Abstract
A total of 18 associative learning/memory tests have been applied to Drosophila melanogaster flies lacking mushroom bodies. Only in paradigms involving chemosensory cues as conditioned stimuli have flies been found to be compromised by a block in the mushroom body pathway. Among the learning tasks not requiring these structures are a case of motor learning (yaw torque/heat), a test of the fly’s spatial orientation in total darkness, conditioned courtship suppression by mated females, and nine different examples of visual learning. The latter used the reinforcers of heat, visual oscillations, mechanical shaking, or sucrose, and as conditioned stimuli, color, intensity contrast, as well as stationary and moving visual patterns. No forms of consolidated memory have been tested in mushroom body-less flies. With respect to short-term memory the mushroom bodies of Drosophila are specially required for chemosensory learning tasks, but not for associative learning and memory in general.
Possible roles of the insect mushroom bodies (corpora pedunculata) in behavior have been debated for nearly 150 years (Dujardin 1850). It took more than a century before some of the hypotheses could be tested experimentally and even then, the means of intervention proved unsatisfactory. Lesions and electrical stimulation in the mushroom bodies (MBs) caused a large variety of behavioral symptoms that were difficult to reconcile with a common function (for review, see Erber et al. 1987). Among the behavioral properties affected by lesions were cocoon spinning in Cecropia larvae (van der Kloot and Williams 1954), the organization of behavioral sequences in bees and locusts (Howse 1974), odor thresholds and optomotor latencies in the cockroach (Drescher 1960), as well as the suppression of locomotor activity in crickets and grasshoppers (Huber 1960). Fortunately, one behavioral impairment has now been consistently found with different lesioning techniques and in different insect species: olfactory learning and memory.
First hints indicating that the MBs are required for the retention of odors came from extensive bilateral lesions in bees (Voskresenskaja 1957) and ants (Vowles 1964). Later, Masuhr (1976) applied more confined lesions in the calyces or α-lobes of the honeybee with similar results. Subsequently, he developed a special probe for locally cooling small areas of the bee brain. With this elegant technique he was able to interfere with short-term memory in the antennal lobe, calyces, or α-lobe (Erber et al. 1980). A decade later, two Drosophila mutants with severe structural defects in the MBs, but entirely different etiologies of the mutant defects, were shown to be impaired in olfactory memory (Heisenberg et al. 1985). A further decade later, it became possible to ablate the MBs in wild-type Drosophila by applying a cytostatic drug, hydroxyurea (HU), to the first larval instar (Prokop and Technau 1994). The result was the same—a severe defect in olfactory memory (de Belle and Heisenberg 1994). Recently, Connolly et al. (1996) went a step farther and confined the block to the Kenyon cells by specifically expressing a constitutively activated Gαs protein subunit in these cells. Again, odor retention was specifically impaired. If the expression of the mutated transgene leaves the circuitry of the MB pathway intact, the experiment shows that blocking modulation of the Kenyon cell output synapses is sufficient to impair olfactory memory.
The cumulative evidence from hymenopterans and flies leaves little doubt that MBs are involved in olfactory memory. This finding calls to mind the proposal of Hanström (1928), suggesting that the MBs may be general association organs. The MBs do receive many types of sensory input (for review, see Erber et al. 1987; Schürmann 1987), and in Drosophila the MBs have been shown recently to be structurally affected by visual experience (Barth and Heisenberg 1997). However, outside the realm of chemoreception little evidence supports Hanström’s view. Flies of the mushroom bodies deranged (mbd) mutant stock with severely defective MBs have been shown to remember colors (Heisenberg et al. 1985; Heisenberg 1989). Moreover, mutant mbd as well as mushroom body miniature1 (mbm1) flies are able to reorganize their visuomotor interface in response to inverted coupling in the flight simulator (preliminary data cited in Heisenberg 1989).
Here we reinvestigate the question whether the MBs might be involved in visual and other kinds of associative learning and memory besides odor preference conditioning. We take advantage of the HU ablation technique (Prokop and Technau 1994) for the generation of MB-less flies (HU-flies). The HU technique seems particularly suited for studying the role of the MBs in visual behavior because the optic anlagen do not start postembryonic development until after HU treatment (Ito and Hotta 1991). The volume of the adult optic lobes is not affected in HU flies (deBelle and Heisenberg 1994). In the time window of the treatment, in each brain hemisphere only five neuroblasts proliferate. Four of them, the MB neuroblasts, give birth to the postembryonic Kenyon cells, and a fifth one generates neurons of the antennal lobe (Ito and Hotta 1991; Prokop and Technau 1994). In the adult animal, HU treatment leads to the virtually complete absence of MBs and, in addition, to a 30% reduction of the volume of the antennal lobe (de Belle and Heisenberg 1994).
