Skip to main content
Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine logoLink to Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine
. 2011 May;104(5):208–218. doi: 10.1258/jrsm.2011.110061

Can an electronic prescribing system detect doctors who are more likely to make a serious prescribing error?

Jamie J Coleman 1,2,, Karla Hemming 1, Peter G Nightingale 2, Ian R Clark 2, Mary Dixon-Woods 3, Robin E Ferner 1, Richard J Lilford 1,2
PMCID: PMC3089874  PMID: 21558099

Abstract

Objectives

We aimed to assess whether routine data produced by an electronic prescribing system might be useful in identifying doctors at higher risk of making a serious prescribing error.

Design

Retrospective analysis of prescribing by junior doctors over 12 months using an electronic prescribing information and communication system. The system issues a graded series of prescribing alerts (low-level, intermediate, and high-level), and warnings and prompts to respond to abnormal test results. These may be overridden or heeded, except for high-level prescribing alerts, which are indicative of a potentially serious error and impose a ‘hard stop’.

Setting

A large teaching hospital.

Participants

All junior doctors in the study setting.

Main outcome measures

Rates of prescribing alerts and laboratory warnings and doctors' responses.

Results

Altogether 848,678 completed prescriptions issued by 381 doctors (median 1538 prescriptions per doctor, interquartile range [IQR] 328–3275) were analysed. We identified 895,029 low-level alerts (median 1033 per 1000 prescriptions per doctor, IQR 903–1205) with a median of 34% (IQR 31–39%) heeded; 172,434 intermediate alerts (median 196 per 1000 prescriptions per doctor, IQR 159–266), with a median of 23% (IQR 16–30%) heeded; and 11,940 high-level ‘hard stop’ alerts. Doctors vary greatly in the extent to which they trigger and respond to alerts of different types. The rate of high-level alerts showed weak correlation with the rate of intermediate prescribing alerts (correlation coefficient, r = 0.40, P = <0.001); very weak correlation with low-level alerts (r = 0.12, P = 0.019); and showed weak (and sometimes negative) correlation with propensity to heed test-related warnings or alarms. The degree of correlation between generation of intermediate and high-level alerts is insufficient to identify doctors at high risk of making serious errors.

Conclusions

Routine data from an electronic prescribing system should not be used to identify doctors who are at risk of making serious errors. Careful evaluation of the kinds of quality assurance questions for which routine data are suitable will be increasingly valuable.

Introduction

The use of routine data for monitoring quality in health systems is well established. The advantages are many: the data are readily available and can be used at far less cost than prospectively designed studies.1 Much of the academic literature has focused on use of routine data for making comparisons across hospitals and enabling detection of variation in process measures and outcomes.2 The increasing use of large-scale information technology (IT) systems across healthcare presents new opportunities to use routine data to direct quality monitoring and improvement activities within organizations.

The absence of data on individual professional practice has been identified as a significant barrier to greater physician involvement in quality improvement3 yet the potential of using routine data for this purpose has remained largely unevaluated. An important question concerns whether some doctors are more likely to make serious errors than others.4 One possibility is that individuals who demonstrate a pattern of multiple low-level or moderate-level errors are at increased risk of making a higher-level, more consequential, error. If individuals at higher risk of making dangerous errors could be identified, they could be offered additional monitoring and support. Electronic systems that capture routine data about practitioners' behaviours provide opportunities for exploring whether different types of behaviours may be correlated with different outcomes.

Prescribing practice is a particularly good area in which to focus such study both because it is an important source of preventable harm57 and because prescribing errors with a low risk of resulting in harm are much more frequent, at a population level, than serious errors.8,9 We aimed to identify the extent to which routine prescribing data might be useful in identifying individuals who are at higher risk of making a serious prescribing error.

Methods

Setting and study population

The study was carried out in a large NHS Foundation Trust with two teaching hospital sites. The Trust has a locally-developed electronic prescribing system known as PICS (Prescribing, Information and Communication System), which is in use throughout all (approximately 1200) inpatient beds and for all prescribing except some chemotherapy regimens.

The system was first installed in the renal unit more than a decade ago,10 and now covers general and specialist medical and surgical specialties apart from obstetrics, paediatrics and mental health. A key feature of the system, for purposes of our study, is that it provides decision support by generating messages alerting prescribers to potential problems. Programmed into the system are approximately 5000 alerts for contraindications, 3400 alerts relating to dose limits, and 1800 for drug interactions. The algorithms that generate the rules are based on the British National Formulary (BNF),11 but are locally configured and updated regularly by a committee of medical and pharmacy specialists.

Computer-generated alerts relating to prescriptions are graded as follows (examples in Table 1):

  • Low-level alerts, where the user is requested to ‘tick a box’, indicating that the message has been considered;

  • Intermediate alerts, where the user must supply a password before the prescription can be continued;

  • High-level alerts, where the user is not permitted to continue with the prescription (‘hard stop’) and prescription is thus disallowed. We used these as a surrogate for major prescription errors with the potential to cause serious harm.

Table 1.

Examples of types of prescribing alerts with descriptions and hierarchy of alerts

Types of alerts
Hierarchy of alerts
Drug contraindication Drug-drug interaction Dose-range checking
Low-level (minor severity) Description Used when the presence of a coded contraindication (usually disease state) means that the use of the medicine could be harmful Used for combinations of medicines which in certain circumstances could cause harm Use of a drug probably outside clinically acceptable or usual limits for dose quantity or timing, but risk of harm not considered high
Specific system example Atorvastatin (a cholesterol-lowering drug) should be used caution with renal impairment due to the risk of side-effects of muscle inflammation The use of atenolol and diuretics (both drugs used in hypertension) together may cause low blood pressure Daily dose warning limit for beclometasone nose drops is 18 drops
Intermediate (moderate severity) Description Used when the presence of a coded contraindication (usually disease state) means that the use of the medicine may be potentially hazardous and requires avoidance or other precautions Used for potentially hazardous interactions of drugs which require avoidance or other special cautions Use of a drug outside clinically acceptable limits for dose quantity or timing based upon the likelihood of severe harm with that dose the usual dosing of that drug
Specific system example Aspirin should not be given in the presence of gastrointestinal ulceration due to the risk of bleeding Using methotrexate and ciclosporin (both immunosuppresants) together can lead to toxicity The infusion rate of vancomycin (antibiotic) is limited to 600 mg/h; a higher rate may cause ‘red man’ syndrome or hypotension
High-level messages (hard stop) Description Used when the presence of a coded contraindication (usually disease state) prohibits the prescription due to risk of harm Used for combinations of drugs that should be avoided in all situations Use of a drug significantly outside clinically acceptable limits for dose quantity or timing based upon the likelihood of serious harm with that dose
Specific system example Abciximab (a potent drug used to prevent thrombosis in cardiac patients) is not prescribable to patients with brain tumours due to high risk of bleeding The antibiotic erythromycin and the antipsychotic pimozide are disallowed in combination due to risk of potentially fatal cardiac rhythm abnormality Amphotericin (Abelcetan) – an antifungal drug – > 6 mg/kg is associated with severe toxicity at any higher weight-based doses

The system also prompts doctors to respond to selected abnormal laboratory test results, as follows:

  • Warnings: Most abnormal laboratory results generate no warning, but more seriously abnormal values, for example, a potassium concentration between 5.5 and 6.5 mmol/L, produces a warning when the doctor logs into a patient's record;

  • Alarms: The most serious abnormal values, such as a potassium concentration above 6.5 mmol/L, produce an interruptive alarm whenever doctors enter the electronic system (regardless of which patient they are viewing) to prompt them to affirm that they have noted and reacted to the specific abnormal result.

The system contains 415 distinct laboratory result warnings and 77 distinct alarms. When responding to them, doctors can either click a button on the electronic system to ‘accept’ the message (and thus show explicitly that they have acknowledged the clinical implications of the decision to proceed) or click a button to ‘ignore’ the message. PICS has a comprehensive audit database of actions, including all prescriptions, messages seen by doctors and responses to warnings or alarms.

