Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Apr 28.
Published in final edited form as: J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2007 Jun;28(3):206–212. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e3180324d87

Use of Videotaped Interactions During Pediatric Well-Child Care: Impact at 33 Months on Parenting and on Child Development

Alan L Mendelsohn *, Purnima T Valdez , Virginia Flynn *, Gilbert M Foley *, Samantha B Berkule *, Suzy Tomopoulos *, Arthur H Fierman *, Wendy Tineo *, Benard P Dreyer *
PMCID: PMC3083927  NIHMSID: NIHMS288862  PMID: 17565287

Abstract

Objective

We performed a randomized, controlled trial to assess the impact of the Video Interaction Project (VIP), a program based in pediatric primary care in which videotaped interactions are used by child development specialists to promote early child development.

Method

Ninety-nine Latino children (52 VIP, 47 controls) at risk of developmental delay based on poverty and low maternal education were assessed at age 33 months. VIP was associated with improved parenting practices including increased teaching behaviors.

Results

VIP was associated with lower levels of parenting stress. VIP children were more likely to have normal cognitive development and less likely to have developmental delays.

Conclusion

This study provides evidence that a pediatric primary care–based intervention program can have an impact on the developmental trajectories of at-risk young preschool children.

Index terms: pediatric prevention, developmental services, parenting


Over the past two decades, a consensus has developed that pediatric primary care represents an important and underused opportunity for preventive intervention.14 One reason is that pediatric well-child visits happen frequently, with 10 to 12 visits in the first 3 years. In addition, participation in well-child care happens almost universally because all children require vaccinations beginning in early infancy. As such, opportunities are present for low-cost interventions that address early child development and the parent-child relationship.

Recent studies have demonstrated that interventions based in pediatric well-child care can be effective in promoting parent-child relationships and early child development. One program that has been widely disseminated is Reach Out and Read, which facilitates early language and literacy development through the promotion of parent-child reading aloud, which has been shown to have an impact on parent-child reading activities and on children’s language development.510 Another approach (Healthy Steps) integrates child development specialists into pediatric care and has been shown to lead to increased parental sensitivity to children’s signals and decreased use of harsh or physical approaches to disciplining.3,11

A program called the Video Interaction Project (VIP) is under study by the Department of Pediatrics at New York University School of Medicine and Bellevue Hospital Center. The VIP approach involves the use of videotaped interactions by child development specialists while parents wait to see their child’s pediatric provider for well-child visits, and it applies the work of Bernstein and McDonough,12 Erickson et al,13 McDonoough,14 and Kubicek15 to the pediatric primary care setting. The goal of VIP is to support the parent-child relationship and thereby enhance cognitive, language, and social-emotional development. The VIP approach is relationship based: a single child development specialist builds a caring relationship with each family that forms the foundation for the intervention. VIP sessions begin at the first visit to the pediatrician when the infant is approximately 2 weeks old and continue regularly until age 3 years. Each 30- to 45-minute session includes (1) discussion of parental expectations and concerns about the child as well as the child’s present and anticipated developmental progress, (2) receipt of a developmentally appropriate learning material (e.g., toy or book) that promotes parent-child engagement, (3) a 5- to 10-minute videotaped recording of the parent and child engaging in activities of the parent’s choice, which is then rewound for the parent and the child development specialist to view together. As the tape is watched, the parent and the specialist each make observations based on the videotape, with the specialist highlighting the parent’s strengths and suggesting activities to practice at home. Because VIP is integrated into pediatric primary care and does not require home visits, its cost is relatively low (approximately $240 per child per year).

In order to assess the impact of the program, VIP was implemented as a randomized, controlled trial. We enrolled a birth cohort of economically disadvantaged Latino children at high risk of developmental delay based on low maternal education. We previously reported on the developmental outcomes of these children when they reached age 21 months, including improved cognitive and language development and a trend toward decreased need for early intervention (EI).16 In this article, we report on the impact of VIP on the cohort at age 33 months, including parenting and child development outcomes.

METHODS

Study Design

The study was conducted as a single-blind, randomized, controlled trial. Institutional review board approval was obtained from New York University School of Medicine, Bellevue Hospital Center, and the New York City Health and Hospitals Corporation. Parents provided informed consent before participation in the study.

