Abstract
Rationale
5-HT1A receptor knockout (1AKO) mice have a robust anxiety phenotype. Tissue-specific “rescue” strategies and electrophysiology have implicated a critical role for postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors, particularly in the CA1 region of the hippocampus.
Objectives
In this study, we evaluated differences in membrane properties and synaptic activity in CA1 hippocampal pyramidal cells between 1AKOs and wild-type (WT) controls to better understand the cellular correlates of anxiety in this mouse model.
Methods
Whole-cell patch-clamp recordings were conducted in CA1 pyramidal cells in hippocampal brain slices from 1AKOs and WTs that had previously been screened for anxiety with the elevated-plus maze. Spontaneous miniature inhibitory and excitatory postsynaptic currents (IPSCs and EPSCs) and stimulus-evoked eIPSCs and eEPSCs were recorded in addition to the effect of the benzodiazepine agonist diazepam or the inverse agonist FG 7142 on γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA)ergic eIPSCs.
Results
Evoked EPSC amplitude was greater in 1AKOs than WTs. When subjects were pooled across genotypes, anxiety measures correlated with eEPSC amplitude, indicating enhanced postsynaptic glutamate synaptic activity under conditions of synaptic activation in anxious subjects. While GABA synaptic activity and sensitivity to diazepam were not affected by genotype or correlated with anxiety, sensitivity to the anxiogenic FG 7142 was smaller in anxious subjects.
Conclusions
These data indicate enhanced postsynaptic glutamate receptor sensitivity and decreased GABAergic inhibition by a benzodiazepine inverse agonist in CA1 hippocampal neurons of anxious mice are produced by deletion of the 5-HT1A receptor. These data provide new information about interactions between 5-HT, GABA, and glutamate systems during the expression of chronic anxiety.
Keywords: Serotonin, 5-HT1A receptor, Hippocampus, Anxiety, Elevated-plus maze, Electrophysiology
Introduction
Dysfunction of γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA) and serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine, 5-HT) systems have been implicated in anxiety disorders (for review, see Charney and Drevets 2002; Millan 2003; Sandford et al. 2000). Much of the evidence for the involvement of these neurochemical systems comes from the finding that certain GABAergic and serotonergic drugs are effective anxiolytics (Bourin and Hascoet 2001). Benzodiazepines, that have anti-anxiety properties, act at the GABAA receptor. In addition, 5-HT has also been linked to anxiety by the observation that activation of 5-HT1A receptors with partial agonists such as buspirone or indirectly through serotonin-selective reuptake inhibitors are effective treatments for anxiety disorders (for review, see Lucki 1996; Ravindran and Stein 2010).
Animal studies have shown that mice with a genetic deletion of the 5-HT1A receptor have a behavioral anxiety phenotype, making 5-HT1A knockouts (1AKOs) a useful model to study anxiety disorders (Heisler et al. 1998; Parks et al. 1998; Ramboz et al. 1998). The behavioral phenotype of these mice also includes decreased hippocampal-dependent learning (Sarnyai et al. 2000) and an antidepressant-like response in tail-suspension and forced swim test models (Heisler et al. 1998; Mayorga et al. 2001; Ramboz et al. 1998). The 1AKO mouse anxiety model is further validated by the finding that 5-HT1A receptors are downregulated in patients with panic disorder (Neumeister et al. 2004). Conditional tissue-specific rescue strategies have more recently implicated a critical role of postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors in the forebrain in anxiety-like behaviors. Specifically, Gross et al. (2002) found that restoration of postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors in the hippocampus but not somatodendritic 5-HT1A autoreceptors in the raphe nuclei re-established normal anxiety-like behavior. In addition, Gross et al. (2002) found that a successful rescue of the anxiety phenotype in these mice was determined by receptor expression early in development, prior to postnatal day 21, rather than in adulthood. This critical period during which forebrain 5-HT1A receptors must be expressed for normal maturation of emotion/anxiety circuits was later refined through pharmacological blockade of the receptor to postnatal days 13 through 34 (Lo and Gross 2008). 1AKO mice have also been shown to have an enhanced neuronal excitability in area CA1 of the hippocampus as well as magnified hippocampal theta oscillations, particularly in anxiety-provoking conditions (Gordon et al. 2005). These data indicate that deletion of the 5-HT1A receptor may cause altered synaptic activity and signaling, specifically in the CA1 region of the hippocampus.
While there is a growing literature generally implicating the ventral rather than the dorsal hippocampus in anxiety (for a recent review, see Fanselow and Dong 2010), the majority of studies characterizing the specific role of the hippocampal 5-HT1A receptor in anxiety, including local administration studies, have indicated a preferential role of the dorsal subdivision (reviewed in Menard and Treit 1999). Stimulation of 5-HT1A receptors in dorsal hippocampus has primarily anxiolytic effects (Carli et al. 1993; Graeff et al. 1996; Jolas et al. 1995; Kataoka et al. 1991; Kostowski et al. 1989; Menard and Treit 1998; Schreiber and De 1993; Stefanski et al. 1993) and occasionally anxiogenic effects (Andrews et al. 1994; Dos et al. 2008; File et al. 1996) in animal models. By contrast, stimulation of 5-HT1A receptors in ventral hippocampus are without effect (File and Gonzalez 1996; Hogg et al. 1994). In addition, Gordon et al. (2005) examined neurophysiological correlates of anxiety in 1AKO mice and found the 1AKOs had elevated theta oscillations in dorsal hippocampus when animals were exposed to the elevated-plus maze (EPM) and further that the magnitude of theta oscillations correlated with behavioral anxiety measures. Similar studies from this group have more recently implicated ventral hippocampus as well in the anxiogenic phenotype of the 1AKO mice (Adhikari et al. 2010). It is clear that both subfields of the hippocampus may contribute to the anxiety phenotype of this mouse model, but the current study focuses first on the role of the dorsal hippocampus while reserving investigations of the ventral hippocampus for future studies.
The purpose of the present study was to further evaluate differences in membrane properties, GABA and glutamatergic synaptic activity, and responses to benzodiazepines at the single-cell level in CA1 pyramidal cells of the dorsal hippocampus. 1AKO mice and wild-type (WT) controls were pre-screened for anxiety in the EPM and later electrophysiology experiments performed using whole-cell patch-clamp techniques in hippocampal brain slices to better understand the cellular correlates of anxiety in this animal model.
