Abstract
This study examined self-reported impulsivity and aggression and performance on the stop-signal task in patients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder and healthy volunteers. Patients show increased stop-signal reaction time but no difference in post-error slowing, suggesting deficient inhibition but spared error monitoring. Response variability was increased in patients compared to controls.
1. Introduction
“Frontal” or “executive” abnormalities are prominent in patients with schizophrenia (Hoptman and Nolan, 2009); they perform poorly on tasks requiring cognitive control and contextual information processing (e.g., the Stroop (Schooler et al., 1997), the AX version of the Continuous Performance Test and go/no-go tasks (Cohen et al., 1999)). Risk for aggression is increased in patients with schizophrenia compared to individuals without serious mental illness (Swanson et al., 1990; Steadman et al., 1998; Brennan et al., 2000). Positive psychotic symptoms may be more severe in violent schizophrenic patients (Krakowski et al., 1999; Fresán et al., 2005), however, frontally mediated functions such as social cognition (Weiss et al., 2006) and impulse control (Hoptman et al., 2002; Kumari et al., 2009) are also impaired and there is evidence for relationships between frontotemporal disconnections and aggression (Hoptman et al., 2002; Hoptman et al., 2010). Aggression is also increased in non-psychotic conditions that affect impulse control. In Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) aggression is increased even in the absence of co-morbid conduct disorder or antisocial personality disorder (Retz and Rösler, 2007) and features of impulsivity and hyperactivity predict aggressive behaviors (Dowson and Blackwell, 2010). Thus, poor impulse control is seen as the basis of at least some aggression.
The stop-signal task (Logan et al., 1984) is widely used to study inhibitory deficits in ADHD (Schachar et al., 2000; Epstein et al., 2001), compulsive disorders (Morein-Zamir et al., 2010) and substance abuse/dependence (Fillmore and Rush, 2002). The SST consists of a primary “Go” task and a secondary “Stop” task which requires withholding responses when a stop-signal occurs. SST performance has been modeled as a race between the go process, initiated by presentation of an imperative go stimulus, and a stop process, triggered by presentation of the stop signal. If the stop process finishes first, the response is successfully inhibited. Successful inhibition depends on both the latency of the stop process and the length of the interval between onset of the go stimulus and presentation of the stop-signal: the stop-signal delay (SSD).
Systematically varying SSD enables estimation of the latency of the inhibition process, the stop signal reaction time (SSRT). In ADHD, deficient SST performance appears to be due to separable impairments in response inhibition and error monitoring. In addition to having significantly longer SSRTs than controls, ADHD participants show less slowing after errors on Stop trials, but post-error slowing is not correlated with SSRT (Schachar et al., 2004).
Patients with schizophrenia display abnormal SST performance, but the underlying impairments remain unclear. In one study, although SSRT was not increased in schizophrenia, the stop process was either more variable or less often triggered (Badcock et al., 2002). In another study, patients with schizophrenia had higher impulsivity ratings and increased SSRT, but there were no significant correlations between stop task inhibition measures and impulsivity ratings (Enticott et al., 2008). In that study, patients’ performance suggested greater response variability rather than impaired inhibition triggering. In light of the partial disagreement between these results, and the implication of impulsivity in aggression, in the current study we examined self-reported impulsivity and aggression and SST performance in patients with schizophrenia and healthy volunteers.
2. Methods
2.1 Participants
Patient participants were diagnosed with schizophrenia (n=40) or schizoaffective disorder (n=7) by DSM-IV criteria using the SCID-I/P (First et al., 2001) and were being treated with either second generation neuroleptics (n=28) or a combination of first- and second generation neuroleptics (n=11, data missing for 3 subjects). Healthy controls (n=49) did not meet criteria for any current Axis I disorders. Individuals meeting criteria for substance abuse within 6 months or substance dependence at any time were excluded. Current substance abstinence was verified by urine toxicology for outpatients (n=17) and healthy controls. All participants provided written informed consent as approved by the local Institutional Review Boards.
2.2 Questionnaires
The Barratt Impulsiveness Scale Version 11 (Patton et al., 1995), Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire (BPAQ; (Buss and Perry, 1992) and Life History of Aggression (LHA; Coccaro et al., 1997) were administered. Based on clinical experience and chart data, and blind to SST data, LHA data were rated for likelihood of validity. Only LHA scores deemed to be of at least medium confidence were retained. Severity of patients’ current psychopathology was rated with the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS; Kay et al., 1987) and Clinical Global Impressions Scale (CGI; Guy, 1976). Substance use was measured using the Kreek-McHugh-Schluger-Kellogg scale (Kellog et al., 2003).
2.3 Stop signal task
The stop signal paradigm was adapted from Schachar et al. (2000). Visual stimuli were presented centrally on a computer display. Subjects responded manually by pressing mouse buttons. The participants’ primary (“go”) task was a choice reaction time (RT) task which required pressing one button in response to the letter “X” and the other button for “O”. Each trial began with a central fixation (+), followed by the stimulus (1000 ms duration). Subjects were instructed to respond as rapidly and accurately as possible to letter stimuli. A visual stop signal, a bright red rectangle surrounding the letter stimulus, appeared on 25% of trials. The SSD was initially set to 250 ms and was decreased by 50 ms following successful (no response) stop trials and increased by 50 ms following unsuccessful stop trials, to approach an overall stop accuracy of 50%. There were 9 32-trial blocks, and the task lasted approximately 15 minutes.
2.4 Data analysis
Only blocks in which Go accuracy exceeded 85% and Stop accuracy fell between 85% and 15% were retained for analysis. Twelve subjects (5 patients and 7 controls) with fewer than 3 usable blocks were dropped from analysis. This resulted in 42 subjects in each group and no between-group difference in number of analyzable blocks (see Table 1). Excluded and retained subjects did not differ demographically. SSRT was calculated per block as average Go RT minus average SSD. Intra-individual coefficient of variation (ICV= standard deviation of Go RT / mean GoRT; (Stuss et al., 2003) indexed within-subject response variability. Group differences in continuous variables were analyzed by independent groups t-test or Mann-Whitney U-tests for nonnormal data; Fisher’s exact test was used for categorical data.
Table 1.
Patients (35 m, 7 f) | Controls (29m, 13 f) | t (df) P+ | |
---|---|---|---|
Age | 35.05 (9.94) | 34.33 (10.77) | 0.32 (82) 0.75 |
Education (years) | 12.48 (2.00) | 16.05 (2.31) | −7.58 (82) <0.001 |
Ethnicity | 21 African American | 14 African American | * p= 0.398 |
19 Caucasian | 26 Caucasian | ||
2 Other | 2 Other | ||
PANSS*** | 77.25 (16.18) | -- | -- |
CGI*** | 3.93 (1.35) | -- | -- |
BIS TOTAL | 61.76 (7.08) | 56.00 (9.95) | 3.06 (74.07) 0.003 |
Go RT (ms) | 577.26 (82.75) | 577.59 (74.16) | −0.02 (82) 0.984 |
SSRT (ms) | 300.32 (50.79) | 249.77 (44.66) | 4.84 (82) <0.001 |
Usable Stop Signal Blocks | 6.83 (1.75) | 6.81 (1.99) | 0.06 (82) 0.954 |
Nonnormally distributed data+ | Mann-Whitney U statistic/p | ||
LHA Total Score** | 14.71 (10.15) | 10.80 (6.37) | 524/0.063 |
BPAQ TOTAL | 62.88 (17.09) | 51.95 (12.24) | 548.5/0.001 |
Tobacco Use++ | 4.00 (4.11) | 6.74 (4.76) | 581/.006 |
Alcohol Use++ | 6.78 (3.32) | 5.07 (3.71) | 627.5/.002 |
Go Accuracy | 0.95 (0.03) | 0.98 (0.02) | 306 /<0.001 |
Stop Accuracy | 0.56 (0.11) | 0.64 (.07) | 481 /<0.001 |
ICV | 0.30 (0.06) | 0.22 (0.05) | 293/<0.001 |
Note.
Fisher’s Exact Test,
35 patients and 40 controls provided valid data for the LHA,
missing for 2 subjects,
as indicated by significant Shapiro-Wilks and/or Kolmogorov-Smirnoff statistics,
from Kreek-McHugh-Scluger-Kellogg (KMSK; Kellogg et al., 2003) scale
3. Results
Demographic and clinical data and aggression and impulsivity scores and SST measures are listed in Table 1. Groups did not differ significantly in age, gender or race. Controls had more years of education, more alcohol use, and less tobacco use. Patients had higher impulsivity scores than controls. There were no group differences in Go RT. Go accuracy was higher in controls than patients. Controls were also more successful at inhibiting responses on Stop Trials. SSRT and ICV were higher in patients than controls. Across groups, there were significant correlations between SSRT and LHA (r=0.29, p=0.012), BPAQ (r=0.30, p=0.005) and BIS (r=0.33, p=0.002) and between ICV and LHA (r=0.26, p=0.025), BPAQ (r=0.42, p<0.001) and BIS (r=0.31, p=0.006). Controlling for group, the SSRT/BIS correlation remained significant (p=.054) as did the ICV/BPAQ correlation (p=.008), with the SSRT/LHA correlation significant at a trend level (p=.07). In patients, PANSS Total scores were significantly and positively correlated with BPAQ total scores (r=0.50, p=0.001) and ICV (r=.48, p=.002) and with SSRT at the trend level (r=.30, p=.059), but not with BIS or LHA scores (ps>0.116).
4. Discussion
Patients with schizophrenia often display behavioral problems that suggest impaired impulse control. In this sample, patients with schizophrenia scored higher than healthy controls on interview and questionnaire measures of impulsivity and aggression. Stop signal performance (SSRT) was correlated with impulsivity ratings and was impaired in patients. Although Go trial accuracy was high in both groups, controls were more accurate than patients. Patients responded as quickly as controls on Go trials. Slower SSRT in the absence of Go RT differences implies a selective deficit in inhibition in schizophrenia, as opposed to a general cognitive deficit. Patients also showed significantly greater response variability (ICV) which has previously been associated with dorsolateral and superior medial frontal lesions (Stuss et al., 2003).
There are several limitations to this study. First, the tracking algorithm used to adjust SSDs did not uniformly induce 50% Stop trial success and controls successfully inhibited more than patients. Nevertheless, control SSRTs were comparable to other published estimates (Badcock et al., 2002; Schachar et al., 2004; Enticott et al., 2006) and showed lawful relationships with other SST measures. Second, all patients were taking neuroleptic medications, which might affect performance. It is possible, for instance, that they might improve performance to the extent that they were efficacious. Third, extrapyamidal symptoms, which were not evaluated, might have affected performance. Fourth, groups differed in tobacco and alcohol use. Finally, questionnaire measures are susceptible to interpretational biases and may be less valid in patients. Nevertheless, predicted relationships were observed in both patients and controls.
In summary, SST performance differences between patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls are consistent with a specific impairment in response inhibition in schizophrenia. These abnormalities were associated with self-report measures of aggression and are consistent with deficits in inferior frontal, dorsolateral prefrontal and superior medial frontal function in schizophrenia.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Badcock JC, Michie PT, Johnson L, Combrinck J. Acts of control in schizophrenia: dissociating the components of inhibition. Psychological Medicine. 2002;32:287–297. doi: 10.1017/s0033291701005128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brennan PA, Mednick SA, Hodgins S. Major mental disorders and criminal violence in a Danish birth cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2000;57:494–500. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.5.494. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buss AH, Perry M. The aggression questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1992;63:452–459. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.63.3.452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coccaro EF, Berman ME, Kavoussi RJ. Assessment of life history of aggression: development and psychometric characteristics. Psychiatry Research. 1997;73:147–157. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(97)00119-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen JD, Barch DM, Carter C, Servan-Schreiber D. Context-processing deficits in schizophrenia: converging evidence from three theoretically motivated cognitive tasks. J Abnorm Psychol. 1999;108:120–133. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.108.1.120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dowson JH, Blackwell AD. Impulsive aggression in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2010;121:103–110. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2009.01460.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Enticott PG, Ogloff JRP, Bradshaw JL. Associations between laboratory measures of executive inhibitory control and self-reported impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences. 2006;41:285–294. [Google Scholar]
- Enticott PG, Ogloff JRP, Bradshaw JL. Response inhibition and impulsivity in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research. 2008;157:251–254. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2007.04.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Epstein JN, Johnson DE, Varia IM, Conners CK. Neuropsychological assessment of response inhibition in adults with ADHD. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2001;23:362–371. doi: 10.1076/jcen.23.3.362.1186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fillmore MT, Rush CR. Impaired inhibitory control of behavior in chronic cocaine users. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2002;66:265–273. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(01)00206-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition (SCID-I/P) New York: Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Fresán A, Apiquian R, de la Fuente-Sandoval C, Löyzaga C, García-Anaya M, Meyenberg N, et al. Violent behavior in schizophrenic patients: relationship with clinical symptoms. Aggressive Behavior. 2005;31:511–520. [Google Scholar]
- Guy W. ECDEU Assessment Manual for Psychopharmacology. Rockville, MD: US Department of Health Education Welfare, National Institute of Mental Health; 1976. Clinical Global Assessment Scale (CGI) pp. 218–222. [Google Scholar]
- Hoptman MJ, D’angelo D, Catalano D, Mauro CJ, Shehzad ZE, Kelly AMC, et al. Amygdalofrontal functional connectivity and aggression in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp012. (in press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoptman MJ, Nolan KA. The Role of Prefrontal Abnormalities in Schizophrenia. In: Javitt DC, Kantrowitz JT, editors. Handbook of Neurochemistry and Molecular Neurobiology: Schizophrenia. Springer; New York: 2009. pp. 383–401. [Google Scholar]
- Hoptman MJ, Volavka J, Johnson G, Weiss E, Bilder RM, Lim KO. Frontal white matter microstructure, aggression, and impulsivity in men with schizophrenia: a preliminary study. Biological Psychiatry. 2002;52:9–14. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01311-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 1987;13:261–276. doi: 10.1093/schbul/13.2.261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krakowski M, Czobor P, Chou JC. Course of violence in patients with schizophrenia: relationship to clinical symptoms. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 1999;25:505–517. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumari V, Barkataki I, Goswami S, Flora S, Das M, Taylor P. Dysfunctional, but not functional, impulsivity is associated with a history of seriously violent behaviour and reduced orbitofrontal and hippocampal volumes in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research. 2009;173:39–44. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Logan GD, Cowan WB, Davis KA. On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 1984;10:276–291. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.10.2.276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morein-Zamir S, Fineberg NA, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. Inhibition of thoughts and actions in obsessive-compulsive disorder: extending the endophenotype? Psychological Medicine. 2010;40:263–272. doi: 10.1017/S003329170999033X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES. Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1995;51:768–774. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::aid-jclp2270510607>3.0.co;2-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Retz W, Rösler M. The relation of ADHD and violent aggression: What can we learn from epidemiological and genetic studies? International Journal of Law and Psychiatry. 2007;32:235–243. doi: 10.1016/j.ijlp.2009.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schachar R, Mota VL, Logan GD, Tannock R, Klim P. Confirmation of an inhibitory control deficit in attention- deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2000;28:227–235. doi: 10.1023/a:1005140103162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schachar RJ, Chen S, Logan GD, Ornstein TJ, Crosbie J, Ickowicz A, et al. Evidence for an error monitoring deficit in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2004;32:285–293. doi: 10.1023/b:jacp.0000026142.11217.f2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schooler C, Neumann E, Caplan LJ, Roberts BR. Stroop theory, memory, and prefrontal cortical functioning: reply to Cohen et al. (1997) J Exp Psychol Gen. 1997;126:42–44. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.126.1.42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steadman HJ, Mulvey EP, Monahan J, Robbins PC, Appelbaum PS, Grisso T, et al. Violence by people discharged from acute psychiatric inpatient facilities and by others in the same neighborhoods. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1998;55:393–401. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.5.393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stuss DT, Murphy KJ, Binns MA, Alexander MP. Staying on the job: the frontal lobes control individual performance variability. Brain. 2003;126:2363–2380. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swanson J, Holzer C, III, Ganju V, Tsutumo Jono R. Violence and psychiatric disorder in the community: Evidence from the epidemiologic catchment area survey. Hospital and Community Psychiatry. 1990;41:761–770. doi: 10.1176/ps.41.7.761. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiss EM, Kohler CG, Nolan KA, Czobor P, Volavka J, Platt MM, et al. The relationship between history of violent and criminal behavior and recognition of facial expression of emotions in men with schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder. Aggressive Behavior. 2006;32:1–8. [Google Scholar]