aplysia. Prog Brain Res. 2008;169:179–198. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(07)00010-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouret S, Sara SJ. Reward expectation, orientation of attention and locus coeruleus-medial frontal cortex interplay during learning. Eur J Neurosci. 2004;20:791–802. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03526.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouret S, Sara SJ. Network reset: a simplified overarching theory of locus coeruleus noradrenaline function. Trends Neurosci. 2005;28:574–582. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2005.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chang FC, Scott TR. Conditioned taste aversions modify neural responses in the rat nucleus tractus solitarius. J Neurosci. 1984;4:1850–1862. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.04-07-01850.1984. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Curran-Everett D. Multiple comparisons: philosophies and illustrations. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2000;279:R1–R8. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.2000.279.1.R1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cury KM, Uchida N. Robust odor coding via inhalation-coupled transient activity in the mammalian olfactory bulb. Neuron. 2010;68:570–585. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.09.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dalo NL, Larson AA. Effects of urethane and ketamine on substance P- and excitatory amino acid-induced behavior in mice. Eur J Pharmacol. 1990;184:173–177. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(90)90679-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doucette W, Milder J, Restrepo D. Adrenergic modulation of olfactory bulb circuitry affects odor discrimination. Learn Mem. 2007;14:539–547. doi: 10.1101/lm.606407. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doucette W, Restrepo D. Profound context-dependent plasticity of mitral cell responses in olfactory bulb. PLoS Biol. 2008;6:e258. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0060258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Egana JI, Aylwin ML, Maldonado PE. Odor response properties of neighboring mitral/tufted cells in the rat olfactory bulb. Neuroscience. 2005;134:1069–1080. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.04.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franks KM, Isaacson JS. Strong single-fiber sensory inputs to olfactory cortex: implications for olfactory coding. Neuron. 2006;49:357–363. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.12.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friedrich RW, Habermann CJ, Laurent G. Multiplexing using synchrony in the zebrafish olfactory bulb. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7:862–871. doi: 10.1038/nn1292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galan RF, Fourcaud-Trocme N, Ermentrout GB, Urban NN. Correlation-induced synchronization of oscillations in olfactory bulb neurons. J Neurosci. 2006;26:3646–3655. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4605-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gao Y, Strowbridge BW. Long-term plasticity of excitatory inputs to granule cells in the rat olfactory bulb. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12:731–733. doi: 10.1038/nn.2319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gire DH, Schoppa NE. Long-term enhancement of synchronized oscillations by adrenergic receptor activation in the olfactory bulb. J Neurophysiol. 2008 doi: 10.1152/jn.01324.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gold JI, Shadlen MN. The neural basis of decision making. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2007;30:535–574. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.113038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray CM, Freeman WJ, Skinner JE. Chemical dependencies of learning in the rabbit olfactory bulb: acquisition of the transient spatial pattern change depends on norepinephrine. Behav Neurosci. 1986;100:585–596. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.100.4.585. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griff ER, Mafhouz M, Chaput MA. Comparison of identified mitral and tufted cells in freely breathing rats: II. Odor-evoked responses. Chem Senses. 2008;33:793–802. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjn040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kashiwadani H, Sasaki YF, Uchida N, Mori K. Synchronized oscillatory discharges of mitral/tufted cells with different molecular receptive ranges in the rabbit olfactory bulb. J Neurophysiol. 1999;82:1786–1792. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.82.4.1786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kay LM, Beshel J. A beta oscillation network in the rat olfactory system during a 2-alternative choice odor discrimination task. J Neurophysiol. 2010;104:829–839. doi: 10.1152/jn.00166.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kay LM, Krysiak M, Barlas L, Edgerton GB. Grading odor similarities in a Go/No-Go task. Physiol Behav. 2006;88:339–346. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.04.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kay LM, Laurent G. Odor- and context-dependent modulation of mitral cell activity in behaving rats. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:1003–1009. doi: 10.1038/14801. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiyokage E, Pan YZ, Shao Z, Kobayashi K, Szabo G, Yanagawa Y, Obata K, Okano H, Toida K, Puche AC, Shipley MT. Molecular identity of periglomerular and short axon cells. J Neurosci. 2010;30:1185–1196. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3497-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Komura Y, Tamura R, Uwano T, Nishijo H, Kaga K, Ono T. Retrospective and prospective coding for predicted reward in the sensory thalamus. Nature. 2001;412:546–549. doi: 10.1038/35087595. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luna VM, Schoppa NE. GABAergic circuits control input-spike coupling in the piriform cortex. J Neurosci. 2008;28:8851–8859. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2385-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mandairon N, Linster C. Odor perception and olfactory bulb plasticity in adult mammals. J Neurophysiol. 2009 doi: 10.1152/jn.00076.2009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin C, Gervais R, Messaoudi B, Ravel N. Learning-induced oscillatory activities correlated to odour recognition: a network activity. Eur J Neurosci. 2006;23:1801–1810. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04711.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matsutani S. Trajectory and terminal distribution of single centrifugal axons from olfactory cortical areas in the rat olfactory bulb. Neuroscience. 