Abstract
Our objective was to ascertain the nature of the associations between C-reactive protein (CRP) and cognition, and to examine how they are affected by gender and obesity. We evaluated 62 females and 63 males between 42 and 82 years of age. There were 20 lean females with a body mass index (BMI) of < 25 kg/m2 and 42 were overweight or obese, with BMIs ≥ 25 kg/m2. There were 14 lean males and 49 with BMIs ≥ 25 kg/m2. CRP was associated with cognitive tests of frontal lobe function only among females and these associations were driven by the overweight/obese female group. No associations between CRP and cognition were found among males. Obesity-associated inflammation appears to be more pronounced in females and is associated with cognitive dysfunction, particularly of frontal lobe tasks.
Keywords: CRP, cognition, gender, overweight, obesity
INTRODUCTION
C-reactive protein (CRP) is an acute phase reactant produced by the liver in response to inflammation within the body. Low-grade elevations in serum CRP have been found to be independently predictive of coronary heart disease and have been implicated in the development of other conditions, such as type 2 diabetes (T2DM) and the metabolic syndrome1, 2, 3. Serum CRP levels less than 1 mg/l are associated with a low risk, levels between 1 and 3 mg/l confer an average risk, and those between 3 and 10 mg/l are associated with increased risk for cardiovascular disease (CVD)4. Roughly 33% of the population in the US has levels between 3 and 10 mg/l, and are thus considered at increased risk for CVD5. A wide variety of conditions have been found to elicit transient elevations in CRP. However, chronic low-grade elevations, accompanied by features of the metabolic syndrome6, may be indicative of a constitutive inflammatory process, which may help explain why these low-grade elevations are linked to increased cardiovascular risk1.
CRP, Obesity and Gender
Obesity, as defined by a body mass index (BMI) of ≥ 30 kg/m2, may be accompanied by a state of chronic low-grade inflammation and CRP levels are often elevated in both men and women with BMIs in the overweight (≥ 25 kg/m2) and obese range7. While CRP elevations are associated with the metabolic syndrome8, they are particularly related to abdominal obesity9, 10, and this association is more prominent among females11, 12, 13. Most studies have found that CRP levels are generally higher in females,14, 15, 16, 17 possibly mediated by estrogen18, a contention that is supported by the fact that hormone replacement therapy is also associated with increased levels of CRP18. However, it is also possible that higher CRP levels in women may reflect sex-related differences in the inflammatory response to obesity19.
CRP, Metabolic Dysregulation, and Cognition
Obesity is frequently associated with metabolic syndrome, insulin resistance, and type 2 diabetes20, 21. It has been well established that type 2 diabetes mellitus (T2DM) and insulin resistance short of T2DM are associated with cognitive deficits22, 23, 24, 25, 26. Our group has demonstrated that both declarative memory deficits and hippocampal volume reductions are associated with impairments in glucose control27, 28.
There is an expanding body of literature linking cognitive deficits with obesity, independent of metabolic dysregulation. Gunstad and colleagues found that a BMI greater than 25 was associated with executive dysfunction in a sample of 408 adult males and females29. A Korean study, utilizing the ethnic-specific criteria for obesity and waist circumference developed for Asian populations30, found that among adults 65 and older, poorer cognition (as reflected by K-MMSE score) was associated with being overweight or obese and particularly with the presence of abdominal obesity31. The Framingham Heart Study reported gender-specific results describing adverse obesity effects on cognition only among men32. Significant associations between waist circumference, hypertension and cognition were found in healthy older adults33. The metabolic syndrome, which is comprised of several risk factors including abdominal obesity34, has been associated with cognitive problems35.
There is a growing literature assessing the associations between CRP and cognitive performance. For example, Yaffe and colleagues36 found that elevated serum levels of CRP were associated with cognitive decline in individuals with the metabolic syndrome. Several studies have shown that CRP predicts cognition longitudinally. For example, Komulainen, et al,37 found that in elderly women higher baseline CRP levels were predictive of poorer memory 12 years later. A meta-analysis of six studies looking at CRP and cognition concluded that higher levels of CRP were predictive of cognitive decline and dementia38. However, most of these studies utilized somewhat insensitive tools for the assessment of cognition, most frequently the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE). The goal of the present study was to ascertain the associations between CRP and cognitive performance employing a comprehensive neuropsychological assessment. In addition, we were particularly interested in assessing how gender and obesity affect those associations.
METHODS
Participants
We evaluated 125 participants consisting of consecutive cases that were part of a study assessing the relationship between peripheral glucose regulation and brain function. All were community-residing individuals living independently. Subjects were either referred by collaborating endocrinologists, responded to advertisements, or were participating in longitudinal studies of normal aging. Participants were between 42 and 82 years of age and had a minimum of a high school education. All study subjects were functioning in the cognitively normal range, as indicated by scores on the Mini-Mental State Exam, and were free of current or historical evidence of significant neurological, medical (other than type 2 diabetes, dyslipidemia, or hypertension), or psychiatric disease. Participants gave informed written consent and were compensated for their participation. The study was approved by the NYU Institutional Board of Research Associates. Subsets of 46 and 60 subjects, respectively, were previously included in two other reports on related topics27, 39. One female subject was excluded due to CRP values exceeding 10 mg/l, which may have been due to an acute infection.
Participants with elevated BMI
Participants qualified for inclusion in this group if they met general population standard criteria40, namely if they had a BMI greater than or equal to 25 kg/m2. As anticipated from national statistics, some two thirds of our study participants (91/125), were overweight or obese. Thirty-six of the 91 overweight or obese subjects (40%) had a diagnosis of T2DM and none were being treated with insulin or insulin secretagogues. Thirty-eight (42%) met criteria for hypertension and 33 (36%) were prescribed statins for dyslipidemia.
Normal BMI participants
Participants qualified for inclusion in this group if they had a BMI between 18 and 24.9 kg/m2. Thirty four of our 125 participants (27%) were in the normal BMI group. Two of the 34 (6%) had a diagnosis of type 2 diabetes, 6 (18%) met criteria for hypertension and 4 (12%) were prescribed statins for dyslipidemia.
Evaluations
All subjects underwent an assessment consisting of a complete medical evaluation, including physical and neurological examinations, as well as neuropsychological and psychiatric evaluations. All subjects had fasting blood tests that included complete cell count, chemistries, liver profile, metabolic profile including lipids, glucose and insulin, and CRP.
Physical Examination
Blood pressure and definition of hypertension
Blood pressure was measured twice during one of the standardized visits, with readings performed at 08:30 h, 30 minutes after the participants arrived, and again at 13:40 h, after the procedure was complete. These readings were then averaged. Hypertension has been linked to both inflammation and cognitive dysfunction. Thus, hypertension was defined based on recommendations of the National Cholesterol Education Program (NCEP) guidelines, namely: (1) systolic value greater than or equal to 130 mm Hg, or (2) diastolic value greater than or equal to 85 mm Hg, or (3) use of anti-hypertensive medication.
C-Reactive Protein (CRP)
We assessed plasma levels of c-reactive protein (CRP) after an overnight fast. CRP levels were measured in our medical center clinical laboratory using an enzymatic immunoassay (Vitros CRP slide, Ortho Clinical Diagnostics).
Neuropsychological and Psychiatric assessment
All cognitive assessments are routinely used standardized neuropsychological tests described in detail elsewhere41. Briefly, declarative memory was assessed with the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT), and subtests from the Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised (WMS-R). Working memory was evaluated using the Digit Span Backwards and Visual Memory Span Backwards from the WMS-R. Executive Function was measured with the computer-administered Tower of London (Colorado Assessment Tests). Attention was assessed with subtests from the Wechsler Memory Scale – Revised (WMS-R). General Intellectual Functioning was assessed with the Shipley Institute of Living Scale scores which were used to estimate WAIS-R full scale IQ scores. Lastly, depressive symptoms were assessed with the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale42.
Statistical Analyses
Differences between groups were analyzed using student's t-tests and analyses of variance (ANOVA) or covariance (ANCOVA) when appropriate. When assessing associations between different variables, correlations or partial correlations were used. A number of recent studies have found that statin medications are effective at lowering CRP levels43,44,45, so, we controlled for statin use in some of our analyses. Because type 2 diabetes has been linked to cognitive dysfunction, we controlled for this diagnosis in the cognitive analyses where merited. Age has been associated with CRP levels46. Partial correlations, controlling for age and statin use, were used to ascertain associations between CRP and other biological variables. Education has been found to be associated with cognitive performance47. Therefore, partial correlations were also used to examine the associations between CRP and cognitive variables, controlling for age, education, diabetes, and statin use. To further assess the relationship between CRP and the cognitive variables that appeared to be associated with CRP in the correlation analyses, linear regression models were used. These models were utilized so as to assess the explanatory power of CRP after accounting for the appropriate potential confounds. Data were analyzed using SPSS for Windows, version 12.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, Ill., USA).
RESULTS
Demographic variables and group descriptors by gender
The demographic variables and group descriptors, listed separately for men and women, are summarized in Table 1.
Table 1.
Females | Males | |
---|---|---|
n = 62 | n = 63 | |
Age (yr) | 61.75 ± 8.77 | 62.06 ± 8.05 |
Education (yr) | 15.82 ± 2.30 | 16.07 ± 2.06 |
Body Mass Index (kg/m2) | 27.30 ± 5.36 | 28.68 ± 5.77 |
Waist Size (in) | 92.98 ± 13.05 | 104.27 ± 12.26 |
HbA1c (%) | 5.83 ± .91 | 6.40 ± 1.87 |
Fasting Glucose (mg/dl) | 93.81 ± 27.05 | 107.21 ± 46.23 |
Fasting Insulin (μIU/ml)† | 7.35 ± 4.15 | 8.74 ± 4.46 |
HDL (mg/dl) | 61.56 ± 17.22 | 45.62 ± 10.55 |
Triglycerides (mg/dl) | 104.98 ± 59.09 | 125.36 ± 75.34 |
LDL (mg/dl)‡ | 119.96 ± 34.72 | 104.67 ± 32.71 |
Fibrinogen (mg/dl) | 351.74 ± 96.31 | 348.65 ± 112.63 |
CRP (mg/l) | 2.24 ± 1.95 | 1.84 ± 2.03 |
Hamilton Depression Score | 2.25 ± 2.65 | 2.36 ± 3.16 |
Hypertension | 20 (32.3%) | 24 (38.1%) |
Type 2 Diabetes | 17 (27.4%) | 21 (33.3%) |
Statin Treatment | 12 (19.4%) | 25 (39.7%) |
Diabetics excluded.
Those on statin treatment excluded.
Given that our main interest was to ascertain whether the associations between CRP and cognition differed by gender and by weight category, we next created descriptions of the sample separately for men and women by BMI groups. As can be seen in Table 2a, we had 20 lean females (32%) with a BMI of 24.9 kg/m2 or less, and 42 females (68%) with a BMI of 25 kg/m2 or greater. As expected, there were significant group differences for HbA1c, HDL, fibrinogen, CRP, and waist circumference between the female BMI groups.
Table 2a.
FEMALES | Normal BMI | Overwt/Obese |
---|---|---|
n = 20 | n = 42 | |
Age (yr) | 60.54 ± 9.54 | 62.33 ± 8.44 |
Education (yr) | 16.30 ± 1.63 | 15.60 ± 2.55 |
Body Mass Index (kg/m2)** | 21.97 ± 2.02 | 29.83 ± 4.52 |
Waist Size (in)** | 80.96 ± 9.02 | 98.85 ± 10.45 |
HbA1c (%)** | 5.33 ± .25 | 6.08 ± 1.01 |
HDL (mg/dl)** | 72.10 ± 13.83 | 56.55 ± 16.51 |
Fibrinogen (mg/dl)** | 310.85 ± 55.64 | 371.21 ± 105.65 |
CRP (mg/l)** | 1.08 ± 1.53 | 2.80 ± 1.90 |
Hypertension** | 2 (10%) | 18 (42.9%) |
Type 2 Diabetes** | 0 (0%) | 17 (40.5%) |
Statin Treatment** | 1 (5%) | 11 (26.2%) |
p < 0.05
The male subjects are presented by BMI group in Table 2b. We had 14 lean males (22%) and 49 (78%) in the overweight or obese category. We found significant differences in HDL, CRP, and waist size, but interestingly, HbA1c did not distinguish the male BMI groups.
Table 2b.
MALES | Normal BMI | Overwt/Obese |
---|---|---|
n = 14 | n = 49 | |
Age | 64.64 ± 7.96 | 61.33 ± 8.01 |
Education (yr) | 16.71 ± 2.40 | 15.89 ± 1.94 |
Body Mass Index (kg/m2)** | 23.03 ± 1.03 | 30.29 ± 5.54 |
Waist Size (in)** | 92.40 ± 5.49 | 107.66 ± 11.54 |
HbA1c (%) | 5.89 ± 1.65 | 6.55 ± 1.92 |
HDL (mg/dl)** | 55.00 ± 9.37 | 42.83 ± 9.25 |
Fibrinogen | 322.43 ± 87.07 | 356.14 ± 118.63 |
CRP** | .84 ± .83 | 2.13 ± 2.19 |
Hypertension | 4 (28.6%) | 20 (40.8%) |
Diabetes** | 2 (14.3%) | 19 (38.8%) |
Statin Treatment* | 3 (21.4%) | 22 (44.9%) |
p < 0.05
Associations between CRP and Clinical Variables
Across all subjects we found positive associations between CRP and waist size (r = .42, p < .001) and HbA1c (r = .34, p < .001). A weak (although statistically significant) negative association existed between CRP and HDL (r = −.24, p = .010), but no significant correlation was detected between CRP and triglycerides (r = .12, p = .189). ANOVA detected no differences in CRP levels between hypertensives and non-hypertensives (F = 1.219, p = .272).
When we looked at these associations separately in men and women, we found that there were some distinct gender differences in the association of CRP with clinical variables.
Among males, CRP was positively associated with waist size (r = .478, p = .000) and HbA1c (r = .419, p = .001), and tended to be negatively associated with HDL (r = −.232, p = .077). Among females, CRP was positively correlated with waist size (r = .421, p = .001) and triglycerides (r = .26, p = .045) and negatively correlated with HDL (r = −.413, p = .001). Interestingly, HbA1c was not significantly associated with CRP among our female group (r = .197, p = .132). No associations between hypertension and CRP were found for either the entire sample (F = 1.219, p =.272) or individual gender groups (males: F = .742, p = .393, females: F = 1.403, p = .241)
Cognitive differences between weight groups
When looking at both males and females together, no significant differences in cognition were found between the normal BMI and overweight/obese groups after adjusting for age, education, statin and diabetes. There were no cognitive differences by BMI group even when we did not adjust for the presence of type 2 diabetes.
We found significant differences in cognition between males and females. As shown in Table 4, males scored significantly poorer (had more excess moves) than females on the Tower of London, a measure of executive function. However, the most marked gender differences were on tests of declarative memory. Scores on the CVLT, a list-learning task, and all of the memory indexes on the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised (WMS-R) showed that males scored significantly lower than females. This is consistent with the fact that our males as a group had significantly worse peripheral glucose regulation, a factor that has been linked to decreased memory performance. There were no gender differences on the Attention/Concentration Index of the WMS-R.
Table 4.
Males | Females | p | |
---|---|---|---|
WAIS Full-Scale IQ | 112.41 ± 12.70 | 113.08 ± 11.23 | ns |
London Excess Moves | 10.5 ± 6.86 | 7.21 ± 6.05 | .013 |
Digit Span Backward | 7.67 ± 2.56 | 7.59 ± 2.55 | ns |
Vis Mem Span Backward | 7.34 ± 1.61 | 7.42 ± 2.08 | ns |
Shipley Total | 61.98 ± 11.80 | 62.71 ± 10.25 | ns |
CVLT Trials 1–5 | 54.30 ± 9.69 | 62.90 ± 10.12 | <.001 |
CVLT Short Delay Free Recall | 10.71 ± 3.63 | 13.20 ± 3.32 | <.001 |
CVLT Long Delay Free Recall | 11.15 ± 3.52 | 12.98 ± 3.22 | <.001 |
WMSR - General Memory Index | 116.85 ± 16.25 | 123.80 ± 13.65 | .013 |
WMSR – Attn/Concentration Index | 109.05 ± 15.09 | 107.20 ± 17.84 | ns |
WMSR – Verbal Memory Index | 113.03 ± 16.52 | 120.31 ± 13.56 | .008 |
WMSR – Visual Memory Index | 116.15 ± 15.18 | 122.81 ± 13.66 | .013 |
WMSR – Delayed Memory Index | 118.54 ± 17.20 | 124.25 ± 16.23 | .064 |
No associations were found between CRP and any of the cognitive variables among males. Because the distribution of CRP values within our male group were skewed, we performed non-parametric analyses to confirm our negative results among the males. In addition, we ran a linear regression analysis using all subjects and found a significant Gender and Gender x CRP interaction in explaining cognition (Partial Eta Squared = .10 and .04 respectively). This interaction was being driven by the females, whose CRP values were much more normally distributed.
It is known that, in general, HDL levels are higher among females, and there is evidence that higher HDL levels may be neuroprotective48. It is also known that CRP levels are higher in both females and among people who are overweight or obese. Given the above-mentioned cognitive differences by gender and the fact that females have both more protective (HDL) and risk factors (higher CRP levels), we chose to examine the effects of CRP on cognition more closely within our female group, paying particular attention to how BMI group affected those associations.
Age, education, and diabetes are all known to be associated with cognition. There are also links between CRP and statin use. Consequently, to ascertain the associations between CRP and cognition within each BMI group, we utilized partial correlations and controlled for age, education, diabetes, and statin use. Significant associations between CRP and cognition were only found among overweight/obese females: WAIS estimated full-scale IQ (r = −.386, p = .015), Tower of London Excess Moves (r = .509, p = .003), figural memory subtest of the WMS-R (r = −.463, p = .004) and Shipley total raw score (r = −.384, p = .016). Trends were detected in two tests of working memory: digit span backward (r = −.286, p = .081) and the visual memory span backward (r = −.313, p = .055) subtests of the WMS-R.
Contribution of CRP to explaining cognition (Linear Regressions)
Because our weight groups differed in peripheral glucose control, which we know plays a role in cognition, and also differed in other factors involved in the metabolic syndrome, we ran regression analyses to further examine the relationship between CRP and the cognitive tests that appeared to be associated with CRP in our female group. After accounting for age, education, and statin use (3.4% of the variance), CRP was entered stepwise in a block with other clinical variables (HDL, triglycerides, hypertension (y/n), waist circumference, and HbA1c) for the entire female group (n=62). The only variables that emerged as related to scores on the Tower of London were CRP and hypertension. We then followed this up with hierarchical regression models using the Tower of London excess moves score as the dependent variable. After age, education, and statin use (3.4% of the variance), hypertension explained an additional 4.5 %, and CRP level when entered last independently explained 16.5% of variance.
When these regression analyses were restricted to the overweight/obese females, after accounting for age, education, and statin use (4% of variance), hypertension explains an additional 6% of the variance, and CRP level, when entered last, independently explained nearly 32% of the variance. Please note that when WAIS Full-Scale IQ was used as the dependent variable, although explaining less of the variance, similar significant results were obtained for CRP levels. Please refer to Table 6.
Table 6.
All Females | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age/Edu/Statin | HTN | CRP | |||||||
Ǝ | ΔR2 | p | Ǝ | ΔR2 | p | Ǝ | ΔR2 | p | |
London Excess Moves | −.092/.143/.359 | .034 | .670 | −.317 | .045 | .152 | .430 | .165 | .004 |
WAIS Full-Scale IQ | .278/.153/−.014 | .063 | .475 | −.277 | .090 | .055 | −.370 | .108 | .028 |
Overweight/Obese Females | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age/Edu/Statin | HTN | CRP | |||||||
β | ΔR2 | p | β | ΔR2 | p | β | ΔR2 | p | |
London Excess Moves | −.240/.225/.477 | .039 | .746 | −.356 | .060 | .176 | .652 | .318 | .001 |
WAIS Full-Scale IQ | .351/.143/.021 | .130 | .043 | −.253 | .074 | .025 | −.247 | .056 | .043 |
DISCUSSION
The objective of this study was to ascertain the associations between CRP and cognition, and to examine how those associations are affected by gender and obesity. Based on literature demonstrating associations between elevated CRP and impaired cognitive performance36,49,50, we hypothesized that CRP levels would be inversely associated with cognitive performance. Furthermore, we sought to examine whether the associations between CRP and cognition would vary by gender or BMI.
Obesity is associated with an inflammatory state51. We therefore predicted that CRP, an inflammatory marker, would be elevated in overweight/obese subjects. Consistent with prior findings9, CRP was significantly elevated in our overweight/obese subjects and was positively associated with waist circumference in both our males and females. We found that CRP was positively associated with HbA1C in males, but not in females. Florez et al similarly found that CRP was associated with insulin resistance only in males9, perhaps suggesting that obesity-related impairments in insulin function, which were more pronounced among our male study participants, may vary by gender in its associated pro-inflammatory response. CRP in females was positively associated with triglycerides and negatively associated with HDL, which is consistent with previous findings of CRP's positive association with the metabolic syndrome8. Females naturally have higher levels of HDL52, and given the inverse associations between HDL and CRP in females, this may help explain the protective role of HDL in cardiovascular health and perhaps brain health. Therefore, this may help explain why the females with lower HDL and larger waist circumference, who also have higher levels of CRP, may have more cognitive deficits.
We found that CRP levels were higher in females than in males, as has been established in other studies14,53. Of interest, women are more prone to autoimmune diseases such as Lupus and Rheumatoid Arthritis54,55, which are characterized by inflammation, elevated CRP56,57, and accelerated rates of microvascular disease and atherosclerosis58,59. Elevated CRP in these women is also associated with cognitive impairments. In one recent study, Shucard and co-investigators documented that elevated CRP in female lupus patients correlated with deficits in executive cognitive function, possibly due to microvascular compromise60. It has been documented that obesity is associated with low-grade systemic inflammation in both men and women. Given that women generally have higher constitutive CRP levels than men and are also more prone to inflammatory diseases61, this may help explain why women have a stronger inflammatory response to obesity11.
When we compared cognition by gender, the most notable finding was that males had lower performance in tasks of declarative memory compared to our females, which was not surprising considering our males had poorer glycemic control and declarative memory performance has been specifically associated with impairments in glucose metabolism27.
We found that there was a significant negative association between CRP and cognition, but only for females, and the overweight/obese subjects were driving this relationship. Given that this group has the highest CRP levels, this led us to speculate that chronic low grade inflammation may be affecting brain health as manifested by cognitive performance. In our regression, CRP explained (after accounting for age, education, statin use, and hypertension) 16.5% of the variance in a test of executive function in all female subjects and 32% of the variance when only overweight/obese women were examined. In these overweight/obese women, the associations with elevated CRP were concentrated in tasks that reflect frontal lobe function.
CRP is a well-established risk factor for cardiovascular disease and endothelial dysfunction62,63. Data suggests that CRP may promote atherothrombosis64 and attenuate nitric oxide production, thereby inhibiting angiogenesis65. This has important implications given that the frontal lobes have less collateral circulation than the rest of the brain and may therefore would be more susceptible to microvascular disease, as suggested by data in vascular dementia or hypertension, two conditions associated with frontal lobe dysfunction66–68. There is some support for this premise from direct in vivo brain assessments. For example, the Rotterdam Scan Study documented that higher CRP levels were associated with the presence and progression of white matter lesions, which are caused by small-vessel disease69.
It has been suggested that CRP concentrates at sites of cell damage, possibly functioning as a ligand for leukocytes in order to assist in the removal of cell debris70. Acute increases in CRP are an important and beneficial part of the body's response to injury and disease71, but chronic constitutive low grade elevations may be detrimental.
In sum, we find that the consequences of obesity differ for men and women. In males, obesity has a stronger association with glucose dysregulation and declarative memory deficits, while overweight and obese women appear to be more susceptible to inflammation and accompanying cardiovascular compromise. However, a larger percentage of men than women in our sample were taking statin medications, and this may have contributed to our results. Nevertheless, our findings may have important clinical implications for treating overweight and obese women, who may benefit from the anti-inflammatory properties of statins, which, given their higher HDL levels and consequently better cholesterol ratios, may less frequently be prescribed.
One limitation of this study was that the study groups were relatively small. We had a particularly small lean male group (n=14) compared to our overweight and obese male group (n=49). Future studies with larger samples are needed to replicate these findings and determine if the results we find here are indeed specific to females. Further research may be warranted examining whether treatment options including weight loss methods or medications with anti-inflammatory properties reduce the CRP and cognitive deficits observed here among overweight and obese women. It would also be interesting to measure other inflammatory markers (interleukin-6, homocysteine, etc.) to determine whether they have significant associations to cognition and whether those associations may be modified by gender and obesity.
Table 3.
Pearson correlation coefficients.
p < 0.05
Table 5.
Females |
||
---|---|---|
Normal BMI | Ovrwt/Obese | |
n=15 | n=37 | |
WAIS-R Estimated IQ | 0.292 | −0.353** |
Tower of London Excess Moves | −.352 | 0.497** |
WMS-R Figural Memory | 0.196 | −0.436** |
WMS-R Digit Span Backwards | 0.209 | −0.247* |
WMS-R Visual Mem Span Backwards | 0.401 | −0.317* |
Shipley Total Raw Score | 0.240 | −0.350** |
Pearson correlation coefficients.
p < 0.05;
p < 0.10
Acknowledgements
The study was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (DK064087 and P30-AG-08051) and support from the New York University General Clinical Research Center (NCRR M01 RR00096).
References
- 1.Lee W-Y, Park J-S, Noh S-Y, Rhee E-J, Sung K-C, Kim B-S, Kang J-H, Kim S-W, Lee M-H, Park J-R. C-reactive protein concentrations are related to insulin resistance and metabolic syndrome as defined by the ATP III report. International Journal of Cardiology. 2004;97:101–6. doi: 10.1016/j.ijcard.2003.08.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Wang Z, Hoy WE. C-reactive protein and the risk of developing type 2 diabetes in Aboriginal Australians. Diabetes Research And Clinical Practice. 2007;76:37–43. doi: 10.1016/j.diabres.2006.07.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Han T, Gonzalez-Villalpando C, Sattar N, Lean M, Williams K, Haffner SM. Prospective Study of C-Reactive protein in Relation to the Development of Diabetes and Metabolic Syndrome in the Mexico City Diabetes Study. Diabetes Care. 2007;25(11):2016–21. doi: 10.2337/diacare.25.11.2016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Pearson TA, Mensah GA, Alexander RW, Anderson JL, Cannon RO, III, Criqui M, Fadl YY, Fortmann SP, Hong Y, Myers GL, Rifai N, Smith SC, Jr., Taubert K, Tracy RP, Vinicor F. Markers of inflammation and cardiovascular disease: application to clinical and public health practice: A statement for healthcare professionals from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention and the American Heart Association. Circulation. 2003 Jan;107(3):499–511. doi: 10.1161/01.cir.0000052939.59093.45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Kushner I, Rzewnicki D, Samols D. What Does Minor Elevation of C-Reactive Protein Signify? American Journal of Medicine. 2006;119:166.e17–166.e28. doi: 10.1016/j.amjmed.2005.06.057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Haffner SM. The Metabolic Syndrome: Inflammation, Diabetes Mellitus, and Cardiovascular Disease. The American Journal of Cardiology. 2006 Jan;97(2, Supplement 1):3–11. doi: 10.1016/j.amjcard.2005.11.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Parikh NI, Pencina MJ, Wang TJ, Lanier KJ, Fox CS, D'Agostino RB, Vasan RS. Increasing Trends in Incidence of Overweight and Obesity over 5 Decades. The American Journal of Medicine. 2007 Mar;120(3):242–50. doi: 10.1016/j.amjmed.2006.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Ye X, Yu Z, Li H, Franco OH, Liu Y, Lin X. Distributions of C-Reactive Protein and its Association With Metabolic Syndrome in Middle-Aged and Older Chinese People. Journal of the American College of Cardiology. 2007 May;49(17):1798–805. doi: 10.1016/j.jacc.2007.01.065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Florez H, Castillo-Florez S, Mendez A, Casanova-Romero P, Larreal-Urdaneta C, Lee D, Goldberg R. C-reactive protein is elevated in obese patients with the metabolic syndrome. Diabetes Research And Clinical Practice. 2006 Jan;71(1):92–100. doi: 10.1016/j.diabres.2005.05.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Wasir JS, Misra A, Vikram NK, Pandey RM, Luthra K. C-reactive protein, obesity, and insulin resistance in postmenopausal women in urban slums of North India. Diabetes and Metabolic Syndrome: Clinical Research and Reviews. 2007 Jun;1(2):83–9. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Thorand B, Baumert J, Doring A, Herder C, Kolb H, Rathmann W, Giani G, Koenig W. Sex differences in the relation of body composition to markers of inflammation. Atherosclerosis. 2006 Jan;184(1):216–24. doi: 10.1016/j.atherosclerosis.2005.04.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Rexrode KM, Pradhan A, Manson JE, Buring JE, Ridker PM. Relationship of total and abdominal adiposity with CRP and IL-6 in women. Annals of Epidemiology. 2003 Nov;13(10):674–82. doi: 10.1016/s1047-2797(03)00053-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Lear S, Chen M, Birmingham CL, Frohlich JJ. The Relationship Between Simple Anthropometric Indices and C-Reactive Protein: Ethnic and Gender Differences. Metabolism. 2003;52(12):1542–6. doi: 10.1016/j.metabol.2003.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Khera A, McGuire D, Murphy S, Stanek H, Das S, Vongpatanasin W, Wians F, Grundy S, de Lemos J. Race and Gender Differences in C-Reactive Protein Levels. Journal of the American College of Cardiology. 2005;46(3):464–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jacc.2005.04.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Pieroni L, Bastard JP, Piton A, Kalil L, Hainique B, Jardel C. Interpretation of circulating C-reactive protein levels in adults: Body Mass Index and gender are a must. Diabetes Metabolism. 2003;29:133–8. doi: 10.1016/s1262-3636(07)70019-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Kraus VB, Stabler TV, Luta G, Renner JB, Dragomir AD, Jordan JM. Interpretation of serum C-reactive protein (CRP) levels for cardiovascular disease risk is complicated by race, pulmonary disease, body mass index, gender, and osteoarthritis. Osteoarthritis and Cartilage. 2007 Aug;15(8):966–71. doi: 10.1016/j.joca.2007.02.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Lakoski S, Cushman M, Criqui M, Rundek T, Blumenthal R, D'Agostino RB, Herrington D. Gender and C-reactive Protein: Data From the Multiethnic Study of Atherosclerosis (MESA) Cohort. American Heart Journal. 2006;152(3):593–8. doi: 10.1016/j.ahj.2006.02.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Kluft C. HRT effects on inflammatory markers: is chronic inflammation a contra-indication for HRT? International Congress Series. 2002;1229:103–8. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Abdullah SM, Khera A, Leonard D, Das SR, Canham RM, Kamath SA, Vega GL, Grundy SM, McGuire DK, de Lemos JA. Sex differences in the association between leptin and CRP: Results from the Dallas Heart Study. Atherosclerosis. 2007 Dec;195(2):404–10. doi: 10.1016/j.atherosclerosis.2006.10.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Ferrannini E, Natali A, Bell P, Cavallo-Perin P, Lalic N, Mingrone G. Insulin Resistance and Hypersecretion in Obesity. J Clin Invest. 1997 Sep;100(5):1166–73. doi: 10.1172/JCI119628. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Smith DO, LeRoith D. Insulin resistance syndrome, pre-diabetes, and the prevention of type 2 diabetes mellitus. Clinical Cornerstone. 2004;6(2):7–13. doi: 10.1016/s1098-3597(04)80050-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Biessels G-J, ter Braak E, Erkelens D, Hijman R. Cognitive function in patients with type 2 diabetes mellitus. Neurosci Res Communications. 2001;28(1):11–22. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Akisaki T, Sakurai T, Takata T, Umegaki H, Araki A, Mizuno S, Tanaka S, Ohashi Y, Iguchi A, Yokono K, Ito H. Cognitive dysfunction associates with white matter hyperintensities and subcortical atrophy on magnetic resonance imaging of the elderly diabetes mellitus Japanese elderly diabetes intervention trial (J-EDIT) Diabetes Metabolism Research Review. 2006;22(5):376–84. doi: 10.1002/dmrr.632. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Strachan M, Deary I, Ewing F, Frier B. Is type II diabetes associated with an increased risk of cognitive dysfunction? A critical review of published studies. Diabetes Care. 1997;20(3):438–45. doi: 10.2337/diacare.20.3.438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Vanhanen M, Koivisto K, Karjalainen L, Helkala EL, Laakso M, Soininen H, Riekkinen P., Sr. Risk for non-insulin-dependent diabetes in the normoglycaemic elderly is associated with impaired cognitive function. NeuroReport. 1997;8(6):1527–30. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199704140-00041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Convit A. Links between cognitive impairment in insulin resistance: An explanatory model. Neurobiology of Aging. 2005 Dec;26(1, Supplement 1):31–5. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.09.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Gold SM, Dziobek I, Sweat V, Tirsi A, Rogers K, Bruehl H, Tsui W, Richardson S, Javier E, Convit A. Hippocampal damage and memory impairments as possible early brain complications of type 2 diabetes. Diabetologia. 2007;50(4):711–9. doi: 10.1007/s00125-007-0602-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Convit A, Wolf OT, Tarshish C, de Leon MJ. Reduced glucose tolerance is associated with poor memory performance and hippocampal atrophy among normal elderly. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA. 2003;100(4):2019–22. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0336073100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Gunstad J, Paul RH, Cohen RA, Tate DF, Spitznagel MB, Gordon E. Elevated body mass index is associated with executive dysfunction in otherwise healthy adults. Comprehensive Psychiatry. 2007;48(1):57–61. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2006.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.The Asia-Pacific perspective Redefining obesity and its treatment. Health Communications Australia Pty Limited. 2000 http://www.obesityasiapacific.com. Available from. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Jeong SK, Nam HS, Son MH, Son EJ, Cho KH. Interactive effect of obesity indexes on cognition. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2005;19(2–3):91–6. doi: 10.1159/000082659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Elias MF, Elias PK, Sullivan LM, Wolf PA, D'Agostino RB. Obesity, diabetes and cognitive deficit: The Framingham Heart Study. Neurobiology of Aging. 2005 Dec;26(1, Supplement 1):11–6. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.08.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Cournot M, Marquie JC, Ansiau D, Martinaud C, Fonds H, Ferrieres J, Ruidavets JB. Relation between body mass index and cognitive function in healthy middle-aged men and women. Neurol. 2006 Oct;67(7):1208–14. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000238082.13860.50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Expert Panel Executive Summary of The Third Report of The National Cholesterol Education Program (NCEP) Expert Panel on Detection, Evaluation, And Treatment of High Blood Cholesterol In Adults (Adult Treatment Panel III) JAMA. 2001 May;285(19):2486–97. doi: 10.1001/jama.285.19.2486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Komulainen P, Lakka TA, kivipelto M, Hassinen M, Helkala EL, Haapala I, Nissinen A, Rauramaa R. Metabolic syndrome and cognitive function: a population-based follow-up study in elderly women. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2007;23(1):29–34. doi: 10.1159/000096636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Yaffe K, Kanaya A, Lindquist K, Simonsick EM, Harris T, Shorr RI, Tylavsky FA, Newman AB. The Metabolic Syndrome, Inflammation, and Risk of Cognitive Decline. JAMA: the Journal of the American Medical Association. 2004 Nov;292(18):2237–42. doi: 10.1001/jama.292.18.2237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Dik MG, Jonker C, Comijs HC, Deeg DJ, Kok A, Yaffe K, Penninx BW. Contribution of Metabolic Syndrome Components to Cognition in Older Persons. Diabetes Care. 2007 Jun; doi: 10.2337/dc06-1190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Kuo HK, Yen CJ, Chang CH, Kuo CK, Chen JH, Sorond F. Relation of C-reactive protein to stroke, cognitive disorders, and depression in the general population: systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Neurol. 2005 Jun;4(6):371–80. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(05)70099-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Bruehl H, Rueger M, Dziobek I, Sweat V, Tirsi A, Javier E, Arentoft A, Wolf OT, Convit A. Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal Axis Dysregulation and Memory Impairments in Type 2 Diabetes. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2007 Jul;92(7):2439–45. doi: 10.1210/jc.2006-2540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Executive Summary of the Clinical Guidelines on the Identification, Evaluation, and Treatment of Overweight and Obesity in Adults. J Am Diet Assoc. 1998 Oct;98(10):1178–91. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Lezak MD. Neuropsychological Assessment. 3 ed. Oxford University Press; New York: 1995. [Google Scholar]
- 42.Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1960;23:56–62. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.23.1.56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Ansell B, Watson K, Weiss R, Fonarow G. hsCRP and HDL Effects of Statins Trial (CHEST): Rapid Effect of Statin Therapy on C-Reactive Protein and High-Density Lipoprotein Levels. Heart Disease. 2003;5:2–7. doi: 10.1097/01.HDX.0000050407.62572.DE. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Hanefeld M, Marx N, Pfutzner A, Baurecht W, Lubben G, Karagiannis E, Stier U, Forst T. Anti-Inflammatory Effects of Pioglitazone and/or Simvastatin in High Cardiovascular Risk Patients With Elevated High Sensitivity C-Reactive Protein: The PIOSTAT Study. Journal of the American College of Cardiology. 2007 Jan;49(3):290–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jacc.2006.08.054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Kinlay S. Low-Density Lipoprotein-Dependent and -Independent Effects of Cholesterol-Lowering therapies on C-reactive Protein. Journal of the American College of Cardiology. 2007;49(20):2003–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jacc.2007.01.083. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Kritchevsky SB, Cesari M, Pahor M. Inflammatory markers and cardiovascular health in older adults. Cardiovascular Research. 2005 May;66(2):265–75. doi: 10.1016/j.cardiores.2004.12.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Neisser U, Boodoo G, Bouchard TJ, Boykin AW, Brody N, Ceci SJ, Halpern DF, Loehlin JC, Perloff R, Sternberg RJ, Urbina S. Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns. American Psychologist. 1996 Feb;51(2):77–101. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Barzilai N, Atzmon G, Derby CA, Bauman JM, Lipton RB. A genotype of exceptional longevity is associated with preservation of cognitive function. Neurol. 2006;67:2170–5. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000249116.50854.65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Ramlawi B, Rudolph JL, Mieno S, Feng J, Boodhwani M, Khabbaz K, Levkoff SE, Marcantonio ER, Bianchi C, Sellke FW. C-Reactive protein and inflammatory response associated to neurocognitive decline following cardiac surgery. Surgery. 2006 Aug;140(2):221–6. doi: 10.1016/j.surg.2006.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Mathew JP, Podgoreanu MV, Grocott HP, White WD, Morris RW, Stafford-Smith M, Mackensen GB, Rinder CS, Blumenthal JA, Schwinn DA, Newman MF. Genetic Variants in P-Selectin and C-Reactive Protein Influence Susceptibility to Cognitive Decline After Cardiac Surgery. Journal of the American College of Cardiology. 2007 May;49(19):1934–42. doi: 10.1016/j.jacc.2007.01.080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.de Luca C, Olefsky JM. Stressed out about obesity and insulin resistance. Nat Med. 2006 Jan;12(1):41–2. doi: 10.1038/nm0106-41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Johnson JL, Slentz CA, Duscha BD, Samsa GP, McCartney JS, Houmard JA, Kraus WE. Gender and racial differences in lipoprotein subclass distributions: the STRRIDE study. Atherosclerosis. 2004 Oct;176(2):371–7. doi: 10.1016/j.atherosclerosis.2004.05.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Abdullah SM, Khera A, Leonard D, Das SR, Canham RM, Kamath SA, Vega GL, Grundy SM, McGuire DK, de Lemos JA. Sex differences in the association between leptin and CRP: Results from the Dallas Heart Study. AtherosclerosisIn Press; Corrected Proof. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Molina MJ, Mayor AM, Franco AE, Morell CA, Lopez MA, Vila LM. Prevalence of systemic lupus erythematosus and associated comorbidities in Puerto Rico. J Clin Rheumatol. 2007 Aug;13(4):202–4. doi: 10.1097/RHU.0b013e318124a8af. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Anonymous Racial/ethnic differences in the prevalence and impact of doctor-diagnosed arthritis--United States, 2002. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. 2005 Feb;54(5):119–23. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Linares LF, Gomez-Reino JJ, Carreira PE, Morillas L, Ibero I. C-reactive protein (CRP) levels in systemic lupus erythematosus (SLE) Clin Rheumatol. 1986 Jan;5(1):66–9. doi: 10.1007/BF02030970. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Popkova TV, Khelkovskaia AN, Mach ES, Aleksandrova EN, Novikov AA, Novikova DS, Nasonov EL. Cardiovascular diseases in rheumatoid arthritis. Ter Arkh. 2007;79(5):9–14. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Urowitz MB, Gladman DD. Atherosclerosis and Lupus The SLICC study. Lupus. 2007;16(12):925–8. doi: 10.1177/0961203307085259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Assous N, Touze E, Meune C, Kahan A, Allanore Y. Cardiovascular disease in rheumatoid arthritis: single-center hospital-based cohort study in France. Joint Bone Spine. 2007 Jan;74(1):66–72. doi: 10.1016/j.jbspin.2006.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Shucard JL, Gaines JJ, Ambrus J, Jr., Shucard DW. C-reactive protein and cognitive deficits in systemic lupus erythematosus. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2007 Mar;20(1):31–7. doi: 10.1097/WNN.0b013e31802e3b9a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Nandula SR, Amarnath S, Molinolo A, Bandyopadhyay BC, Hall B, Goldsmith CM, Zheng C, Larsson J, Sreenath T, Chen W, Ambudkar IS, Karlsson S, Baum BJ, Kulkarni AB. Female mice are more susceptible to developing inflammatory disorders due to impaired transforming growth factor beta signaling in salivary glands. Arthritis Rheum. 2007 Jun;56(6):1798–805. doi: 10.1002/art.22715. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Pradhan AD, Manson JE, Rossouw JE, Siscovick DS, Mouton CP, Rifai N, Wallace RB, Jackson RD, Pettinger MB, Ridker PM. Inflammatory biomarkers, hormone replacement therapy, and incident coronary heart disease: prospective analysis from the Women's Health Initiative observational study. JAMA. 2002 Aug;288(8):980–7. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.8.980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Singh U, Devaraj S, Vasquez-Vivar J, Jialal I. C-reactive protein decreases endothelial nitric oxide synthase activity via uncoupling. J Mol Cell Cardiol. 2007 Dec;43(6):780–91. doi: 10.1016/j.yjmcc.2007.08.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Jialal I, Devaraj S, Venugopal SK. C-reactive protein: risk marker or mediator in atherothrombosis? Hypertension. 2004 Jul;44(1):6–11. doi: 10.1161/01.HYP.0000130484.20501.df. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Verma S, Wang CH, Li SH, Dumont AS, Fedak PW, Badiwala MV, Dhillon B, Weisel RD, Li RK, Mickle DA, Stewart DJ. A self-fulfilling prophecy: C-reactive protein attenuates nitric oxide production and inhibits angiogenesis. Circulation. 2002 Aug;106(8):913–9. doi: 10.1161/01.cir.0000029802.88087.5e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Schmidt R, Enzinger C, Ropele S, Schmidt H, Fazekas F. Subcortical vascular cognitive impairment: Similarities and differences with multiple sclerosis. Journal of the Neurological Sciences. 2006 Jun;245(1–2):3–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2005.06.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Gold SM, Dziobek I, Rogers K, Bayoumy A, McHugh PF, Convit A. Hypertension and hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis hyperactivity affect frontal lobe integrity. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism. 2005 Jun;90(6):3262–7. doi: 10.1210/jc.2004-2181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Raz N, Rodrigue KM, Acker JD. Hypertension and the brain: vulnerability of the prefrontal regions and executive functions. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2003 Dec;117(6):1169–80. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.117.6.1169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.van Dijk EJ, Prins ND, Vermeer SE, Vrooman HA, Hofman A, Koudstaal PJ, Breteler MMB. C-Reactive Protein and Cerebral Small-Vessel Disease: The Rotterdam Scan Study. Circulation. 2005 Aug;112(6):900–5. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.104.506337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Swanson SJ, McPeek MM, Mortensen RF. Characteristics of the binding of human C-reactive protein (CRP) to laminin. J Cell Biochem. 1989 May;40(1):121–32. doi: 10.1002/jcb.240400112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Pepys MB, Baltz ML. Acute phase proteins with special reference to C-reactive protein and related proteins (pentaxins) and serum amyloid A protein. Adv Immunol. 1983;34:141–212. doi: 10.1016/s0065-2776(08)60379-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]