Materials and methods
Subjects
Male homozygote 5-HT1A receptor knockout and wild-type mice on a 129/Sv background (for details, see Phillips et al. 1999) were generated by homozygous mutant or wild-type breeding colonies at Temple University. These homozygote breeding colonies were derived from heterozygous breeding pairs generously provided by Dr. Mark Geyer at UCSD, San Diego, CA. While homozygous breeding colonies are at risk for genetic drift due to spontaneous gene mutations that develop over-successive generations (Crawley 2007), random mating strategies were employed to minimize this confounding factor. As a test for phenotype changes due to genetic drift, behavioral data (see anxiety and locomotor measures described in the EPM section below) from the initial 1AKO litters were compared with data from the final 1AKO litters (and a similar comparison made in the WT group) and no statistical difference was obtained. Mice were 3–6 months of age at the time of study and were housed 4 per cage on a 12 h light schedule (lights on at 0,700 h) in a temperature-controlled (20°C) colony room. Mice were given access to standard mouse chow and water ad libitum. Animal protocols were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee and were conducted in accordance with the NIH Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals.
Elevated-plus maze
Mice were pre-screened for anxiety in a 50-cm-high black Plexiglas EPM. Lighting conditions were adjusted using a single fluorescent lamp on the room periphery to achieve dim overall illumination but to maintain visibility on the video recordings. The EPM consisted of two open arms (28×7 cm), with a 0.5 cm lip on each open arm, and two enclosed arms (30×7×13.5 cm) that extended from a central platform (7×7 cm). Subjects were videotaped in the maze for 10 min. Time spent and numbers of entries into open and closed EPM arms and EPM center were measured in the first 5 min of EPM exposure. Time spent and the number of entries into open arms are considered anxiety measures; whereas, the number of total or closed arm entries are considered locomotor measures (File 2001; Hogg 1996).
Brain slice preparation
In order to ensure that exposure to the EPM did not influence electrophysiological measures, animals were returned to the animal colony for a minimum of 3 weeks following the EPM test. Mice were rapidly decapitated and the head placed in ice cold artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF) in which sucrose (248 mM) was substituted for NaCl. The brain was rapidly removed, dorsal hippocampal slices (250 μm thick) were cut using a Leica microslicer (Leica, Allendale, NJ) or Vibratome 3000 Plus (Vibratome, St. Louis, MO) and placed in a holding vial containing ACSF at 35°C bubbled with 95% O2/5% CO2 for 1 h. Slices were then maintained in room temperature ACSF bubbled with 95% O2/5%CO2. The composition of the ACSF was (mM), NaCl 124, KCl 2.5, NaH2PO4 2, CaCl2 2.5, MgSO4 2, Dextrose 10, and NaHCO3 26.
Electrophysiological recording
Slices were transferred to a recording chamber (Warner Instruments, Hamden, CT) and continuously perfused with ACSF at 1.5–2.0 ml/min at 32–34°C maintained by an inline solution heater (TC-324, Warner Instruments). Only one cell was recorded per brain slice. CA1 pyramidal neurons were visualized using a Nikon E600 upright microscope fitted with a 40× water-immersion objective, differential interference contrast and infrared filter (Optical Apparatus, Ardmore, PA). The image from the microscope was enhanced using a CCD camera and displayed on a computer monitor. Whole-cell recording pipettes were fashioned on a P-97 micropipette puller (Sutter Instruments, Novato, CA) using borosilicate glass capillary tubing (1.2 mm OD, 0.69 mm ID; Warner Instruments). Recording pipettes (4–8MΩ) were filled with an intracellular solution (in mM): K-gluconate 70, KCl 70, NaCl 2, EGTA 4, HEPES 10, adenosine 5′-triphosphate magnesium salt (MgATP) 4, guanosine 5′-triphosphate sodium salt hydrate (NaGTP) 0.3, pH 7.3 for recording inhibitory postsynaptic currents (IPSCs) or (in mM): K-gluconate 120, KCl 10, MgCl2 1, HEPES 10, EGTA 0.02, MgATP 2, Na-phosphocreatinine 10, NaGTP 0.5, pH 7.3 for recording excitatory postsynaptic currents (EPSCs).
The majority of the cells recorded were pre-screened for the presence of strong spike frequency accommodation in response to a depolarizing current pulse which distinguishes pyramidal cells from interneurons (Freund and Buzsaki 1996; Lacaille et al. 1987). A visualized cell was approached with the electrode, a gigaohm seal established and the cell membrane ruptured to obtain a whole-cell recording using a Multiclamp 700B amplifier (Axon Instruments, Foster City, CA). Series resistance was monitored throughout the experiment. If the series resistance was unstable or exceeded four times the electrode resistance, the cell was discarded. Once the whole-cell recording was obtained, IPSCs and EPSCs were recorded in voltage-clamp mode (Vm=−70 mV). Signals were digitized by a Digidata 1320-series analog-to-digital converter and stored on-line using pClamp 9 software (Axon Instruments). Signals were filtered at 1 kHz and digitized at 10 kHz. The liquid junction potential was approximately 9–10 mV between the pipette solution and the ACSF and was not subtracted from the data obtained.
For stimulation experiments, bipolar tungsten stimulating electrodes (World Precision Instruments, Sarasota, FL) were placed ventral (100–200 μm distance) to the recorded cell in the Schaffer collateral pathway. For each cell, stimulus response curves were generated and a stimulus intensity that produced 50% of the maximum response was selected. Paired pulses (50 ms interval, every 20 s) of constant current were delivered with an Isoflex stimulus isolator (A.M.P.I., Jerusalem, Israel). The mean stimulus intensity was 0.62±0.19 mA with a range of 0.02 to 2.5 mA for evoked IPSC (eIPSC) experiments and 0.45±0.10 mA with a range of 0.02 to 3.0 mA for evoked EPSC (eEPSC) experiments.
GABAergic IPSCs were isolated by addition of the non-N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor antagonist 6,7-dinitroquinoxaline-2,3(1H,4H)-dione (DNQX; 20 μM) and the NMDA receptor antagonist DL-2-amino-5-phosphonopentanoic acid (APV; 50 μM) and frequently confirmed by the addition of the GABAA receptor antagonist bicuculline (20 μM) at the end of the experiment. Glutamatergic EPSCs were isolated by the addition of bicuculline (20 μM). Under these recording conditions (holding current=−70 mV with the presence of magnesium in the electrolyte), EPSCs should be mediated by non-NMDA receptors. This is further supported by the finding in multiple cells and in multiple animals that addition of the α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methylisoxazole-4-propionic acid (AMPA)/kainate receptor antagonist DNQX alone (20 μM) at the end of an experiment completely suppressed EPSCs. The decay kinetics of miniature spontaneous EPSCs were rapid and fit by a single exponential that are characteristic of AMPA rather than slower kainate-mediated events (Cossart et al. 2002). Spontaneous inhibitory or excitatory postsynaptic currents (sIPSCs or sEPSCs) were recorded in the absence and miniature spontaneous IPSCs or EPSCs (mIPSCs or mEPSCs) in the presence of tetrodotoxin (1 μM) to block action potential-mediated GABA or glutamate release.
Data analysis
MiniAnalysis software (Synaptosoft, Inc., Decatur, GA) was used to analyze spontaneous and miniature IPSC and EPSC events on the basis of amplitude, rate of rise, duration and area. Initially, noise analysis was conducted for each cell and amplitude detection thresholds set to exceed noise values (usually around 5 pA). Events were automatically selected, analyzed for double peaks, then visually inspected and confirmed. Event amplitude histograms were generated and compared with the noise histogram to ensure that they did not overlap. Synaptic activity was analyzed for frequency, amplitude and baseline holding current. Baseline synaptic activity data was compared between experimental groups by unpaired Student's t-test. To examine tonic inhibition by GABA or tonic excitation by glutamate, the effect of GABA or glutamate receptor blockade on resting membrane potential or holding current was compared between genotypes by two-way repeated measures ANOVA.
Evoked IPSC or EPSC amplitude was calculated by subtracting the peak current from the current obtained during a 5 ms window immediately preceding the stimulus artifact. Baseline eIPSC or eEPSC amplitude was averaged from at least 60 consecutive trials calculated over at least a 10 min period. Paired-pulse ratio (PPR) was calculated as the amplitude of the second eIPSC or eEPSC divided by the amplitude of the first eIPSC or eEPSC, respectively. The effects of diazepam or FG 7142 on eIPSCs were determined from the average of at least 60 consecutive trials 3–6 min following drug application when drug effect had reached steady state. The effect of these drugs on eIPSC amplitude or PPR was analyzed by comparing pre- and postdrug values by paired Student's t-test. Physiological characteristics and measures of synaptic activity were correlated with the percent of time in open arm index of anxiety measured during the EPM test by Pearson product-moment correlation. A probability of p<0.05 was considered significant. Data are reported as mean±SEM.
Drugs
Drugs were bath applied and most chemicals for making the ACSF and electrolyte solution obtained from Sigma–Aldrich (St. Louis, MO). DNQX was purchased from Tocris (St. Louis, MO) and TTX was purchased from Calbiochem (San Diego, CA). FG 7142 was obtained from Sigma-Aldrich and diazepam (DZ) was generously provided by NIDA. Diazepam, FG 7142 and DNQX were dissolved in DMSO (final DMSO concentration in bath 0.015%). Diazepam has been shown to have a KD of 6.9 nM in mouse brain membranes and FG 7142 inhibits [3H]-diazepam binding with an IC50 of 27.9 nM (Allan et al. 1992). We selected concentrations of diazepam (1 μM) and FG 7142 (3 μM) to fully saturate the receptor.
Results
Elevated-plus maze
Figure 1 shows the behavioral data from the EPM model. Figure 1a demonstrates that 1AKOs make significantly fewer open arm entries (as a percent of total arm entries; p<0.01) and spend significantly less time in the open arms (as a percent of total time in the maze; p<0.05) than WT controls. Both of these measures are indications of elevated anxiety in this animal model. Figure 1b shows that there is no effect of genotype on locomotor activity, as indicated by number of total arm entries.
Fig. 1.
1AKOs demonstrated an anxiety phenotype without alterations in locomotor activity when compared with WT controls in the elevated-plus maze (EPM) model. a Summary of the anxiety-like behavior of the 1AKOs in the EPM. 1AKOs had fewer open-arm entries (relative to total entries) and spent less time in the open arms of the EPM (relative to total time in EPM) than WT controls. b Total number of entries into both arms did not differ between groups. Data are represented by mean±SEM. *p<0.05; **p<0.01 (statistically significant differences from WT controls by unpaired Student's t test)
Membrane characteristics and response to GABAA and glutamate receptor blockade
There was no significant effect of genotype on either resting membrane potential (in current-clamp recordings) or holding current (in voltage-clamp recordings). Cells from WT mice had a resting membrane potential of −63.8±1.2 mV (N=27). Cells from 1AKO mice had a resting membrane potential of −64.2±1.1 mV (N=35). In a subset of cells, the effect of blockade of GABAA receptors on resting membrane potential was examined and compared between genotypes. Cells from both WTs and 1AKOs showed a similar depolarization in response to bicuculline [from −66.1±0.8 mV to −63.6±0.8 mV in WTs (N=18) and from −68.0±1.0 mV to −64.4±0.9 mV in 1AKOs (N=12)]. There was a significant main effect of the drug (F(1,28)=50.19; p<0.001) but no genotype×drug interaction (F(1,28)=1.79; n.s.), indicating similar tonic inhibition by GABA in this preparation in the two groups. The effect of glutamate receptor blockade on holding current was also examined under voltage-clamp (I=−70 mV) conditions. A combination of APV and DNQX to block NMDA and non-NMDA receptors produced a small reduction of holding current in both groups [from −22.7±3.5 pA to −31.0±6.0 pA in WTs (N=4) and from −39.1±6.2 pA to −41.5±7.7 pA in IAKOs (N=16)]. There was not a significant main effect of drug or a significant genotype×drug interaction, indicating a mild tonic influence of glutamate that was similar in the two groups.
Glutamate synaptic activity
Fig. 2 compares glutamate synaptic activity characteristics in CA1 hippocampal pyramidal cells under spontaneous (Fig. 2a) and evoked release conditions (Fig. 2b) in WT and 1AKO mice. Figure 2a shows that there is no difference in basal sEPSC or mEPSC frequency or amplitude between the two groups. In addition, there is no difference in mEPSC kinetics (rise or decay time) between the two groups (data not shown). In contrast, Fig. 2b shows that basal eEPSC amplitude is elevated in 1AKOs (p<0.05) when compared with WT controls. Since there is no difference in PPR between the two groups, these data indicate that 1AKOs have elevated postsynaptic AMPA receptor number or sensitivity, relative to controls. In addition, when the two groups are pooled, Fig. 3 shows a significant negative correlation between anxiety measures (percent of time in open arms of the EPM) and eEPSC1 amplitude (R=−.491, p<0.001, N=61) where eEPSC1 indicates the first EPSC evoked by the paired-pulse stimulus. There was also a similar negative correlation between this anxiety measure and eEPSC2 amplitude (R=−.405; p<0.01; data not shown) where eEPSC2 indicates the second EPSC evoked by the paired-pulse stimulus. Similar correlations were also observed when genotype was analyzed separately. There was a significant negative correlation between % time in open arms and eEPSC1 amplitude in the 1AKO group (R=−.425; p<0.05; N=23) and in the WT group (R=−.482; p<0.01; N=38). Nonetheless, these data indicate that anxious subjects, regardless of genotype, have elevations in postsynaptic AMPA receptor number or sensitivity.
Fig. 2.
CA1 hippocampal pyramidal cells recorded from 5-HT1A receptor knockout mice (1AKOs) had greater postsynaptic glutamate receptor sensitivity under evoked release conditions. Figure 2a contains values for basal release conditions; 1AKOs did not significantly differ from WT controls in spontaneous or miniature spontaneous excitatory postsynaptic current (sEPSC or mEPSC) frequency or amplitude. Figure 2b contains the values for evoked potentials; 1AKOs had a higher evoked EPSC1 (eEPSC1) amplitude (p=0.039) but did not differ significantly from WT controls in eEPSC2 amplitude or paired-pulse ratio (PPR). Next to Fig. 2b is an example of an averaged eEPSC1 from a WT (240 pA) and a 1AKO (328 pA). EPSCs were isolated by the addition of the GABAA antagonist bicuculline (20 μM). Spontaneous EPSCs were recorded in the absence and mEPSCs in the presence of tetrodotoxin (1 μM). Evoked EPSCs were elicited by stimulation of the Schaffer collateral pathway and were 50% of the maximum response. Data are expressed as mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM). Abbreviations: amp=amplitude, freq=frequency
Fig. 3.
Anxiety is correlated with increased postsynaptic glutamate receptor density or sensitivity in CA1 hippocampal pyramidal neurons. Regardless of genotype, there is a significant negative correlation between time spent exploring the open arms of the EPM and the glutamatergic eEPSC amplitude
GABA synaptic activity
Table 1 compares GABA synaptic activity characteristics in CA1 hippocampal pyramidal cells under spontaneous and evoked release conditions in WT and 1AKO mice. There is no difference in basal sIPSC or mIPSC frequency or amplitude or eIPSC amplitude or PPR between the two groups. In addition, there is no difference in mIPSC kinetics (rise or decay time) between the two groups (data not shown). These data indicate no effect of genotype on either spontaneous or evoked GABA synaptic activity.
Table 1.
Genotype had no effect on GABA synaptic activity under basal or evoked release conditions in CA1 pyramidal cells
sIPSC freq (Hz) | mIPSC freq (Hz) | sIPSC amp (pA) | mIPSC amp (pA) | |
WT (N=14) | 5.9±1.0 | 4.7±0.9 | 23.1±1.8 | 19.7±2 |
KO (N=19) | 6.1±0.8 | 5.2±0.7 | 24.5±2.6 | 17.6±1.2 |
p | .876 | .687 | .680 | .383 |
eIPSC1 amp (pA) | eIPSC2 amp (pA) | PPR | ||
WT (N=33–46) | 186.8±16.0 | 156.1±14.9 | 0.84±0.04 | |
KO (N=39–58) | 206.7±19.6 | 154.2±13.4 | 0.78±0.04 | |
p | .446 | .925 | .356 |
The data show that 1AKOs did not differ significantly from WT controls in spontaneous or miniature spontaneous inhibitory post synaptic current (sIPSC or mIPSC) frequency or amplitude. The data shows that under evoked release conditions, 1AKOs did not significantly differ from WT controls in eIPSC amplitude or PPR. IPSCs were isolated by the addition of the glutamate receptor antagonists 6,7-dinitroquinoxaline-2,3(1H,4H)-dione (DNQX; 20 μM) and DL-2-amino-5-phosphonopentanoic acid (APV; 50 μM). Spontaneous IPSCs were recorded in the absence and mIPSCs in the presence of tetrodotoxin (1 μM). Evoked IPSCs (eIPSCs) were elicited by stimulation of the Schaffer collateral pathway and were 50% of the maximum response. Data are expressed as mean ± SEM. Abbreviations: amp = amplitude, freq = frequency
Response to benzodiazepines
Figure 4 compares the effects of the anxiolytic benzodiazepine agonist, diazepam (Fig. 4a), and the anxiogenic partial inverse agonist at the benzodiazepine allosteric site on the GABAA receptor, FG 7142 (Fig. 4b), on eIPSC activity in CA1 hippocampal pyramidal cells in WT and 1AKO mice. Diazepam significantly elevated amplitude of the first but not the second eIPSC (p<0.01 in WTs and p<0.05 in 1AKOs) and reduced PPR (p<0.01 in both groups). These data indicate that diazepam similarly augments evoked presynaptic GABA release in both genotypes. FG 7142 significantly reduced eIPSC amplitude (p<0.01 in both groups) without changing PPR. These data indicate that FG 7142 similarly inhibits evoked GABA synaptic activity in both groups. However, when data from both groups were pooled and correlated with anxiety measures from EPM performance (Fig. 5), there was a significant positive correlation between percent of time in open arms of the EPM and FG 7142 response (R=.448, p<0.05, N=24). When this correlation was examined in the separate genotypes, it was not significant in either due to the smaller N and consequent loss of statistical power (1AKOs: R=.233, n.s., N=14; WTs: R=.556, n.s., N=10). These data indicate that anxious subjects, regardless of genotype, show a decreased response to this anxiogenic benzodiazepine inverse agonist.
Fig. 4.
Genotype had no effect on sensitivity to the anxiolytic diazepam or the anxiogenic FG 7142. Figure 4a contains summary data demonstrating that diazepam (1 μM) elevated eIPSC1 amplitude and reduced PPR in both genotypes. Next to Fig. 4a is an example of an averaged eIPSC from a WT at baseline (157 pA) and following diazepam application (207 pA). The data in Fig. 4b shows that FG 7142 (3 μM) reduced eIPSC1 amplitude in both groups. Next to Fig. 4b is an example of an averaged eIPSC from a WT at baseline (180 pA) and following FG 7142 application (124 pA). Asterisks indicate a significant difference from predrug baseline by paired Student's t-test (* p<0.05, ** p<0.01). Data are expressed as mean+SEM. Abbreviation: amp=amplitude
Fig. 5.
Anxiety is correlated with a reduced response to the anxiogenic benzodiazepine inverse agonist FG 7142. Figure 5 illustrates a significant positive correlation between time spent exploring the open arms of the EPM and the magnitude of the change in GABAergic eIPSC amplitude in response to FG 7142 in CA1 hippocampal pyramidal cells
Physiological correlates of anxiety: a summary
Table 2 summarizes the correlation data of anxiety measures (percent of time in open arms) with physiological measures described above. Anxiety correlated with elevations in eEPSC amplitude and reduced eIPSC amplitude response to FG 7142. These findings indicate elevated postsynaptic AMPA receptors measured under conditions of synaptic activation and a desensitized response to this anxiogenic benzodiazepine inverse agonist in anxious subjects.
Table 2.
Summary of correlations between anxiety-like behavior of WT and 1AKOs and electrophysiology differences in CA1 hippocampal pyramidal cells
Physiological correlates of anxiety in CA1 hippocampal neurons | |||
---|---|---|---|
Correlation coefficient (R) | N | Statistical significance | |
RMP (mV) | .140 | 62 | n.s. |
eEPSC1 amplitude (pA) | −.491 | 61 | p<0.01 |
eEPSC2 amplitude (pA) | −.405 | 61 | p<0.01 |
eEPSC PPR | .004 | 61 | n.s. |
eIPSC1 amplitude (pA) | .028 | 72 | n.s. |
eIPSC2 amplitude (pA) | .158 | 72 | n.s. |
eIPSC PPR | .175 | 72 | n.s. |
sEPSC frequency (Hz) | .054 | 23 | n.s. |
mEPSC frequency (Hz) | −.067 | 22 | n.s. |
sEPSC amplitude (pA) | .246 | 23 | n.s. |
mEPSC amplitude (pA) | .365 | 22 | n.s. |
sIPSC frequency (Hz) | .061 | 33 | n.s. |
mIPSC frequency (Hz) | .120 | 33 | n.s. |
sIPSC amplitude (pA) | −.097 | 33 | n.s. |
mIPSC amplitude (pA) | .320 | 33 | p=0.07 |
DZ response (Δ eIPSC amp; pA) | −.076 | 28 | n.s. |
DZ response (Δ PPR) | .018 | 28 | n.s. |
FG response (Δ eIPSC amp; pA) | .448 | 24 | p<0.05 |
FG response (Δ PPR) | .029 | 24 | n.s. |
Anxiety-like behavior was measured by percent of time spent in the open arms of the elevated-plus maze
RMP resting membrane potential, amp amplitude, PPR paired-pulse ratio, DZ diazepam, FG FG 7142
Discussion
While behavioral characterization of the 5-HT1A receptor knockout mouse describes a robust anxiety phenotype that persists on a variety of background strains, as measured by multiple anxiety models in multiple laboratories, an understanding of the physiological basis of the anxiety phenotype has been limited. The current study used a combination of behavioral measures, pharmacological challenges and hippo-campal whole-cell recording techniques to uncover novel physiological correlates of anxiety in these subjects. Consistent with previous findings using the EPM (Bailey and Toth 2004;Ramboz et al. 1998; Sibille et al. 2000) and other anxiety models (Heisler et al. 1998; Parks et al. 1998), 1AKOs demonstrated elevated anxiety levels in the EPM model. Interestingly, many of the WT controls also exhibited anxiety behavior that overlapped with that seen in the 1AKOs. As a result, for correlation analysis, subjects were pooled to provide a larger overall range of anxiety measurements so that physiological endpoints could be correlated with anxiety measures. Anxious subjects demonstrated elevated eEPSC amplitude, indicating elevated postsynaptic AMPA receptor sensitivity under conditions of synaptic activation. Anxious subjects also showed diminished GABAergic responses to the anxiogenic benzodiazepine inverse agonist FG 7142. These correlations indicate a link between anxiety and altered sensitivity of hippocampal neurons to excitatory glutamatergic inputs as well as to anxiogenic, GABA-inhibiting benzodiazepines.
One general technical issue to consider in these experiments concerns the limitations of constitutive deletion of the 5-HT1A receptor. Because the gene is constitutively deleted, it may result in developmental, physiological or behavioral compensatory processes which, rather than the targeted gene, could contribute to the observed phenotype (Gerlai 2001). These compensatory changes could also mask the functional consequence of the gene mutation, obscuring potential phenotypes in the animal (Crawley 1996). Our data demonstrate that in fact these compensatory responses do occur since we found a change in the glutamate neurotransmitter system which may be a physiological correlate underlying the anxiety phenotype. Another technical issue which is more specific to the current study concerns the potential influence of exposure to the behavioral model on the physiological endpoints examined. The EPM model is known to be stressful, resulting in elevated levels of stress hormones (Appenrodt et al. 1999; File et al. 1994; Pellow et al. 1985; Rodgers et al. 1999). There is some disagreement in the literature as to whether exposure to the EPM, by itself, elevates anxiety levels. Some studies have shown that subjects tested in repeated EPM trials show stable measures of anxiety across trials (File et al. 1990;Lister 1987; Pellow et al. 1985) while others have shown progressive increases in anxiety with subsequent trials (Griebel et al. 1993; Rodgers et al. 1992; Rodgers and Shepherd 1993; Treit et al. 1993). Nonetheless, we chose to conduct the electrophysiology experiments at least 3 weeks following EPM testing in order to minimize any potentially confounding effects of the test itself on the electrophysiological measures, particularly in an area such as the hippocampus which is well-known for its responsiveness to stressors (McEwen et al. 1992; McEwen and Magarinos 1997). Our goal in this design was to capture two potential consequences of the 5-HT1A receptor deletion: a behavioral measure of anxiety and changes in hippocampal physiology while minimizing the influence of one endpoint on the other.
In glutamate synaptic activity experiments, under conditions of spontaneous glutamate release, there were no genotypic differences or correlations with anxiety measures. By contrast, under conditions of synaptic activation, postsynaptic AMPA receptor number or sensitivity was significantly enhanced in 1AKOs and also significantly correlated with behavioral anxiety. One potential explanation of this discrepancy is that Schaeffer collateral stimulation to evoke glutamate release engages a different population and a greater number of synapses than those engaged during s/mEPSC recordings during which only synapses within the range of the patch electrode contribute. Another potential factor is that behavioral differences between 1AKOs and WT controls are not readily apparent until subjects are challenged in animal models of anxiety, all of which involve stressful procedures to elicit the anxious behavior (i.e. active avoidance of open, exposed spaces (open-field test) or open arms (elevated-plus maze)). So, it is perhaps not surprising that this physiological hypersensitivity of hippocampal neurons to glutamatergic input is not observed in 1AKOs until the glutamate system is challenged by synaptic activation, as would be present during behavioral activation and stress conditions in vivo. This heightened glutamatergic sensitivity of hippocampal neurons is consistent with and may contribute to earlier findings in 1AKO mice of enhanced neuronal excitability in area CA1 of the hippocampus as well as magnified hippocampal theta oscillations that occur during anxiety-provoking conditions (Gordon et al. 2005). Since we observed greater sensitivity to glutamate in 1AKOs than WTs, it is possible that it represents a developmental response to the constitutive deletion of the 5-HT1A receptor. It is unclear if the anxiety phenotype of 1AKOs is driven by the receptor deletion, per se, or potentially by secondary developmental responses in other systems such as the excitatory glutamatergic system as shown in the current study. Nonetheless, it is clear that greater excitatory drive to hippocampal neurons could contribute to anxiety as the hippocampus has long been associated with emotional behaviors, neural control of the hypothalamopituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and stress psychopathology (Gray and McNaughton 2003; Jacobson and Sapolsky 1991;McEwen 2007).
Previous studies have also examined the sensitivity of 1AKO mice to benzodiazepines. Sibille, et al. (2000) initially reported that 1AKOs on a mixed Swiss-Webster × 129/Sv background demonstrated benzodiazepine-resistant anxiety coupled with reduced benzodiazepine receptor binding in cortex and amygdala, reduced GABAA receptor binding in hippocampus, impaired GABA function in hippocampus and abnormal GABAA receptor subunit expression in all three regions. Later reports showed that 1AKOs on a mixed Swiss-Webster × C57Bl6 background were similarly benzodiazepine-resistant (Bailey and Toth 2004) but that 1AKOs on the pure 129/Sv (Pattij et al. 2002) or C57/Bl6 backgrounds (Bailey and Toth 2004) had normal benzodiazepine sensitivity. We compared benzodiazepine responsivity in 1AKOs and WTs and found no effect of genotype on the response to either benzodiazepine agonists (increase in eIPSC) or inverse agonists (decrease in eIPSC), consistent with the normal benzodiazepine sensitivity reported for 1AKOs on a pure 129/Sv background (Pattij et al. 2002). However, when subjects were pooled and anxiety measures compared with benzodiazepine responsivity, we found a significant correlation between anxiety and diminished GABA synaptic response to the inverse agonist. These data indicate that anxious behavior rather than genotype predict this decreased sensitivity to an anxiogenic benzodiazepine inverse agonist. Endogenous ligands for the benzodiazepine receptor have been proposed. For example, diazepam-binding inhibitor (DBI) is a neuropeptide that has been identified in both CNS and the periphery which acts an inverse agonist at benzodiazepine receptors (Costa and Guidotti 1991). The decreased sensitivity of the anxious subjects to the exogenous benzodiazepine inverse agonist could actually reflect a desensitization of the receptor system in these animals as a compensatory mechanism to high circulating levels of an endogenous inverse agonist such as DBI. In fact, DBI has anxiogenic behavioral effects when administered exogenously (Barbaccia et al. 1986;Costa and Guidotti 1991) and elevated expression of DBI has been reported in human depression with a strong anxiety component (Barbaccia et al. 1986). This elevation in endogenous inverse agonist could contribute to anxiety directly or indirectly via its effect on other anxiety-related neuropeptides. For example, inverse agonists modulating GABAA receptor function have been shown to elevate levels of corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) in vitro (Calogero et al. 1988) and in vivo (Skelton et al. 2000) and to enhance the behavioral effects of CRF (Britton et al. 1988). CRF is in turn responsible for initiating the hormonal, physiological and behavioral response to stress mediated by the HPA axis. In this manner, reduced sensitivity demonstrated by hippocampal neurons to an exogenously applied benzodiazepine inverse agonist maybe an adaptation to excessive circulating levels of an endogenous inverse agonist such as DBI which either directly or indirectly contributes to anxiety.
Previous studies have also examined physiological correlates of anxiety using different animal models. For example, Smith et al. showed decreased GABAA receptor-mediated current in hippocampus and insensitivity to benzodiazepine agonists in a steroid withdrawal-induced anxiety model in rats (Smith et al. 1998). Cagetti et al. showed reduced benzodiazepine agonist sensitivity and reduced mIPSC decay in hippocampal neurons of rats during an ethanol withdrawal-induced anxiety model (Cagetti et al. 2003). Van Sickle et al. found increased AMPA but not NMDA receptor-mediated excitation of CA1 neurons in rats during benzodiazepine withdrawal-induced anxiety (Van Sickle et al. 2004). Maguire et al. showed reduced GABAA receptor-mediated tonic inhibition of hippocampal neurons and elevated neuronal excitability during the estrus, as compared with diestrus, period of the ovarian cycle in female mice, a time of heightened anxiety (Maguire et al. 2005). All of these studies using different animal models are consistent with our findings and indicate a number of different mechanisms whereby heightened hippocampal neuron excitability may contribute to anxiety.
These experiments highlight at the single-cell level the potential importance of CA1 hippocampal pyramidal neurons and their GABA and glutamatergic synaptic inputs in the generation of anxiety. They identify altered sensitivity of hippocampal neurons to GABA modulators which ultimately may reflect underlying hyperactivity of an endogenous GABA-inhibiting system, an effect which could be linked to anxiety. In addition, these experiments identify enhanced glutamate synaptic drive to hippocampal neurons of anxious subjects. This mechanism would increase the excitability of hippocampal neurons, stimulating hippocampal output and would contribute to hippocampal-mediated behaviors that could include exaggerated emotional responses to environmental stressors and anxiety disorders.
Acknowledgments
We thank Drs. Mark Geyer and Victoria Risbrough in the Dept. of Psychiatry at the University of California, San Diego for the generous donation of breeding pairs of 129/Sv 5-HT1A knockout mice and wild-type controls for use in these studies. We thank Alessandra Cathel for her technical assistance and contributions to electrophysiological studies. This work was supported by a Young Investigator Award from the National Association of Research on Schizophrenia and Depression (NARSAD) and a National Institute of Mental Health grant (MH 63301) issued to Dr. Kirby and by National Institute of Mental Health grants (MH 48125 and MH 63078) issued to Dr. Beck.
References
- Adhikari A, Topiwala MA, Gordon JA. Synchronized activity between the ventral hippocampus and the medial prefrontal cortex during anxiety. Neuron. 2010;65:257–269. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.12.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allan AM, Baier LD, Zhang X. Effects of lorazepam tolerance and withdrawal on GABAA receptor-operated chloride channels. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1992;261:395–402. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andrews N, Hogg S, Gonzalez LE, File SE. 5-HT1A receptors in the median raphe nucleus and dorsal hippocampus may mediate anxiolytic and anxiogenic behaviours respectively. Eur J Pharmacol. 1994;264:259–264. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(94)00473-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Appenrodt E, Kroning G, Schwarzberg H. Increased plasma ACTH in rats exposed to the elevated plus-maze is independent of the pineal gland. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1999;24:833–838. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(99)00040-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bailey SJ, Toth M. Variability in the benzodiazepine response of serotonin 5-HT1A receptor null mice displaying anxiety-like phenotype: evidence for genetic modifiers in the 5-HT-mediated regulation of GABA(A) receptors. J Neurosci. 2004;24:6343–6351. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0563-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barbaccia ML, Costa E, Ferrero P, Guidotti A, Roy A, Sunderland T, Pickar D, Paul SM, Goodwin FK. Diazepam-binding inhibitor. A brain neuropeptide present in human spinal fluid: studies in depression, schizophrenia, and Alzheimer's disease. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1986;43:1143–1147. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1986.01800120029007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bourin M, Hascoet M. Drug mechanisms in anxiety. Curr Opin Investig Drugs. 2001;2:259–265. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Britton KT, Lee G, Koob GF. Corticotropin releasing factor and amphetamine exaggerate partial agonist properties of benzodiazepine antagonist Ro 15-1788 in the conflict test. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 1988;94:306–311. doi: 10.1007/BF00174680. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cagetti E, Liang J, Spigelman I, Olsen RW. Withdrawal from chronic intermittent ethanol treatment changes subunit composition, reduces synaptic function, and decreases behavioral responses to positive allosteric modulators of GABAA receptors. Mol Pharmacol. 2003;63:53–64. doi: 10.1124/mol.63.1.53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calogero AE, Gallucci WT, Chrousos GP, Gold PW. Interaction between GABAergic neurotransmission and rat hypothalamic corticotropin-releasing hormone secretion in vitro. Brain Res. 1988;463:28–36. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(88)90523-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carli M, Tatarczynska E, Cervo L, Samanin R. Stimulation of hippocampal 5-HT1A receptors causes amnesia and anxiolytic-like but not antidepressant-like effects in the rat. Eur J Pharmacol. 1993;234:215–221. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(93)90956-i. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Charney DS, Drevets WC. Neurobiological basis of anxiety disorders. In: Davis KL, Charney DS, Coyle J, Nemeroff C, editors. Neuropsychopharmacology: the Fifth Generation of Progress. American College of Neuropsychopharmacology. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; Baltimore, MD: 2002. pp. 901–930. [Google Scholar]
- Cossart R, Epsztein J, Tyzio R, Becq H, Hirsch J, Crepel V. Quantal release of glutamate generates pure kainate and mixed AMPA/kainate EPSCs in hippocampal neurons. Neuron. 2002;35:147–159. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00753-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Costa E, Guidotti A. Diazepam binding inhibitor (DBI): a peptide with multiple biological actions. Life Sci. 1991;49:325–344. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(91)90440-m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crawley JN. Unusual behavioral phenotypes of inbred mouse strains. Trends Neurosci. 1996;19:181–182. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(96)20021-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crawley J. What's wrong with my mouse: behavioral phenotyping of transgenic and knockout mice. John Wiley and Sons; Hoboken: 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Dos SL, De Andrade TG, Zangrossi JH. 5-HT1A receptors in the dorsal hippocampus mediate the anxiogenic effect induced by the stimulation of 5-HT neurons in the median raphe nucleus. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2008;18:286–294. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2007.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fanselow MS, Dong HW. Are the dorsal and ventral hippocampus functionally distinct structures? Neuron. 2010;65:7–19. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.11.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- File SE. Factors controlling measures of anxiety and responses to novelty in the mouse. Behav Brain Res. 2001;125:151–157. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(01)00292-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- File SE, Gonzalez LE. Anxiolytic effects in the plus-maze of 5-HT1A-receptor ligands in dorsal raphe and ventral hippocampus. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1996;54:123–128. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(95)02108-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- File SE, Gonzalez LE, Andrews N. Comparative study of pre-and postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptor modulation of anxiety in two ethological animal tests. J Neurosci. 1996;16:4810–4815. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-15-04810.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- File SE, Mabbutt PS, Hitchcott PK. Characterisation of the phenomenon of “one-trial tolerance” to the anxiolytic effect of chlordiazepoxide in the elevated plus-maze. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 1990;102:98–101. doi: 10.1007/BF02245751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- File SE, Zangrossi H, Jr, Sanders FL, Mabbutt PS. Raised corticosterone in the rat after exposure to the elevated plus-maze. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 1994;113:543–546. doi: 10.1007/BF02245237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freund TF, Buzsaki G. Interneurons of the hippocampus. Hippocampus. 1996;6:347–470. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1996)6:4<347::AID-HIPO1>3.0.CO;2-I. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerlai R. Gene targeting: technical confounds and potential solutions in behavioral brain research. Behav Brain Res. 2001;125:13–21. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(01)00282-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gordon JA, Lacefield CO, Kentros CG, Hen R. State-dependent alterations in hippocampal oscillations in serotonin 1A receptor-deficient mice. J Neurosci. 2005;25:6509–6519. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1211-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Graeff FG, Guimaraes FS, De Andrade TG, Deakin JF. Role of 5-HT in stress, anxiety, and depression. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1996;54:129–141. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(95)02135-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray JA, McNaughton N. The neuropsychology of anxiety: an enquiry into the function of the septo-hippocampal system. Oxford University Press; Oxford: 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Griebel G, Moreau JL, Jenck F, Martin JR, Misslin R. Some critical determinants of the behavior of rats in the elevated plus-maze. Behav Process. 1993;29:37–47. doi: 10.1016/0376-6357(93)90026-N. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gross C, Zhuang X, Stark K, Ramboz S, Oosting R, Kirby L, Santarelli L, Beck S, Hen R. Serotonin1A receptor acts during development to establish normal anxiety-like behaviour in the adult. Nature. 2002;416:396–400. doi: 10.1038/416396a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heisler LK, Chu HM, Brennan TJ, Danao JA, Bajwa P, Parsons LH, Tecott LH. Elevated anxiety and antidepressant-like responses in serotonin 5-HT1A receptor mutant mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1998;95:15049–15054. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.25.15049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hogg S. A review of the validity and variability of the elevated plus-maze as an animal model of anxiety. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1996;54:21–30. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(95)02126-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hogg S, Andrews N, File SE. Contrasting behavioural effects of 8-OH DPAT in the dorsal raphe nucleus and ventral hippocampus. Neuropharmacology. 1994;33:343–348. doi: 10.1016/0028-3908(94)90064-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacobson L, Sapolsky RM. The role of the hippocampus in feedback regulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis. Endocr Rev. 1991;12:118–134. doi: 10.1210/edrv-12-2-118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jolas T, Schreiber R, Laporte AM, Chastanet M, De VJ, Glaser T, Adrien J, Hamon M. Are postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors involved in the anxiolytic effects of 5-HT1A receptor agonists and in their inhibitory effects on the firing of serotonergic neurons in the rat? J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1995;272:920–929. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kataoka Y, Shibata K, Miyazaki A, Inoue Y, Tominaga K, Koizumi S, Ueki S, Niwa M. Involvement of the dorsal hippocampus in mediation of the antianxiety action of tandospirone, a 5-hydroxytryptamine1A agonistic anxiolytic. Neuropharmacology. 1991;30:475–480. doi: 10.1016/0028-3908(91)90009-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kostowski W, Plaznik A, Stefanski R. Intra-hippocampal buspirone in animal models of anxiety. Eur J Pharmacol. 1989;168:393–396. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(89)90803-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lacaille JC, Mueller AL, Kunkel DD, Schwartzkroin PA. Local circuit interactions between oriens/alveus interneurons and CA1 pyramidal cells in hippocampal slices: electrophysiology and morphology. J Neurosci. 1987;7:1979–1993. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.07-07-01979.1987. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lister RG. The use of a plus-maze to measure anxiety in the mouse. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 1987;92:180–185. doi: 10.1007/BF00177912. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lo IL, Gross C. Alpha-Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II contributes to the developmental programming of anxiety in serotonin receptor 1A knock-out mice. J Neurosci. 2008;28:6250–6257. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5219-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lucki I. Serotonin receptor specificity in anxiety disorders. J Clin Psychiatry. 1996;57(Suppl 6):5–10. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maguire JL, Stell BM, Rafizadeh M, Mody I. Ovarian cycle-linked changes in GABA(A) receptors mediating tonic inhibition alter seizure susceptibility and anxiety. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:797–804. doi: 10.1038/nn1469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayorga AJ, Dalvi A, Page ME, Zimov-Levinson S, Hen R, Lucki I. Antidepressant-like behavioral effects in 5-hydroxytryptamine(1A) and 5-hydroxytryptamine(1B) receptor mutant mice. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2001;298:1101–1107. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McEwen BS. Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: central role of the brain. Physiol Rev. 2007;87:873–904. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00041.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McEwen BS, Gould EA, Sakai RR. The vulnerability of the hippocampus to protective and destructive effects of glucocorticoids in relation to stress. Br J Psychiatry. 1992;15(Suppl):18–23. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McEwen BS, Magarinos AM. Stress effects on morphology and function of the hippocampus. Ann NY Acad Sci. 1997;821:271–284. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1997.tb48286.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Menard J, Treit D. The septum and the hippocampus differentially mediate anxiolytic effects of R(+)-8-OH-DPAT. Behav Pharmacol. 1998;9:93–101. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Menard J, Treit D. Effects of centrally administered anxiolytic compounds in animal models of anxiety. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1999;23:591–613. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(98)00056-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Millan MJ. The neurobiology and control of anxious states. Prog Neurobiol. 2003;70:83–244. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0082(03)00087-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neumeister A, Bain E, Nugent AC, Carson RE, Bonne O, Luckenbaugh DA, Eckelman W, Herscovitch P, Charney DS, Drevets WC. Reduced serotonin type 1A receptor binding in panic disorder. J Neurosci. 2004;24:589–591. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4921-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parks CL, Robinson PS, Sibille E, Shenk T, Toth M. Increased anxiety of mice lacking the serotonin1A receptor. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1998;95:10734–10739. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.18.10734. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pattij T, Groenink L, Oosting RS, van der GJ M, RA OB. GABA(A)-benzodiazepine receptor complex sensitivity in 5-HT (1A) receptor knockout mice on a 129/Sv background. Eur J Pharmacol. 2002;447:67–74. doi: 10.1016/s0014-2999(02)01893-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pellow S, Chopin P, File SE, Briley M. Validation of open: closed arm entries in an elevated plus-maze as a measure of anxiety in the rat. J Neurosci Methods. 1985;14:149–167. doi: 10.1016/0165-0270(85)90031-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips TJ, Hen R, Crabbe JC. Complications associated with genetic background effects in research using knockout mice. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 1999;147:5–7. doi: 10.1007/s002130051128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramboz S, Oosting R, Amara DA, Kung HF, Blier P, Mendelsohn M, Mann JJ, Brunner D, Hen R. Serotonin receptor 1A knockout: an animal model of anxiety-related disorder. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1998;95:14476–14481. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.24.14476. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ravindran LN, Stein MB. The pharmacologic treatment of anxiety disorders: a review of progress. J Clin Psychiatry. 2010;71:839–854. doi: 10.4088/JCP.10r06218blu. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodgers RJ, Haller J, Holmes A, Halasz J, Walton TJ, Brain PF. Corticosterone response to the plus-maze: high correlation with risk assessment in rats and mice. Physiol Behav. 1999;68:47–53. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(99)00140-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodgers RJ, Lee C, Shepherd JK. Effects of diazepam on behavioural and antinociceptive responses to the elevated plus-maze in male mice depend upon treatment regimen and prior maze experience. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 1992;106:102–110. doi: 10.1007/BF02253596. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodgers RJ, Shepherd JK. Influence of prior maze experience on behaviour and response to diazepam in the elevated plus-maze and light/dark tests of anxiety in mice. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 1993;113:237–242. doi: 10.1007/BF02245704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sandford JJ, Argyropoulos SV, Nutt DJ. The psychobiology of anxiolytic drugs. Part 1: Basic neurobiology. Pharmacol Ther. 2000;88:197–212. doi: 10.1016/s0163-7258(00)00082-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sarnyai Z, Sibille EL, Pavlides C, Fenster RJ, McEwen BS, Toth M. Impaired hippocampal-dependent learning and functional abnormalities in the hippocampus in mice lacking serotonin(1A) receptors. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2000;97:14731–14736. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.26.14731. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schreiber R, De Vry J. Neuronal circuits involved in the anxiolytic effects of the 5-HT1A receptor agonists 8-OH-DPAT ipsapirone and buspirone in the rat. Eur J Pharmacol. 1993;249:341–351. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(93)90531-l. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sibille E, Pavlides C, Benke D, Toth M. Genetic inactivation of the Serotonin(1A) receptor in mice results in downregulation of major GABA(A) receptor alpha subunits, reduction of GABA(A) receptor binding, and benzodiazepine-resistant anxiety. J Neurosci. 2000;20:2758–2765. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-08-02758.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skelton KH, Nemeroff CB, Knight DL, Owens MJ. Chronic administration of the triazolobenzodiazepine alprazolam produces opposite effects on corticotropin-releasing factor and urocortin neuronal systems. J Neurosci. 2000;20:1240–1248. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-03-01240.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith SS, Gong QH, Li X, Moran MH, Bitran D, Frye CA, Hsu FC. Withdrawal from 3alpha-OH-5alpha-pregnan-20-One using a pseudopregnancy model alters the kinetics of hippocampal GABAA-gated current and increases the GABAA receptor alpha4 subunit in association with increased anxiety. J Neurosci. 1998;18:5275–5284. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-14-05275.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stefanski R, Palejko W, Bidzinski A, Kostowski W, Plaznik A. Serotonergic innervation of the hippocampus and nucleus accumbens septi and the anxiolytic-like action of midazolam and 5-HT1A receptor agonists. Neuropharmacology. 1993;32:977–985. doi: 10.1016/0028-3908(93)90062-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Treit D, Menard J, Royan C. Anxiogenic stimuli in the elevated plus-maze. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1993;44:463–469. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(93)90492-c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Sickle BJ, Xiang K, Tietz EI. Transient plasticity of hippocampal CA1 neuron glutamate receptors contributes to benzodiazepine withdrawal-anxiety. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:1994–2006. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300531. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]