In UK hospitals, most prescribing is done by junior doctors below the grade of registrar, and they were therefore chosen as the population of interest for this study. We used the database to evaluate associations, for each doctor, between numbers of low-level and high-level prescribing alerts, and between the numbers of intermediate and high-level alerts. Response to prescribing alerts was classified as either ‘heeding’, where a prescription was altered or abandoned so that the alert was no longer generated, or ‘overriding’, where the prescription proceeded to completion despite the alert. Similarly, response to laboratory messages was classified as either ‘accepting’ or ‘ignoring’.

Permission to perform this evaluation was obtained from the Clinical Governance Support Unit of the University Hospitals Birmingham NHS Foundation Trust, which deemed this study to be service evaluation not requiring research ethics committee approval.

Data capture

To ensure the study focused on a relatively stable group of doctors over the annual rotation, which begins each year in August, we analysed data from PICS between 8 August 2007 and 31 July 2008. Data were extracted for each junior doctor on all prescriptions generated by PICS, together with information on date and directorate (clinical service division), type and level of any alert, and whether the prescription was completed with or without modification. Only those doctors making more than 20 completed prescriptions during the study period were included (to exclude very short term attachments such as locum doctors prescribing during a few shifts only). All data were fully anonymized.

Information on completed prescriptions was matched to corresponding data on the alerts generated. So too was information on total numbers of completed prescriptions made by each doctor within each directorate. Where matching was not possible, data on such alerts or prescriptions were excluded. The number of laboratory warnings and alarms posted to the doctor and associated responses (accepting/ignoring) were also extracted from the database. All inpatient hospital directorates were included, with the exception of the oncology directorate, where PICS was not fully operational at the time.

Analysis

Rates of prescribing alerts for every 1000 completed prescriptions were calculated for each doctor, by grade of alert, type of warning (e.g. drug–drug interaction), and directorate. The slopes (with standard error and P value) and correlation coefficients for associations between rates of high-level alerts and rates of intermediate and of low-level alerts were evaluated using generalized linear models, weighted by the number of completed prescriptions made. Similar methods were used to measure associations between rates of high-level alerts and propensity to override intermediate alerts and low-level alerts. These methods were also used to measure association between prescribing alerts and ignoring a warning or alarm triggered by an abnormal laboratory result. To reduce errors due to multiple comparisons, P values of less than 0.01 were taken to indicate a statistically significant association.

Funnel plots of rates of high-level alerts against number of completed prescriptions were evaluated with 95% and 99% confidence bands in order to explore heterogeneity between doctors. Prescribers outside these confidence bands may be thought of as outliers, in the sense that they are at the extreme of what would be considered a normal high-level alert rate. To further explore associations between high-level alerts and intermediate or low-level alerts, indicator variables for doctors falling outside the 99% confidence bands for intermediate and low-level alerting rates were superimposed on the funnel plots. Results are presented for all directorates combined and for the three directorates where most prescriptions were completed.

Results

Altogether 849,153 completed prescriptions were issued and 1,094,693 prescribing alerts were generated by PICS during the one year study period. In total, 432 junior doctors used the system (and made 70.8% of all prescriptions in the Trust over this period), but data relating to 50 doctors who completed fewer than 20 prescriptions each over the study period were excluded, along with their 369 prescriptions and 419 alerts. Matching of one further doctor, 93 prescriptions, and 14,856 alerts proved technically impossible and related data were excluded. In addition, 13 prescriptions (associated with 15 alerts) that erroneously entered the database from the oncology directorate were excluded. Available for analysis therefore were 1,079,403 alerts relating to 381 doctors, who completed 848,678 prescriptions over the study period.

Prescribing alerts generated

Most (895,029; 83%) of the 1,079,403 prescribing alerts generated by PICS were low-level alerts. Fewer (172,424; 16%) were intermediate alerts. A very few (11,940; 1%) were high-level alerts indicative of a serious prescribing error.

Rates of heeding prescribing alerts

Of the 1,079,403 prescribing alerts, 352,025 (33%) overall were heeded (i.e. the prescription was abandoned or changed so that an alert was no longer generated). The remaining alerts were over-ridden. Of the 172,434 intermediate alerts, 38,007 (22%) were heeded, while of the 895,029 low-level alerts, 302,078 (34%) were heeded. All 11,940 high-level alerts had to be heeded as the alert could not be overridden (‘hard stop’ warnings).

Issues generating prescribing alerts

Of all prescribing alerts, 621,142 (58%) related to dose-range anomalies, 340,286 (31%) to drug–drug interaction messages, and 117,975 (11%) to contraindications (Table 2). For dose-range anomalies, 47% (99%CI 46.1–47.2%) of intermediate alerts and 45% (99%CI 45.0–45.3%) of low-level alerts were heeded. For contraindications, 32% (99%CI 30.0–33.6%) of intermediate alerts and 21% (99%CI 20.4–21.0%) of low-level alerts were heeded. For drug interactions, doctors were less likely to heed intermediate alerts (8.7% heeded; 99%CI 8.5–8.9%) than low-level alerts (12.8%, 99%CI 12.6–12.9%).

Table 2.

Alerts by type of anomaly

Alerts (n)
Heeded alerts (n)
Heeded alerts (%)
High-level Intermediate Low-level High-level Intermediate Low-level High-level Intermediate Low-level
Contraindications 908 4535 112,532 908 1443 23,291 100 32 21
Dose 10,529 57,807 552,806 10,529 26,965 249,503 100 47 45
Interaction 503 110,092 229,691 503 9599 29,284 100 9 13
All 11,940 172,434 895,029 11,940 38,007 302,078 100 22 34

High-level alerts accounted for only a very small proportion of each category: 908/117,975 (0.76%) of all alerts for contraindications; 503/340,286 (0.15%) for drug interactions; and 10,529/621,142 (1.7%) for dose-range anomalies.

Laboratory test result warnings and alarms

Doctors failed to acknowledge 206,580 of the 342,929 warnings (median percentage ignored per doctor 57%; IQR 28–88%) and 49,746 of the 60,062 alarms (median 90%; IQR 70–100%) relating to abnormal laboratory results.

Variations by directorate

The number of completed prescriptions varied considerably by directorate. For example, 274,774 completed prescriptions were issued over the period in the general medical directorate, compared with just 6,791 in the burns surgical directorate (Table 3). The rate of prescribing alerts per 1000 completed prescriptions also varied between directorates. For example, there were eight high-level alerts for every 1000 completed prescriptions in the vascular surgery directorate, compared with 34 per 1000 in the critical care directorate. Rates of low-level alerts varied from 837 per 1000 completed prescriptions in the ear, nose and throat directorate to 1451 in haematology. Rates of heeding of prescription warnings also varied by directorate, for example, 24% of 34,288 low-level warnings generated in the cardiothoracic surgical directorate were accepted, compared with 38% of 60,736 warnings generated in the cardiology medical directorate.

Table 3.

Alerts by directorate: number, rate and percentage heeded (ranked by high-level alerting rate)

Alerts (n)
Alert rate per 1000 scripts
Alerts heeded (%)
Directorate High-level Intermediate Low-level Scripts (n) High-level Intermediate Low-level High-level Intermediate Low-level
Critical Care 1571 16,604 64,467 45,934 34 361 1403 100 28 37
Burns Surgery 155 3328 7277 6791 23 490 1072 100 16 34
Trauma / Orthopaedics 1012 20,252 49,238 58,247 17 348 845 100 11 31
Maxillofacial surgery 242 2810 13,503 14,359 17 196 940 100 20 38
Plastics 315 4707 15,261 20,115 16 234 759 100 15 26
Liver 798 10,794 50,866 49,165 16 220 1035 100 15 31
Neurosciences 688 9352 40,634 46,367 15 202 876 100 20 34
Urology 437 5516 26,079 29,889 15 185 873 100 17 31
Surgery 1476 25,176 98,790 105,045 14 240 940 100 16 34
Medicine 3393 40,575 285,252 274,774 12 148 1038 100 27 35
Ear Nose Throat 236 2815 16,084 19,226 12 146 837 100 20 32
Haematology 268 3365 36,225 24,962 11 135 1451 100 18 34
Cardiothoracic 253 4993 34,288 24,197 10 206 1417 100 23 24
Renal 493 6587 71,858 57,443 9 115 1251 100 25 32
Vascular surgery 175 5602 24,471 20,770 8 270 1178 100 14 32
Cardiology 428 9958 60,736 51,394 8 194 1182 100 49 38
All directorates 11,940 172,434 895,029 848,678

Variation among doctors

The median number of low-level alerts per 1000 completed prescriptions per junior doctor was 1033 (IQR 903–1205), of which 34% (IQR 31–39) were heeded. The median number of intermediate alerts per junior doctor was lower, at 196 (IQR 159–266), of which 23% (IQR 16–30) were heeded. The median number of high-level alerts (resulting in the prescription being disallowed) was 13 per 1000 prescriptions (IQR 9–20).

A funnel plot of rate of high-level alerts against the number of completed prescriptions reveals considerable heterogeneity in rates of prescribing alerts between doctors (Figure 1). Outliers on the high-level alerts funnel plot were not consistently identified as outliers for intermediate or low-level alerts (Figure 1a). When stratified by directorate, funnel plots showed less heterogeneity and fewer outliers (Figure 1b–d), but again outliers for high-level alerts were not consistently identified as outliers for rates of low-level or intermediate alert.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Funnel plots of rates of high-level (hard stop) alerts against number of prescriptions. Rates of high-level (hard stop) alerts per 1000 completed prescriptions with mean (solid line) and 95 (dashed lines) and 99 (dotted lines) percent confidence bands. Doctors whose rate of intermediate (password) alerts exceeds the corresponding 95% confidence band are marked with ‘x’ and doctors whose rate of low-level (tickbox) alerts exceeds the 95% confidence band are marked with a solid dot. a. All Directorates. b. General Surgery Directorate. c. Trauma/Orthopaedic Directorate. d. General Medicine Directorate

Rates of intermediate alerts were statistically associated with the rate of high-level alerts (Table 4), but the correlation was weak (r = 0.40, P = 0.000), and the correlation between rates of low-level and high-level alerts was even weaker (r = 0.12, P = 0.019). Correlations between rates of high-level alerts and rates of not heeding low-level or intermediate alerts were also low, (or even negative), ranging from 0.04 to –0.22 across directorates.

Table 4.

Association between rates of hard stop alerts and (a) intermediate and low-level alerts, (b) intermediate and low-level alerts that were not heeded and (c) laboratory warnings and alarms not heeded

All directorates
Surgery Directorate
Trauma and Orthopaedics Directorate
Medical Directorate
Slope (SE) P value Correlation Slope (SE) P value Correlation Slope (SE) P value Correlation Slope (SE) P value Correlation
Rate intermediate alerts 0.045 (0.005) 0.000 0.40 0.016 (0.007) 0.030 0.15 0.015 (0.006) 0.001 0.19 0.022 (0.011) 0.046 0.12
Rate low-level alerts 0.005 (0.002) 0.019 0.12 0.006 (0.004) 0.138 0.10 0.006 (0.004) 0.220 0.09 0.002 (0.003) 0.494 0.05
Intermediate alerts not heeded (%) 0.031 (0.043) 0.469 0.04 −0.543 (0.188) 0.004 −0.22 −0.104 (0.150) 0.518 −0.06 0.008 (0.063) 0.904 0.01
Low-level alerts not heeded (%) −0.318 (0.087) 0.000 −0.19 −0.566 (0.210) 0.008 −0.19 −0.223 (0.179) 0.214 −0.10 −0.194 (0.105) 0.066 −0.11
Laboratory warnings not heeded (%) −0.002 (0.001) 0.226 −0.06 −0.009 (0.010) 0.377 −0.06 −0.000 (0.003) 0.882 −0.01 −0.002 (0.001) 0.324 −0.06
Laboratory alarms not heeded (%) −0.004 (0.002) 0.044 −0.11 −0.016 (0.012) 0.193 −0.09 −0.007 (0.004) 0.091 −0.13 −0.005 (0.002) 0.005 −0.18

P values significant at the 99% level are in bold italics

P values significant at the 95% level are in bold

Correlations between rates of failure to heed intermediate alerts and failure to heed low-level alerts were weak (ranging from 0.21 to 0.50 between directorates) (Table 5). Not heeding prescription-associated alerts correlated poorly, and sometimes negatively, with ignoring laboratory warnings or alarms (Table 5). Analysis of prescribing alerts by category – dose-range anomaly, interaction and contraindication – gave broadly similar findings (Table 6).

Table 5.

Association between not heeding prescription alerts and not heeding laboratory warnings and alarms

All directorates
Surgery Directorate
Trauma and Orthopaedics Directorate
Medical Directorate
Slope (SE) P value Correlation Slope (SE) P value Correlation Slope (SE) P value Correlation Slope (SE) P value Correlation
Associations with ignoring intermediate alerts
Low-level prescribing alerts not heeded (%) 1.041 (0.094) 0.000 0.50 0.313 (0.111) 0.005 0.21 0.429 (0.104) 0.000 0.37 0.682 (0.106) 0.000 0.40
Laboratory warnings not heeded (%) 0.003 (0.002) 0.080 0.09 0.002 (0.002) 0.239 0.09 −0.007 (0.002) 0.000 −0.34 −0.001 (0.002) 0.712 −0.02
Laboratory alarms not heeded (%) −0.001 (0.002) 0.816 −0.01 −0.000 (0.002) 0.936 −0.00 −0.007 (0.003) 0.019 −0.23 0.005 (0.002) 0.012 0.18
Associations with ignoring low-level alerts
Laboratory warnings not heeded (%) 0.002 (0.001) 0.003 0.15 0.000 (0.001) 0.497 0.05 −0.000 (0.001) 0.954 −0.03 0.002 (0.000) 0.021 0.14
Laboratory alarms not heeded (%) 0.001 (0.002) 0.675 0.02 0.001 (0.001) 0.282 0.08 −0.000 (0.002) 0.790 −0.02 0.002 (0.001) 0.090 0.11

P values significant at the 99% level are in bold italics

P values significant at the 95% level are in bold

Slope is the coefficient from a fitted generalized linear model, modelling the linear association between rate of hard stop alerts and each of the listed independent variables within the table

Table 6.

Association between rates of high-level (hard stop) alerts and intermediate and low-level alerts by category of warning

Interactions
Dose
Contraindications
Slope (SE) P value Correlation Slope (SE) P value Correlation Slope (SE) P value Correlation
Rate intermediate alerts 0.000 (0.001) 0.000 0.23 0.038 (0.010) 0.000 0.19 −0.027 (0.015) 0.080 −0.09
Rate low-level alerts −0.001 (0.000) 0.079 −0.09 0.015 (0.003) 0.000 0.29 −0.000 (0.001) 0.806 −0.01
Intermediate alerts not heeded (%) 0.008 (0.008) 0.021 0.12 −0.010 (0.038) 0.802 −0.01 −0.000 (0.003) 0.748 −0.02
Low-level alerts not heeded (%) 0.013 (0.008) 0.021 0.10 −0.108 (0.081) 0.182 −0.07 −0.012 (0.008) 0.105 −0.08
Laboratory warnings not heeded (%) −0.000 (0.000) 0.028 −0.11 −0.001 (0.001) 0.359 −0.05 −0.000 (0.000) 0.342 −0.05
Laboratory alarms not heeded (%) −0.000 (0.000) 0.135 −0.08 −0.003 (0.002) 0.053 −0.11 −0.000 (0.000) 0.654 −0.02

P values significant at the 99% level are in bold italics

P values significant at the 95% level are in bold

Slope is the coefficient from a fitted generalized linear model, modelling the linear association between rate of hard stop alerts and each of the listed independent variables within the table

Discussion

This single NHS foundation trust generated over a million prescribing alerts in the one-year period. Only 1% of prescriptions produced high-level alerts, but this amounts to over 10,000 such instances per year. If high-level alerts are reasonable surrogates for serious prescribing errors, then error in hospitals is frequent, as others have also found.12,13 In targeting serious errors, it would of course be useful if it were possible to screen or monitor individuals most at risk of making a catastrophic prescribing error. Our data suggest that some doctors trigger intermediate alerts more frequently than others, and some doctors trigger high-level alerts more frequently than others, but these ‘outliers’ tend not to be the same doctors, and nor is there a relationship between tendency to provoke alerts or warnings/alarms and ‘heeding’ behaviour in response. Our analysis suggests that it is not possible to use routine prescribing data recording behaviour in relation to alerts, warnings and alarms to identify doctors who are more likely to generate an alert indicative of a serious prescribing error.

Our study does have a number of limitations. It was conducted in a single NHS Teaching Hospital Trust, using a specific computer system. Other IT systems might produce different findings, and it may be useful to replicate the same study in other systems and to use methods of triangulation to assess more holistically issues of individual variation in prescribing behaviour.

Our data reveal considerable variation between doctors in rates of high-level alerts: some doctors generate such alerts frequently, while others do so infrequently. Similarly, doctors vary widely in rates of low and intermediate level alerts they generate, and in rates of generated alerts that they heed. The number of high-level alerts a doctor generated correlates only weakly with the number of intermediate alerts, or the number of low-level alerts the same doctor generates. Furthermore, there is little or no correlation between the number of prescribing alerts of any grade a doctor generates, and the doctor's propensity to heed intermediate or low-level alerts. This means that doctors who are at highest risk of making serious prescribing errors, as reflected by triggering high-level alerts, cannot be identified from the rate of low-level or intermediate alerts they generate, or from whether they heed them. One interpretation is that the search for the phenotype of a generally error-prone person may prove as elusive in medicine as it has been elsewhere,1419 but the available data do not allow more certain conclusions to be drawn.

More generally our analysis demonstrates some of the limitations of using routine data from a computerized prescribing system as a way of detecting ‘error’ or aspects of individual performance. The extent to which different levels of alert are reasonable surrogates for varying gravity of error is, for example, poorly understood. Doctors routinely override prescribing alerts20 as most can be safely ignored. Our analysis suggests that there are clear risks of using electronic traces of rule-breaking (such as apparent breaches of protocols or overrides of warning recorded on IT systems) as an indication that something that might injure a patient has occurred. For example, we found no association between doctors breaking the rule that a box should be ticked to confirm that an abnormal laboratory result has been noted and the chance that they will make a serious prescribing error indicated by a ‘hard stop’ on the computer system. Here not only is there no positive correlation, but the correlations turn negative. Such an observation supports the possibility that some rule-following may be little more than a ritualised display of compliance rather more than a signal of safe practice. This finding points to the possible risks of using data from hospital IT systems to try to alter behaviour. For example, our data provide some indications that encouraging doctors to respond to low-level computer-generated warnings will have little impact on major errors, and risks diverting efforts from preventing low-frequency high-harm events to events that are high-frequency but low-harm or no-harm. Further, encouraging staff to respond to such prompts, which they may see as purely bureaucratic, could damage the overall legitimacy of the patient safety enterprise.

IT systems are widely and increasingly used to collect routine data in healthcare organizations. Organizations may see these data as a potential source of quality assurance information, but need to consider what criteria need to be met before acting on it. Evaluations of what kinds of questions such systems should be used for will become increasingly valuable. There is little evidence, on the basis of our analysis, that routine prescribing systems can or should be used to detect doctors who are more likely to make a serious prescribing error. Though caution is required in this particular application, use of routine data from such systems may, nonetheless, have an important role in monitoring quality in healthcare organizations, and future studies should investigate their potential in this area.

DECLARATIONS

Competing interests

All authors have completed the Unified Competing Interest form at www.icmje.org/coi_disclosure.pdf (available on request from the corresponding author) (JJC). JJC, IRC, and PGN work within the University Hospital Birmingham NHS Foundation Trust which is collaborating with CSE Healthcare Systems to commercialise the PICS system in the UK. All other authors report no financial relationships with commercial entities that might have an interest in the submitted work; no spouses, partners, or children of the authors have relationships with commercial entities that might have an interest in the submitted work; none of the authors have non-financial interests that may be relevant to the submitted work.

Funding

The National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Collaborations for Leadership in Applied Health Research and Care (CLAHRC) for Birmingham and Black Country and the Department of Health Policy Research Programme supported the undertaking of this work. The views expressed in this work do not necessarily reflect those of the funders

Ethical approval

Not applicable

Guarantor

JJC

Contributorship

All authors had full access to statistical reports and tables in the study and take responsibility for the accuracy of the data analysis. All authors contributed to the writing of the manuscript, the interpretation of data, and approved the final version. KH, PGN and IRC had access to the raw data

Acknowledgements

The authors are grateful for the support of the Birmingham Clinical Research Academy. The authors thank Dave Thompson and Ian Young for assistance with data extraction from the audit database. The authors are also grateful to James Reason and Frank Davidoff for their independent reviews and discussion of the contents of the manuscript

REFERENCES

  • 1.Powell AE, Davies HTO, Thomson RG Using routine comparative data to assess the quality of healthcare: understanding and avoiding common pitfalls. Qual Safety Health Care 2003;12:122–8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Marshall MN, Shekelle P, Davies HTO, Smith PC Public reporting on quality in the United States and the United Kingdom. Health Affairs 2003;22:134–48 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Davies H Measuring and reporting the quality of care: issues and evidence from the international research literature. Edinburgh: NHS Quality Improvement Scotland, 2006. See http://www.nhsqis.org.uk/nhsqis/files/Davies%20Paper.pdf [Google Scholar]
  • 4.McManus C, Vincent C Selecting and educating safer doctors. : Vincent C, Ennis M, Audley R, Medical Accidents. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2003 [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Dornan T, Ashcroft D, Heathfield H, et al. An in depth investigation into causes of prescribing errors by foundation trainees in relation to their medical education – EQUIP Study London: General Medical Council, 2009 [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Leape LL, Brennan TA, Laird N, et al. The nature of adverse events in hospitalized patients. Results of the Harvard medical practice study II. N Engl J Med 1991;324:377–84 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Pirmohamed M, James S, Meakin S, et al. Adverse drug reactions as cause of admission to hospital: prospective analysis of 18 820 patients. BMJ 2004;329:15–19 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Dobrzanski S, Hammond I, Khan G, Holdsworth H The nature of hospital prescribing errors. Brit J Clin Govern 2002;7:187–93 [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Ferner RE The epidemiology of medication errors: the methodological difficulties. Brit J Clin Pharmacol 2009;67:614–20 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Nightingale PG, Adu D, Richards NT, Peters M Implementation of rules based computerised bedside prescribing and administration: intervention study. BMJ 2000;320:750–3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.British Medical Association/Royal Pharmaceutical Society of Great Britain British National Formulary No. 60. London: BMA, 2010 [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Lin CP, Payne TH, Nichol WP, Hoey PJ, Anderson CL, Gennari JH Evaluating clinical decision support systems: monitoring CPOE order check override rates in the Department of Veterans Affairs' Computerized Patient Record System. J Am Med Inform Assoc 2008;15:620–6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Van Der Sijs H, Aarts J, Vulto A, Berg M Overriding of drug safety alerts in computerized physician order entry. J Am Med Inform Assoc 2006;13:138–47 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Lawton R, Parker D Individual differences in accident liability: A review and integrative approach. Hum Factors 1998;40:655–71 [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Lawton R, Parker D, Manstead ASR, Stradling SG The role of affect in predicting social behaviours: the case of road traffic violations. J Appl Soc Psychol 1997;27:1258–76 [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Booysen AE, Erasmus JAK The relationship between some personality factors and accident risk. S Afr J Psychol 1989;19:144–52 [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Hilakivi I, Veilahti J, Asplund P, Sinivuo J, Laitinen L, Koskenvuo K A 16-factor personality test for predicting automobile driving accidents of young drivers. Accident Anal Prev 1989;21:413–18 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Elander J, West R, French D Behavioural-correlates of individual-differences in road-traffic crash risk – an examination of methods and findings. Psychol Bull 1993;113:279–94 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Lardent CL Pilots who crash – personality constructs underlying accident prone behaviour of fighter pilots. Multivar Exp Clin Res 1991;10:1–25 [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Isaac T, Weissman JS, Davis RB, et al. Overrides of medication alerts in ambulatory care. Arch Intern Med 2009;169:305–11 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine are provided here courtesy of Royal Society of Medicine Press

RESOURCES