In this study we test MB-less flies in six learning paradigms, four of which involve visual orientation. Five are operant and one is a classic form of conditioning. In four paradigms, the animals are tethered in flight; in two paradigms, freely walking flies are tested. Different kinds of reinforcements and various conditioned stimuli are used. In all these situations MB-less flies perform as well as normal ones. The involvement of the MBs in learning and memory seems to be confined to olfaction and possibly taste.
Materials and Methods
FLIES
All flies tested, with or without MBs, were between 2 and 6 days old and were maintained at 25°C on standard corn meal/molasses medium (for recipe, see Guo et al. 1996) in a 16-hr light/8 hr dark cycle at 60% humidity. For all flight experiments only female flies were used. Wild-type flies were from the strain “Berlin” (WT Berlin). For the experiment in Figure 3 (below), the mutant mbm1 (Heisenberg et al. 1985) was used, which is kept as a homozygous stock in the genetic background of WT Berlin.
For ablation of MB neuroblasts (Prokop and Technau 1994) ∼150 first instar larvae (maximum age, 1 hr) were collected and mixed with 1 ml of a suspension containing precooked baker’s yeast and 60 mg of HU. After 4 hr of exposure to HU, larvae were carefully rinsed with water and transferred to fresh food vials. To avoid any confusion from handling artifacts, HU flies and controls were bred strictly in parallel, with the only difference that HU was omitted for the controls. All HU flies that successfully passed the behavioral tests were subjected to postmortem histology. Rare individuals with detectable remnants of MBs (∼2%–4%) were not included in the behavioral data.
APPARATUS
FLIGHT SIMULATOR
For the flight simulator (Heisenberg and Wolf 1988), single flies were glued by their head and thorax to a small hook of copper wire (0.05 mm diam.); the hook was attached to a torque meter. The torque meter was positioned in the center of a circular arena that could be rotated by a fast electric motor. The angular velocity of the arena was made negatively proportional to the voltage output of the torque meter (flight simulator). Thus, instead of rotating the fly in a stationary surround, the fly’s yaw torque rotated the panorama around the stationary fly. As reinforcement the fly was heated by an infrared beam (Wolf and Heisenberg 1991) that could be intercepted by an electric shutter (Fig. 1) controlled, in turn, by the fixed schedule of a computer program (classic conditioning, Fig. 5, below), or by the fly’s flight direction with respect to the arena (operant conditioning, Figs. 2–4). The arena carried two upright and two inverted T-shaped landmarks (width, 39°; height, 36°; thickness of horizontal and vertical T-bars, 12°; as seen from the position of the fly in the center of the arena). For the experiment of Figure 4, instead of heat the arena was oscillated (peak-to-peak amplitude, App = ±7.5°; frequency, f = 5 Hz) as a negative reinforcement. The exafferent oscillations were generated by a computer program and superimposed on the reafferent movement of the arena at the appropriate flight directions. Yaw torque and flight direction were recorded continuously and stored in the computer memory (sampling frequency, 20 Hz) for further evaluation of data. Experimental procedures are further described in the text and figure legends.
HEATING CHAMBER
The heating chamber has been described by Wustmann et al. (1996). The upper and lower surfaces of the laterally transparent rectangular chamber (40 × 4 × 2.5 mm3) consisted of Peltier elements. A control circuit and a thermosensor in the chamber kept the Peltier elements at a permissive “cold” (25°C ± 2°C) or restrictive “hot” (37°C ± 2°C) temperature. The chamber was virtually subdivided perpendicular to its long axis into two halves by a directionally selective infrared light gate (Fig. 7e, below) invisible to the fly (for spectral sensitivity of Drosophila, see Heisenberg and Wolf 1984). The chamber was illuminated by four small lamps close to the corners of the alley (Fig. 7a,b) that were switched off in the experiment of Figure 7, c and d. The supervising computer continuously monitored the time and directions of transitions at the light gate. During training, in addition, it turned heat on when the fly entered one side and turned it off when the fly moved to the other side. A rack with 15 chambers was used for the experiments. Therefore, it was possible to test 15 flies at the same time (for further details, see also Wustmann and Heisenberg 1997). The experiments consisted of three phases: a preference test block of 30-sec duration, four training blocks (30 sec each), and two memory tests (30 sec each). After training, the memory tests did not start until the fly crossed the light gate from the previously hot to the cold side. Therefore, inactive flies were excluded and all flies experienced the low temperature on the previously hot side before the onset of the memory test period. Between consecutive trials the sides for the permissive and restrictive conditions were switched.
ANALYSIS OF DATA
The performance of individual flies was calculated as performance index: PI = (ta − tb)/(ta + tb). During training, tb indicates the time the fly was exposed to the reinforcer and ta the time without reinforcement. Because during test the reinforcement was permanently switched off, ta and tb refer to the times in which the experimental situations corresponded to the respective permissive or restrictive training situations. To test the significance of the differences between memory scores (PIs of memory tests) in MB-less and control flies the two-sided P value [alternate t-test for assumed Gaussian populations with different standard deviations (Welch test)] was calculated (Sachs 1992). All error bars in figures are s.e.m.
Results
OPERANT VISUAL LEARNING IN THE FLIGHT SIMULATOR
HU flies showed no obvious impairments. In the culture vials, they were actively walking around and could be observed grooming, jumping, and courting. For the flight experiments single females of the HU and control groups were cooled to ∼6°C–8°C and glued to small hooks of silver wire. The next day they were attached to the torque meter in the flight simulator (Fig. 1) and their pattern preference with respect to an upright and an inverted T-shaped figure was recorded for consecutive 2-min periods (Fig. 2). HU flies, like the controls, flew readily once tarsal contact was removed. The percentage that kept flying during the whole 18-min recording period was similar in the HU and control flies (HU flies: 83.3%; controls: 91.4%; P >> 0.05, contingency χ2).
To adjust the zero setting of the angular velocity with respect to the fly’s yaw torque range at the onset of the experiment, the flight simulator mode was interrupted and the arena was briefly rotated exafferently in both directions. MB-less flies showed normal-sized optomotor responses to these motion stimuli (no data were recorded). During the first 2 min in the flight simulator mode no heat was applied. Flies had the opportunity to gather experience with the highly artificial feedback conditions (Guo et al. 1996). The HU flies kept optomotor balance without any difficulty and generated typical torque spikes at a normal rate (Heisenberg and Wolf 1979, 1993). Like the controls, they had the tendency to fixate on the patterns and showed a slight preference for the inverted T (not shown).
After 2 min the training began. Flies were conditioned with heat as reinforcer to avoid certain flight directions with respect to the two patterns in the panorama. The flies quickly learned to keep their course out of the heat. Already during the first 2-min period of training flies reached an avoidance score of P = 0.5 implying that they were heated for <25% of the time. HU flies avoided the heat as effectively as the controls (Fig. 2) suggesting that neither the MBs nor the missing parts of the antennal lobes are essential for heat reception or active heat avoidance (see Discussion).
After the 4-min training period, heat was switched off and the fly’s pattern preference was tested for 2 min. In the MB-less flies, as well as in the controls, the conditioning procedure had shifted the pattern preference toward the “no heat” pattern. This result indicates that neither pattern recognition nor learning and memory of visual pattern preferences depend on the MBs. This is in contrast to the requirement of the MBs in odor recognition or odor preference learning as discussed in the Introduction. After the first memory test the training was repeated for another 4 min and finally the fly was subjected to a second memory test. No significant difference was observed between MB-less and normal flies (Fig. 2).
A similar conclusion was reached using the mutant mbm1. The present mbm1 stock was backcrossed to WT Berlin, as described by de Belle and Heisenberg (1996), and contains >90% females with severely reduced MBs. These flies, however, have a low motivation to fly. Many of them stop during the recording period and have to be reactivated by sudden air puffs. Only ∼60% of the mbm1 flies glued to the hook (see Materials and Methods) flew long enough to finish the entire 18-min experiment (control flies, 85%). Although this difference was not statistically significant (see Materials and Methods for statistical tests), the observation is supported by many similar studies. Nevertheless, for the flies that finished the experiment, flight performance, heat avoidance, pattern recognition, and spontaneous pattern preferences were normal. Although the memory test scores appeared lower than those of the wild type, the differences were not statistically significant (Fig. 3).
VISUAL OSCILLATIONS AS NEGATIVE REINFORCEMENT
In the following experiment we changed the reinforcer. Instead of heating the flies we superimposed a 5-Hz oscillation (±7.5° App) upon the reafferent movement of the panorama at the nonpermissive pattern orientations. Normal flies readily avoided this “turbulence” during the training and later, in the memory test, showed a marked preference for those patterns that promised a calm flight (Fig. 4a). HU flies had no obvious impairment in any phase of this experiment. They perceived the fast oscillation, avoided it, and developed a preference for flight orientations for which oscillation was not to be expected. Evidently, this purely visual learning task does not require the MBs (Fig. 4b).
CLASSIC CONDITIONING AT THE TORQUE METER
In olfactory classic conditioning (Tully and Quinn 1985) intact MBs are required (de Belle and Heisenberg 1994; Connolly et al. 1996). We considered the possibility that classic conditioning might present a greater challenge than operant conditioning and that intact MBs might be required only in classic forms of learning.
In the flight simulator the training phase can easily be changed into a classic procedure. The feedback loop between the fly’s yaw torque and the angular velocity of the arena was opened and the arena movement was controlled by a fixed schedule. The arena alternated between two orientations at 3-sec intervals, one with the upright T and the other with the inverted T in front of the fly (Fig. 5a). Flies were heated with one of the pattern orientations as before, but now they received heat for 50% of the time. Therefore, the heat had to be reduced slightly to make it tolerable for the fly. During training no PI was calculated (see Fig. 5b–d). In the memory test the apparatus was switched back to the flight simulator mode as this is the only way the fly’s pattern preference can be assessed. The overall time schedule was the same as in the experiments of Figure 3, that is, 4 min of pretest in the flight simulator without heat, 4 min of training (stationary patterns), a 2-min memory test (flight simulator mode), again 4 min of training (stationary patterns), and a final 4-min memory test (flight simulator mode). The result for normal flies is shown in Figure 5b. After classic conditioning, memory scores are about as high as in the operant experiment.
The operant nature of the conditioning in the original experiment of Figure 2 had been revealed by the so-called replay experiment (Wolf and Heisenberg 1991), which is reminiscent of a yoked control. Flies were passively exposed to a sequence of pattern movements and heat/no heat alternations that had been recorded from flies trained in the flight simulator mode. With this kind of classic conditioning no positive memory scores were obtained. The result could be interpreted to indicate that the motivational state of flies without visual feedback was incompatible with learning or memory (e.g., stress as a result of the lack of feedback). The new finding rules out this kind of explanation. Flies are able to acquire classically conditioned pattern preferences and display them subsequently in the flight simulator mode if sufficient reinforcement is applied. The fly’s active role in controlling pattern orientation and temperature seems to facilitate the association between conditioned and unconditioned stimulus (B. Brembs and M. Heisenberg, in prep.).
In the operant conditioning procedure the fly has the opportunity to modify its motor programs by trial and error until it finds a strategy that improves its situation (conditioned response). For instance, the fly might find out that it is advantageous to immediately reverse its turning direction when the temperature gets too high. Obviously, during classic conditioning the fly has no opportunity to acquire any such strategies. Thus, the new result demonstrates that positive memory scores do not necessarily depend on learned motor programs. The acquired pattern preference in Drosophila, therefore, can be interpreted as a central state that is largely independent of motor output.
The classic conditioning experiment addresses a further point of interest. In human subjects stabilized retinal images quickly fade away (e.g., Riggs et al. 1953). In the classic training of the experiment in Figure 5b the flies are exposed to stabilized retinal images. The positive memory scores show that they recognize and remember them. It is a remote possibility that the flies learn the patterns while they are changing their angular position. To rule out this possibility, we have darkened the arena during the 220-msec displacement period (“dark flashes”). With this regimen we obtain memory scores in the normal range (Fig. 5c; in Fig. 5b,d memory scores are unusually high; differences with and without dark flashes are not significant). Thus, in the present case the memory template cannot be a “motion signature,” as has been discussed for Drosophila (Dill et al. 1995) and other insects (e.g., Zeil 1993). Srinivasan et al. (1993) have shown that honeybees can learn the orientation of contours without motion cues. In our case, even flickering the image seems not to be essential for visibility (although switching on the light once after the dark flash might be considered a minimal flicker stimulus).
The main question posed in this section was whether the MBs are required in visual learning and memory if the flies are prevented from learning by trial and error. In the experiment shown in Figure 5d HU flies were subjected to the classic conditioning procedure. They learned and remembered as well as the controls. As mentioned in Materials and Methods, the flies were sacrificed for histology after the experiment and were individually inspected to verify the MB ablation. Obviously, MBs are not involved in this associative process. [R. Wolf and M. Heisenberg (unpubl.) have reported an experiment showing that in HU flies dark flashes interfere with short-term memory in this paradigm. Work is in progress investigating whether this behavioral deficit can be assigned definitively to the MBs.]
MOTOR LEARNING AT THE TORQUE METER
In the next experiment MB-less flies were tested in a motor learning task not involving vision. This paradigm has been described by Wolf and Heisenberg (1991). The fly was flying at the torque meter in an illuminated arena without any visual motion feedback or explicit stationary patterns. The whole range of spontaneously generated yaw torque was arbitrarily divided by the computer into two halves corresponding roughly to the domains for left and right turns in free flight. During training heat was applied whenever yaw torque was in one of the domains. The fly readily adjusted its yaw torque range to avoid being heated and kept this reduced range even after heat was permanently switched off. Figure 6 shows avoidance and memory scores for HU flies and control flies. As pointed out earlier (Wolf and Heisenberg 1991), the first difficulty for the fly in this experiment is to detect that it is the yaw torque that matters with respect to temperature. The second step is the lasting modification of the yaw torque range. Apparently, the MBs are not involved in either of these processes.
SPATIAL LEARNING OF FREELY WALKING FLIES IN THE HEATING CHAMBER
Recently, Wustmann et al. (1996) have developed a simple device to test spatial learning in freely walking single flies. It consists of a small chamber that can be heated instantaneously by Peltier elements above and below the fly. A directional light gate in the middle of the chamber monitors whether the fly is in one or the other half. The experiment consists of a preference test, a training test, and a memory test. During training the entire chamber is heated to 37°C whenever the fly is on one side of the light gate and cooled to 25°C if the fly is on the other side. Most flies rapidly learn to avoid the hot side and keep their acquired side preference even during the subsequent memory test without heat (for details of the procedure, see Materials and Methods).
The entire experiment can be conducted in complete darkness. Wustmann and Heisenberg (1997) have shown that in this case the fly has only tactile and ideothetic (self-generated) cues for its spatial orientation in the chamber. If in a different experiment visual landmarks [yellow and blue light-emitting diodes (LEDs)] are provided, normal flies rely on both visual and tactile/ideothetic cues. In both versions of the test MB-less flies were not measurably impaired (Fig. 7), indicating that the MBs are not essential for telling the fly where in the chamber it is in relation to the invisible potential heat threat. The LEDs as visual landmarks for orientation did not confuse the flies. Because HU flies have a largely normal visual system (see above) we assume that like the control flies they use the visual cues for orientation if these are provided. It cannot be excluded, however, that they just ignore them.
Discussion
A total of 18 associative learning tasks have now been tested in MB-less flies (Table 1). Together they provide an unambiguous answer to the question whether the MBs are a general device for associative learning and memory. The answer is no. Fortunately, presence or absence of MBs is easily and unambiguously shown for the individual fly and if MB-less flies obtained with one kind of technique can be shown to perform normally in a learning paradigm, no further controls and no other methods of blocking the MB pathway need to be explored. Only if a behavioral deficit is observed, as in the case of odor discrimination conditioning, it is important to use several different routes of intervention to make sure that it is indeed the block in the MBs causing the abnormality.
Table 1.
(HU) Hydroxyurea; (Gαs) transgenic expression of a constitutively activated G-protein αs subunit in the Kenyon cells; for further explanations see text.
It may not be a big surprise that motor learning at the torque meter works properly in MB-less flies. After all, posture learning and the plasticity of simple sensory motor responses can be observed in decapitated insects including Drosophila (Horridge 1962; Hoyle 1980; Booker and Quinn 1981; Corfas and Dudai 1989). Taking this older study into account there could not have been any doubt that some instances of associative learning work without MBs. However, because in Drosophila thermoreception is mediated by the antennae (Sayeed and Benzer 1996), an involvement of the MB pathway did not seem totally unlikely. It would be most interesting to determine where in the central nervous system the efference copy of the yaw torque motor program is correlated with the temporal modulation of thermoreception (Wolf and Heisenberg 1991). Our present results indicate that the MBs are not involved in this process.
More surprisingly, the MBs are dispensable in all visual learning tasks investigated so far. These involve freely walking and stationarily flying animals, as well as both classic and operant associative learning. Several suggestions regarding the function of the MBs can now be rejected. For instance, the hypothesis that MBs might serve as matrices for memory templates (Heisenberg 1989, 1994) must be discarded regarding visual pattern recognition. Stationary and moving visual patterns as well as different light intensities and colors (cited in Heisenberg et al. 1985; Heisenberg 1994) can be learned by MB-less flies as conditioned stimuli. Another suggestion had been that the MBs might be required to adjust and accommodate the different time courses of the conditioned and unconditioned stimuli (summarized in Erber et al. 1987). Again, independent of the stimulus used as negative or positive reinforcer (heat, vigorous shaking, visual oscillations, sucrose), MB-less flies have no difficulty to learn the task. Even the remarkable ability of Drosophila to cope with the inversion of visual feedback in the flight simulator (Heisenberg and Wolf 1984) is not impaired in MB-less flies (Heisenberg 1989). In all these cases, no tests for long-term memory have been performed. It cannot be excluded, therefore, that these memory traces are less stable than in the controls.
In three of the experiments we tested visual learning in the flight simulator. The finding that MB-less flies learn and remember properly under these conditions also implies that they can perform basic visual flight control. Drescher (1960) has reported an increased latency of the optomotor response in cockroaches with lesioned calyces. Such an effect, if present in MB-less Drosophila, would probably have dramatic consequences in the flight simulator and would hardly remain undiscovered. Nevertheless, the tests conducted so far account only for a few selected aspects of the fly’s visual abilities. The recent finding that visual experience influences the size of the calyx (Barth and Heisenberg 1997) should encourage a detailed search for visual functions that require the MBs.
Recently, Mizunami et al. (1993) generated mechanical lesions in the MBs of Periplaneta americana. The operated animals performed poorly in a spatial memory task. Our result with MB-less flies performing the spatial learning test in the heating chamber is not in accord with their observation. Several factors may account for the discrepancy. The two paradigms have very different designs (size of arena, difficulty of search, sensory cues, etc.). Moreover, Drosophila has a lobe-type MB, and Periplaneta has a calyx-type MB. Also, it should be kept in mind that with one kind of intervention alone it is difficult to assign a behavioral function to a particular neural structure, as one cannot be certain that the intervention has no effects outside of the target structure.
So far, the only behavioral function shown to require intact MBs is olfactory learning. As discussed in the Introduction, converging evidence from honeybees, ants, and Drosophila indicates beyond a reasonable doubt that learning or remembering odors is impaired whenever the MB pathway is blocked. No other behavioral paradigm has been sufficiently tested with several independent methods of intervention to warrant a similar conclusion (but see Martin et al. 1998). Considering that so far all conditioning experiments involving chemical signals as conditioned stimuli have been sensitive to MB ablation, it is interesting that conditioned courtship suppression by mated females works properly without MBs [preliminary data on the mutant mbd cited in Heisenberg 1994; and recent experiments of E. Savvateeva (in prep.) using the HU ablation technique]. This finding underscores the importance of nonchemical cues [the physical presence of the courted fly or dummy (Tompkins et al. 1983)], which the courting male associates with the anti-aphrodisiac pheromones of the mated female.
For Drosophila, MB mutants, the HU ablation technique, and a growing collection of specific MB enhancer strains allowing to direct suitable effector genes to the Kenyon cells, now make a systematic search for MB functions in behavior feasible. Several candidate behaviors all involving chemoreception have been tentatively assigned to the MBs. Among them are gustatory learning (J. Médioni, cited in Heisenberg 1989), mate recognition (Ferveur et al. 1995; O’Dell et al. 1995), and spontaneous odor preferences (Heisenberg 1989). It remains a great challenge to determine in more detail how the MB pathway, or more specifically the properties of the Kenyon cells influence behavior. Behavioral analysis will be indispensible to understand how the MB pathway contributes to the functioning of the brain.
Acknowledgments
We thank S. Flurschütz for technical assistance and S. Kramer for valuable comments on the manuscript. This work was supported by Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (He986) and Alexander-von-Humbold-Foundation (L.L.).
The publication costs of this article were defrayed in part by payment of page charges. This article must therefore be hereby marked “advertisement” in accordance with 18 USC section 1734 solely to indicate this fact.
References
- Aceves-Piña EO, Quinn WG. Learning in normal and mutant Drosophila larvae. Science. 1979;26:93–96. doi: 10.1126/science.206.4414.93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barth M, Heisenberg M. Vision affects mushroom bodies and central complex in Drosophila melanogaster. Learn & Mem. 1997;4:219–229. doi: 10.1101/lm.4.2.219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Booker R, Quinn WG. Conditioning of leg position in normal and mutant Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 1981;78:3940–3944. doi: 10.1073/pnas.78.6.3940. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Connolly JB, Roberts IJH, Armstrong JD, Kaiser K, Forte M, Tully T, O’Kane CJ. Associative learning disrupted by impaired GS signaling in Drosophila mushroom bodies. Science. 1996;274:2104–2107. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5295.2104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corfas G, Dudai Y. Habituation and dishabituation of a cleaning reflex in normal and mutant Drosophila. J Neurosci. 1989;9:56–62. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.09-01-00056.1989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Belle JS, Heisenberg M. Associative odor learning in Drosophila abolished by chemical ablation of mushroom bodies. Science. 1994;263:692–695. doi: 10.1126/science.8303280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- ————— Expression of Drosophila mushroom body mutations in alternative genetic backgrounds: A case study of the mushroom body miniature gene (mbm) Proc Natl Acad Sci. 1996;93:9875–9880. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.18.9875. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeJianne D, McGuire TR, Pruzan-Hotchkiss A. Pavlovian counterconditioning of proboscis extension in Drosophila melanogaster. J Comp Psychol. 1984;99:74–80. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dill M, Wolf R, Heisenberg M. Behavioral analysis of Drosophila landmark learning in the flight simulator. Learn & Mem. 1995;2:152–160. doi: 10.1101/lm.2.3-4.152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drescher W. Regenerationsversuche am Gehirn von Periplaneta americana unter Berücksichtigung von Verhaltensänderung und Neurosekretion. Z Morphol Ökol Tiere. 1960;48:576–649. [Google Scholar]
- Dujardin F. Memoires sur le systeme nerveux des insectes. Ann Sci Nat (Zool) 1850;14:195–205. [Google Scholar]
- Erber J, Masuhr T, Menzel R. Localization of short-term memory in the brain of the bee Apis mellifera. Physiol Entomol. 1980;5:343–358. [Google Scholar]
- Erber J, Homberg U, Gronenberg W. Functional roles of the mushroom bodies in insects. In: Gupta AP, editor. Arthropod brain. Its evolution, development, structure, and functions. New York, NY: John Wiley; 1987. pp. 485–511. [Google Scholar]
- Ferveur JF, Stoertkuhl KF, Stocker RF, Greenspan RJ. Genetic feminization of brain structures and changed sexual orientation in male Drosophila melanogaster. Science. 1995;267:902–905. doi: 10.1126/science.7846534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guo A, Liu L, Xia S-Z, Feng C-H, Wolf R, Heisenberg M. Conditioned visual flight orientation in Drosophila: Dependence on age, practice and diet. Learn & Mem. 1996;3:49–59. doi: 10.1101/lm.3.1.49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanström B. Vergleichende Anatomie des Nervensystems der wirbellosen Tiere. Berlin, Germany: Springer Verlag; 1928. [Google Scholar]
- Heisenberg M. Genetic approach to learning and memory (mnemogenetics) in Drosophila melanogaster. Neuronal plasticity and brain function. Fortschr Zool. 1989;37:3–45. [Google Scholar]
- ————— Central brain function in insects: Genetic studies on the mushroom bodies and central complex in Drosophila. Neural Basis of behavioural adaptations. Fortschr Zool. 1994;39:61–79. [Google Scholar]
- Heisenberg M, Wolf R. On the fine structure of yaw torque in visual flight orientation of Drosophila melanogaster. J Comp Physiol. 1979;130:113–130. [Google Scholar]
- ————— . Vision in Drosophila. In: Braitenberg V, editor. Studies of bran function. XII. Berlin, Germany: Springer-Verlag; 1984. [Google Scholar]
- ————— Reafferent control of optomotor yaw torque in Drosophila melanogaster. J Comp Physiol A. 1988;163:373–388. [Google Scholar]
- ————— . The sensory-motor link in motion-dependent flight control of flies. In: Miles FA, Wallmann J, editors. Visual motion and its role in the stabilization of gaze. New York, NY: Elsevier Science; 1993. pp. 265–283. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heisenberg M, Borst A, Wagner S, Byers D. Drosophila mushroom body mutants are deficient in olfactory learning. J Neurogenet. 1985;2:1–30. doi: 10.3109/01677068509100140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horridge GA. Learning of leg positions by headless insects. Nature. 1962;193:697–698. doi: 10.1038/193697a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Howse PE. Design and function in the insect brain. In: Barton-Brown L, editor. Experimental analysis of insect behaviour. Berlin, Germany: Springer; 1974. pp. 180–194. [Google Scholar]
- Hoyle G. Learning using natural reinforments in insect preparations that permit cellular neuronal analysis. J Neurobiol. 1980;11:323–354. doi: 10.1002/neu.480110402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huber F. Untersuchungen über die Funktion des Zentralnervensystems und insbesondere des Gehirns bei der Fortbewegung und der Lauterzeugung der Grillen. Z Vergl Physiol. 1960;44:60–132. [Google Scholar]
- Ito K, Hotta Y. Proliferation pattern of postembryonic neuroblasts in the brain of Drosophila melanogaster. Dev Biol. 1991;149:134–148. doi: 10.1016/0012-1606(92)90270-q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kloot van der WG, Williams CM. Cocoon construction by the Cecropia silkworm. III. The alteration of spinning behavior by chemical and surgical techniques. Behaviour. 1954;6:233–255. [Google Scholar]
- Martin, J.R., R. Ernst, and M. Heisenberg. 1998. Mushroom bodies suppress locomotor activity in Drosophila melanogaster. Learn. & Mem. (this issue). [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Masuhr T. “Lokalisation und Funktion des Kurzzeitgedächtnisses der Honigbiene (Apis mellifera L.).” Ph.D. Dissertation. Germany: Fachbereich Biologie, TH Darmstadt; 1976. [Google Scholar]
- Menne D, Spatz HC. Color vision in Drosophila melanogaster. J Comp Physiol A. 1977;114:301–312. [Google Scholar]
- Mizunami M, Weibrecht JM, Strausfeld NJ. A new role for the insect mushroom bodies: Place memory and motor control. In: Beer RD, Ritzman RE, McKenna T, editors. Biological neural networks in invertebrate neuroethology and robotics. New York, NY: Academic Press; 1993. pp. 199–225. [Google Scholar]
- O’Dell KMC, Armstrong JD, Yang MY, Kaiser K. Functional dissection of the Drosophila mushroom bodies by selective feminization of genetically defined subcompartments. Neuron. 1995;15:55–61. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(95)90064-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prokop A, Technau GM. Normal function of the mushroom body defect gene of Drosophila is required for the regulation of the number and proliferation of neuroblasts. Dev Biol. 1994;161:321–337. doi: 10.1006/dbio.1994.1034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quinn WG, Harris WA, Benzer S. Conditioned behavior in Drosophila melanogaster. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 1974;71:708–712. doi: 10.1073/pnas.71.3.708. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riggs LA, Ratliff F, Cornsweet JC, Cornsweet TN. The disappearance of steadily fixated visual test objects. J Opt Soc Am. 1953;43:495–501. doi: 10.1364/josa.43.000495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sachs L. Angewandte statistik. 7th ed. Berlin, Germany: Springer; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Sayeed O, Benzer S. Behavioral genetics of thermosensation and hygrosensation in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 1996;93:6079–6084. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.12.6079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schürmann FW. The architecture of the mushroom bodies and related neuropils in the insect brain. In: Gupta AG, editor. Arthropod brain: Its evolution, development, structure, and functions. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons; 1987. pp. 231–264. [Google Scholar]
- Siegel RW, Hall JC. Conditioned responses in courtship behavior of normal and mutant Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 1979;76:3430–3434. doi: 10.1073/pnas.76.7.3430. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Srinivasan MV, Zhang SW, Rolfe B. Is pattern vision in insects mediated by “cortical” processing? Nature. 1993;362:539–540. [Google Scholar]
- Tempel BL, Bonini N, Dawson DR, Quinn WG. Reward learning in normal and mutant Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 1983;80:1482–1486. doi: 10.1073/pnas.80.5.1482. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tompkins L, Hall JC, Hall LM. Conditioned courtship in Drosphila and its mediation by association of chemical cues. Behav Genet. 1983;13:565–578. doi: 10.1007/BF01076402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tully T, Quinn WG. Classical conditioning and retention in normal and mutant Drosophila melanogaster. J Comp Physiol A. 1985;157:263–277. doi: 10.1007/BF01350033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voskresenskaja AK. On the role played by mushroom bodies (corpora pedunculata) on the supraesophageal ganglion in the conditioned reflexes of the honey bee. Dokl Akad Nauk SSSR. 1957;112:964–967. [Google Scholar]
- Vowles DM. Olfactory learning and brain lesions in the wood ant (Formica rufa) J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1964;58:105–111. doi: 10.1037/h0042609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolf R, Heisenberg M. Basic organization of operant behavior as revealed in Drosophila flight orientation. J Comp Physiol A. 1991;169:699–705. doi: 10.1007/BF00194898. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- ————— Visual space from visual motion: Turn integration in tethered flying Drosophila. Learn & Mem. 1997;4:318–327. doi: 10.1101/lm.4.4.318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wustmann G, Heisenberg M. Behavioral manipulation of retrieval in a spatial memory task for Drosophila melanogaster. Learn & Mem. 1997;4:328–336. doi: 10.1101/lm.4.4.328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wustmann G, Rein K, Wolf R, Heisenberg M. A new paradigm for operant conditioning of Drosophila melanogaster. J Comp Physiol A. 1996;179:429–436. doi: 10.1007/BF00194996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeil J. Orientation flights of solitary wasps (Cerceris; Sphecidae; Hymenoptera) II. Similarities between orientation and return flights and the use of motion parallax. J Comp Physiol A. 1993;172:207–222. [Google Scholar]