Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2012 Jun 1.
Published in final edited form as: Pediatr Clin North Am. 2011 Apr 1;58(3):649–665. doi: 10.1016/j.pcl.2011.03.002

Cognitive, Behavioral, and Functional Consequences of Inadequate Sleep in Children and Adolescents

Dean W Beebe a,b,a,b
PMCID: PMC3100528  NIHMSID: NIHMS283201  PMID: 21600347

Synopsis

During the past few decades, studies using multiple research designs have examined whether sleep during childhood and adolescence is related to cognition, behavior, and other aspects of daytime functioning. This paper summarizes recent correlational, case-control, quasi-experimental, and experimental studies, highlighting how the strengths and limitations of each research design are complementary, thereby allowing one to more confidently draw conclusions when viewing the research literature as a whole. Viewed in this manner, published findings suggest that inadequate sleep quality and/or quantity can cause sleepiness, inattention and, very likely, other cognitive and behavioral deficits that significantly impact children and adolescents in important functional settings (e.g., school). This paper then integrates findings from longitudinal studies within a developmental psychopathology model. In this model, inadequate sleep is viewed as a noxious exposure that can, over time, fundamentally alter a child or adolescent's development, resulting in poorer long-term outcomes. Important research questions remain, but the available evidence supports the integration of sleep screening and interventions into routine clinical care and also supports advocacy for public policy changes to improve the sleep of children and adolescents.

Keywords: sleep deprivation, sleep quality, pediatrics, cognition, psychological, school functioning


The premise that inadequate sleep can cause problems with cognition, behavior, or other aspects of daytime functioning has been long discussed in Western culture. Relevant writings date back to antiquity 1 and, once technological advances allowed for a popular press, that press disseminated definitively-worded advice on “proper” sleep patterns for children 2. Physicians and the lay public could not be blamed, however, for a healthy dose of skepticism. After all, the advice seemed to change from author to author, and it did not take much digging to see that it was based on thinly-veiled personal observations and opinions. Over the past several decades, however,,scientific data have largely replaced subjective observations and personal speculation, and have confirmed that at least some of early observers’ impressions were correct. This paper reviews the available data, then outlines why that evidence is particularly concerning from a developmental perspective.

Methodological Considerations

This review will focus on data collected in children and adolescents. There is a large and well-developed literature on sleep deprivation in adults that can provide initial guidance for pediatric research, but can not be extrapolated to children without studying children, for several reasons. Whereas the large majority of adult experimental studies have examined the impact of 1-2 nights of complete sleep deprivation, it is reasonable to question how these findings generalize to children, whose overall sleep need is greater and who far more often experience chronic partial sleep restriction than total sleep deprivation. Moreover, circadian rhythms shift developmentally, and adolescence brings tremendous changes in sleep physiology, particularly within the lower “slow wave” EEG frequency ranges, that may alter the response to sleep restriction 3-5. Finally, the contexts in which children must function differ substantially from adults. Adult research findings on how experimental sleep deprivation impacts truck drivers, medical residents, or other professionals are important, but these findings provide only a rough guess as to the effect of chronic sleep restriction on classroom behaviors or learning, the development of new driving skills, or behavioral and social functioning in developing children.

Studying children and adolescents poses methodological challenges. The primary research design for adult sleep deprivation studies, in which participants stay “in lab” to allow for greater control of sleep and activity schedules, is less of an option for child researchers because parents are often reluctant to leave their children in the care of unfamiliar adults, participants may be resistant to a prolonged stay away from home, and children's sleep may be particularly subject to disruption in an unfamiliar environment 6-7. Further, children are a vulnerable population, for whom additional protections against risk may be required. Although it appears that any health effects of short-term sleep restriction are reversible with 1-2 nights of recovery sleep, researchers must consider the risk of events that could occur during sleep restriction (e.g., a poor school grade, auto accident for young drivers) and attempt to mitigate that risk. As a result, there have been few experimental studies.

High-quality studies using multiple research designs are needed, because each has strengths and weaknesses. Correlational, case-control and quasi-experimental studies lend themselves well to assessing the real-world associations between sleep and daytime function, often in impressively large samples that promote subgroup analyses and generalization of findings. However, the potential for uncontrolled confounding factors (e.g., parent work schedules, parenting styles, family structure, child daytime activities/habits, teen employment) limits causal inferences, and measurements of both sleep and outcome variables are often imprecise. In contrast, experimental studies allow for more confident attribution of causality and more measurement precision, but in much smaller samples and employing methods and measures that do not map cleanly onto real-world circumstances.

Like research designs, different cognitive and behavioral assessment techniques have complementary strengths and weaknesses 8. Questionnaires have the advantages of easy administration, low cost, and ready assessment of real-world functioning, but are prone to reporter biases. Office-based standardized neuropsychological tests avoid such bias and can parse out specific cognitive skills. However, some domains of functioning, particularly attention and executive functioning (e.g., planning, organization, mood and behavior regulation) are difficult to assess in an office-based testing environment 9-10. Direct, systematic ratings of child behaviors by trained observers can provide an objective perspective in applied settings, but are logistically very difficult; the two studies that have used such techniques in the sleep-behavior literature used “simulated” classrooms, rather than embedding raters in subjects’ schools 11-12.

In the end, there is no single “best” way to study the impact of sleep on the daytime functioning of children and adolescents. The best conclusions tend to be drawn from an accumulation of studies with complementary strengths and weaknesses.

Correlational and Case-Control Studies

The largest research base that links sleep in children to daytime functioning comes from correlational studies in epidemiological samples. As summarized in recent major reviews 13-16, children's quantity and/or quality of sleep repeatedly has been shown to correlate with their levels of daytime sleepiness and performance in school. The strength of that association may vary by student age and sex; one recent meta-analysis of sleep and school functioning reported that studies of younger children, particularly those that enrolled more boys, tended to show the largest effects.16 To some degree, research in this area could be criticized for an overreliance on parent- or self-report of sleep and academic performance. However, such reports correlate well with objective measures 15, 17 and, importantly, the association between sleep and academic functioning has been reported even when both constructs were measured objectively 18.