2010;169:436–448. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller P, Katz DB. Stochastic transitions between neural states in taste processing and decision-making. J Neurosci. 2010;30:2559–2570. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3047-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mori K, Nagao H, Yoshihara Y. The olfactory bulb: coding and processing of odor molecule information. Science. 1999;286:711–715. doi: 10.1126/science.286.5440.711. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nagayama S, Takahashi YK, Yoshihara Y, Mori K. Mitral and tufted cells differ in the decoding manner of odor maps in the rat olfactory bulb. J Neurophysiol. 2004;91:2532–2540. doi: 10.1152/jn.01266.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nissant A, Bardy C, Katagiri H, Murray K, Lledo PM. Adult neurogenesis promotes synaptic plasticity in the olfactory bulb. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12:728–730. doi: 10.1038/nn.2298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pandipati S, Gire DH, Schoppa NE. Adrenergic receptor-mediated disinhibition of mitral cells triggers long-term enhancement of synchronized oscillations in the olfactory bulb. J Neurophysiol. 2010;104:665–674. doi: 10.1152/jn.00328.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pantoja J, Ribeiro S, Wiest M, Soares E, Gervasoni D, Lemos NA, Nicolelis MA. Neuronal activity in the primary somatosensory thalamocortical loop is modulated by reward contingency during tactile discrimination. J Neurosci. 2007;27:10608–10620. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5279-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pupo AS, Minneman KP. Adrenergic pharmacology: focus on the central nervous system. CNS Spectr. 2001;6:656–662. doi: 10.1017/s1092852900001346. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quiroga RQ, Nadasdy Z, Ben Shaul Y. Unsupervised spike detection and sorting with wavelets and superparamagnetic clustering. Neural Comput. 2004;16:1661–1687. doi: 10.1162/089976604774201631. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramón y Cajal S. Textura del sistema nervioso del hombre y de los vertebrados. Madrid: Moya; 1904. [Google Scholar]
- Restrepo D, Doucette W, Whitesell JD, McTavish TS, Salcedo E. From the top down: flexible reading of a fragmented odor map. Trends Neurosci. 2009;32:525–531. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2009.06.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rinberg D, Koulakov A, Gelperin A. Sparse odor coding in awake behaving mice. J Neurosci. 2006;26:8857–8865. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0884-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts AC, Glanzman DL. Learning in Aplysia: looking at synaptic plasticity from both sides. Trends Neurosci. 2003;26:662–670. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2003.09.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoenfeld TA, Macrides F. Topographic organization of connections between the main olfactory bulb and pars externa of the anterior olfactory nucleus in the hamster. J Comp Neurol. 1984;227:121–135. doi: 10.1002/cne.902270113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoenfeld TA, Marchand JE, Macrides F. Topographic organization of tufted cell axonal projections in the hamster main olfactory bulb: an intrabulbar associational system. J Comp Neurol. 1985;235:503–518. doi: 10.1002/cne.902350408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoppa NE. Synchronization of olfactory bulb mitral cells by precisely timed inhibitory inputs. Neuron. 2006;49:271–283. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.11.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shepherd GM, Chen WR, Greer CA. Olfactory bulb. In: Shepherd GM, editor. The Synaptic Organization of the Brain. New York: Oxford University Press; 2004. pp. 159–204. [Google Scholar]
- Shuler MG, Bear MF. Reward timing in the primary visual cortex. Science. 2006;311:1606–1609. doi: 10.1126/science.1123513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slotnick BM, Restrepo D. Olfactometry with mice. In: Crawley JN, Gerefen CR, Rogawski MA, Sibley DR, Skolnick P, Wray S, editors. Current Protocols in Neuroscience. New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc; 2005. pp. 1–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stopfer M, Bhagavan S, Smith BH, Laurent G. Impaired odour discrimination on desynchronization of odour-encoding neural assemblies. Nature. 1997;390:70–74. doi: 10.1038/36335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takahashi S, Sakurai Y. Sub-Millisecond Firing Synchrony of Closely Neighboring Pyramidal Neurons in Hippocampal CA1 of Rats During Delayed Non-Matching to Sample Task. Front Neural Circuits. 2009;3:9. doi: 10.3389/neuro.04.009.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tan J, Savigner A, Ma M, Luo M. Odor information processing by the olfactory bulb analyzed in gene-targeted mice. Neuron. 2010;65:912–926. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uchida N, Mainen ZF. Speed and accuracy of olfactory discrimination in the rat. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6:1224–1229. doi: 10.1038/nn1142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wachowiak M, Shipley MT. Coding and synaptic processing of sensory information in the glomerular layer of the olfactory bulb. Semin Cell Dev Biol. 2006;17:411–423. doi: 10.1016/j.semcdb.2006.04.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wallis JD, Kennerley SW. Heterogeneous reward signals in prefrontal cortex. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2010;20:191–198. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.02.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang Y, Barakat A, Zhou H. Electrotonic coupling between pyramidal neurons in the neocortex. PLoS One. 2010;5:e10253. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0010253. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wesson DW, Donahou TN, Johnson MO, Wachowiak M. Sniffing behavior of mice during performance in odor-guided tasks. Chem Senses. 2008;33:581–596. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjn029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson DA, Leon M. Spatial patterns of olfactory bulb single-unit responses to learned olfactory cues in young rats. J Neurophysiol. 1988;59:1770–1782. doi: 10.1152/jn.1988.59.6.1770. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson RI, Mainen ZF. Early events in olfactory processing. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2006;29:163–201. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.112950. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.