Study Sample

The study sample has been previously described.16 In brief, enrollment was performed on the postpartum floor of an inner-city public hospital between July 1999 and January 2002. Latino mother-newborn dyads were considered eligible for the study if the mother had low education (defined as not having graduated high school). Exclusion criteria included medical complications (e.g., prematurity or postnatal medical complications), psycho-social issues (e.g., adolescent mother, maternal history of substance abuse), or follow-up care planned outside our institution. Following enrollment, dyads were randomized to the Video Interaction Project (VIP) intervention or a control group based on the toss of a coin. Intervention and control families received the same well-child care by the same primary care pediatricians, including the same anticipatory guidance and periodic routine screening according to the guidelines of the American Academy of Pediatrics.17

Measures

Assessments of parenting and child developmental-behavioral outcomes were performed when the child was 33 months. Bilingual research assistants masked to group assignment performed these assessments.

Parenting was assessed in two domains: parenting practices important for child development (provision of toys, reading, teaching, and verbal responsivity), and attitudes and perceptions related to parent-child interaction (parenting stress and maternal depressive symptoms). Parenting practices were assessed using the (StimQ-Toddler [StimQ-T]).18 The StimQ-T assesses parenting practices related to child development. It is based on a structured interview with the child’s caregiver and is validated for use in low socioeconomic status populations in English and Spanish.18 It has good internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = .88), test-retest reliability (intraclass correlation coefficient of 0.93), and criterion-related validity (correlation with Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment [HOME] Inventory19: r = .55, p < .001) and is gender neutral. It also has good concurrent validity with developmental measures and is correlated with Bayley Scales of Infant Development Mental Development Index (MDI)20 (semi-partial regression (sr) = 0.45, p < .00118) and One Word Picture Vocabulary Tests21 (Receptive: sr = 0.38, p = .01; Expressive: sr = 0.33, p = .03)22 It has been used in several recent studies of early child development performed with urban economically disadvantaged populations.10,23,24 The StimQ-T consists of four subscales, which are summed together for a total score (range, 0–39). Availability of Learning Materials (ALM) assesses learning materials such as toys provided by the caregiver in the home (range, 0–7). Reading Activities (READ) assesses number and diversity of books that are read to the child as well as frequency of reading activities (range, 0–18). The Parental Involvement in Developmental Advancement (PIDA) assesses caregiver teaching and play activities (e.g., teaching letters, words, and colors), teaching the child to play with toys (e.g., stacking blocks, pressing buttons), and playing make believe games with the child (range, 0–10). Parental Verbal Responsivity (PVR) assesses caregiver-child verbal interactions such as talking while feeding and playing rhyming games (range, 0–4).

Stress related to parenting was assessed using the Parenting Stress Index-Short Form (PSI-SF).25 The PSI-SF is a 36-item scale that consists of three subscales: The Parental Distress subscale measures symptoms of anxiety and depression related to parenting. The Dysfunctional Interaction subscale measures perceived engagement between parent and child. The Difficult Child subscale measures perceived difficult child temperament. For each subscale and for the overall scale, the 85th percentile is used as a cutoff to define severe parenting stress.26

Maternal depression was assessed using the Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression Scale (CES-D).27 This 21-item scale measures self-reported depressive symptoms. Participants answered questions about the frequency of feelings and experiences during the past week on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from “rarely or none of the time” to “most or all of the time.” An example of a question would be “Did you feel that you could not shake off the blues, even with help from your family or friends?” Mothers were considered to screen positive for depressive symptoms if the score was 16 or higher.

Child developmental-behavioral assessments were conducted in three domains: cognitive, language, and social-emotional/behavioral. Child cognitive development was assessed using the MDI of Bayley Scales of Infant Development-Second Edition.20 The MDI was administered in the families’ preferred language (English, Spanish, or both). No formal Spanish version exists; therefore, the raters agreed on common, appropriate language to use during administration of the MDI in Spanish. This approach represents our clinical practice in assessing for early intervention (EI) eligibility, which has been accepted by New York State EI assessment guidelines.28 Language development was assessed using the Preschool Language Scale-3 (PLS-3), which is normed in English and Spanish.29

Both the MDI and PLS-3 provide standardized scores, with a mean of 100 and an SD of 15. For each of the these assessments, performance was considered to be in the normal range if the score was at or above a cut point of 1 SD below the mean (≥85 and above), borderline if the score was between 1 and 2 SDs below the mean (70 through 84), and delayed if the score was below a cut point of 2 SDs below the mean (69 and below).

Social-emotional/behavioral development was assessed using the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) for ages 1½ to 5 years. The CBCL is validated in English and Spanish and consists of 100 items describing behaviors that the parent rates as “not true,” “somewhat or sometimes true,” or “very often or often true.” Based on these items, T scores (mean, 50; SD = 10) for Internalizing Problems (e.g., irritability, withdrawal, anxiety, depression, somatization), Externalizing Problems (e.g., aggression, oppositionality, inattention), and Total Problems were calculated. As recommended in the manual, clinically significant behavior was defined as a T score of 64 or greater.

In addition, eligibility for EI services was determined based on the criteria used in our state.30 Children were considered EI eligible if the score was 2 SDs below the mean in any of the three domains that were measured (cognitive [MDI], language [PLS-3], social-emotional / behavioral CBCL), or 1.5 SDs below the mean in any two domains.

Finally, demographic and other data characterizing the sample were collected based on parental interview. For the parents, this included mother’s age, country of origin, education level, primary language spoken, and occupation and father’s education level and occupation and whether the father lived in the household. Family socioeconomic status was determined based on parental education and occupation (Hollingshead Four Factor Index of Social Status).31 For the child, we assessed gender, birth order, and whether EI services were received before this assessment.

Statistical Analysis

Statistical analyses were performed based on intention to treat, i.e., outcomes were assessed according to group (and not according to degree of participation in the intervention).32 Data were analyzed using SPSS.33 VIP and control families were compared for sociodemographic characteristics and for parenting and developmental behavioral outcomes. Comparisons of means were performed using independent samples t tests. Comparisons of frequencies were performed using χ2 (with continuity correction for 2 × 2 tables) for nominal data and Spearman rank correlation for ordinal data (e.g., frequency of normal, borderline, or delayed development). We had decided a priori to assess whether the impact of the intervention might differ by level of maternal education and had found at the earlier 21-month assessment that maternal education moderated the impact of VIP on developmental outcomes.16 As at 21 months, we performed subgroup analyses for children of mothers whose level of education was above (seventh to 11th grade) and below (sixth grade or lower) the median of the sample.

RESULTS

Sample Description

Enrollment and randomization took place from July 1999 through January 2002 and has been previously described.16 Of 150 enrolled families, one or more measures was obtained at mean (SD) 33.6 (1.7) months for 99 (66.0%) families, including 52 of 77 (67.5%) Video Interaction Program (VIP) and 47 of 73 (64.4%) controls (χ2 = 0.06, p = .82). When compared to families lost to follow-up, families undergoing the 33-month assessment were more likely to have mothers who were immigrants (p < .001), were Spanish speaking (p < .001), and had lower education (p = .04), but were similar for child’s gender, birth order, and having a father living at home.

VIP and control families included in this analysis are compared in Table 1. Families in the two groups were similar for most variables, including child’s gender and previous receipt of early intervention (EI) services, mother’s age, country of origin, language spoken in the home, father living in the household, and Hollingshead Four Factor Index of Social Status. VIP children were less likely to be first born and VIP mothers had less education, but these were not statistically significant.

Table 1.

Sociodemographic Characteristics of Families Assessed at 33 Months

VIPa Controla Testb p
Childc
 Female gender   40.4% 36.2% 0.05 .82
 First born child   21.2% 36.2% 2.05 .15
 Received EI services   37.5% 39.5% 0.00 1.00
Mother/familyd
 Age, yr   29.8 (5.8) 29.8 (6.2) 0.03 .97
 Immigrated from outside US 100.0% 95.7% 0.60 .43
 Language spoken: monolingual Spanish   96.2% 91.5% 0.30 .58
 Father living in household   82.7% 83.0% 0.05 .82
 Education, yr     6.8 (2.1)   7.3 (2.5) 1.20 .22
 Hollingshead social class 5 100.0% 95.7% 0.62 .43

VIP, Video Interaction Program; EI, early intervention.

a

Values are mean (SD) or percentage.

b

Values are t or χ2.

c

VIP, n = 52; control, n = 47 except receipt of early intervention services (VIP, n = 48; control, n = 43.)

d

VIP, n = 52; control, n = 47.

Parenting Outcomes

Parenting outcomes are compared for VIP and control families in Table 2. On the StimQ-Toddler (StimQ), VIP families scored higher on the Parental Involvement in Developmental Advancement (PIDA) (teaching) subscale (p = .003). Trends were seen for differences in Availability of Learning Materials (ALM) and Parental Verbal Responsivity (PVR), but these were not statistically significant (p < .10). Total parenting stress was lower among VIP families (p = .048); in addition, a trend was seen in which fewer VIP mothers scored in the clinical range (39.2% vs 58.7%; p = .09). The subscale of the Parenting Stress Index-Short Form (PSI-SF) that appeared to contribute most to the difference in overall parenting distress was the Parenting Distress subscale, which measures stress related to anxiety and depression and was somewhat lower for VIP families (p = .09). Maternal depression did not differ between the two groups.

Table 2.

Parenting Outcomes

VIPa Controla Testb p
Parenting practices (StimQ)c
 Total score 23.1 (5.7) 21.7 (7.5) 1.06 .29
 ALM subscale   5.7 (1.5)   5.0 (1.8) 1.90 .06
 READ subscale   7.4 (3.7)   8.4 (3.8) 1.44 .15
 PIDA subscale   7.5 (1.7)   6.1 (2.8) 3.07 .003
 PVR subscale   2.6 (1.2)   2.1 (1.3) 1.82 .07
Parenting stress (PSI)d,e
 Total score 71.8 (23.8) 80.6 (18.8) 2.01 .048
 In clinical rangef 39.2% 58.7% 2.94 .09
 Parenting Distress subscale 60.9 (25.1) 68.9 (19.6) 1.72 .09
 In clinical rangef 23.5% 34.8% 0.99 .32
Parent-Child Dysfunctional Interaction subscale 76.0 (22.9) 80.9 (18.4) 1.15 .25
 In clinical rangef 37.3% 47.8% 0.72 .40
Difficult Child subscale 58.0 (25.8) 63.6 (26.6) 1.05 .30
 In clinical rangef 29.4% 28.3% 0.00 1.00
Maternal depression (CES-D)g
 Total score 10.6 (12.0) 12.4 (9.5) 0.81 .42
 In clinical rangeh 19.2% 25.5% 0.26 .61

VIP, Video Interaction Program; ALM, Availability of Learning Materials; READ, Reading Activities; PIDA, Parental Involvement in Developmental Advancement; PVR, Parental Verbal Responsivity; PSI, Parenting Stress Index; CES-D, Center for Epidemiological Studies–Depression Scale.

a

Values are mean (SD) or percentage.

b

Values are t or χ2.

c

VIP, n = 52; control, n = 45.

d

VIP, n = 51; control, n = 46.

e

Lower scores on the PSI are indicative of lower levels of parenting stress.

f

Scores at or above the 85th percentile are considered to be within the clinical range for parenting stress.

g

VIP, n = 51; control, n = 46.

h

Scores at or above 16 are considered to be within the clinical range for depression.

Child Development and Behavior Outcomes

VIP children were more likely to have normal cognitive development scores (63.5% vs 44.0%) and less likely to have developmental delays (1.9% vs 6.7%), with p = .048 (Table 3). Statistically significant differences were not seen for language, behavior, or EI eligibility.

Table 3.

Child Developmental and Behavioral Outcomes: Sample as a Whole

VIPa Controla Testb p
Cognitive (MDI)c
 MDI score 86.1 (7.5) 83.9 (9.7) 1.30 .20
  % Normal 63.5 44.4 0.20 .048
  % Borderline 34.6 48.9
  % Delayed   1.9   6.7
Language (PLS-3)c
 PLS-3 score 80.7 (10.2) 81.1 (10.6) 0.20 .86
  % Normal 30.8 35.6 0.04 .69
  % Borderline 55.8 51.1
  % Delayed 13.5 13.3
Behavior (CBCL)d,e
 CBCL total 50.2 (10.0) 53.2 (9.7) 1.50 .13
  In clinical rangef   7.7% 17.0% 0.14 .16
 CBCL externalizing 50.0 (9.8) 51.8 (9.4) 0.90 .36
  In clinical rangef 11.5% 10.6% 0.01 .89
 CBCL internalizing 52.9 (9.9) 53.8 (9.3) 0.40 .66
  In clinical rangef 17.3% 14.9% 0.03 .75
Early interventionc
 Eligible 25.0% 28.9% 0.04 .67

MDI, Mental Development Index; PLS-3, Preschool Language Scale–3; CBCL, Child Behavior Checklist.

a

Values are mean (SD) or percentage.

b

t Test or Spearman r.

c

VIP, n = 52; control, n = 45.

d

Lower scores are indicative of less problematic behavior.

e

VIP, n = 52; control, n = 47.

f

Clinically significant behavior was defined as a T score of 64 or greater.

As with the results at 21 months,16 differences between VIP and control children for developmental outcomes were most apparent for the subgroup of mothers with seventh to 11th grade education (Table 4). In this subgroup, the frequency of normal Mental Development Index (MDI) was more than double among VIP children (73.7% vs 36.4%), and the frequency of developmental delay was reduced (0.0% vs 9.1%), with p = .01. MDI score was mean 5.5 (95% confidence interval: 0.0–10.9) points higher in VIP children (p = .049). Although VIP children tended to have lower Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) scores and less frequent EI eligibility, these differences were not statistically significant. In comparison, no differences in developmental-behavioral outcomes were found between VIP and control children whose mothers had sixth grade or lower education.

Table 4.

Child Developmental and Behavioral Outcomes: Mothers with Seventh to 11th Grade Education

VIPa Controla Testb p
Cognitive (MDI)c
 MDI score 88.0 (5.8) 82.5 (10.8) 2.04 .049
  % Normal 73.7 36.4 0.39 .01
  % Borderline 26.3 54.5
  % Delayed   0.0   9.1
Language (PLS-3)c
 PLS-3 score 82.3 (8.4) 80.2 (9.6) 0.70 48
  % Normal 36.8 31.8 0.09 58
  % Borderline 52.6 50.0
  % Delayed 10.5 18.2
Behavior (CBCL)d,e
 CBCL total 49.9 (9.1) 55.6 (9.9) 1.90 .07
  In clinical rangef   5.3% 21.7% 0.23 .14
 CBCL externalizing 50.1 (9.2) 54.6 (10.9) 1.40 .16
  In clinical rangef 10.5% 21.7% 0.15 . 34
 CBCL internalizing 52.9 (7.4) 54.9 (9.2) 0.80 .44
  In clinical rangef 15.8% 17.4%. 0.02 .89
Early interventionc
 Eligible 15.8% 31.8% 0.19 .24

VIP, Video Interaction Project; MDI, Mental Development Index; PLS-3, Preschool Language Scale–3; CBCL, Child Behavior Checklist.

a

Values are mean (SD) or percentage.

b

t Test or Spearman r.

c

VIP, n = 19; control, n = 22.

d

Lower scores are indicative of less problematic behavior.

e

VIP, n = 19; control, n = 23.

f

Clinically significant behavior was defined as a T score of 64 or greater.

DISCUSSION

In this study, we have documented continuing developmental effects of Video Interaction Program (VIP) at 33 months as well as effects on parenting. Children randomized to VIP had improved cognitive development, with more than a 40% increase in the percentage of children performing in the normal range and more than a 33% reduction in the percentage of children performing in the borderline or delayed range. In addition, VIP was associated with improved parenting practices and reduced parenting stress.

As at the 21-month report,16 children of mothers with seventh to 11th grade education showed the greatest improvement. In this group, there was more than a SD improvement in cognitive score, with a doubling of children scoring in the normal range and no child found to be delayed. In addition, there was a 50% reduction in early intervention (EI) eligibility in this group; although this was not statistically significant, this may have been a result of smaller sample size and lower power within this subgroup. Although Ramey and colleagues demonstrated that mothers with low education (less than 12th grade) may benefit more from parenting interventions than mothers who have completed high school, it is possible that interventions of greater frequency or intensity may be required to alter developmental trajectories for children whose mothers have very low levels of education, below seventh grade.3436

Importantly, we documented improved parenting activities in VIP families, particularly regarding teaching and playing. Numerous longitudinal studies using either the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME) Inventory or direct measures of verbal interaction have documented the importance of these activities in influencing early childhood developmental tractories.3740 In addition, we have previously documented the importance of learning materials in promoting parent-child interaction and developmental outcomes in this sample.41 While VIP had no impact on reading activities, this is perhaps not surprising given that Reach Out and Read had been in place for children presenting to our pediatric clinic during the period in which this study was performed and all of the enrolled families had been exposed to this intervention.

In addition, VIP was associated with reduced parenting stress. This is important as parenting stress is associated with difficulties in the parent-child relationship, the potential for child maltreatment, and long-term child adjust-ment.42 The Parenting Distress subscale, which in part reflects depressive symptoms related to parenting, appeared to contribute most to this difference. In contrast, depressive symptoms in general (and not necessarily related to parenting) as measured on the Center for Epide-miological Studies–Depression Scale (CES-D) were not different between the two groups. Depressive symptoms are prevalent and multifactorial in economically disadvan-taged populations,43,44 and it is not surprising that VIP should have had an impact only on those symptoms related to parenting.

We were unable to show an impact of VIP on language development. Although the rate of delay was lower in the subgroup with seventh to 11th grade maternal education, this difference was not statistically significant, possibly as a result of limited power. In addition, close to 40% of families in each of the groups had received EI services before the 33-month assessment, and almost all these children had received speech therapy. At 21 months, differences had been noted for language measures, and it is possible that EI services had the effect of bringing the VIP and control groups closer together for both cognitive and language outcomes. Although our results were consistent with this possibility, we were unable to definitively prove this hypothesis.

A limitation of this study is that we were only able to obtain assessments for approximately two thirds of enrolled families. It is therefore possible that differential loss to follow-up could have accounted for some of the findings. However, rate of loss to follow-up was similar in the two groups. In addition, demographic and other characteristics, including receipt of EI services, were comparable between the groups. Although the VIP families had slightly lower maternal education, this might have made it more difficult to show differences between the groups.

An additional limitation of this study is that while we were able to study the aggregate impact of VIP, we were unable to determine the individual importance of each component. However, the importance of the relationship between developmental specialists and the family in preventive interventions has been documented in other studies. In the Infant Health and Development Program, improved child developmental outcomes were seen for families that were more engaged with the interventionists.45 There is also evidence that videotape with feedback has an impact beyond that of written materials in studies of attachment based interventions.46

In conclusion, we have provided further evidence that a pediatric primary care–based intervention program can have an impact on the developmental trajectories of atrisk young preschool children. Furthermore, we have shown that this intervention has an impact on measures of parenting practices important for child development as well as on parenting stress. Long-term follow-up of this sample is in progress to assess whether VIP has an impact as children attend kindergarten and first grade. Finally, a large-scale replication study funded by National Institute of Child Health and Human Development is in progress, assessing the impact of VIP on a large birth cohort of newborns from ethnically and educationally diverse background (5 R01 HD047740-02).

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors gratefully acknowledge the Rhodebeck Charitable Trust, the Tiger Foundation, the New York Community Trust and Children of Bellevue, Inc. for their funding of the Video Interaction Project. They also acknowledge the commitment and dedication of the child development specialists who implemented the program: Ana Aguilar, Florencia Catanzaro, Susan Linker, Alyse Mandel, Kim Mc-Garvey, and Gizzelle Rojas. They thank Nancy Linn for photography and for design of the VIP parenting pamphlets. They acknowledge the hard work and support of the following individuals: Kathy Angkustsiri, Claudia Aristy, Evelyn Dickey, Tania Halcon, Harris Huberman, Asha Mandava, Gloria Mattera, Marilia Neves, Lali Piera, Catherine Tamis-LeMonda, Abigail Nixon, Charissa Pebenito, Irene Rovira, Omolara Thomas, Yvette Tola Thompson, Carmen Torres, Heidi Torres, and Linda van Schaick. Finally, they express their gratitude to the families who participated, without whom this study could not have been performed.

The Video Interaction Project was funded by the Rhodebeck Charitable Trust, the Tiger Foundation, the New York Community Trust, and Children of Bellevue.

Footnotes

This study was presented in part at the annual meeting of the Society for Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, September 2005, San Diego, CA.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Stein MT, Plonsky C, Zuckerman B, Carey WB. Reformatting the 9-month health supervision visit to enhance developmental, behavioral and family concerns. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2005;26:56–60. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Dworkin PH. 2003 C. Anderson Aldrich Award Lecture: enhancing developmental services in child health supervision—an idea whose time has truly arrived. Pediatrics. 2004;114:827–832. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-0416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Zuckerman B, Parker S, Kaplan-Sanoff M, Augustyn M, Barth MC. Healthy steps: a case study of innovation in pediatric practice. Pediatrics. 2004;114:820–826. doi: 10.1542/peds.2003-0999-L. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Dinkevich E, Ozuah PO. Well-child care: effectiveness of current recommendations. Recent review of the literature on evidence for anticipatory guidance during well child care. Clin Pediatr. 2002;41:211–217. doi: 10.1177/000992280204100403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Klass P, Needlman R, Zuckerman B, et al. Reach Out and Read Training Manual. 2nd ed. Boston, MA: Reach Out and Read National Center; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Mendelsohn AL. Promoting language and literacy through reading aloud: the role of the pediatrician. Curr Probl Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2002;32:183–210. doi: 10.1067/mps.2002.125467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Needlman R, Toker KH, Klass P, Dreyer BP, Mendelsohn AL. Effectiveness of a primary care intervention to support reading aloud: a multi-center evaluation. Ambul Pediatr. 2005;5:209–215. doi: 10.1367/A04-110R.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Needlman R, Silverstein M. Pediatric interventions to support reading aloud: how good is the evidence? J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2004;25:352–363. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200410000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.High PC, LaGasse L, Becker S, et al. Literacy promotion in primary care pediatrics: can we make a difference? Pediatrics. 2000;105:927–934. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Mendelsohn AL, Mogilner LN, Dreyer BP, et al. The impact of a clinic-based literacy intervention on language development in inner-city preschool children. Pediatrics. 2001;107:130–134. doi: 10.1542/peds.107.1.130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Minkovitz C, Hughart N, Strobino D, et al. A practice-based intervention to enhance quality of care in the first 3 years of life: the Healthy Steps for Young Children Program. JAMA. 2003;290:3081–3091. doi: 10.1001/jama.290.23.3081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Bernstein VJ, McDonough S. Using video to identify family strengths. Paper presented at: Zero to Three 10th Annual Training Institute, Pre-Institute Seminar; December 1995; Atlanta, GA. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Erickson MF, Endersbe J, Simon J. Seeing Is Believing. Videotaping Families and Using Guided Self-observation to Build on Parenting Strengths. Minneapolis, MN: Regents of the University of Minnesota; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.McDonough SC. Promoting positive early parent-infant relationships through interaction guidance. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 1995;4:661–672. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Kubicek LF. Helping young children become competent communicators: the role of relationships. Zero to Three. 1996:25–30. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Mendelsohn AL, Dreyer BP, Flynn V, et al. Use of videotaped interactions during pediatric well-child care to promote child development: an RCT. J Dev Behavioral Pediatr. 2005;26:34–41. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.American Academy of Pediatrics. Guidelines for Health Supervision III. Grove, IL: American Academy of Pediatrics; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Dreyer BP, Mendelsohn AL, Tamis-LeMonda CS. [accessed May 4, 2006];StimQ. Available at: http://stimq.med.nyu.edu/stimq/
  • 19.Dreyer BP, Mendelsohn AL, Tamis-LeMonda CS. Assessing the child’s cognitive home environment through parental report: reliability and validity. Early Dev Parent. 1996;5:271–287. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant Development. 2nd Edition. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation, Harcourt Brace; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Gardner MF. Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test (Revised) Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Mendelsohn AL, Dreyer BP, Tamis-LeMonda CS, Ahuja P. Validity of StimQ, a Scale for Assessing the Cognitive Home Environment. Paper presented at: The Annual Meeting of the Society for Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics; September 1999; Seattle, WA. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Sharif I, Reiber S, Ozuah PO. Exposure to “Reach Out and Read” and vocabulary outcomes in inner-city preschoolers. J Natl Med Assoc. 2002;94:171–177. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Albright MB, Tamis-LeMonda CS. Maternal depressive symptoms in relation to dimensions of parenting in low-income mothers. Appl Dev Sci. 2002;6:24–34. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Abidin R. Short Form of the Parenting Stress Index. Charlottesville, VA: Pediatric Psychology Press; 1986. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Abidin R. Parenting Stress Index: Professional Manual. 3rd ed. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Radloff LS. The CES-D Scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Measure. 1977;1:385–401. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.New York City Department of Health and Mental Hygiene. [Accessed October 20, 2006];NYC Early Intervention Program Evaluation Requirements Under The Early Intervention Program. 2003 Available at: http://www.nyc.gov/html/doh/pdf/ei/ei-manual-add.pdf.
  • 29.Zimmerman L, Steiner V, Pond R. Preschool Language Scale–3. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.New York State Department of Health. [accessed June 18, 2006];Early Intervention Program—memorandum 2005-02: eligibility criteria. 2005 August; Available at: http://www.health.state.ny.us/community/infants_children/early_intervention/memoranda/2005-02/eligibility_crite-ria.htm.
  • 31.Hollingshead AB. Four Factor Index of Social Status. New Haven, CT: Department of Sociology, Yale University; 1975. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Hulley SB, Cummings SR, Browner WS, Grady D, Hearst N, Newman TB. Designing Clinical Research. 2nd ed. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.SPSS for Windows. Chicago, IL: SPSS, Inc; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Campbell FA, Ramey CT. Effects of early intervention on intellectual and academic achievement: a follow-up study of children from low-income families. Child Dev. 1994;65:684–698. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Burchinal MR, Campbell FA, Bryant DM, Wasik BH, Ramey CT. Early intervention and mediating processes in cognitive performance of children of low-income African American families. Child Dev. 1997;68:935–954. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1997.tb01972.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Bryant DM, Ramey CT. An analysis of the effectiveness of early intervention for environmentally at-risk children. In: Guralnick MJ, Bennett FC, editors. The Effectiveness of Early Intervention for At-Risk and Handicapped Children. Orlando, FL: Academic; 1987. pp. 33–78. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Stevens JH, Bakeman R. A factor analytic study of the HOME Scale for infants. Dev Psychol. 1985;21:1196–1203. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Bradley RH, Caldwell BM. The relation of infants’ home environments to achievement test performance in first grade: a follow-up study. Child Dev. 1984;55:803–809. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1984.tb03817.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Huttenlocher PR, Dabholkar AS. Regional differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex. J Comp Neurol. 1997;387:167–178. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(19971020)387:2<167::aid-cne1>3.0.co;2-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Hart B, Risley TR. Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children. Baltimore, MD: Paul Brookes; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Tomopoulos S, Dreyer BP, Tamis-LeMonda CS, et al. Books, toys, parents-child interaction and development in young Latino children. Ambul Pediatr. 2006;6:72–80. doi: 10.1016/j.ambp.2005.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Abidin R. Parenting Stress Index Manual. 3rd ed. Charlottesville, VA: Pediatric Psychology Press; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Zuckerman BS, Beardslee WR. Maternal depression: a concern for pediatricians. Pediatrics. 1987;79:110–117. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Kavanaugh M, Halterman JS, Montes G, Epstein M, Hightower AD, Weitzman M. Maternal depressive symptoms are adversely associated with prevention practices and parenting behaviors for preschool children. Ambul Pediatr. 2006;6:32–37. doi: 10.1016/j.ambp.2005.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Liaw F, Meisels SJ, Brooks-Gunn J. The effects of experience of early intervention on low birth weight, premature children: The Infant Health and Development Program. Early Child Res Q. 1995;10:405–431. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Juffer F, Hoksbergen RAC, Riksen-Walraven JM, Kohnstamm GA. Early intervention in adoptive families: supporting maternal sensitive responsiveness, infant-mother attachment, and infant competence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1997;38:1039–1050. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01620.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES