Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2012 Jul 1.
Published in final edited form as: Psychophysiology. 2010 Dec 10;48(7):1015–1021. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01155.x

Support Vector Machines to improve physiologic hot flash measures: Application to the ambulatory setting

Rebecca C Thurston a,b, Javier Hernandez c, Jose M Del Rio c, Fernando De la Torre c
PMCID: PMC3107378  NIHMSID: NIHMS254825  PMID: 21143609

Abstract

Most midlife women have hot flashes. The conventional criterion (≥2 μmho rise/30 seconds) for classifying hot flashes physiologically has shown poor performance. We improved this performance in the laboratory with Support Vector Machines (SVMs), a pattern classification method. We aimed to compare conventional to SVM methods to classify hot flashes in the ambulatory setting. Thirty-one women with hot flashes underwent 24 hrs of ambulatory sternal skin conductance monitoring. Hot flashes were quantified with conventional (≥2 μmho/30 sec) and SVM methods. Conventional methods had low sensitivity (sensitivity=.57, specificity=.98, positive predictive value (PPV)=.91, negative predictive value (NPV)=.90, F1=.60), with performance declining with higher body mass index (BMI). SVMs improved this performance (sensitivity=.87, specificity=.97, PPV=.90, NPV=.96, F1=.88) and reduced BMI variation. SVMs can improve ambulatory physiologic hot flash measures.

Introduction

Approximately 70% of midlife women experience hot flashes at some point during the menopausal transition (Gold, et al., 2006). Previously thought to persist only during the several years around the menopausal transition, it is now clear that a significant minority of women experience hot flashes well into their 60's and 70's (Barnabei, et al., 2005; Barnabei, et al., 2002). Hot flashes are associated with pronounced decrements in quality of life, including physical, social, and emotional functioning (Avis, et al., 2009; Avis, et al., 2003). They are a consistent predictor of depressed mood during the menopausal transition (Bromberger, et al., 2007). More recently, hot flashes have been linked to cardiovascular risk (Thurston, Sutton-Tyrrell, Everson-Rose, Hess, & Matthews, 2008) and bone loss (Crandall, et al., 2009). Given findings of potential health risk associated with hormone therapy (Rossouw, et al., 2002), the leading treatment for hot flashes, developing an improved understanding of the physiology of hot flashes and new treatments for hot flashes have been of increased scientific interest.

One factor that has limited research on hot flashes has been issues with the physiologic measurement of hot flashes. Physiologic measures of hot flashes in addition to self-report measures are desirable for research on hot flashes given the many factors that influence the reporting of hot flashes (Miller & Li, 2004). These factors include subject adherence, distraction, sleep, and psychological factors such as anxiety, which may increase the likelihood of reporting hot flashes in the absence of a physiologic change (Thurston, Blumenthal, Babyak, & Sherwood, 2005; Thurston, Matthews, Hernandez, & De La Torre, 2009). Physiologic measures address these limitations, allowing quantification of the occurrence of hot flashes without reliance on subject reporting. They have the potential to precisely quantify hot flashes, including their precise frequency and the timing of their occurrence during sleep and wake, not possible when relying solely upon subject reporting.

The most widely-used physiologic measure of hot flashes is sternal skin conductance. The standard criterion for the physiologic occurrence of a hot flash is a 2 μmho rise in skin conductance in a 30 second period (Freedman, 1989). However, we previously demonstrated that although sternal skin conductance reliably changes with hot flashes, this 2 μmho criterion had low sensitivity in the laboratory setting (Thurston, Matthews, Hernandez, & De La Torre, 2009). Further, the performance of this criterion varied by subject characteristics, showing particularly poor performance among overweight/obese women. Others have similarly shown issues with the performance of this measure (de Bakker & Everaerd, 1996; Hanisch, Palmer, Donahue, & Coyne, 2007; Sievert, et al., 2002). We improved upon this criterion by using the classification approach of Support Vector Machines (SVMs) (Thurston, Matthews, Hernandez, & De La Torre, 2009). SVMs are state-of-the-art classification methods particularly useful for complicated pattern recognition problems (Guyon, Weston, Barnhill, & Vapnik, 2002; Joachims, 1998; Michel & Kaliouby, 2003). As opposed to applying a single magnitude-based threshold to classify hot flashes, SVMs can characterize the distinct skin conductance patterns associated with hot flashes.

Our prior work considered laboratory measured hot flashes only. We now extend our prior work to the ambulatory setting, the setting in which much of clinical research on hot flashes occurs. However, measuring hot flashes in the ambulatory setting is more challenging than in the laboratory. The ambulatory setting is characterized by a range of factors increasing error in skin conductance signals, such as baseline drift; artifact producing factors such as physical activity, seat belts, and purses; and a changing electrode-subject interface over longer monitoring periods. Further, hot flash reporting may become less reliable in the ambulatory setting, with increased distractions and greater subject adherence demands for reporting over 24 hours or more.

In this investigation, we evaluate the conventional criterion (2 μmho rise in 30 second period) for quantifying hot flashes from sternal skin conductance in the ambulatory setting. We also apply SVMs to sternal skin conductance signals to improve the performance of this measure. For both conventionally-quantified and SVM-quantified hot flashes, we evaluate any variation in the algorithms by subject characteristics such as body mass index (BMI), race/ethnicity, and anxiety, factors that have been previously linked to variations in the physiologic detection and/or reporting of hot flashes (Sievert, 2007; Thurston, Matthews, Hernandez, & De La Torre, 2009).

Methods

Subjects

Thirty-four African American and Caucasian women between the ages of 40 and 60 were recruited from the surrounding community via newspaper advertisements, fliers in local businesses, and online message board postings. Inclusion criteria included late perimenopausal (amenorrhea last 3-12 months) or postmenopausal (amenorrhea ≥12 months) status, reporting ≥4 hot flashes a day, and having a uterus and both ovaries. Women were excluded if having taken hormone therapy (oral or transdermal estrogen and/or progesterone), oral contraceptives, selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors or serotonin norepinepherine reuptake inhibitors, clonidine, methyldopa, bellergal, gabapentin, aromatase inhibitors, selective estrogen receptor modulators in the past 3 months, having taken isoflavone supplements or black cohosh in the past month, currently undergoing acupuncture for the treatment of hot flashes, reporting medical or psychiatric conditions associated with hot flash sensations (panic disorder, pheochromocytoma, leukemia, pancreatic tumor), or inability to provide informed consent and follow study procedures. Of these 34 women, two women were excluded due to equipment failure during the session and one woman withdrew from the study for a final sample of 31 women.

Procedures

Participants underwent measurement of height, weight, and waist circumference, and completed a battery of questionnaires for assessment of medical, demographic, and psychological characteristics. Participants were equipped with an ambulatory sternal skin conductance monitor and an electronic diary, and instructed to wear the monitor over 24 hours. They were instructed to avoid heavy physical activity, swimming, and showering while wearing the monitor. At each experience of a hot flash, participants were instructed to press two event mark buttons on the monitor and complete the electronic diary entry, both of which provide date- and time-stamped subjective hot flash reports. For five subjects, problems with electrode adhesion compromised data quality; these subjects conducted a make-up monitoring session with all usable monitoring data reported here. All study procedures were approved by the University of Pittsburgh Institutional Review Board and all participants provided written informed consent.

Measures

Sternal skin conductance was recorded via the Biolog ambulatory monitor (UFI, model 3991/2-SCL; Morro Bay, CA), a portable device worn in a pouch around the waist. The Biolog measures sternal skin conductance during daily life, sampled at 1 Hz from the sternum via a 0.5 Volt constant voltage circuit passed between two Ag/AgCl electrodes (Vermed; Bellows Falls, VT) filled with 0.05M KCL Velvachol/glycol paste (Dormire & Carpenter, 2002). The Biolog has two event mark buttons providing a date/time-stamped subjective event report. During waking monitoring hours, participants completed a portable electronic diary (Palm Zire, Palm, Inc.) when experiencing a hot flash, with questions about the experience of the hot flash programmed with Palm-compatible software (Satellite Forms; Thacker Network Technologies). Height and weight were measured via a fixed stadiometer and a calibrated balance beam scale, respectively. Menstrual history, parity, education, marital status, alcohol use, and smoking status were assessed by standard demographic and medical history questionnaires. Depressive symptoms were assessed via the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Survey (Radloff, 1977) and state and trait anxiety via the Spielberger State Trait Anxiety Inventory (Spielberger, 1983).

Data reduction

Physiologic hot flashes were classified with two methods: 1) 2 μmho rise in 30 s (conventional criterion), and 2) SVM-defined hot flashes.

Conventional Criterion

For the conventional criterion, skin conductance increases of 2 μmho in 30 seconds (Freedman, 1989) were flagged automatically by custom software and edited for artifact using standard methods (Carpenter, Andrykowski, Freedman, & Munn, 1999). A 20-minute lockout period was implemented after the start of the flash, after which no hot flashes were coded.

Support Vector Machines

Building an SVM model, or algorithm, consists of both a training phase, in which the model is developed, and a testing phase, in which model performance is tested. Data were first preprocessed to normalize skin conductance signals and to remove noise. Since we have previously found dramatic differences in the magnitude of skin conductance rises associated with hot flashes (Thurston, Matthews, Hernandez, & De La Torre, 2009), signals were scaled between zero and one within subject. Momentary voltage drops and baseline drift was removed, and exponential smoothing (α=0.08) (Holt, 2004) was applied to reduce signal noise.

Next, a dataset is created in which all hot flash events are labeled. Given the presence of reported hot flashes accompanied by no skin conductance changes and the known psychological influences on hot flash reporting limiting the validity of reports (Thurston, Blumenthal, Babyak, & Sherwood, 2005), and consistent with our prior work (Thurston, Matthews, Hernandez, & De La Torre, 2009), all data were labeled via expert-defined physiologic hot flashes. Skin conductance changes associated with hot flashes show a sharp and rapid rise following by a sloping return to baseline, or “swishy tail” that can be distinguished from the “sawtooth” pattern characteristic of activity or other sweating-related artifact (Carpenter, Andrykowski, Freedman, & Munn, 1999). Thus, all data were visually reviewed and hot flash and non-hot flash intervals labeled, based upon the characteristic skin conductance shape of the hot flash rather than solely the magnitude of the rise. The reliability of this manual labeling from two separate coders was κ=.86. All available skin conductance training data is then divided into smaller segments belonging to one of two classes (hot flash or non-hot flash).

The SVM model is then trained, learning the characteristics associated with hot flash and non-hot flash segments. Key features of the hot flash-associated skin conductance rises versus the non-hot flash associated skin conductance are extracted, and a non linear decision boundary is used to separate hot flash and non-hot flash events (see Appendix). Once the SVM model has been trained, the performance of this model is evaluated on a new set of testing sequences. The same pre-processing steps, data segmentation and feature extraction techniques are used for the testing set. We implemented a leave-one-subject-out strategy, useful when limited quantities of data are available for training and testing (Witten & Frank, 2005). The leave-one-subject-out cross-validation approach uses a single subject's session from the original sample as testing, and the remaining sessions (other subjects) as the training data from which the SVM is learned. This is repeated with a re-training of the SVM in each round, such that each session is used only once as testing, and the remainder as training data. The SVM model classifies each new segment of skin conductance data in this testing set as a hot flash or non-hot flash, and the performance of the algorithm is calculated.

LIBSVM, a publicly available MATLAB library, is used to implement the core of SVM training and testing (Chih-Chung Chang and Chih-Jen Lin, LIBSVM: a library for support vector machines, 2001, http://www.csie.ntu.edu.tw/cjlin/libsvm/). Although specialized expertise is required to build an SVM, this publicly available library provides SVM tutorials, sample programming code, and program code libraries that form the core of training and testing of the SVM algorithm. See the Appendix for more technical details of preprocessing, segmentation, features extracting, training and testing of the SVM.

Calculation of performance indices

True positive (TP), false positive (FP), false negative (FN), and true negative (TN) hot flashes were scored relative to self-reported hot flashes and expert-defined hot flashes. Expert label was the primary referent, given the known psychological and related influences on hot flash reporting (Thurston, Blumenthal, Babyak, & Sherwood, 2005; Thurston, Matthews, Hernandez, & De La Torre, 2009), although the referent of self-report was also calculated. For models using self-reported hot flashes as the referent, only waking hours of data were used, given the low reliability of hot flash reporting during sleep(Thurston, Blumenthal, Babyak, & Sherwood, 2006). Given the tendency for participants to report hot flashes multiple times via multiple methods, hot flash reports within 15 minutes were grouped as one report. Physiologic hot flashes were defined both by the conventional criterion as well as SVM. A physiologic hot flash was considered concordant with a report/expert label if it was met by a report/label within 5 minutes before and 20 after the physiologic hot flash. The 5 minute pre-flash interval is consistent with the published literature (Carpenter, Andrykowski, Freedman, & Munn, 1999); the 20 minute post-flash interval was selected for consistency with the traditional inter-flash lockout period, to account for the duration over which skin conductance changes associated with a single hot flash occur, and after empirical examination of the distribution of hot flash reporting revealing some delay in hot flash reporting in the ambulatory setting. A TP was a physiologic hot flash prediction met by an expert-labeled/self-reported reported hot flash. A FP was a physiologic hot flash prediction not met by an expert-labeled/self-reported hot flash. A FN was an expert label/self-report not followed by a physiologic hot flash prediction. A TN was all 20-minute intervals lacking both an expert label/self-report and a physiologic hot flash. For each physiologic hot flash detection method (conventional, SVM), sensitivity (TP/TP+FN), specificity (TN/FP+TN), positive predictive value (PPV; TP/TP+FP), negative predictive value (NPV; TN/TN+FN), and F1 (2 TN /(2 TN + FP + FN)) were calculated. Sensitivity corresponds to the percentage of labeled/reported hot flashes accompanied by a hot flash prediction, specificity to the percentage of 20-minute segments without a labeled/reported hot flash also lacking a hot flash prediction, PPV to the percentage of hot flash predictions also accompanied by a labeled/reported hot flash, NPV to the percentage of 20-minute segments without a hot flash prediction that also lack a labeled/reported hot flash. F1 provides an overall index of performance, a summary index unbiased by the large number of true negatives in the sample (Jardine & van Rijsbergen, 2002).

Data analysis

Comparisons of performance indices by subject characteristics were performed using Spearman's rho, Pearson correlation coefficients, and linear and logistic regression, with transformation of rates as necessary. Comparisons between SVM and conventional criterion performance were conducted using paired t-tests. Due to skew, transformations were applied to meet models assumptions of normality: all SVM performance indices were exponentiated for models with the referent of expert label, specificity was exponentiated for SVM models using the referent of self-report, specificity was exponentiated for conventional criterion for both referents, and NPV and PPV exponentiated for the conventional criterion with the referent of expert label. Statistical comparisons for PPV were limited by a zero denominator for multiple PPV values; these observations were set to missing, and sample sizes noted when deviating from the full sample. Variations in performance by the subject characteristics race/ethnicity, BMI, and state and trait anxiety were evaluated in linear regression models, with each performance index considered separately. Since state and trait anxiety were highly correlated (0.67, p<.0001), and findings for trait anxiety were stronger than those for state anxiety, only trait anxiety findings are reported here. Analyses were performed using SAS v.9.2 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC) and MATLAB v.7.0 (MathWorks, Natick, Massachusetts). Tests were 2-sided with α=.05.

Results

Participants were on average 53 years old, postmenopausal, and overweight (Table 1). Women reported on average 10 hot flashes during the waking hours of the ambulatory monitoring. An average of 24.62 (SD=2.86) monitoring hours were conducted per woman.

Table 1. Subject characteristics.

N 31
Age, M (SD) 53.0 (4.8)
Race, n (%)
 White 17 (54.8)
 Black 14 (45.2)
Education, n (%)
 High school or less 9 (29.0)
 Vocational/some college 13 (41.9)
 College or higher 9 (29.0)
Menopausal stautus, n (%)
 Postmenopausal 19 (61.29)
 Late perimenopaual 12 (38.71)
BMI, M (SD) 28.7 (6.0)
Marital Status, n (%)
 Never Married 7 (22.6)
 Married 17 (54.8)
 Divorced/widowed 7 (22.6)
Current smoker, n (%) 8 (25.81)
Parous, n (%) 24 (77.4)
State anxiety, M (SD) 29.4 (7.2)
Trait anxiety, M (SD) 32.5 (6.8)
CESD, M (SD) 5.0 (3.6)
Reported hot flashes (waking), Median (IQR) 10 (7)
Conventionally-defined hot flashes, Median (IQR) 10 (18)
SVM-defined hot flashes, Median (IQR) 14 (10)

Waking hours only;

Waking and sleeping hours

Conventional criterion

The conventional 2 μmho criterion showed low sensitivity relative to both expert-labeled and subject-reported hot flashes (Table 2). Further, the performance of the conventional criterion significantly varied by BMI. Consistent with prior findings in the laboratory (Thurston, Matthews, Hernandez, & De La Torre, 2009), when compared to expert labeled hot flashes, the conventional criterion showed lower sensitivity (b (SE) = -2.44 (1.13), p=0.04) and a lower F1 score (b (SE) = -2.45 (1.09), p=0.03) as BMI, considered as a continuous variable, increased (Table 3). Using the referent of subject reports, with higher BMI, sensitivity of the conventional criterion was lower (b (SE) =-2.80 (1.14), p=0.02), specificityexp higher (b=511.59 (210.20), p=0.02), and NPVexp lower (b (SE)=-510.31 (192.44), p=0.01). Thus, the conventional criterion differentially missed both the expert-labeled and subject-reported hot flashes among heavier women. Comparisons by obesity status category are presented in Table 3. There were no differences in the performance of the conventional criterion for either referent by anxiety or race/ethnicity.

Table 2. Performance of sternal skin conductance 2 μmho/30 sec criterion to classify hot flashes.

Referent

Expert-defined hot flashes Subject-reported hot flashes
Sensitivity 0.57 0.52
Specificity 0.98 0.92
PPV 0.91 0.59
NPV 0.90 0.92
F1 0.60 0.44

Note: N=31 for all models except PPV for expert-defined hot flashes referent (N=24), and PPV for subject-reported hot flash referent (N=23)

Table 3. Performance of conventional criterion (2 μmho/30 sec criterion) to classify hot flashes from sternal skin conductance by obesity status.

Referent: expert-defined hot flashes Referent: subject-reported hot flashes

Normal
(N=10)
Overweight
(N=7)
Obese
(N=14)
Normal
(N=10)
Overweight
(N=7)
Obese
(N=14)
Sensitivity 0.75 0.63 0.41 0.69 0.68 0.32*
Specificity 0.98 0.99 0.99 0.86 0.91 0.96*
PPV 0.94 0.93 0.86 0.51 0.64 0.63
NPV 0.93 0.90 0.88 0.96 0.93 0.88*
F1 0.77 0.68 0.44 0.53 0.60 0.30

Note: Normal weight BMI <25 (referent), Overweight BMI 25-29.9, Obese BMI ≥30; Low state anxiety ≤27.5 (referent), high state anxiety >27.5; N's are as noted except PPV for expert-defined hot flashes referent (normal weight N=9, overweight N=6, obese N=9) and PPV for subject-reported hot flashes referent (normal weight N=9, overweight N=6, obese N=8)

*

p<0.05, obesity status comparison

Similar to laboratory findings (Thurston, Matthews, Hernandez, & De La Torre, 2009), the magnitude of the 30-second skin conductance rises associated with hot flashes significantly and inversely varied by BMI (r=-0.36, p=0.045). Particularly low rises were observed among the obese women (M (SD)=1.62 (1.28)) in contrast to normal weight (M (SD)=2.93 (1.05)) and overweight women (M (SD)=2.68 (1.41); F (2, 28) = 3.73, p=0.04). Thus, the attenuated skin conductance rises associated with obesity were likely misclassified using the conventional single threshold criterion.

SVM

Application of SVMs improved the performance of sternal skin conductance-assessed hot flashes when compared to both expert-labeled hot flashes and self-reported hot flashes (Table 4). These improvements were statistically significant when compared to the conventional criterion in the case of sensitivity (t(30)=4.47, p<0.0001), NPV (t(30)=3.92, p=0.0005) and F1 score (t(30)=4.40, p=0.0001) for the referent of expert labeled hot flashes, and the F1 score (t(30)=3.12, p=0.004) and lesser extent sensitivity (t(30)=1.88, p=0.07) for the referent of subject-reported hot flashes.

Table 4. Performance of sternal skin conductance using SVM methods to classify hot flashes.

Referent: expert-defined hot flashes Referent: subject-reported hot flashes
Sensitivity 0.87 0.61
Specificity 0.97 0.91
PPV 0.90 0.61
NPV 0.96 0.92
F1 0.88 0.56

Note: N=31 for all models except PPV for subject-reported hot flash as referent (N=28)

No differences were observed in the performance of SVM models by BMI for the expert label referent. The one condition in which SVM performance varied by BMI was for NPV when using the referent of subject report (b (SE) =-0.34 (0.16), p=0.04). Thus, more of the 20-min intervals containing a hot flash report among higher BMI women were not accompanied by an SVM hot flash. Comparisons by obesity status category are presented in Table 5.

Table 5. Performance of SVM to classify hot flashes from sternal skin conductance by obesity status.

Referent: expert-defined hot flashes Referent: subject-reported hot flashes

Normal
(N=10)
Overweight
(N=7)
Obese
(N=14)
Normal
(N=10)
Overweight
(N=7)
Obese
(N=14)
Sensitivity 0.92 0.89 0.84 0.71 0.72 0.48
Specificity 0.98 0.95 0.98 0.88 0.90 0.94
PPV 0.95 0.88 0.88 0.55 0.66 0.63
NPV 0.97 0.97 0.96 0.95 0.92 0.90
F1 0.93 0.88 0.85 0.60 0.62 0.50

Note: Normal weight BMI <25 (referent), Overweight BMI 25-29.9, Obese BMI ≥30; N's are as noted except PPV for subject-reported hot flashes as referent (normal weight N=10, overweight N=6, obese N=12)

Similar to prior work (Thurston, Blumenthal, Babyak, & Sherwood, 2005; Thurston, Matthews, Hernandez, & De La Torre, 2009), for the referent of self-reported hot flashes only, women higher in trait anxiety had a somewhat lower F1 score for SVM models (b(SE)=-1.36(0.68), p=0.05). There were no differences in the performance of SVM models by anxiety for the expert label referent, nor by race/ethnicity for either referent.

Additional analyses

We compared the two referents, self-report and expert label, to understand how differently these two referents characterized the data. Relative to expert labels, self-report had a sensitivity of 0.57, a specificity of .96, a PPV of .80, a NPV of .87 and an F1 score of 0.63. This pattern can be interpreted as a tendency to under-report hot flashes, similar to prior work (Carpenter, Monahan, & Azzouz, 2004). Further, given our prior work showing associations between anxiety and the reporting of hot flashes (Thurston, Blumenthal, Babyak, & Sherwood, 2005; Thurston, Matthews, Hernandez, & De La Torre, 2009), we evaluated the association between anxiety and reporting of hot flashes by comparing reported hot flashes to our referent of expert labeled hot flashes. These models indicated that higher trait anxiety was associated with lower specificityexp (b (SE)=445.83 (130.00), p=0.003), lower PPV (b (SE)=-1.76 (0.72), p=0.02), and a lower F1 score (b (SE)= -1.49 (0.63), p=0.03) of subject reporting of hot flashes when compared to expert labeled hot flashes. Further, higher trait anxiety was associated with the tendency to report hot flashes accompanied by no discernable change in skin conductance (OR (95% CI) =1.22 (1.03-1.45), p=0.02). Thus, when subject reports were compared to our primary referent of expert labels, trait anxiety was associated with elevated hot flash reporting, even in the absence of any skin conductance change.

Discussion

The present investigation showed that the conventional criterion had low sensitivity. Moreover, the performance of this conventional criterion varied by BMI, showing particularly low sensitivity among women with higher BMI. Applying SVMs improved the performance of sternal skin conductance in detecting hot flashes. Further, variations by obesity status were reduced with use of SVMs.

This investigation extends our prior work in the laboratory (Thurston, Matthews, Hernandez, & De La Torre, 2009) to the ambulatory setting. While the ambulatory setting is the primary setting in which clinical research on hot flashes is carried out, particularly clinical trials, the ambulatory setting is a more difficult setting for physiologic hot flash measurement, characterized by greater baseline shifts, variations in signal quality, and artifact from factors such as activity, which can produce skin conductance elevations that meet the conventional threshold. SVMs are a class of machine learning models that are particularly well-suited to these types of complex pattern recognition problems (Guyon, Weston, Barnhill, & Vapnik, 2002; Joachims, 1998; Michel & Kaliouby, 2003). They present several advantages over the conventional criterion. In contrast to the conventional criterion's application of a single threshold, SVMs characterize the shape of the skin conductance changes associated with the hot flashes versus artifact. This approach not only more carefully quantifies the hot flash, but can reduce systematic variation by subject characteristics such as BMI, which may reduce the magnitude of hot flash-associated skin conductance rises. Further attenuating systematic variation by subject characteristics is SVMs' ability to be developed to vary by key subject characteristics, or ultimately, by woman. Multiple sources of information, such as subject characteristics, or dynamic physiologic or subjective information, can also be used in training SVM models (Bundele & Banerjee, 2009; Sommer, Golz, Trutschel, & Edwards, 2008). Finally, SVMs will ultimately classify hot flashes in a largely automated fashion, reducing the extensive visual editing characteristic of the conventional criterion approach. A valid, automated coding approach for physiologic hot flash measures is essential to the use of these measures with larger samples and by a wider range of researchers (Miller & Li, 2004).

Similar to our prior findings in the laboratory (Thurston, Matthews, Hernandez, & De La Torre, 2009) variations in the performance of the conventional criterion were observed by BMI. Lower performance of the conventional criterion, particularly low sensitivity, was observed with higher BMI, with strikingly poor performance among obese women. Skin conductance recordings among higher BMI women are often characterized by greater artifact (ambient sweating). However, similar to our findings in the laboratory, the magnitude of hot flash-associated skin conductance rises were also attenuated among higher BMI women. The conventional threshold's single magnitude-based threshold frequently misclassified these smaller magnitude hot flashes among higher BMI women as nonevents. Thus, with use of the conventional criterion, bias by BMI is introduced, particularly worrisome as obesity is a leading risk factor for hot flashes (Gold, et al., 2006; Thurston, et al., 2008) and the majority of women in the US are overweight or obese (Flegal, Carroll, Ogden, & Curtin, 2010). These BMI differences in performance were reduced with application of SVMs. Notably, SVMs characterize the pattern, as opposed to only the amplitude of the rise, of sternal skin conductance changes associated with hot flashes.

In our previous work, we showed that elevated anxiety assessed at the time of the laboratory testing was associated with poorer performance of conventional and SVM models when compared to self-report, largely due to elevated reporting of hot flashes lacking any physiologic change among more anxious women (Thurston, Matthews, Hernandez, & De La Torre, 2009). In this ambulatory investigation, anxiety showed less pronounced and consistent associations with hot flash reporting than in the laboratory setting. However, trait anxiety was associated with elevated hot flash reporting when self-reports were compared to expert labels, the leading referent. Trait anxiety was also associated with the tendency to report hot flashes in the absence of any discernable skin conductance changes. Finally, higher trait anxious women had somewhat lower F1 scores for SVM models when compared to self-report. Notably, anxiety has been shown to be a leading predictor of hot flash reporting in epidemiologic investigations (Freeman, et al., 2005; Gold, et al., 2006) as well as elevated ambulatory hot flash reporting when compared to physiologically assessed hot flashes (Thurston, Blumenthal, Babyak, & Sherwood, 2005). These findings are consistent with a large body of literature linking anxiety to elevated symptom reporting (Cohen, et al., 1995; Pennebaker, 1982).

Two referents were considered here: expert labeled hot flashes and self-reported hot flashes. While self-reported hot flashes are traditionally considered as a referent, the many reporting biases make this a less ideal referent, including subject non-adherence, inability to reliably report hot flashes during sleep, as well as psychological factors discussed above. Reporting may be particularly unreliable in the ambulatory setting, characterized by multiple distractions, attentional demands, and settings in which hot flash reporting may be impossible (e.g., driving). Thus, key performance indices may be somewhat lower with the referent of self-report in the ambulatory versus laboratory setting (Thurston, Matthews, Hernandez, & De La Torre, 2009). No other gold standard measures of hot flashes exist. Coders can be trained to visually distinguish hot flashes from skin conductance signals with high reliability as demonstrated here. Therefore, consistent with our prior work (Thurston, Matthews, Hernandez, & De La Torre, 2009), we present findings considering both self-report and expert-labeled hot flashes as a referent. However, due to its demand for rigorous training and extensive personnel coding time, this approach is not feasible for larger investigations or for longer monitoring durations, underscoring the need for development of a more automated approach.

Several limitations deserve mention. First, the study sample was relatively small. While the SVM models were stable as the number of hot flashes across women was high, certain secondary comparisons by subject characteristics involved fairly small numbers, limiting power. In calculating PPV, particularly for the conventional criterion, zero denominator problems (no physiologic hot flashes) further limited sample size PPV. Further, while SVM libraries and documentation are publicly available, building SVM models from raw data requires specialized experience. Finally, this SVM model was a preliminary model for the ambulatory setting requiring further elaboration, refinement, and further testing in independent samples. With further testing, a final model can be made available to the wider scientific community.

This study has several strengths. This study was a detailed analysis of subjectively reported and physiologically recorded hot flashes over 24 hours, allowing a careful comparison of these indices. This study included two racial/ethnic groups, allowing validation of these ambulatory measurement approaches across these groups. This study assessed multiple physiologic and psychological characteristics, allowing comparison of the performance of these measurement approaches by these subject characteristics. Most importantly, this study is the first to apply SVMs to quantifying hot flashes physiologically in the ambulatory setting. This approach improved the detection of hot flashes, further advancing the physiologic measurement of hot flashes.

The present research showed that the standard single threshold criterion to quantify hot flashes physiologically had poor performance, particularly among higher BMI women which may have attenuated skin conductance rises with hot flashes. This research indicates the importance of using more sophisticated pattern recognition models, SVMs, to characterize hot flashes from physiologic signals, models which improved the overall performance of this index and reduced variation by BMI. This improved method to quantify hot flashes has the potential to further advance research on hot flashes, supporting research aimed at better understanding the physiology of hot flashes as well as the development of new treatments for hot flashes.

Acknowledgments

This publication was supported by The Fannie E. Rippel Foundation/American Federation Research New Investigator Award on Gender Differences in Aging (PI: Thurston), the Caja Madrid Foundation (Hernandez), the National Institutes of Health (K23AG029216), and the Pittsburgh Mind-Body Center (National Institute of Heath grants HL076852/076858).

References

  1. Avis NE, Colvin A, Bromberger JT, Hess R, Matthews KA, Ory M, Schocken M. Change in health-related quality of life over the menopausal transition in a multiethnic cohort of middle-aged women: Study of Women's Health Across the Nation. Menopause. 2009;16:860–869. doi: 10.1097/gme.0b013e3181a3cdaf. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Avis NE, Ory M, Matthews KA, Schocken M, Bromberger J, Colvin A. Health-related quality of life in a multiethnic sample of middle-aged women: Study of Women's Health Across the Nation (SWAN) Med Care. 2003;41:1262–1276. doi: 10.1097/01.MLR.0000093479.39115.AF. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Barnabei VM, Cochrane BB, Aragaki AK, Nygaard I, Williams RS, McGovern PG, Young RL, Wells EC, O'Sullivan MJ, Chen B, Schenken R, Johnson SR. Menopausal symptoms and treatment-related effects of estrogen and progestin in the Women's Health Initiative. Obstet Gynecol. 2005;105:1063–1073. doi: 10.1097/01.AOG.0000158120.47542.18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Barnabei VM, Grady D, Stovall DW, Cauley JA, Lin F, Stuenkel CA, Stefanick ML, Pickar JH. Menopausal symptoms in older women and the effects of treatment with hormone therapy. Obstet Gynecol. 2002;100:1209–1218. doi: 10.1016/s0029-7844(02)02369-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bromberger JT, Matthews KA, Schott LL, Brockwell S, Avis NE, Kravitz HM, Everson-Rose SA, Gold EB, Sowers M, Randolph JF., Jr Depressive symptoms during the menopausal transition: the Study of Women's Health Across the Nation (SWAN) J Affect Disord. 2007;103:267–272. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2007.01.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bundele M, Banerjee R. An SVM classifier for fatigue-detection using skin conductance for use in the BITS-Lifeguard Wearable Computing System. International Conference on Emerging Trends in Engineering & Technology; Nagpur, Maharashtra, India. 2009. pp. 934–939. [Google Scholar]
  7. Carpenter JS, Andrykowski MA, Freedman RR, Munn R. Feasibility and psychometrics of an ambulatory hot flash monitoring device. Menopause. 1999;6:209–215. doi: 10.1097/00042192-199906030-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Carpenter JS, Monahan PO, Azzouz F. Accuracy of subjective hot flush reports compared with continuous sternal skin conductance monitoring. Obstet Gynecol. 2004;104:1322–1326. doi: 10.1097/01.AOG.0000143891.79482.ee. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cohen S, Doyle WJ, Skoner DP, Fireman P, Gwaltney JM, Jr, Newsom JT. State and trait negative affect as predictors of objective and subjective symptoms of respiratory viral infections. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1995;68:159–169. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.68.1.159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Crandall CJ, Zheng Y, Crawford SL, Thurston RC, Gold EB, Johnston JM, Greendale GA. Presence of vasomotor symptoms is associated with lower bone mineral density: a longitudinal analysis. Menopause. 2009;16:239–246. doi: 10.1097/gme.0b013e3181857964. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. de Bakker IP, Everaerd W. Measurement of menopausal hot flushes: validation and cross-validation. Maturitas. 1996;25:87–98. doi: 10.1016/0378-5122(96)01046-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Dormire SL, Carpenter JS. An alternative to Unibase/glycol as an effective nonhydrating electrolyte medium for the measurement of electrodermal activity. Psychophysiology. 2002;39:423–426. doi: 10.1017.S0048577201393149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Flegal KM, Carroll MD, Ogden CL, Curtin LR. Prevalence and trends in obesity among US adults, 1999-2008. JAMA. 2010;303:235–241. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.2014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Freedman RR. Laboratory and ambulatory monitoring of menopausal hot flashes. Psychophysiology. 1989;26:573–579. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1989.tb00712.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Freeman EW, Sammel MD, Lin H, Gracia CR, Kapoor S, Ferdousi T. The role of anxiety and hormonal changes in menopausal hot flashes. Menopause. 2005;12:258–266. doi: 10.1097/01.gme.0000142440.49698.b7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Gold E, Colvin A, Avis N, Bromberger J, Greendale G, Powell L, Sternfeld B, Matthews K. Longitudinal analysis of vasomotor symptoms and race/ethnicity across the menopausal transition: Study of Women's Health Across the Nation (SWAN) Am J Public Health. 2006;96:1226–1235. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2005.066936. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Guyon I, Weston J, Barnhill S, Vapnik V. Gene selection for cancer classification using support vector machines. Machine Learning. 2002;46:389–422. [Google Scholar]
  18. Hanisch LJ, Palmer SC, Donahue A, Coyne JC. Validation of sternal skin conductance for detection of hot flashes in prostate cancer survivors. Psychophysiology. 2007;44:189–193. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00492.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Holt CC. Forecasting seasonals and trends by exponentially weighted moving averages. International Journal of Forecasting. 2004;20:5–10. [Google Scholar]
  20. Jardine N, van Rijsbergen C. The use of hierarchic clustering in information retrieval. Information Storage and Retrieval. 2002;7:217–240. [Google Scholar]
  21. Joachims T. Text categorization with support vector machines: Learning with many relevant features. Proceedings of the European Conference on Machine Learning; Berlin: Springer; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  22. Michel P, Kaliouby RE. Real time facial expression recognition in video using support vector machines. Proceedings of the 5th international conference on multimodal interfaces; New York: Association for Computing Machinery; 2003. pp. 258–264. [Google Scholar]
  23. Miller HG, Li RM. Measuring hot flashes: summary of a National Institutes of Health workshop. Mayo Clin Proc. 2004;79:777–781. doi: 10.4065/79.6.777. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Pennebaker J. The psychology of physical symptoms. New York: Springer-Verlag; 1982. [Google Scholar]
  25. Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement. 1977;1:385–401. [Google Scholar]
  26. Rossouw JE, Anderson GL, Prentice RL, LaCroix AZ, Kooperberg C, Stefanick ML, Jackson RD, Beresford SA, Howard BV, Johnson KC, Kotchen JM, Ockene J. Risks and benefits of estrogen plus progestin in healthy postmenopausal women: principal results From the Women's Health Initiative randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2002;288:321–333. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.3.321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Sievert LL. Variation in sweating patterns: implications for studies of hot flashes through skin conductance. Menopause. 2007;14:742–751. doi: 10.1097/gme.0b013e3180577841. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Sievert LL, Freedman RR, Garcia JZ, Foster JW, del Carmen Romano Soriano M, Longcope C, Franz C. Measurement of hot flashes by sternal skin conductance and subjective hot flash report in Puebla, Mexico. Menopause. 2002;9:367–376. doi: 10.1097/00042192-200209000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Sommer D, Golz M, Trutschel U, Edwards D. Assessing driver's hypovigilance from biosignals. 4th European Conference of the International Federation for Medical and Biological Engineering; 2008. pp. 152–155. [Google Scholar]
  30. Spielberger CD. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  31. Thurston RC, Blumenthal JA, Babyak MA, Sherwood A. Emotional antecedents of hot flashes during daily life. Psychosom Med. 2005;67:137–146. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000149255.04806.07. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Thurston RC, Blumenthal JA, Babyak MA, Sherwood A. The association between hot flashes, sleep complaints, and psychological functioning among healthy menopausal women. Int J Behav Med. 2006;13:163–172. doi: 10.1207/s15327558ijbm1302_8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Thurston RC, Matthews KA, Hernandez J, De La Torre F. Improving the performance of physiologic hot flash measures with support vector machines. Psychophysiology. 2009;46:285–292. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00770.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Thurston RC, Sowers MR, Chang Y, Sternfeld B, Gold EB, Johnston JM, Matthews KA. Adiposity and reporting of vasomotor symptoms among midlife women: the study of women's health across the nation. Am J Epidemiol. 2008;167:78–85. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwm244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Thurston RC, Sutton-Tyrrell K, Everson-Rose SA, Hess R, Matthews KA. Hot flashes and subclinical cardiovascular disease: Findings from the Study of Women's Health Across the Nation Heart Study. Circulation. 2008;118:1234–1240. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.108.776823. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Witten I, Frank E. Data mining: Practical machine learning tools and techniques. San Francisco: Morgan Kaufmann Publishers; 2005. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES