Skip to main content
The Gerontologist logoLink to The Gerontologist
. 2010 Nov 19;51(3):379–388. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnq094

Social Network Type and Subjective Well-being in a National Sample of Older Americans

Howard Litwin 1,*, Sharon Shiovitz-Ezra 2
PMCID: PMC3095651  PMID: 21097553

Abstract

Purpose: The study considers the social networks of older Americans, a population for whom there have been few studies of social network type. It also examines associations between network types and well-being indicators: loneliness, anxiety, and happiness. Design and Methods: A subsample of persons aged 65 years and older from the first wave of the National Social Life, Health, and Aging Project was employed (N = 1,462). We applied K-means cluster analysis to derive social network types using 7 criterion variables. In the multivariate stage, the well-being outcomes were regressed on the network type construct and on background and health characteristics by means of logistic regression. Results: Five social network types were derived: “diverse,” “friend,” “congregant,” “family,” and “restricted.” Social network type was found to be associated with each of the well-being indicators after adjusting for demographic and health confounders. Respondents embedded in network types characterized by greater social capital tended to exhibit better well-being in terms of less loneliness, less anxiety, and greater happiness. Implications: Knowledge about differing network types should make gerontological practitioners more aware of the varying interpersonal milieus in which older people function. Adopting network type assessment as an integral part of intake procedures and tracing network shifts over time can serve as a basis for risk assessment as well as a means for determining the efficacy of interventions.

Keywords: Social relations, NSHAP, Loneliness, Anxiety, Happiness, Social work


Social network type is an important indicator of social capital—“the array of social contacts that give access to social, emotional, and practical support” (Gray, 2009, 6). Network type is a composite characterization of the interpersonal milieu in which people are embedded. The measurement of network type and the analysis of its role in promoting well-being among older adults should be of concern to gerontologists, and particularly to practitioners who serve the older population. However, there is still only limited attention paid to the notion of social network type in the gerontological literature.

Studies of Social Network Type

Groundbreaking social network studies in England and Wales initially derived five social network types (Wenger, 1991) that effectively identified community-dwelling older people at risk (Wenger, 1997) and later served as diagnostic criteria for gerontological social work practice (Wenger & Tucker, 2002). Subsequent analysis of survey data on older adults residing in the community in Israel derived five slightly different social network groupings termed diverse, friend focused, neighbor focused, family focused, and restricted (Litwin, 2001). The respective network types mainly reflected variations in social network composition. It was found, moreover, that the networks with a wider range of social ties, such as the diverse and friend-focused network types, had the best outcomes. In comparison, the social network grouping with the most limited ties—the restricted network—revealed the poorest mental health, the least physical activity, and the greatest seven-year mortality (Litwin, 2003; Litwin & Shiovitz-Ezra, 2006).

Efforts to replicate the Israeli typology in an American sample have been recently reported. Analysis of data from the Americans’ Changing Lives study derived diverse, family, friends, and restricted social network types, with some variation—that is, two types of restricted networks were discerned (Fiori, Antonucci, & Cortina, 2006). As noted, restricted network types are those with the most limited extent of social ties. In the American study, depressive symptomatology was highest for individuals embedded in non-friends restricted networks and lowest for those who maintained diverse networks.

Similar analyses of data from a study of older people in the United States and Japan derived mostly similar network types (Fiori, Antonucci, & Akiyama, 2008). However, although the network types were associated with well-being in the American sample in the study, they were not related in the Japanese sample. In a recent Korean study of older adults, a simpler social network typology was identified and an association between network type and well-being was discerned. Those in diverse networks reported the best health and those in isolated networks reported the worst (Cheon, 2010).

Analysis of data from the Australian Longitudinal Study of Aging (ALSA) found that social networks rich in friends were protective against 10-year mortality among 70-year olds (Giles, Glonek, Luszcz, & Andrews, 2005). However, the ALSA study considered characteristics of network composition rather than composite social network types per se. An analysis of data from the Berlin Aging Study derived six multidimensional network types, taking into account network structure, function, and quality. The investigators found a relationship between the resultant network types and depressive symptoms, subjective well-being, and morbidity (Fiori, Smith, & Antonucci, 2007).

Well-being

The construct of well-being reflects a rather large collection of states that range from objective functional health to subjective perceptions of life quality (George, 2010). Subjective well-being most often refers to a positive orientation toward life and is generally based upon such feelings as happiness, morale, positive affect, and life satisfaction. Research has shown that these various measures are mostly interrelated and that they frequently constitute a single dimension (Slocum-Gori, Zumbo, Michalos, & Diener, 2009). Negative affect is also addressed by investigators as an independent measure of well-being (Ranzijn & Luszcz, 2000).

The indicators of subjective well-being in late life are associated with several background factors. Meta analyses show that age, income, and education are positively related to subjective well-being, whereas female gender is negatively related (Pinquart & Sorensen, 2000, 2001). Blacks (Yang, 2008) and Hispanics (Barger, Donoho, & Wayment, 2009) score lower than Whites on various subjective well-being measures. Health has emerged as the primary correlate, but the principal health measure—self-rated health—may confound with the subjective well-being outcome (Pinquart & Sorensen, 2000).

The current analysis focuses upon three selected indicators of subjective well-being: loneliness, anxiety, and happiness. Loneliness is a discrete, subjective construct that reflects the perceived discrepancy between desired and achieved social relationships (Peplau & Perlman, 1982). In the United States, some 17% of people aged 50 years and above report loneliness (Theeke, 2010). Studies of older people show that loneliness is negatively associated with emotional well-being (Lee & Ishii-Kuntz, 1987), a predictor of cognitive decline (Wilson et al., 2007) and a risk factor for depressive symptoms (Cacioppo, Hughes, Waite, Hawkley, & Thisted, 2006). Furthermore, loneliness poses substantial mortality risk, even after controlling for demographic and clinical characteristics (Shiovitz-Ezra & Ayalon, 2010).

Although anxiety is highly comorbid with depression, it constitutes a distinct indicator of poor mental health. Moreover, anxiety is associated with medical illness, cognitive decline, disability, sleep disturbance, and hospitalization, all matters of concern to older people (Brenes et al., 2009; Kvaal & Laake, 2003; Wolitzky-Taylor, Castriotta, Lenze, Stanley, & Craske, 2010). Happiness is a common outcome measure employed in studies of subjective well-being (Baker, Cahalin, Gerst, & Burr, 2005; McAuley et al., 2000; Menec, 2003; Theurer & Wister, 2010). Surveys indicate that the vast majority of American adults are happy (George, 2010). Moreover, older adults, particularly men, tend to report higher levels of happiness (Yang, 2008), whereas health impairment is an inverse predictor of happiness among centenarians (Bishop, Martin, & Poon, 2006).

Social Network, Loneliness, Anxiety, and Happiness

There is little reported research on the association between social network types among older adults and loneliness, anxiety, or happiness. However, analyses based upon data from the Framingham Heart Study seem to suggest that loneliness and happiness may both be products of one's social network. It was found that people tended to cluster by the traits and states of members (happiness or loneliness, respectively) within three degrees of their network. Moreover, happy networks seemed to make their members happier and lonely networks further isolated lonely people (Cacioppo, Fowler, & Christakis, 2009; Fowler & Christakis, 2008).

A study in Dublin examined the Wenger social network typology in relation to anxiety and depression among very old persons (Golden et al., 2009). The analysis considered the effects of loneliness and network type separately. It found that loneliness accounted for most of the depressed mood, but that network type was an independent predictor as well, albeit to a lesser degree. A longitudinal study of very old people in the Kungsholmen District of Stockholm, in Sweden, found that anxiety among those with no previous psychiatric disorder was associated with having an insufficient network, that is, no children and no regular visitors (Forsell, 2000).

Research Questions

Based upon the preceding review, the study reported here addresses three major questions. Firstly, can a robust typology of social network types indeed be discerned among older Americans? Secondly, are social network types among older Americans unique to this population, or do they reflect the kinds of social networks that are found in other settings? Thirdly, does the social network type in which one is embedded really matter, that is to say, is there a significant association between network type and well-being among older Americans?

Design and Methods

The analysis is based upon data from the first wave of the National Social Life, Health, and Aging Project (NSHAP), a survey that examines health and interpersonal connections among older Americans. Generated using the field operation employed for the 2004 wave of the Health and Retirement Study, the NSHAP sample is representative of the population of noninstitutionalized Americans aged 57–85 years (Suzman, 2009). Data collection was executed in 2005-06, yielding 3,005 respondents. African Americans, Latinos, men, and the older age group were oversampled in order to provide adequate representation of these subgroups (O’Muircheartaigh, Eckman & Smith, 2009).

The main NSHAP instrument was a 2-hr in-home computer-assisted personal interview (CAPI) conducted in English or Spanish with a weighted response rate of 75.5%. Data collection included a brief self-administrated post-interview questionnaire, the response rate for which was about 84% (Smith et al., 2009). The current analysis focused on respondents aged 65 years and older within the NSHAP sample and included only those who completed both the CAPI interview and the self-administrated questionnaire. The resultant analytic sample was N = 1,462. Given that we addressed a selected part of the sample only and sought primarily to derive social network types and to clarify the relationships between selected variables, we did not employ the weights that are available for the entire NSHAP sample.

Study Variables

As in previous analyses, “network type” was derived through the application of K-means cluster analysis to the following social capital variables relevant to older adults: current marital status, number of children, number of close relatives, number of friends, frequency of getting together with neighbors, frequency of attendance at religious services, and frequency of attendance at organized group meetings (Gray, 2009; Litwin, 2001). These particular variables reflect the main components of the social networks of older people as they are reported in the social support literature (Berkman & Syme, 1979; Lubben et al., 2006; Wenger, 1991).

Current marital status was measured as a dichotomy: 1 = married or living with a partner and 0 = other. Number of children was a simple count (0–6) in which the final category reflected six or more children. Number of close relatives and number of friends were both measured on a 6-point scale, as it was solicited in the original NSHAP questionnaire (0 = none, 1 = 1, 2 = 2-3, 3 = 4-9, 4 = 10-20, and 5 = more than 20). Frequency of getting together with neighbors was tapped on a 5-point scale ranging from hardly ever (1) to daily or almost daily (5). Frequency of attendance at religious services and at organized group meetings were also measured on 5-point scales ranging from never (0) to weekly or more (4). The frequency ratings for attendance at religious services and organized group meetings were asked in relation to the previous 12 months, whereas the frequency of neighbor contact was queried in general.

The dependent variable in the analysis was well-being, as measured on three separate constructs: loneliness, anxiety, and happiness. The loneliness measure was the specific item in the Center for Epidemiological Studies—Depression scale that asks how often the respondent felt lonely in the previous week. We collapsed the 4-point answer scale into a dichotomous response (0 = rarely or never felt lonely and 1 = felt lonely sometimes or more often).

Anxiety was tapped by a modified version of the seven-item anxiety subscale of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression scale (Zigmond & Snaith, 1983). Respondents report on feelings of anxious mood, thoughts, and restlessness over the past week, with higher values indicating higher anxiety levels. The modified anxiety measure achieved satisfactory concurrent validity and significant correlations with other quality of life indicators in NSHAP (Shiovitz-Ezra et al., 2009). The total score for the anxiety subscale ranges from 0 to 21. The suggested cutoff point for the presence of possible anxiety is between 7 and 8 (Herrmann, 1997). We employed this cutoff to create a dichotomous anxiety measure.

Happiness was measured by a single probe: “If you were to consider your life in general these days, how happy or unhappy would you say you are on the whole … extremely happy, very happy, pretty happy, unhappy sometimes, or unhappy usually”? This question was adopted by NSHAP in light of its equivalence to an item from the General Social Survey, which established concurrent validity, and its use in several different studies (Kousha & Mohseni, 2000; Maselko & Kubzansky, 2006). It was dichotomized here to distinguish between very happy or more (1) and pretty happy or less (0).

Correlates included sociodemographic background and health. The background variables—age (65-74, 75-85), gender (men/women), education (less than high school, high school, some college, bachelor's degree or more), ethnicity (White, Black, Hispanic, and others), and subjective income—were gathered through self-report. In the case of subjective income, the subjects were asked “compared with American families in general, would you say that your household income is far below average, below average, average, above average or far above average.” Subjective income has been found elsewhere to constitute a robust reflection of economic status (Litwin & Sapir, 2009).

Health was measured in terms of respondents’ functional capacity, as reflected in the difficulty experienced with six basic activities of daily living (ADL): walking across a room, dressing, bathing, eating, getting in or out of bed, and using the toilet. Measured on a 4-point ordinal scale ranging from no difficulty to unable to do so, we dichotomized the functional health indicator to reflect no ADL difficulty and one or more such difficulties.

Analysis

In order to test the association between social network type and well-being, bivariate and multivariate analyses were executed. Cross-tabulations were carried out between network type and the three dichotomous well-being outcomes. Bivariate associations were also examined between the correlates and the outcome measures and between the correlates and the network type construct. The chi-square statistic was consulted in each case. In the multivariate stage of the analysis, the well-being outcomes were regressed on the collection of study variables by means of logistic regression. Odds ratios were computed to show the relative likelihood that people with a given characteristic felt lonely, anxious, or happy. The reference categories for each variable in the analysis are indicated in the appropriate table.

Results

Table 1 presents the characteristics of the social network types that were identified through the clustering process. The results of post hoc group comparisons that were performed on each of the delineating characteristics, using the Tukey HSD test, are also shown in the table. The numbers that appear in bold italics constitute the subsets with the highest values whereas the numbers that are underlined constitute the subsets with the lowest values.

Table 1.

Network Delineation Criteria and Social Network Type Distribution Among Older Americans: Cluster Analysis

Values of Criterion Variablesa
Marriedb Number of Childrenb Number of close relativesb Number of friendsb Get-together with neighborsb Attend religious servicesb Attend organized group meetingsb
Meanc Meanc Meanc Meanc Meanc Meanc Meanc
Network type
    Diverse 0.67 5.14 3.52 3.76 2.64 3.73 2.43
    Friend 0.57 2.17 2.93 3.79 2.58 3.56 3.56
    Congregant 0.58 2.16 2.98 3.20 2.48 3.60 0.58
    Family 0.61 4.55 2.80 2.30 2.15 1.21 0.74
    Restricted 0.55 1.61 2.43 3.05 2.29 0.50 1.29
    Total 0.59 2.96 2.92 3.30 2.44 2.56 1.92
a

Variable scale ranges: married (0-1), number of children (0-6), number of close relatives (0-5), number of friends (0-5), get-together with neighbors (1-5), attend religious services (0-4), and attend organized group meetings (0-4).

b

Analysis of variance: married (F = 2.55, p < .04), number of children (F = 685.10, p < .00), number of close relatives (F = 46.14, p < .00), number of friends (F = 71.33, p < .00), get-together with neighbors (F = 6.66, p < .00), attend religious services (F = 1009.05, p < .00), and attend organized group meetings (F = 423.99, p < .00).

c

Post hoc group comparisons—Tukey HSD test: numbers that appear in bold italics (e.g., 0.67) constitute subsets with the highest values; numbers that appear underlined (e.g., 0.55) constitute subsets with the lowest values.

As may be seen in the table, five network types were derived. The “diverse network” in the NSHAP sample demonstrated the greatest degree of sociability, on the whole. Its members had the greatest percentage of married members as well as the greatest number of children and close family members, the highest degree of neighbor get-togethers, and the most frequent attendance at religious services. Respondents in this network type also exhibited a considerable number of friends and fairly frequent attendance at organized group meetings.

Respondents clustered in the grouping termed “friend network” reported the greatest number of friends and the most frequent attendance at organized group meetings, as well as relatively frequent attendance at religious services. The social strengths of the friend network, therefore, were in the extra-familial ties maintained by its members. In comparison, those included in the “congregant network” type also had frequent attendance at religious services but were indistinguishable on most of the other clustering criteria. They also showed the lowest rate of attendance at organized group meetings. It can be assumed for these reasons that the people in the congregant network engaged in social exchange primarily with other congregants at their place of worship.

The next grouping, labeled “family network,” was characterized by its relatively high number of children, on average, and by the relative dearth of other kinds of ties in the entourage, especially extrafamilial ties. It seems that respondents in the family network type could count mainly on their children. Finally, the members of the “restricted network” cluster in the NSHAP sample reported the lowest scores on four of the seven criterion variables. Moreover, the remaining three criteria, on which they showed only mid-range rankings, were all extra-familial in nature. Respondents in the restricted network type had the least social capital and were most at risk in terms of sociability.

The frequency distributions of the social network types show that almost one fifth of the sample was embedded in a diverse network (Table 2). Friend networks accounted for more than a quarter. About a sixth was in congregant networks and a bit less in family networks. More than a fifth of sample respondents belonged to restricted networks. As for the well-being outcomes, about a third of all respondents felt lonely in the previous week, some 13% felt anxious and a bit more than half were very happy in general.

Table 2.

The Well-being of Older Americans by Social Network Type and Background Characteristics: Fequenciesa and Cross-tabulations (χ2)

Frequencies
Lonely
Anxious
Happy
Variables Categories N % % χ2 % χ2 % χ2
Network Diverse 276 18.9 28.3 9.9* 9.6 13.4* 56.9 17.5**
Friend 398 27.2 27.9 9.4 61.8
Congregant 239 16.3 31.9 13.2 55.9
Family 220 15.0 39.1 14.6 51.5
Restricted 329 22.5 32.8 17.3 47.1
Background
    Age (years) 65-74 824 56.4 29.0 4.9* 11.2 3.3+ 58.7 10.4**
75-85 638 43.6 34.5 14.4 50.2
    Gender Men 682 46.6 24.7 27.0*** 9.9 8.4** 61.1 19.1***
Women 780 53.4 37.3 15.0 49.7
    Ethnic group White 1,102 75.6 29.3 9.6* 11.1 10.8* 53.8 2.9
Black 195 13.4 36.4 16.8 58.5
Hispanic 131 9.0 40.5 19.5 59.5
Other 29 2.0 34.5 10.7 58.6
    Education Less than high school 333 22.8 43.8 48.8*** 18.0 20.1*** 48.5 12.3**
High school 409 28.0 32.1 13.1 54.3
Vocational/some college 434 29.7 30.2 12.5 55.8
BA or more 286 19.6 17.8 6.0 62.5
    Income Far below average 166 11.7 45.8 42.5*** 16.7 19.9** 42.2 38.7***
Below average 364 25.7 36.4 17.0 50.4
Average 596 42.0 30.4 12.0 54.6
Above average 244 17.2 18.0 5.8 68.4
Far above average 49 3.5 22.4 8.3 73.5
    Health No ADL difficulty 1,045 71.6 28.3 16.3*** 9.6 30.2*** 59.0 22.9***
One + ADL 414 28.4 39.1 20.3 45.2
    Total 1,462a 100.0 32.0 12.6 55.4

Note: ADL = activities of daily living.

a

Due to missing data, category totals vary: ethnic group, N = 1,457; income, N = 1,419; health, N = 1,459; and all other variables, N = 1,462.

+.05 > p < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

At the bivariate level, social network type was found to be related to all three well-being outcome measures. The vast majority of background and health variables were also related to loneliness, anxiety, and happiness. For example, lower education was related to greater loneliness, greater anxiety, and less happiness. Additional bivariate analyses (not shown) revealed that the respondents’ background and health characteristics were also related to the network types: age (χ2 = 14.9, p < .01), gender (χ2 = 28.5, p < .001), ethnic group (χ2 = 74.5, p < .001), education (χ2 = 81.4, p < .001), income (χ2 = 46.6, p < .001), and health (χ2 = 32.2, p < .01). Thus, for example, more high income respondents belonged to friend networks than to other network types, and those in the family network had worse functional health than respondents in the other networks. The bivariate analyses substantiated the need to control for the background and health characteristics in examination of the association between social network type and well-being.

Table 3 presents the multivariate regressions in which each of the dichotomous well-being outcomes was regressed on the network, background, and health variables. Looking first at the background and health characteristics, the following associations were observed. Female respondents felt lonelier than men, had greater anxiety, and were less happy. The same was true for respondents with one or more functional health disabilities when compared with those with no disability. Other findings showed that those with less education were lonelier than persons with college degrees and those with lower income were less happy than persons in the high-income reference category. Finally, the young–old reported being happier than the old–old (but not feeling lonelier or more anxious). Blacks and Hispanics expressed a greater degree of anxiety when compared with Whites.

Table 3.

Background Characteristics, Social Network Type, and Well-being Among Persons Aged 65+ Years in the NSHAP Sample: Logistic Regressions

Lonelya(N = 1,410) Anxiousa(N = 1,390) Happya(N = 1,410)
Variables Categoriesb OR 95% CI OR 95% CI OR 95% CI
Network Diverse 0.69* 0.47, 1.00 0.42** 0.25, 0.71 1.38+ 0.98, 1.95
Friend 0.81 0.58, 1.14 0.49** 0.31, 0.79 1.82*** 1.32, 2.49
Congregant 0.77 0.53, 1.14 0.54* 0.32, 0.92 1.70*** 1.19, 2.44
Family 1.09 0.75, 1.60 0.59* 0.36, 0.98 1.25 0.86, 1.80
Age (years) 75-85 1.16 0.91, 1.47 1.24 0.89, 1.73 0.77* 0.61, 0.96
Gender Women 1.74*** 1.36, 2.23 1.49* 1.05, 2.11 0.65*** 0.52, 0.82
Ethnic group Black 1.21 0.85, 1.72 1.69* 1.06, 2.70 1.37+ 0.97, 1.94
Hispanic 1.29 0.84, 1.99 1.94* 1.11, 3.39 1.29 0.85, 1.97
Other 1.54 0.68, 3.50 1.15 0.33, 4.02 1.39 0.61, 3.15
Education Less than high school 2.07** 1.34, 3.19 1.94+ 0.99, 3.78 0.89 0.60, 1.31
High school 1.51* 1.01, 2.26 1.87+ 1.00, 3.52 1.09 0.77, 1.54
Vocational or some college 1.51* 1.03, 2.24 1.88* 1.01, 3.49 1.08 0.77, 1.50
Income Far below average 1.47 0.67, 3.24 1.09 0.33, 3.57 0.36** 0.17, 0.76
Below average 1.25 0.59, 2.65 1.50 0.48, 4.67 0.45* 0.23, 0.91
Average 1.08 0.52, 2.23 1.08 0.35, 3.31 0.48* 0.25, 0.96
Above average 0.69 0.32, 1.49 0.64 0.19, 2.13 0.85 0.42, 1.73
Health One or more ADL difficulty 1.31* 1.02, 1.70 2.09*** 1.49, 2.93 0.65** 0.51, 0.83

Notes: NSHAP = National Social Life, Health, and Aging Project; OR = odds ratio; CI = confidence interval.

a

Outcome variables—loneliness: 0 = rarely felt lonely, 1 = felt lonely sometimes or more; anxiety: 0 = score of 0-7 on Hospital Anxiety and Depression scale, 1 = score of 8-21; happiness: 0 = pretty happy or less, 1 = very happy or more.

b

Reference categories—age: 65-74 years; gender: men; ethnic group: White; education: Bachelor degree or more; income: far above average; health: no ADL difficulty; network—restricted network type; loneliness (0); anxiety (0); happiness (0).

+.05 > p <.10. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

After controlling for respondents’ background and health characteristics, the construct of network type maintained an independent association with each of the outcome measures. Thus, when compared with the reference category (restricted network), respondents in the diverse network were less likely to have felt lonely and less likely to have felt anxious. Moreover, a positive association with happiness had borderline significance (p = .067). Respondents who belonged to the friend network were less likely to feel anxious and more likely to feel happy. However, their scores on the loneliness outcome measure were not different than those of respondents in the reference category. Members of the congregant network were also found to be happier, and people in the family network were less anxious than those in the restricted network comparison group. Also worthy of note is that the associations between the network types and the happiness/loneliness outcome variables were mostly the same when linear regressions were performed on the original ordinal scales (not shown).

Discussion

Using a methodology that was applied in previous research on a non-American sample (Litwin, 2001), the current analysis identified five discernable network groupings among respondents in the NSHAP. These groupings were appreciably different in terms of seven different aspects of social capital. The resultant network constellations that were derived in this procedure were termed “diverse,” “friend,” “congregant,” “family,” and “restricted” network types. Although the groupings varied somewhat in their relative distribution within the sample, each social network type was nevertheless representative of a meaningful proportion of the study population, reflecting the different interpersonal milieus in which older Americans are embedded.

The analysis also revealed that four of the five network types derived from the NSHAP data reflect the main social network types that have been identified in previous studies (Fiori et al., 2006; Litwin, 2001). These include the diverse, friend, family, and restricted networks. However, a fifth network grouping also emerged, one that is unique to the current study sample. Its exceptional characteristic was its high relative frequency of attendance at religious services as well as its minimal attendance at organized group meetings of other kinds. We termed this grouping the congregant network, insofar as its outstanding feature was its social exchange with other people at a place of worship.

It seems that the social networks of older Americans in the NSHAP sample are mostly similar to the main types that have been identified earlier. This suggests that the network type construct can indeed be used as an assessment measure for characterizing the nature of the social worlds in which older adults live and function. At the same time, the study also points to a new social aggregate that seems to draw support from its religious social connections. This network grouping accounts for about a sixth of the sample respondents, suggesting that faith-based social networks may have particular importance for certain segments of the current older American population. Future inquiry on the antecedents and the concomitants of belonging to a congregant network type is indeed warranted.

The present study also considered whether there is an association between social network type and subjective well-being among older Americans. The findings confirm that networks with a wider range of social ties are indeed related to better well-being, independent of the effects of demographic and health confounders. Respondents embedded in diverse, friend, and congregant network types expressed a superior sense of subjective well-being as reflected, to varying degrees, in their levels of loneliness, anxiety, and/or happiness. It is important, therefore, to recognize the potential effect of different social network types on the subjective well-being of their members.

A few limitations of the current analysis should be cited. The distributions of the network types as reported here reflect their relative proportion in the unweighted sample, as specific weights for the 65+ cohort were not available. For more precise specification of network type distribution in the older American population, therefore, additional analysis would be required. Nevertheless, the current distribution still provides useful initial parameters for consideration of social network types and their concomitants.

A second limitation is that the analysis was based upon available measures in the database. This is a constraint that is inherent in all secondary analysis. However, this obstacle was only a minor limitation, insofar as the NSHAP data provided a wealth of relevant indicators. Finally, we note that the current analysis is based upon a single wave of cross-sectional data. Thus, we cannot yet determine whether it is the social network type that affects older persons’ subjective well-being or whether it is the nature of lonely, anxious, and/or happy people that tends to lead them to cluster, a priori, in different social network types. Further research on this question is certainly warranted.

Given the instructive findings from this research, a supplementary question to be considered concerns how the notion of social network type can aid in the work of gerontological practitioners. There are four practical implications in this regard. First, the awareness of existing network types may help sensitize gerontological practitioners to the varied interpersonal environments in which older adults are embedded. This may result in improved critical consideration of how social networks function to enhance or restrain the well-being of their members. Second, efforts should be made to create practitioner-friendly instruments that can “type” clients’ social networks in a cost-effective and an efficient manner. Additional empirical work that tests the parameters of network type criterion variables and formulates appropriate algorithms for network typing is recommended. Attention should also be given as to how to involve older clients in the assessment of their social networks.

Third, the social network type construct can serve as a basis for risk assessment as well as a means for determining the efficacy of interventions. For example, the documented movement of a person from a diverse network to a restricted network could be a warning signal that he or she is at increased risk. Conversely, one's transition from a family network to a congregant network could indicate that networking interventions carried out on his or her behalf were indeed successful. These applications imply that periodic assessments of older adults’ social network types are warranted. Fourth, service agency practitioners should think further about how the notion of social network type might enhance their own professional practice, leading to the development of additional field-driven applications of this construct.

In sum, the construct of social network type has much potential for gerontological research and practice. Future inquiry should address additional aspects of the interpersonal milieu that can be included in the derivation and identification of different network types. Research can clarify, for example, whether social network types derived by the present methodology are as effective, in terms of assessment, as more structurally driven social network inventories, such as those that employ name generating mechanisms. Further inquiry in this respect is likely to provide additional useful benefit and should be encouraged.

Funding

National Institute on Aging, Office of Women's Health Research, Office of AIDS Research, and Office of Behavioral and Social Science Research (5R01AG021487) to NSHAP.

References

  1. Baker LA, Cahalin LP, Gerst K, Burr JA. Productive activities and subjective well being among older adults: The influence of number of activities and time commitment. Social Indicators Research. 2005;73:431–458. doi:10.1007/s11205-005-0805-6. [Google Scholar]
  2. Barger SD, Donoho CJ, Wayment HA. The relative contributions of race/ethnicity, socioeconomic status, health, and social relationships to life satisfaction in the United States. Quality of Life Research. 2009;18:179–189. doi: 10.1007/s11136-008-9426-2. doi:10.1007/s11136-008-9426-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Berkman LF, Syme SL. Social networks, host-resistance and mortality: 9-Year follow-up-study of Alameda County residents. American Journal of Epidemiology. 1979;109:186–204. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a112674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bishop AJ, Martin P, Poon L. Happiness and congruence in older adulthood: A structural model of life satisfaction. Aging and Mental Health. 2006;10:445–453. doi: 10.1080/13607860600638388. doi:10.1080/13607860600638388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Brenes GA, Miller ME, Stanley MA, Williamson JD, Knudson M, McCall WV. Insomnia in older adults with generalized anxiety disorder. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2009;17:465–472. doi: 10.1097/jgp.0b013e3181987747. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Cacioppo JT, Fowler JH, Christakis NA. Alone in the crowd: The structure and spread of loneliness in a large social network. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2009;97:977–991. doi: 10.1037/a0016076. doi:10.1037/a0016076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Cacioppo JT, Hughes ME, Waite LJ, Hawkley LC, Thisted RA. Loneliness as a specific risk factor for depressive symptoms: Cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses. Psychology and Aging. 2006;21:140–151. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.140. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cheon EY. Correlation of social network types on health status of Korean elders. Journal of Korean Academy of Nursing. 2010;40:88–98. doi: 10.4040/jkan.2010.40.1.88. doi:10.4040/jkan.2010.40.1.88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Fiori KL, Antonucci TC, Akiyama H. Profiles of social relations among older adults: A cross-cultural approach. Ageing and Society. 2008;28:203–231. doi:10.1017/s0144686x07006472. [Google Scholar]
  10. Fiori KL, Antonucci TC, Cortina KS. Social network typologies and mental health among older adults. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences. 2006;61:P25–P32. doi: 10.1093/geronb/61.1.p25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Fiori KL, Smith J, Antonucci TC. Social network types among older adults: A multidimensional approach. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences. 2007;62:P322–P330. doi: 10.1093/geronb/62.6.p322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Forsell Y. Predictors for depression, anxiety and psychotic symptoms in a very elderly population: Data from a 3-year follow-up study. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. 2000;35:259–263. doi: 10.1007/s001270050237. doi:10.1017/s0144686x07006472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Fowler JH, Christakis NA. Dynamic spread of happiness in a large social network: Longitudinal analysis over 20 years in the Framingham Heart Study. British Medical Journal. 2008;337:a2338. doi: 10.1136/bmj.a2338. doi:10.1136/bmj.a2338. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. George LK. Still happy after all these years: Research frontiers on subjective well being in later life. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences. 2010;65:S331–S339. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbq006. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbq006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Giles LC, Glonek GF, Luszcz MA, Andrews GR. Effect of social networks on 10 year survival in very old Australians: The Australian longitudinal study of aging. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2005;59:574–579. doi: 10.1136/jech.2004.025429. doi:10.1136/jech.2004.025429. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Golden J, Conroy RM, Bruce I, Denihan A, Greene E, Kirby M, et al. Loneliness, social support networks, mood and wellbeing in community-dwelling elderly. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2009;24:694–700. doi: 10.1002/gps.2181. doi:10.1002/gps.2181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Gray A. The social capital of older people. Ageing and Society. 2009;29:5–31. doi:10.1017/s0144686x08007617. [Google Scholar]
  18. Herrmann C. International experiences with the hospital anxiety and depression scale—A review of validation data and clinical results. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 1997;42:17–41. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(96)00216-4. doi:10.1016/s0022-3999(96)00216-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Kousha M, Mohseni N. Are Iranians happy? A comparative study between Iran and the United States. Social Indicators Research. 2000;52:259–289. doi:10.1023/a:1007061431186. [Google Scholar]
  20. Kvaal K, Laake K. Anxiety and well being in older people after discharge from hospital. Journal of Advanced Nursing. 2003;44:271–277. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2648.2003.02802.x. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2648.2003.02802.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Lee GR, Ishii-Kuntz M. Social-interaction, loneliness, and emotional well being among the elderly. Research on Aging. 1987;9:459–482. doi: 10.1177/0164027587094001. doi:10.1177/0164027587094001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Litwin H. Social network type and morale in old age. The Gerontologist. 2001;41:516–524. doi: 10.1093/geront/41.4.516. doi:10.1093/geront/41.4.516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Litwin H. Social predictors of physical activity in later life: The contribution of social-network type. Journal of Aging and Physical Activity. 2003;11:389–406. [Google Scholar]
  24. Litwin H, Sapir EV. Perceived income adequacy among older adults in 12 countries: Findings from the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe. The Gerontologist. 2009;49:397–406. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnp036. doi:10.1093/geront/gnp036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Litwin H, Shiovitz-Ezra S. Network type and mortality risk in later life. The Gerontologist. 2006;46:735–743. doi: 10.1093/geront/46.6.735. doi:10.1093/geront/gnp036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Lubben J, Blozik E, Gillmann G, Iliffe S, Kruse WV, Beck JC, et al. Performance of an abbreviated version of the Lubben Social Network Scale among three European community-dwelling older adult populations. The Gerontologist. 2006;46:503–513. doi: 10.1093/geront/46.4.503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Maselko J, Kubzansky LD. Gender differences in religious practices, spiritual experiences and health: Results from the US General Social Survey. Social Science and Medicine. 2006;62:2848–2860. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.11.008. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. McAuley E, Blissmer S, Marquez DX, Jerome GJ, Kramer AF, Katula J. Social relations, physical activity, and well being in older adults. Preventive Medicine. 2000;31:608–617. doi: 10.1006/pmed.2000.0740. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Menec VH. The relation between everyday activities and successful aging: A 6-year longitudinal study. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences. 2003;58:S74–S82. doi: 10.1093/geronb/58.2.s74. doi:10.1093/geronb/58.2.S74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. O’Muircheartaigh C, Eckman S, Smith S. Statistical design and estimation for the National Social Life, Health and Aging Project. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences. 2009;64:I12–I19. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbp045. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbp045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Peplau LA, Perlman D. Loneliness: A sourcebook of current theory, research and therapy. New York: A Wiley-Interscience Publication; 1982. [Google Scholar]
  32. Pinquart M, Sorensen S. Influences of socioeconomic status, social network, and competence on subjective well being in later life: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging. 2000;15:187–224. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.15.2.187. doi:10.1037//0882- 7974.15.2.187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Pinquart M, Sorensen S. Gender differences in self-concept and psychological well being in old age: A meta-analysis. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences. 2001;56:P195–P213. doi: 10.1093/geronb/56.4.p195. doi:10.1093/geronb/56.4.P195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Ranzijn R, Luszcz M. Measurement of subjective quality of life of elders. International Journal of Aging and Human Development. 2000;50:263–278. doi: 10.2190/4B0W-AMGU-2NDX-CYUQ. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Shiovitz-Ezra S, Ayalon L. Situational versus chronic loneliness as risk factors for all-cause mortality. International Psychogeriatrics. 2010;22:455–462. doi: 10.1017/S1041610209991426. doi:10.1017/s1041610209991426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Shiovitz-Ezra , S., Leitsch S, Graber J, Karraker A. Quality of life and psychological health indicators in the National Social Life, Health and Aging Project. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences. 2009;64:I30–I37. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbn020. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbn020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Slocum-Gori SL, Zumbo BD, Michalos AC, Diener E. A note on the dimensionality of quality of life scales: An illustration with the Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS) Social Indicators Research. 2009;92:489–496. doi:10.1007/s11205-008-9303-y. [Google Scholar]
  38. Smith S, Jaszczak A, Graber J, Lundeen K, Leitsch S, Wargo E, et al. Instrument development, study design implementation, and survey conduct for the National Social life, Health and Aging Project. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences. 2009;64:I20–I29. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbn013. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbn013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Suzman R. The National Social Life, Health and Aging Project: An introduction. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences. 2009;64:I5–I11. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbp078. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbp078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Theeke LA. Socidemographic and health-related risks for loneliness and outcome differences by loneliness status in a sample of U.S. older adults. Research in Gerontological Nursing. 2010;3:113–125. doi: 10.3928/19404921-20091103-99. doi:10.3928/19404921-20091103-99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Theurer K, Wister A. Altruistic behaviour and social capital as predictors of well being among older Canadians. Ageing and Society. 2010;30:157–181. doi:10.1017/S0144686X09008848. [Google Scholar]
  42. Wenger GC. A network typology: From theory to practice. Journal of Aging Studies. 1991;5:147–162. [Google Scholar]
  43. Wenger GC. Social networks and the prediction of elderly people at risk. Aging and Mental Health. 1997;1:311–320. [Google Scholar]
  44. Wenger GC, Tucker I. Using network variation in practice: Identification of support network type. Health and Social Care in the Community. 2002;10:28–35. doi: 10.1046/j.0966-0410.2001.00339.x. doi:10.1046/j.0966-0410.2001.00339.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Wilson RS, Krueger KR, Arnold SE, Schneider JA, Kelly JF, Barnes LL, et al. Loneliness and risk of Alzheimer Disease. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2007;64:234–240. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.2.234. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Wolitzky-Taylor KB, Castriotta N, Lenze EJ, Stanley MA, Craske MG. Anxiety disorders in older adults: A comprehensive review. Depression and Anxiety. 2010;27:190–211. doi: 10.1002/da.20653. doi:10.1002/da.20653. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Yang Y. Social inequalities in happiness in the United States, 1972 to 2004: An age-period-cohort analysis. American Sociological Review. 2008;73:204–226. doi:10.1177/000312240807300202. [Google Scholar]
  48. Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 1983;67:361–370. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1983.tb09716.x. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.1983.tb09716.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The Gerontologist are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES