Abstract
Patients seeking deep brain stimulation (DBS) surgery for Parkinson’s disease (PD) typically undergo neuropsychological assessment to determine candidacy for surgery, with poor memory performance interpreted as a contraindication. Patients with PD may exhibit worse memory for word lists than for stories due to the lack of inherent organization in a list of unrelated words. Unfortunately, word list and story tasks are typically developed from different normative datasets, and the existence of a memory performance discrepancy in PD has been challenged. We compared recall of stories and word lists in 35 non-demented PD candidates for DBS. We administered commonly-used neuropsychological measures of word list and story memory (Hopkins Verbal Learning Test, Logical Memory), along with a second word list task that was co-normed with the story task. Age-corrected scores were higher for the story task than for both word list tasks. Compared to story recall, word list recall correlated more consistently with motor severity and composite measures of processing speed, working memory, and executive functioning. These results support the classic view of fronto-subcortical contributions to memory in PD and suggest that executive deficits may influence word list recall more than story recall. We recommend a multi-componential memory battery in the neuropsychological assessment of DBS candidates to characterize both mesial temporal and frontal-executive memory processes. One should not rely solely on a word list task because patients exhibiting poor memory for word lists may perform better with stories and therefore deserve an interdisciplinary discussion for DBS surgery.
Introduction
Memory changes are a primary concern for patients with Parkinson’s disease (PD), who are at an elevated risk for dementia (Aarsland, Zaccai, & Brayne, 2005). Among non-demented patients, cognitive difficulties are common and can include slowing, executive dysfunction (e.g., temporal ordering, organization, set-shifting), and episodic memory impairments (Pahwa, Paolo, Tröster, & Koller, 1998; Weintraub, Moberg, Culbertson, Duda, & Stern, 2001; Muslimovic, Post, Speelman, & Schmand, 2005; McKinlay, Grace, Dalrymple-Alford, & Roger, 2010). Executive impairments have been attributed to disruption of fronto-subcortical systems (Alexander, DeLong, & Strick, 1998; Taylor, Saint-Cyr, & Lang, 1986; Zgaljardic, Borod, Foldi, & Mattis, 2003). An accurate assessment of memory is a crucial goal in the neuropsychological evaluation of candidates for deep brain stimulation (DBS) because cognitive impairment may increase surgical risk. Neuropsychological evaluation helps to identify individuals with PD dementia or Dementia with Lewy Bodies, who are less likely to benefit from DBS, and aids in decisions regarding selection of surgical site (pallidum vs. subthalamic nucleus) based on cognitive status (Okun et al., 2007).
The assessment of memory can take many forms. Initial correlational research suggested that tests evaluating memory for word lists versus stories were essentially interchangeable (Delis, Butters, Cairns, Prifitera, 1988). Both require rapid processing of a constant stream of information that must be maintained in working memory to facilitate encoding and consolidation via hippocampally-based mechanisms. However, contemporary research highlights important differences (Randolph, et al., 1994; Wicklund, Johnson, Rademaker, Weitner, & Weintraub, 2006; Helmstaedter, Wietzke, & Lutz, 2009). Performance on story tasks typically benefits from intrinsic semantic organization of the material, while word-list tasks require one to self-generate organizational strategies. Memory for word lists, compared to stories, has been associated with cognitive measures of executive function (Tremont, Halpert, Javorsky, & Stern, 2000). Patients with focal frontal lobe lesions (Kopelman & Stanhope, 1998) and executive dysfunction (Brooks, Weaver, & Scialfa, 2006) exhibit worse memory for unrelated words than semantically-related information. Conversely, memory for semantically-related information, including stories, may be more directly related to temporal lobe integrity (Lezak, Howieson, & Loring, 2004; Tremont et al., 2000).
Because many patients with PD exhibit slowing and executive dysfunction, it logically follows that these patients would perform worse on word-list memory tasks than story memory tasks. At least three studies have examined this issue. One provided empirical support for this performance discrepancy but did not assess memory with common clinical instruments (Hartikainen, Helkala, Soininen, & Riekkinen, 1993). Another reported the opposite finding (better memory for word lists than for stories) but used tasks developed from different normative samples (Dulay, et al., 2008). The third did not observe a discrepancy between word-list and story recall in PD using “co-normed” tests from the Repeatable Battery for Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS), a relatively easy tool that employs a single normative sample (Beatty et al., 2003). These highly discrepant results raise the possibility that performance differences on word-list versus story tests may reflect differences in tests’ normative properties rather than PD-related “executive” impairment. The pragmatic importance of this issue relates to the assumption that all memory measures are equivalent during clinical decision making. Poor performance on one word-list memory task sensitive to PD-type executive impairment may be inaccurately attributed to dementia (“false positive”).
The purpose of the present study was to directly test two competing hypotheses for why patients with PD may perform worse on word-list versus story memory tests. Performance discrepancies may reflect: a) a disease effect related to reduced executive skills in PD; b) an artifact of different test normative properties. To address these hypotheses, we administered conormed memory tests (story and a word-list tasks from the Weschler Memory Scale-III), and the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test (HVLT). The HVLT has been commonly used as a word list task and was developed from a different normative sample.
Methods
Participants
Participants included 35 patients with probable idiopathic PD being considered for DBS at the University of Florida Movement Disorders Center. Diagnoses were made according to U.K. brain bank criteria by fellowship-trained neurologists specializing in movement disorders (Hughes, Daniel, Kilford, & Lees, 1992). Patients were excluded if they evidenced dementia according to recent Movement Disorder Society recommendations (Dubois et al., 2007). Specifically, participants were excluded if they scored below 26 on the Mini-Mental State Exam and below 136 on the Mattis Dementia Rating Scale, . Thirty-two participants self-identified as white, three as Hispanic.
Memory Measures
Wechsler Memory Scale-Third Edition (WMS-III; Wechsler, 1997)
The normative sample used to develop the WMS-III involved a geographically and demographically diverse group of 1,250 individuals. Older groups reported relatively lower education than the rest of the sample (Lezak et al., 2004). Two WMS-III subtests were used in this study:
Logical Memory
Two brief stories are presented orally. Patients are asked to freely recall each story immediately after it was read and again 25-35 minutes later.
Word List
This test includes four learning trials of 12 unrelated words, an interference learning trial of 12 new words, and a delayed recall trial of the initial 12 words 25-35 minutes later. Patients are told they would be tested after a delay.
Hopkins Verbal Learning Test (HVLT; Benedict, Schretlen, Groninger, & Brandt, 1998)
The normative sample used to develop the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test (HVLT) involved a relatively homogenous group of 541 individuals living in the northeastern United States reporting an average of 13.8 years of education (Benedict et al., 1998). This test involves three learning trials for 12 words belonging to three semantic categories and a delayed recall trial 20-25 minutes later. No interference list is presented, and patients are not told they will be tested after a delay.
Other Cognitive and Motor Measures
We created composites representing four cognitive domains: executive functioning, processing speed, working memory, and language. The executive functioning domain comprised Trail Making Test (Part B), Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT), and the Stroop Test (Color-Word trial). The processing speed domain comprised Trail Making Test (Part A), the Digit Symbol subtest from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Third Edition, and the Stroop Test (Word Reading trial). The working memory domain comprised Digit Span Forward and Backward from the WAIS-III. The language domain comprised the Boston Naming Test (BNT) and the Vocabulary subtest from the WAIS-III.
Executive functioning (i.e., inhibition, set-shifting and directed search), processing speed, working memory and motor functioning have all been associated with fronto-subcortical circuitry. Language measures including confrontation naming and vocabulary have been associated with temporal lobe integrity and were included as a control domain (Lezak et al., 2004).
Procedures
Informed consent was obtained according to university and federal guidelines. Cognitive assessments used for pre-DBS screening were completed over two days while patients were on dopaminergic medications. Day one: patients underwent a standardized neuropsychological assessment that included WMS-III Logical Memory and HVLT. Day two: patients were administered the WMS-III Word List subtest during a research visit. Motor testing on dopaminergic medications was conducted by neurologists using the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS-III; Fahn, Elton, and Committee, 1987).
Statistical Analyses
Using normative data provided by test publishers for each patient’s age group, performances on the memory tasks were converted to T-scores (mean=50, standard deviation=10). Primary aims were assessed with repeated-measure analyses of variance. Cognitive composite scores were calculated as averaged z-scores derived from raw scores on each test within a domain. Williams’ t-tests were used to evaluate the statistical significance of differences in Pearson correlation magnitudes (Williams, 1959; Steiger, 1980).
Results
Table 1 provides sample characteristics. The participants were predominantly well-educated men with moderate PD severity. Table 2 displays scores on the memory tests. Compared to age peers, patients performed statistically below the normative average (T-score=50) on three of the four word list variables but neither story variable.
Table 1.
Sample characteristics
Mean | SD | |
---|---|---|
Demographic | ||
Age | 63.0 | 8.9 |
Education (years) | 14.5 | 2.9 |
Sex (M / F) | 28 / 7 | |
Disease | ||
Disease duration (months) | 136.9 | 56.2 |
UPDRS-III “on” | 23.4 | 9.9 |
UPDRS-III “off” | 36.5 | 13.3 |
HY (2 / 2.5 / 3)^ | 21 / 2 / 3 | |
LEDD | 1102.3 | 577.4 |
Emotion | ||
BDI-II | 10.6 | 6.4 |
AS | 10.3 | 6.7 |
STAI state (percentile) | 62.4 | 32.4 |
STAI trait (percentile) | 58.1 | 32.1 |
General Cognitive | ||
MMSE | 28.4 | 1.7 |
DRS-2 | 137.8 | 5.4 |
SD: Standard deviation; UPDRS-III: Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale, Motor Section; HY: Hoehn & Yahr Stage Scale; LEDD: Levodopa equivalent daily dose; BDI-II: Beck Depression Inventory, 2nd edition; AS: Apathy Scale; STAI: State-Trait Anxiety Inventory; MMSE: Mini Mental State Exam; DRS-2: Dementia Rating Scale, 2nd edition.
Note that Hoehn & Yahr data were not available for 9 patients
Table 2.
Mean age-adjusted T-scores on memory tasks relative to normative sample^
Mean T-score | SD | t | |
---|---|---|---|
HVLT Word List-1 | |||
Immediate | 42.3 | 12.0 | −3.717 * |
Delay | 41.6 | 11.3 | −4.349 * |
| |||
WMS-III Word List | |||
Immediate | 43.9 | 11.8 | −3.077 * |
Delay | 52.1 | 8.3 | 1.404 |
| |||
WMS-III LM (Stories) | |||
Immediate | 51.6 | 12.3 | 0.768 |
Delay | 54.9 | 10.0 | 2.929 |
p<.006 in one-sample t-tests (test value=50)
Normative sample T Score: Mean=50, SD=10
WMS-III: Wechsler Memory Scale, 3rd edition; HVLT: Hopkins Verbal Learning Test
Note: normative scores for both the WMS-III and HVLT-R are based on age (but not sex or education)
Memory (Figure 1)
Figure 1.
Age-corrected T scores on the three memory tests.
HVLT: Hopkins Verbal Learning Test; WMS-III: Wechsler Memory Test, Third Edition
Immediate recall scores differed across the three memory tests (F(2,66)=13.909; p<.001; ηp2=.30.) The two word-list tasks did not differ (WMS-III, HVLT, p=.37). Scores on both were significantly lower than scores on Logical Memory (p’s<.001). Delayed recall scores also differed across the three memory tests (F(1.6,47.9)=25.248; p<.001; ηp2=.46). Logical Memory scores were higher than scores on both WMS-III Word List (p<.01) and HVLT (p<.001). Scores were higher on WMS-III Word list than HVLT (p<.001).
Other Cognitive Domains (Table 3)
Table 3.
Pearson correlations between memory measures, cognitive composites, and motor severity
Processing Speed |
Working Memory |
Executive Functioning |
Language |
UPDRS-III On |
|
---|---|---|---|---|---|
HVLT | |||||
Immediate | .398 * | .337 + | .175 | .305 | −.144 |
Delay | .502 ** | .058 | .387 * | .282 | −.415 ** |
| |||||
WMS-III Word List |
|||||
Immediate | .384 * | .553 ** | .269 | .549 ** | −.266 |
Delay | .378 * | .356 * | .411 * | .323 | −.126 |
| |||||
WMS-III Logical Memory |
|||||
Immediate | .072 | .227 | .174 | .370 * | −.162 |
Delay | .226 | .253 | .280 | .290 | −.234 |
| |||||
UPDRS-III On | −.351 + | −.047 | −.386 + | −.072 |
p<.01
p<.05
p<.1
WMS-III: Wechsler Memory Scale, 3rd edition; HVLT: Hopkins Verbal Learning Test; UPDRS-III: Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale, Motor Portion
Table 3 displays correlations between the memory tasks and specific cognitive domains. Williams’ t-tests revealed that several correlations between composites reflecting frontal-related cognition (processing speed, working memory, executive functioning) and word-list variables were statistically stronger than correlations between these composites and corresponding story variables. Processing Speed: Correlations with immediate word-list recall (HVLT, WMS-III) were greater than those with immediate story recall (p’s < .05). There was a trend for a similar pattern involving delayed recall (HVLT vs. Logical Memory; t(32)=1.40; p=.09). Working Memory: A greater correlation occurred with WMS-III Word List than with Logical Memory for immediate recall (t(32)=2.74; p<.01). Executive Functioning: The executive functioning composite correlated significantly with delayed recall scores for both word list tasks but not with either of the story memory variables. However, none of these pairs of correlations were statistically different (p’s>.05).
Motor Severity (Table 3)
Greater motor severity was associated with worse performance on HVLT Delayed. Medium-sized, non-significant relationships between UPDRS-III and Executive Functioning and Processing Speed were noted, with greater motor severity associated with worse performance.
Discussion
These findings support the view that non-demented individuals with PD exhibit better episodic memory for stories than for word lists. We found that both immediate and delayed memory scores were significantly higher for a story task than a co-normed word-list task. Similar findings have been demonstrated with non-demented patients with PD using non-standard measurement instruments (Hartikainen et al., 1993). This study is the first to document this verbal memory performance discrepancy in PD using co-normed measures. Results are in line with current theoretical conceptualizations of mesial temporal versus subcortical memory difficulties, with the former associated with encoding and storage deficits and the latter associated with higher-order encoding and retrieval deficits (Cummings, 1990; Perry & Hodges, 1996; Panegyres, 2004).
We did not replicate the findings of Beatty et al. (2003), who reported similar story and word-list recall among patients with PD on co-normed measures from the RBANS. This discrepancy may relate to psychometric properties of the instruments. Specifically, a recent study examining the RBANS in a sample of non-demented patients with PD reported low reliability and validity and failed to replicate its proposed factor structure in this population (Yan et al., 2009).
Our findings support an association between fronto-subcortically mediated cognition and word-list learning. Executive functioning (i.e., Trailmaking, Part B, Stroop Color-Word, verbal fluency), working memory, and processing speed are often reduced in PD (Lees & Smith, 1983; Stebbins et al., 1994; Cooper et al., 1992; Cooper et al., 1991; Fabrieli, Singh, Stebbins, & Goetz, 1996). Composite scores representing these domains were more consistently associated with word-list recall than story recall. Further support for the hypothesis that fronto-subcortical dysfunction underlies poorer word-list performance was provided by a medium-sized (Cohen, 1988) correlation between motor severity and a word-list variable (HVLT Delayed). Motor severity was not associated with either story variable.
Results also highlight an influence of normative data on the size of discrepancies between story and word-list recall. Specifically, WMS-III Word List scores were higher than HVLT scores. Clinicians should be cognizant of normative sample differences when drawing conclusions about the magnitude of impairments. A relevant limitation of this study is that the order of the two word-list tasks was not counterbalanced due to our unwillingness to alter standard clinical procedures during the DBS candidacy evaluation. As a result, all patients received the HVLT before the WMS-III Word List. However, we believe the impact is minimal because practice effects for word-list learning tasks with different memoranda are small (Wilson, et al., 2000). Also, our finding of better performance on WMS-III Logical Memory than WMS-III Word List despite a potential practice effect that would presumably reduce that difference only strengthens our conclusions.
Another potential factor that could influence recall of these different memoranda that was not explored in this study relates to the various attributes of the words used in the various tests, including frequency, length, and concreteness. While systematic differences in these attributes between the tests may not be apparent in corrected scores, it is possible that patients with PD may be more susceptible to increasing difficulty on one or more of these dimensions. Of note, there are word list tests that have been created with this issue in mind. Specifically, the Philadelphia Repeatable Verbal Learning Test includes words matched on frequency for category based on category exemplars identified using an older adult sample, and the California Verbal Learning Test is rank-based for frequency in the English language. Future studies should explore the contribution of word attributes to the detectability of impairments. Researchers and clinicians comparing word list and story tasks should be mindful of issues related to the unique properties of most story tasks, such as the inclusion of non-nouns and proper nouns, and the acceptance of certain alternative phrasings for scoring purposes.
This study highlights the importance of a multi-componential memory battery in the clinical neuropsychological assessment of patients with PD, particularly candidates for DBS. If verbal memory were assessed only with a word-list task, patients could be misclassified with memory system impairment (false positive). Conversely, assessment with only a story memory task may result in a failure to identify higher-order memory deficits commonly experienced by individuals with PD (false negative). Such higher-order aspects of memory are more heavily emphasized by word list tasks, which demand one to self-generate organizational strategies. If a patient were impaired on both word list and story memory tasks, a clinician may be more confident in concluding a primary memory impairment. For DBS screenings, clinicians should not dismiss patients with poor word-list memory without confirming deficits with a story task. Identification of a primary memory impairment should be brought to the attention of the multidisciplinary team as an example of a contraindication to surgery. Such an impairment may suggest that an additional pathological process is present, such as PD dementia or Alzheimer’s disease. Surgical intervention in these patients is considered to pose greater risk for complications and result in less robust outcomes (Okun et al., 2004). Similar consideration should be taken in the assessment of post-DBS cognitive functioning. Compared to medically-managed patients with PD, DBS patients are more likely to experience reliable declines in word-list recall than in story recall (Mikos, Zahodne, Okun, Foote, & Bowers, 2010).
Acknowledgements/Financial Disclosure/Conflict of Interest
The present study was made possible by support from the University of Florida (UF) National Parkinson Foundation Center of Excellence, the UF Foundation, and the National Institutes of Health (NINDS: NS50633, NS044997, K23NS060660; NIA: T32-AG020499). No authors have conflicts of interest related to the research reported in this manuscript.
Footnotes
The authors certify that this manuscript has not been published elsewhere and has not been submitted simultaneously for publication in another source.
References
- Aarland D, Zaccai J, Brayne C. A systematic review of prevlanece studies of dementia in Parkinson’s disease. Movement Disorders. 2005;20:1255–1263. doi: 10.1002/mds.20527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alexander GE, DeLong MR, Strick PL. Parallel organization of functionally-segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 1986;9:357–381. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.09.030186.002041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed. APA; Washington, DC: 2000. Revised. [Google Scholar]
- Beatty WW, Ryder KA, Gontkovsky ST, Scott JG, McSwan KL, Bharucha KJ. Analyzing the subcortical dementia syndrome of Parkinson’s disease using the RBANS. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2003;8:509–520. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benedict RHB, Schretlen D, Groninger L, Brandt J. Hopkins Verbal Learning Test – Revised: normative data and analysis of inter-form and test-retest reliability. The Clinical Neuropsychologist. 1998;12:43–55. [Google Scholar]
- Brooks BL, Weaver LE, Scialfa CT. Does impaired executive functioning differentially impact verbal memory measures in older adults with suspected dementia? The Clinical Neuropsychologist. 2006;20:230–242. doi: 10.1080/13854040590947461. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. second ed. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; Mahwah, NJ: 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Cooper JA, Sagar HJ, Doherty SM, Jordan N, Tidswell P, Sullivan EV. Different effects of dopaminergic and anticholinergic therapies on cognitive and motor function in PD a follow-up study of untreated patients. Brain. 1992;115:1701–1725. doi: 10.1093/brain/115.6.1701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper JA, Sagar HJ, Jordan N, Harvey NS, Sullivan EV. Cognitive impairment in early, untreated Parkinson’s disease and its relationship to motor disability. Brain. 1991;114:2095–2122. doi: 10.1093/brain/114.5.2095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cummings JL. Subcortical dementia. Oxford University Press; New York: 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Delis DC, Cullum M, Butters N, Cairns P, Prifitera A. Wechsler Memory Scale – Revised and California Verbal Learning Test: convergence and divergence. The Clinical Neuropsychologist. 1988;2:188–196. [Google Scholar]
- Dubois B, Burn D, Goetz C, Emre M. Dioagnostic procedures for Parkinson’s disease dementia: recommendations from the Movement Disorder Society task force. Movement Disorders. 2007;22:2314–2324. doi: 10.1002/mds.21844. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dulay MF, Strutt AM, Levin HS, Jankovic J, Lai EC, Grossman RG, York MK. Depressed mood and memory impairment before and after unilateral posteroventral pallidotomy in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 2008;20:357–363. doi: 10.1176/jnp.2008.20.3.357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fahn S, Elton RL, Committee . Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale. Macmillan Healthcare Information; Florham Park, NJ: 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Gabrieli JDE, Singh J, Stebbins GT, Goetz CG. Reduced working memory span in Parkinson’s disease: evidence for the role of a fronto-subcortical system in working and strategic memory. Neuropsychology. 1996;10:322–332. [Google Scholar]
- Hartikainen P, Helkala EL, Soininen H, Riekkinen P., Sr. Cognitive and memory deficits in untreated Parkinson’s disease and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis patients: a comparative study. Journal of Neural Transmission. Parkinson’s Disease and Dementia Section. 1993;6:127–137. doi: 10.1007/BF02261006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Helmstaedter C, Witzke J, Lutz MT. Unique and shared validity of the “Wechsler logical memory test,” the “California verbal learning test,” and the “verbal learning and memory test” in patients with epilepsy. Epilepsy Research. 2009;87:203–212. doi: 10.1016/j.eplepsyres.2009.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hughes AJ, Daniel SE, Kilford L, Lees AJ. Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson’s disease: a clinico-pathological study of 100 cases. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 1992;55:181–184. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.55.3.181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kopelman MD, Stanhope N. Recall and recognition memory in patients with focal frontal, temporal lobe and diencephalic lesions. Neuropsychologia. 1998;36:785–796. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(97)00167-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lees AJ, Smith E. Cognitive deficits in the early stages of Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 1983;106:257–270. doi: 10.1093/brain/106.2.257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW. Neuropsychological Assessment. Fourth Edition Oxford University Press; New York: 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Muslimovic D, Post B, Speelman JD, Schmand B. Cognitive profile of patients with newly diagnosed Parkinson disease. Neurology. 2005;65:1239–1245. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000180516.69442.95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKinlay A, Grace RC, Dalrymple-Alford JC, Rogers D. Characteristics of executive function impairment in Parkinson’s disease patients without dementia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2010;16:268–277. doi: 10.1017/S1355617709991299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mikos A, Zahodne L, Okun MS, Foote K, Bowers D. Cognitive declines after unilateral Deep Brain Stimulation surgery in Parkinson’s disease: a controlled study using Reliable Change, part II. The Clinical Neuropsychologist. 2010;24:235–245. doi: 10.1080/13854040903277297. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Okun MS, Rodriguez RL, Mikos A, Miller K, Kellison I, Kirsch-Darrow L, et al. Deep brain stimulation and the role of the neuropsychologist. The Clinical Neuropsychologist. 2007;21:162–189. doi: 10.1080/13825580601025940. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pahwa R, Paolo A, Tröster A, Koller W. Cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. European Journal of Neurology. 1998;5:431–441. doi: 10.1046/j.1468-1331.1998.550431.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Panegyres PK. The contribution of the study of neurodegenerative disorders to the understanding of human memory. Quarterly Journal of Medicine. 2004;97:555–567. doi: 10.1093/qjmed/hch096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perry RJ, Hodges JR. Spectrum of memory dysfunction in degenerative disease. Current Opinion in Neurology. 1996;9:281–285. doi: 10.1097/00019052-199608000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Randolph C, Gold JM, Kozora E, et al. Estimating memory function: disparity of Wechsler Memory Scale – Revised and California Verbal Learning Test indices in clinical and normal samples. The Clinical Neuropsychologist. 1994;8:99–108. [Google Scholar]
- Stebbins GT, Gabrieli JDE, Burton K, Rinaldi J, Fleischeman D, Monti L. Dissociation between processing speed and mnemonic components of working memory in patients with Parkinson’s disease and patients with global amnesia. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts. 1994;20:430. [Google Scholar]
- Steiger JH. Tests for comparing elements of a correlation matrix. Psychological Bulletin. 1980;87:245–251. [Google Scholar]
- Taylor AE, Saint-Cyr JA, Lang AE. Frontal lobe dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 1986;109:845–883. doi: 10.1093/brain/109.5.845. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tremont G, Halpert S, Javorsky DJ, Stern RA. Differential impact of executive dysfunction on verbal list learning and story recall. The Clinical Neuropsychologist. 2000;14:295–302. doi: 10.1076/1385-4046(200008)14:3;1-P;FT295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. The Wechsler Memory Scale. Third Edition Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Weintraub D, Moberg PJ, Culbertson WC, Duda JE, Stern MB. Evidence for impaired encoding and retrieval memory profiles in Parkinson disease. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology. 2001;17:195–200. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wicklund AH, Johnson N, Rademaker A, Weitner BB, Weintraub S. Word list versus story memory in Alzheimer disease and frontotemporal dementia. Alzheimer’s Disease and Associated Disorders. 2006;20:86–92. doi: 10.1097/01.wad.0000213811.97305.49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams EJ. The comparison of regression variables. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B (Methodology) 1959;21:396–399. 1959. [Google Scholar]
- Wilson BA, Watson PC, Baddeley AD, Emslie H, Evans JJ. Improvement or simply practice? The effects of twenty repeated assessments on people with and without brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2000;6:469–479. doi: 10.1017/s1355617700644053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang C, Garrett-Mayer E, Schneider JS, Gollomp SM, Tilley BC. Repeatable Battery for Assessment of Neuropsychological Status in early Parkinson’s disease. Movement Disorders. 2009;24:1453–1460. doi: 10.1002/mds.22552. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zgaljardic DJ, Borod JC, Foldi NS, Mattis P. A review of the cognitive and behavioral sequelae of Parkinson’s disease: relationship to fronto-subcortical circuitry. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology. 2003;16:193–210. doi: 10.1097/00146965-200312000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]