Even so, findings have not been universal. At least one large study has suggested that sleep is minimally associated with academic knowledge 19. Of note, that study relied heavily on office-based tests of academic knowledge, which are only partial predictors of classroom performance. Classroom performance is also dependent upon skills that are difficult to test in the office, including sustained attention, behavior regulation, planning, and organization 20. Indeed, school-identified learning problems did significantly correlate with poor parent-reported sleep quality in that study. However, this effect disappeared after statistically covarying for symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), which led the authors to speculate that any apparent link between sleep and learning problems could be due to the confounding effects of ADHD. Alternatively, however, symptoms of ADHD may not represent confounding factors, but rather the mechanism by which poor sleep quality was linked to learning problems.

Indeed, inadequate sleep has been linked to difficulties with attention, impulse control, and behavior regulation 21-24, with potential consequences that extend beyond the classroom. Poor sleep quality is associated with crash risk in teen drivers 25, and short sleep has been linked to accidental injuries in young children 26-28 and adolescents 27, as well as risk-taking behaviors in adolescents 29. Because poor regulation of attention and behavior are key features of ADHD, some have concluded that a subgroup of children with primary sleep problems may be misdiagnosed with ADHD. The relationship between ADHD and sleep is complex, however, and the reader is referred to the relevant chapter in this volume and several other recent reviews for detailed coverage 30-32.

The links between sleep and other psychiatric diagnoses, including depression and anxiety disorders, are similarly complex and extend beyond the current discussion. Briefly, sleep problems are disproportionately present in many psychiatric conditions, and the direction of causation appears to be reciprocal, rather than unidirectional (see ref 33 for reviews). There is also evidence that the presence or severity of sleep disturbance predicts psychiatric symptom severity and functional impairment e.g. 34-35. However, it is difficult to know how to apply this information to the general population; whereas sleep disruption appears to be linked to mood, studies have yielded mixed associations between sleep duration and emotional functioning 36-38. Indeed, within one study, parent-reported mood problems and behavior problems had variable associations with sleep duration depending on the source of information on behavior/ mood (parent vs self-report) and sleep duration (parent versus actigraphy)39.

Of perhaps greater interest are studies of the daytime functioning of children and adolescents with obstructive sleep apnea (OSA), a largely treatable disorder in which the upper airway is chronically and/or repeatedly obstructed during sleep. As reviewed by Beebe 40, OSA has been linked to poor classroom grades, sleepiness, inattention, hyperactivity, oppositional behaviors, and mood dysregulation (but not ongoing mood disturbance) in the vast majority of relevant studies, most of which have targeted children aged 5-12 years of age. We recently extended those findings through the adolescent years 20. Office-based tests of intelligence have yielded inconsistent results in children with OSA, with the most consistent evidence of IQ deficits during the preschool and early grade-school years 40. Other tests of cognition have yielded mixed results, but there is some evidence of poor scores on tests of attention, executive functioning, and learning/memory in children with OSA 40-42. OSA is of particular interest here not only because a large number of studies have examined behavioral outcomes of children with this condition, but also because treatments focus on the airway and would not be expected to impact behavior in a manner independent of sleep. Non-randomized studies have shown improved daytime functioning following surgical intervention for uncomplicated OSA 40, 43, bolstering the suggestion that OSA is causally related to daytime dysfunction. However, the field awaits the results of the CHildhood AdenoTonsillectomy (CHAT) study, an ongoing large randomized adenotonsillectomy trial for OSA with blinded outcome measures.

There have been studies that have linked other treatable sleep conditions, most notably Restless Legs Syndrome (RLS) and Periodic Limb Movement Disorder (PLMD), to daytime dysfunction, particularly to hyperactivity/impulsivity and inattention 44-45. However, compared to OSA, studies have been few, causal implications are unclear, and intervention data are difficult to interpret because the relevant sleep treatments can also directly impact daytime functioning. For more information on RLS, see Durmer and colleagues’ paper in this volume.

Outside of OSA, only a handful of correlational and case-controlled studies of pediatric sleep have employed objective measures of cognitive functioning. In two, poor quality sleep was significantly associated with poor attention, working memory, and/or impulse control 22-23. A recent study linked objectively-defined short sleep with lower IQ test scores 46. However, others have reported either no relationship between sleep duration and IQ 47 or an association only for males and on selected aspects of intelligence 48. Two large-sample studies on young children have arrived at conflicting results with respect to overall cognitive functioning and sleep 49-50.

In summary, correlational and case-control studies have yielded good evidence of associations between inadequate sleep and disturbances in children's behavior and attention regulation, daytime sleepiness, academic performance and, to the extent that it has been explored, executive functioning. However, the possibility of uncontrolled confounding factors limits the degree to which causal inferences can be drawn these studies.

Quasi-Experimental Studies

As summarized in Table 1, there have been several quasi-experimental studies in which scientists have carefully observed middle- and high-school students whose sleep duration was systematically influenced by school start times. In an impressive illustration of how public policy can impact health, starting school later in the morning is associated with students getting more sleep, regardless of whether comparing across schools (between groups) 51-54, within a group of students over time 55-56, or within individuals over time 57. The link between more sleep and later school start time is due primarily to when students awaken; bedtimes are relatively unchanged. Not surprisingly, later start times also are associated with less subjective and physiological sleepiness 51-54, 57. Finally, later start times appear to be associated with improved enrollment stability 53-54, better attendance among the least stable students 53-54, less tardiness 53-54, fewer teen driving accidents 55, and slightly fewer sick days and depressive symptoms 53-54.

Table 1.

Quasi-Experimental Studies Involving Differences in School Start Times

Authors Grade Level Analytic Design Comparison Findings
Dexter et al.51 10th-11th grades Between-Groups Two nearby schools serving students with similar demographics, one starting at 7:50 am, the other at 8:35 am. Students at the school that started at 7:50 reported significantly less sleep and a trend towards more sleepiness. Sleepiness ratings at both approached the pathologically sleepy range.
Wolfson et al.52 7th-8th grades Between-Groups Two nearby schools serving students with similar demographics, one starting at 7:15 am, the other at 8:37 am. Students at the school that started at 7:15 reported significantly less sleep on school nights and greater sleepiness, and had more school-documented tardiness. Reported class grades differed across schools for 8th graders, but not 7th graders.
Hansen et al.56 Incoming 9th grade honors students Within-Group Student sleep diaries the month before school started were compared to those completed the first two weeks of high school and again several months later. Weeknight sleep duration dropped dramatically from summer into the school year. It then lengthened a bit across the school year, but remained well below summertime levels.
Carskadon et al.57 Students Shifting 9th to 10th grades Within-Subjects Students wore actigraphs in the spring of 9th grade at a school that started at 8:25 am, and again in the fall of 10th after transitioning to a school that started at 7:20 am. Sleepiness was also assessed during each time period. School night bedtimes did not change over time, but rise time became significantly earlier with the transition to the 7:20 am start time, resulting in less sleep. Students’ physiological sleepiness was higher during the second time point as well.
Wahlstrom53-54 9th-12th grades Mixed Within-Group and Between-Groups 7 schools in one district (District A) changed start time from 7:15 am to 8:40 am. Letter grades and enrollment data were compared before and after the schedule change. Also, after the school start time change, student-reported sleep and affect ratings were compared against analogous ratings in a nearby, demographically-similar district that had a 7:30 am start time (District B). After start time change, students in District A changed schools less, and those who changed schools had higher attendance rates. There were no differences in overall student grades from before to after the start time change, but methodological issues complicated analyses. Focusing on the post-change time period, compared to District B, students in District A had only slightly later bedtimes, but markedly later rise times, resulting in almost an hour more sleep per night. Also students in District A reported less daytime sleepiness, less tardiness due to oversleeping, and marginally fewer sick days and symptoms of depression.
Danner & Phillips55 9th-12th grades Mixed Within-Group and Between-Groups Students reported on sleep before and after start times were shifted from 7:30 am to 8:30 am in a county-wide district. Motor vehicle crash data for 17-18 year-olds in the county were contrasted with trends for other counties in the state. Compared to before the change in start times, after the start time had been changed students’ school night sleep time increased significantly, weekend night sleep time decreased significantly, and there was a reduction in motor vehicle crashes by teens in the county. During the same time period, teen crashes were stable or increased slightly elsewhere in the same state.

These are impressive findings. However, limitations of many of these studies include an unknown risk for uncontrolled confounding factors (e.g., school management, historical events, child development), reliance on self-report of daytime functioning, and samples comprised of “superhealthy” individuals who may not reflect the general population. Importantly, evidence of academic gains or improvements on standardized test scores is sparse, and none of these studies has yet demonstrated that students learn more with later school start times.

Experimental Studies

Since 1896, hundreds of publications have documented the impact of experimental sleep deprivation or restriction on adults’ sleep-wake regulation, affect regulation, cognitive performance, real-world functioning (e.g., driving), and neuronal activity 58. In contrast, as of late 2010 there had been only 7 analogous published studies of pediatric populations, all in print since 1980 (Table 2). These studies so far allow for five broad conclusions.

Table 2.

Experimental Studies of Cognitive and Behavioral Effects of Sleep Restriction in Children and Adolescents

Authors Sample Analytic Design Comparison Findings
Carskadon et al59 9 subjects aged 11-13 years Within-Subject, No cross-over Objective sleepiness and cognitive testing after a baseline night (10 hrs in bed), one night of 4 hrs in bed, and a recovery night (10 hrs in bed). Objective sleepiness increased after sleep restriction. No significant changes across nights were noted on the cognitive tests, which included measures of complex addition, word learning/memory, and sustained auditory attention.
Carskadon et al60 12 subjects aged 11-14 years. Within-Subject, No cross-over Objective and subjective sleepiness and cognitive testing after baseline night (10 hrs in bed), one night of no sleep at all, and two recovery nights (10 hrs each). After sleep deprivation, objective and subjective sleepiness increased and performance on all cognitive tests diminished, reaching significance on complex addition and word learning/memory, and showing trends on sustained attention tests.
Randazzo et al63 16 subjects aged 10-14 years Between-Subject Cognitive test scores after random assignment to 11 hours in bed or 5 hours in bed for a single night. Significant effects on 3 indexes of creativity and a measure of concept-formation/reasoning. No such effects were evident on a second measure of concept-formation/reasoning, a test of verbal learning, or 7 other indexes of creativity.
Sadeh, Gruber, & Raviv61 77 subjects aged 9-12 years Mixed within & between-subject design Cognitive tests after 2 nights of normal sleep and 3 nights of a randomly-assigned sleep condition (normal sleep duration plus 1 hour or minus 1 hour) There were cross-condition effects for sleepiness and session-by-group interactions for reaction time and attention span, favoring the lengthened sleep condition. No such interactions were evident on tests of finger tapping speed, sustained attention, impulse control, working memory, or learning/memory.
Fallone et al.11 82 subjects aged 8-15 years Between-subjects Cognitive testing, sleepiness, and behaviors in a simulated academic setting after one in an optimized sleep condition (10 hrs in bed) or restricted sleep condition (4 hrs in bed). Observers rated those with restricted sleep as less attentive but not more hyper/impulsive. In the academic setting, subjects who had restricted sleep were sleepy but not hyper/impulsive. Self-report and objective sleepiness were higher in the restricted sleep condition. There were few cross-group effects on attention tests.
Fallone et al.62 74 subjects aged 6-12 years Within Subject, With Cross-Over Teacher behavior ratings while subjects underwent 3-week protocol: Baseline week (self-selected sleep duration), followed in counterbalanced order by optimized sleep (10+ hrs/night) versus sleep restriction (6.5 - 8 hrs/night). Cross-condition effects were seen on teacher ratings of academic problems, sleepiness, and inattention; the worst ratings occurred during the restricted sleep condition, though otherwise the pattern of scores varied. There were no significant cross-condition effects on teacher ratings of hyperactivity/impulsivity, internalizing/mood issues, or oppositional/aggressive behaviors.
Beebe et al.7, 12, 64 19 subjects aged 13-16 years Within Subject, With Cross-Over Parent and subject ratings of behavior, simulated classroom performance, and EEG and fMRI assessments during a 3-week protocol: Baseline (self-selected sleep duration), followed in counter-balanced order by extended (10 hrs/nt) versus restricted sleep (6.5 hrs/nt). Parents rated their teens as sleepier and having more problems with attention, oppositionality, behavior regulation and meta-cognition; similar effects were also self-reported. Effects on reported hyperactivity/impulsivity were minimal. In the simulated classroom, learning was poorer and there was behavioral and EEG evidence of sleepiness/low arousal during sleep restriction. Functional MRI suggested compensatory neural mechanisms.

First, compared to when they are well-rested, sleep-deprived children fall asleep more easily during the day, self-report sleepiness, and look sleepier 7, 11, 59-62. Second, children are less attentive when sleep-deprived. It has proven difficult to demonstrate this on formal attention tests, but shortened sleep results in visibly more inattentive behaviors, whether reported by individuals who are not blind to sleep condition 7, 11, teachers who were likely blind to sleep condition 62, or observers whose condition-blind state was rigorously maintained 17. Third, no experimental study has yet shown that sleep restriction induces hyperactivity, impulsivity, or other externalizing behaviors in children, despite correlational and case-control evidence that this “should” occur. Fourth, there is some evidence that depriving children of sleep affects their higher-level cognitive skills. One study documented diminished creativity and reasoning skills following a single night of shortened sleep, but overall study results were mixed63. Another reported that sleep-deprived adolescents showed diminished higher-level executive functioning skills according to parent- and self-report forms 7. Fifth, there is evidence that that the impact of sleep deprivation is substantial enough to result in real-world impairment. Sleepiness and inattention in real-world settings have been reported by teachers, parents, and subjects 7, 61-62. One study also found deficits in executive functioning as applied to daily life 7, and two studies reported learning difficulties in a simulated or real classroom 12, 62.

These findings are important because they allow one to infer that inadequate sleep causes daytime deficits. However, to date there simply have been too few studies to answer many important questions. In children, we have little to no knowledge of how circadian rhythms interact with sleep restriction to impact functioning, the response to sleep restriction as it accumulates over time, or whether and why some individuals are more vulnerable to sleep restriction. Moreover, the existing studies have generally had limited statistical power due to small samples and/or the use of between-subjects research designs. The samples also have been largely comprised of “superhealthy” individuals, with limited ethnic and socioeconomic diversity. Young children have been overlooked entirely in published work, though this age range may show unique symptoms (e.g., greater impulsivity). In addition, while it is clear that sleep restriction results in impairments that extend beyond simple sleepiness, some constructs remain underexplored (e.g., problem-solving, memory) and others are unexplored but theoretically at risk (e.g., affect regulation). Finally, only one pilot study so far has examined how pediatric sleep restriction affects waking neural activity 12, 64.

Adding a Developmental Context

The findings presented so far are important, but few address developmental issues that are particularly salient during childhood, such as the parallel development of the brain and its cognitive and behavioral functions, as well as unique contexts in which children are situated.

Developmental changes are evident in the brain throughout the lifespan, but the most dramatic neurodevelopment occurs during childhood, guided by an interaction between genetic programming and environmental factors 65-66. Chronic or extreme exposure to stress or toxins during development can lead to aberrant neural connections, resulting in disruption of cognitive, behavioral, or emotional functioning 66. Inadequate sleep may be one such exposure; animal models have demonstrated that even short-term sleep deprivation can alter neural plasticity 67-73. Further, a rodent model of OSA suggests a developmental gradient, with particularly marked effects on the brain and on learning during the time period equivalent to early childhood 74-75. These findings are notable in light of human data which suggest that OSA has the greatest effect on behavioral functioning in boys under age 8 76, and on intelligence in preschool children 40. Human exposures to inadequate sleep can be prolonged, so even “low level” exposure might result in changed neurodevelopmental patterns and trajectories over time. Notably, the anterior brain regions that show the most protracted development across childhood are also those that are believed to experience the greatest functional impact of sleep deprivation 77-79.

There have been few publications on the neural response to sleep deprivation in children, but non-invasive technologies hold promise for shedding additional light. Four relevant studies have been published. Magnetic resonance spectroscopy conducted on school-aged children with severe OSA found region-specific chemical abnormalities suggestive of neuronal injury 80. Also, in young children with “subclinical” sleep-disordered breathing, altered neuronal processing of speech sounds has been detected via evoked response potentials, leading study authors to speculate that the brain may be attempting to compensate for sleep disturbance 81. A similar explanation was evoked to explain altered activation-deactivation patterns in attention-related brain regions of adolescents during sleep restriction 64. These adolescents also showed electroencephalographic slowing in a simulated classroom while sleep-deprived 12. All of these findings are preliminary, and at present it is impossible to draw coherent conclusions due to differences in samples, research designs, and measures. However, these studies support the suggestion that inadequate sleep can substantively alter neural processing.

Paralleling neurodevelopment, key functional skills develop across childhood. While academic skills are most easily appreciated as requiring learning, it is no exaggeration to state that every foundation skill necessary for adult functioning matures substantially during childhood and adolescence. To the degree that sleep deprivation impacts a young child's ability to engage with and learn from his or her environment—and the evidence reviewed above suggests that it does—maturation may be delayed or disrupted. Older children and adolescents may be vulnerable in other ways, as their behaviors can have costly, irreversible consequences 78. For example, there is a spike in accidental injuries during adolescence 78, and adolescent school underperformance increases the odds of school drop-out, failure to enroll in or complete college, adult mental illness or substance abuse, and low occupational attainment 82-86. If sleep quality or quantity affect injury risk or school success—and the above evidence suggests this to be the case—then the long-term legacy of pediatric sleep problems may be considerable.

In humans, it is neither feasible nor ethical to experimentally expose children to prolonged sleep restriction. However, longitudinal studies can examine the natural associations between inadequate sleep and later functioning. Over a dozen relevant studies have been published, and a well-replicated finding is that childhood sleep problems (variously defined) predict the development of anxiety and depressive symptoms over time, even after controlling for baseline mood difficulties and other potential confounds. This has been reported across timeframes spanning preschool to mid-childhood 87-88, preschool to mid-adolescence 89-90, mid-childhood to late childhood 39, 91, mid-childhood to young adulthood 92, and adolescence to young adulthood 93. Longitudinal associations between sleep problems and externalizing behaviors such as hyperactivity, aggression or conduct have tended to be weaker or less consistent, but also have been reported 39, 87-88, 92. Consistent with such associations, the presence of loud snoring, a hallmark symptom of OSA, in young children has been shown to predict later hyperactivity and poor school performance 94-95. Finally, sleep problems at ages 3-8 years have also been found to predict the early onset of substance use in adolescents 90.

Such longitudinal relationships have not always been straightforward, however. One group has suggested that inadequate sleep has the greatest effects in children from homes in lower socioeconomic strata 39, 47. In several other studies, the initial presence of sleep problems was less important in predicting later functioning than whether these problems persisted or worsened over time 38, 96-97. Such complexity has also been evident in studies of cognitive and learning outcomes. Especially among children from low income homes, the presence of sleep problems in 3rd grade predicts intellectual stagnation, and longer sleep predicts better reading development, over the following two years 47. Persistent or worsening sleep problems during childhood also predict poorer scores later on tests of executive functioning, but not memory, nonverbal reasoning, vocabulary, or fine motor skills 97-99.

Taken together, these longitudinal findings can not prove causation, but are consistent with a developmental model in which inadequate sleep is viewed as a noxious exposure that results, over time, in increased risk for adverse functional outcomes.

Conclusions

Findings from studies that used complementary research methods have converged to strongly suggest that inadequate sleep quality and quantity are causally linked to sleepiness, inattention, and probably other cognitive and behavioral deficits that impact daytime functioning, with potential implications for long-term development. Important research questions remain, but the available data support the integration of sleep screening and interventions into routine clinical care (see related chapter in this volume) and also support advocacy for public policy changes to improve the sleep of children and adolescents, with the goal of preventing long-term functional deficits 53-54.

As the pediatric sleep discipline moves forward, it is worth reflecting on other domains of public health that focus on prevention. When adult conditions have etiologies earlier in development, treatments performed in adulthood are usually inefficient and not entirely efficacious. Consider skin cancer, which can be treated in adulthood, but with mixed success and sometimes at great personal and societal cost. In contrast, better understanding of the early effects of sun exposure during childhood has led to more effective primary prevention 100. Effective prevention has also emerged when childhood exposure to a suspected pathogen is studied to determine its deleterious mechanisms and lifetime effects. This was the case with inorganic lead exposure which, until studied in children, was tolerated at levels which were later shown to cause long-term adaptive deficits 101. In both of these examples, it is noteworthy that causal conclusions—and the effective preventive strategies that followed—were derived from a combination of translational, experimental, correlational, and longitudinal data. If similarly diverse data continue to bear out the developmental model described above, chronic sleep problems or sleep restriction might not be considered any more tolerable during childhood or adolescence than are noxious serum lead levels or unregulated artificial tanning facilities.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by Grant No. R01 HL092149 from the National Institutes of Health.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

The author has nothing to disclose.

References

  • 1.Thorpy MJ. History of sleep and man. In: Thorpy MJ, Yager J, editors. The Encylopedia of Sleep and Sleep Disorders. Facts on File, Inc.; New York, NY: 1991. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Stearns PN, Rowlands P, Giarnella L. Children's sleep: Sketching historical change. J Soc Hist. 1996;30:345–366. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Carskadon MA, Acebo C. Regulation of sleepiness in adolescents: update, insights, and speculation. Sleep. 2002;25:606–614. doi: 10.1093/sleep/25.6.606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Jenni OG, Achermann P, Carskadon MA. Homeostatic sleep regulation in adolescents. Sleep. 2005;28:1446–1454. doi: 10.1093/sleep/28.11.1446. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Campbell IG, Higgins LM, Trinidad JM, et al. The Increase in Longitudinally Measured Sleepiness Across Adolescence is Related to the Maturational Decline in Low-Frequency EEG Power. Sleep. 2007;30:1677–1687. doi: 10.1093/sleep/30.12.1677. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Fallone G, Seifer R, Acebo C, et al. How well do school-aged children comply with imposed sleep schedules at home? Sleep. 2002;25:739–745. doi: 10.1093/sleep/25.7.739. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Beebe DW, Fallone G, Godiwala N, et al. Feasibility and behavioral effects of an at-home multi-night sleep restriction protocol for adolescents. Jn Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008;49:915–923. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01885.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Beebe DW. Assessing Neurobehavioral Outcomes in Childhood Sleep Disordered Breathing: A primer for non-neuropsychologists. In: Marcus C, Carroll J, Loughlin G, Rosen CL, editors. Sleep in Children: Developmental Changes in Sleep Patterns. 2nd ed. Informa Healthcare; New York: 2008. pp. 345–365. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, et al. BRIEF -- Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function. Psychological Assessment Resources; Odessa, FL: 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Baron IS. Neuropsychological evaluation of the child. Oxford University Press; New York: 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Fallone G, Acebo C, Arnedt JT, et al. Effects of acute sleep restriction on behavior, sustained attention, and response inhibition in children. Percept Mot Skills. 2001;93:213–229. doi: 10.2466/pms.2001.93.1.213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Beebe DW, Rose D, Amin R. Attention, Learning, and Arousal of Experimentally Sleep-restricted Adolescents in a Simulated Classroom. J Adolesc Health. 2010;47:523–525. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2010.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Drake C, Nickel C, Burduvali E, et al. The pediatric daytime sleepiness scale (PDSS): sleep habits and school outcomes in middle-school children. Sleep. 2003;26:455–458. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Fallone G, Owens JA, Deane J. Sleepiness in children and adolescents: clinical implications. Sleep Med Rev. 2002;6:287–306. doi: 10.1053/smrv.2001.0192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Wolfson AR, Carskadon MA. Understanding adolescents’ sleep patterns and school performance: A critical appraisal. Sleep Med Rev. 2003;7:491–506. doi: 10.1016/s1087-0792(03)90003-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Dewald JF, Meijer AM, Oort FJ, et al. The influence of sleep quality, sleep duration and sleepiness on school performance in children and adolescents: A meta-analytic review. Sleep Med Rev. 2010;14:179–189. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2009.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Wolfson AR, Carskadon MA, Acebo C, et al. Evidence for the validity of a sleep habits survey for adolescents. Sleep. 2003;26:213–216. doi: 10.1093/sleep/26.2.213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Keller PS, El-Sheikh M, Buckhalt JA. Children's attachment to parents and their academic functioning: sleep disruptions as moderators of effects. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2008;29:441–449. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e318182a9b4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Mayes SD, Calhoun SL, Bixler EO, et al. Nonsignificance of sleep relative to IQ and neuropsychological scores in predicting academic achievement. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2008;29:206–212. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e31816d924f. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Beebe DW, Ris MD, Kramer ME, et al. The Association Between Sleep-Disordered Breathing, Academic Grades, and Cognitive and Behavioral Functioning Among Overweight Subjects during Middle to Late Childhood. Sleep. 2010;33:1447–1456. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.11.1447. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Paavonen EJ, Raikkonen K, Lahti J, et al. Short sleep duration and behavioral symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in healthy 7- to 8-year-old children. Pediatrics. 2009;123:e857–864. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-2164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Sadeh A, Gruber R, Raviv A. Sleep, neurobehavioral functioning, and behavior problems in school-age children. Child Dev. 2002;73:405–417. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Steenari MR, Vuontela V, Paavonen EJ, et al. Working memory and sleep in 6- to 13-year-old schoolchildren. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2003;42:85–92. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200301000-00014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Paavonen EJ, Porkka-Heiskanen T, Lahikainen AR. Sleep quality, duration and behavioral symptoms among 5-6-year-old children. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;18:747–754. doi: 10.1007/s00787-009-0033-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Pizza F, Contardi S, Antognini AB, et al. Sleep quality and motor vehicle crashes in adolescents. J Clin Sleep Med. 2010;6:41–45. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Koulouglioti C, Cole R, Kitzman H. Inadequate sleep and unintentional injuries in young children. Public Health Nurs. 2008;25:106–114. doi: 10.1111/j.1525-1446.2008.00687.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Stallones L, Beseler C, Chen P. Sleep patterns and risk of injury among adolescent farm residents. Am J Prev Med. 2006;30:300–304. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2005.11.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Owens JA, Fernando S, Mc Guinn M. Sleep disturbance and injury risk in young children. Behav Sleep Med. 2005;3:18–31. doi: 10.1207/s15402010bsm0301_4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.O'Brien EM, Mindell JA. Sleep and risk-taking behavior in adolescents. Behav Sleep Med. 2005;3:113–133. doi: 10.1207/s15402010bsm0303_1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Gruber R. Sleep characteristics of children and adolescents with attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 2009;18:863–876. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2009.04.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Owens JA. Sleep disorders and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2008;10:439–444. doi: 10.1007/s11920-008-0070-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Cortese S, Faraone SV, Konofal E, et al. Sleep in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: meta-analysis of subjective and objective studies. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48:894–908. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181ac09c9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Ivanenko A, Crabtree VM, Gozal D. Sleep in children with psychiatric disorders. Pediatr Clin North Am. 2004;51:51–68. doi: 10.1016/s0031-3955(03)00181-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Liu X, Buysse DJ, Gentzler AL, et al. Insomnia and hypersomnia associated with depressive phenomenology and comorbidity in childhood depression. Sleep. 2007;30:83–90. doi: 10.1093/sleep/30.1.83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Liu X, Hubbard JA, Fabes RA, et al. Sleep disturbances and correlates of children with autism spectrum disorders. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2006;37:179–191. doi: 10.1007/s10578-006-0028-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Moore M, Kirchner HL, Drotar D, et al. Relationships among sleepiness, sleep time, and psychological functioning in adolescents. J Pediatr Psychol. 2009;34:1175–1183. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsp039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Wolfson AR, Carskadon MA. Sleep schedules and daytime functioning in adolescents. Child Dev. 1998;69:875–887. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Fredriksen K, Rhodes J, Reddy R, et al. Sleepless in Chicago: tracking the effects of adolescent sleep loss during the middle school years. Child Dev. 2004;75:84–95. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00655.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.El-Sheikh M, Kelly RJ, Buckhalt JA, et al. Children's sleep and adjustment over time: the role of socioeconomic context. Child Dev. 2010;81:870–883. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01439.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Beebe DW. Neurobehavioral Effects of Childhood Sleep-Disordered Breathing (SDB): A comprehensive review. Sleep. 2006:1115–1134. doi: 10.1093/sleep/29.9.1115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Spruyt K, Capdevila OS, Kheirandish-Gozal L, et al. Inefficient or insufficient encoding as potential primary deficit in neurodevelopmental performance among children with OSA. Dev Neuropsychol. 2009;34:601–614. doi: 10.1080/87565640903133566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Kheirandish-Gozal L, De Jong MR, Spruyt K, et al. Obstructive sleep apnoea is associated with impaired pictorial memory task acquisition and retention in children. Eur Respir J. 2010;36:164–169. doi: 10.1183/09031936.00114209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Garetz SL. Behavior, cognition, and quality of life after adenotonsillectomy for pediatric sleep-disordered breathing: summary of the literature. Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2008;138:S19–26. doi: 10.1016/j.otohns.2007.06.738. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Cortese S, Konofal E, Lecendreux M, et al. Restless legs syndrome and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a review of the literature. Sleep. 2005;28:1007–1013. doi: 10.1093/sleep/28.8.1007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.O'Brien LM. The neurocognitive effects of sleep disruption in children and adolescents. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 2009;18:813–823. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2009.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Gruber R, Laviolette R, Deluca P, et al. Short sleep duration is associated with poor performance on IQ measures in healthy school-age children. Sleep Med. 2010;11:289–294. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2009.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Buckhalt JA, El-Sheikh M, Keller PS, et al. Concurrent and longitudinal relations between children's sleep and cognitive functioning: the moderating role of parent education. Child Dev. 2009;80:875–892. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01303.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Ortega FB, Ruiz JR, Castillo R, et al. Sleep duration and cognitive performance in adolescence. The AVENA study. Acta Paediatr. 2010;99:454–456. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2009.01618.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Touchette E, Petit D, Seguin JR, et al. Associations between sleep duration patterns and behavioral/cognitive functioning at school entry. Sleep. 2007;30:1213–1219. doi: 10.1093/sleep/30.9.1213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Nixon GM, Thompson JM, Han DY, et al. Short sleep duration in middle childhood: risk factors and consequences. Sleep. 2008;31:71–78. doi: 10.1093/sleep/31.1.71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Dexter D, Bijwadia J, Schilling D, et al. Sleep, sleepiness and school start times: a preliminary study. WMJ. 2003;102:44–46. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Wolfson AR, Spaulding NL, Dandrow C, et al. Middle school start times: the importance of a good night's sleep for young adolescents. Behav Sleep Med. 2007;5:194–209. doi: 10.1080/15402000701263809. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Wahlstrom KL. Accommodating the sleep patterns of adolescents within current educational structures: An uncharted path. In: Carskadon MA, editor. Adolescent Sleep Patterns: Biological, Social, and Psychological Influences. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge, UK: 2002. pp. 172–197. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Wahlstrom KL. Changing times: Findings from the first longitudinal study of later high school start times. NAASP Bulletin. 2002;96:3–21. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Danner F, Phillips B. Adolescent sleep, school start times, and teen motor vehicle crashes. J Clin Sleep Med. 2008;4:533–535. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Hansen M, Janssen I, Schiff A, et al. The impact of school daily schedule on adolescent sleep. Pediatrics. 2005;115:1555–1561. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-1649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Carskadon MA. Adolescent sleep patterns: Biological, social, and psychological influences. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge, UK: 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Goel N, Rao H, Durmer JS, et al. Neurocognitive consequences of sleep deprivation. Semin Neurol. 2009;29:320–339. doi: 10.1055/s-0029-1237117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Carskadon MA, Harvey K, Dement WC. Acute restriction of nocturnal sleep in children. Percept Mot Skills. 1981;53:103–112. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Carskadon MA, Harvey K, Dement WC. Sleep loss in young adolescents. Sleep. 1981;4:299–312. doi: 10.1093/sleep/4.3.299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Sadeh A, Gruber R, Raviv A. The effects of sleep restriction and extension on school-age children: What a difference an hour makes. Child Dev. 2003;74:444–455. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.7402008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Fallone G, Acebo C, Seifer R, et al. Experimental restriction of sleep opportunity in children: effects on teacher ratings. Sleep. 2005;28:1561–1567. doi: 10.1093/sleep/28.12.1561. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Randazzo AC, Muehlback MJ, Schweitzer PK, et al. Cognitive function following acute sleep restriction in children ages 10-14. Sleep. 1998;21:861–868. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Beebe DW, Difrancesco MW, Tlustos SJ, et al. Preliminary fMRI findings in experimentally sleep-restricted adolescents engaged in a working memory task. Behav Brain Funct. 2009;5:9. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-5-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Beebe DW. Sleep and behavior in children: A multi-system, developmental heuristic model. In: Ivanenko A, editor. Sleep and Psychiatric Disorders in Children and Adolescents. Informa; New York: 2008. pp. 1–10. [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Michel GF. A developmental-psychobiological approach to developmental neuropsychology. Dev Neuropsychol. 2001;19:11–32. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN1901_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Mirmiran M, Ariagno RL. Role of REM sleep in brain development and plasticity. In: Maquet P, Smith C, Stickgold R, editors. Sleep and brain plasticity. Oxford University Press; New York: 2003. pp. 181–187. [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Frank MG, Stryker MP. The role of sleep in the development of central visual pathways. In: Maquet P, Smith C, Stickgold R, editors. Sleep and brain plasticity. Oxford University Press; New York: 2003. pp. 190–206. [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Dang-Vu TT, Desseilles M, Peigneux P, et al. A role for sleep in brain plasticity. Pediatr Rehabil. 2006;9:98–118. doi: 10.1080/13638490500138702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Frank MG. The mystery of sleep function: current perspectives and future directions. Rev Neurosci. 2006;17:375–392. doi: 10.1515/revneuro.2006.17.4.375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Guzman-Marin R, Ying Z, Suntsova N, et al. Suppression of hippocampal plasticity-related gene expression by sleep deprivation in rats. J Physiol. 2006;575:807–819. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.115287. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Lopez J, Roffwarg HP, Dreher A, et al. Rapid eye movement sleep deprivation decreases long-term potentiation stability and affects some glutamatergic signaling proteins during hippocampal development. Neurosci. 2008;153:44–53. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.01.072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Tartar JL, Ward CP, McKenna JT, et al. Hippocampal synaptic plasticity and spatial learning are impaired in a rat model of sleep fragmentation. Eur J Neurosci. 2006;23:2739–2748. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04808.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Gozal E, Row BW, Schurr A, et al. Developmental differences in cortical and hippocampal vulnerability to intermittent hypoxia in the rat. Neurosci Lett. 2001;305:197–201. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(01)01853-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Row BW, Kheirandish L, Neville JJ, et al. Impaired spatial learning and hyperactivity in developing rats exposed to intermittent hypoxia. Pediatr Res. 2002;52:449–453. doi: 10.1203/00006450-200209000-00024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Chervin RD, Archbold KH, Dillon JE, et al. Inattention, hyperactivity, and symptoms of sleep-disordered breathing. Pediatrics. 2002;109:449–456. doi: 10.1542/peds.109.3.449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Harrison Y, Horne JA. The impact of sleep deprivation on decision making: A review. J Exp Psychol Appl. 2000;6:236–249. doi: 10.1037//1076-898x.6.3.236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Dahl RE. Adolescent brain development: a period of vulnerabilities and opportunities. Keynote address. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2004;1021:1–22. doi: 10.1196/annals.1308.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Dahl RE, Lewin DS. Pathways to adolescent health sleep regulation and behavior. J Adolesc Health. 2002;31:175–184. doi: 10.1016/s1054-139x(02)00506-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Halbower AC, Degaonkar M, Barker PB, et al. Childhood obstructive sleep apnea associates with neuropsychological deficits and neuronal brain injury. PLoS Med. 2006;3:e301. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0030301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Key AP, Molfese DL, O'Brien L, et al. Sleep-disordered breathing affects auditory processing in 5-7-year-old children: evidence from brain recordings. Dev Neuropsychol. 2009;34:615–628. doi: 10.1080/87565640903133608. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Iramaneerat C. Predicting academic achievement in the medical school with high school grades. J Med Assoc Thai. 2006;89:1497–1505. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Dubow EF, Huesmann LR, Boxer P, et al. Middle childhood and adolescent contextual and personal predictors of adult educational and occupational outcomes: a mediational model in two countries. Dev Psychol. 2006;42:937–949. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.5.937. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Crum RM, Juon HS, Green KM, et al. Educational achievement and early school behavior as predictors of alcohol-use disorders: 35-year follow-up of the Woodlawn Study. J Stud Alcohol. 2006;67:75–85. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2006.67.75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Locke TF, Newcomb MD. Adolescent predictors of young adult and adult alcohol involvement and dysphoria in a prospective community sample of women. Prev Sci. 2004;5:151–168. doi: 10.1023/b:prev.0000037639.78352.3c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Rosenbaum JE. Beyond college for all. Sage; New York: 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Jaspers M, de Winter AF, de Meer G, et al. Early findings of preventive child healthcare professionals predict psychosocial problems in preadolescence: the TRAILS study. J Pediatr. 2010;157:316–321. e312. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2010.02.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Gregory AM, Eley TC, O'Connor TG, et al. Etiologies of associations between childhood sleep and behavioral problems in a large twin sample. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2004;43:744–751. doi: 10.1097/01.chi/0000122798.47863.a5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Gregory AM, O'Connor TG. Sleep problems in childhood: a longitudinal study of developmental change and association with behavioral problems. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002;41:964–971. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200208000-00015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Wong MM, Brower KJ, Zucker RA. Childhood sleep problems, early onset of substance use and behavioral problems in adolescence. Sleep Med. 2009;10:787–796. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2008.06.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Gregory AM, Rijsdijk FV, Lau JY, et al. The direction of longitudinal associations between sleep problems and depression symptoms: a study of twins aged 8 and 10 years. Sleep. 2009;32:189–199. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.2.189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Gregory AM, Van der Ende J, Willis TA, et al. Parent-reported sleep problems during development and self-reported anxiety/depression, attention problems, and aggressive behavior later in life. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2008;162:330–335. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.162.4.330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Roane BM, Taylor DJ. Adolescent insomnia as a risk factor for early adult depression and substance abuse. Sleep. 2008;31:1351–1356. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Chervin RD, Ruzincka DL, Archbold KH, et al. Snoring predicts hyperactivity four years later. Sleep. 2005;28:885–890. doi: 10.1093/sleep/28.7.885. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Gozal D, Pope D. Snoring during early childhood and academic performance at ages thirteen to fourteen years. Pediatrics. 2001;107:1394–1399. doi: 10.1542/peds.107.6.1394. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Gregory AM, Caspi A, Eley TC, et al. Prospective longitudinal associations between persistent sleep problems in childhood and anxiety and depression disorders in adulthood. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2005;33:157–163. doi: 10.1007/s10802-005-1824-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Quach J, Hiscock H, Canterford L, et al. Outcomes of child sleep problems over the school-transition period: Australian population longitudinal study. Pediatrics. 2009;123:1287–1292. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-1860. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Friedman NP, Corley RP, Hewitt JK, et al. Individual differences in childhood sleep problems predict later cognitive executive control. Sleep. 2009;32:323–333. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.3.323. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Gregory AM, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, et al. Sleep problems in childhood predict neuropsychological functioning in adolescence. Pediatrics. 2009;123:1171–1176. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-0825. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.MacNeal RJ, Dinulos JG. Update on sun protection and tanning in children. Curr Opin Pediatr. 2007;19:425–429. doi: 10.1097/MOP.0b013e3282294936. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Dietrich KN. Environmental Toxicants and psychological development. In: Yeates KO, Ris MD, Taylor HG, Pennington BF, editors. Pediatric Neuropsychology: Research, Theory, and Practice. 2nd ed. Guilford; New York: 2010. pp. 211–264. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES