Skip to main content
Indian Journal of Psychological Medicine logoLink to Indian Journal of Psychological Medicine
. 2010 Jul-Dec;32(2):131–135. doi: 10.4103/0253-7176.78511

Effect of Acute Moderate Exercise on Cognitive Event-Related Potentials N100, P200, N200, and Interpeak Latencies

Naresh Kumar 1,, Sushma Sood 1, Manjeet Singh 1, Beena 1, Sakshi 1
PMCID: PMC3122546  PMID: 21716775

Abstract

Background:

Effect of acute moderate exercise on cognition has been studied recently, although there are controversial reports in this context. This is the reason we performed this study, to observe the effect of acute moderate exercise on the cognitive functions of persons having a sedentary lifestyle, by measuring the latencies of event-related potentials N100, P200, N200, and the inter-peak latencies N100-P200, P200-N200, and N200-P300.

Materials and Methods:

Sixty right-handed participants (34 males, 26 females) of age group 15 – 30 years, having a sedentary lifestyle, were recruited for the study. A baseline recording of event-related potentials was conducted and then they were subjected to acute moderate exercise (60 – 80% of the maximum load of effort during exercise, where 100%=200 - Age), again the recording was carried out using the paired student's ′t′ test, to compare the present values with the initial values.

Results:

After performing the exercise there was a significant decrease in the latency of wave N1 and P2 of females for both frequent and rare stimuli. A significant decrease in the latency was seen in wave N2 with rare stimulus in both females and males. While, for frequent stimulus a significant decrease was seen only in females. Also the N2-P3 interpeak latency significantly decreased in males, while there was no significant decrease in females.

Conclusion:

There was a definite role of exercise in enhancing the cognitive functions as evidenced by its effect on the latencies of event-related potentials N100, P200, N200, and interpeak latencies.

Keywords: Event-related potential, exercise, interpeak latencies

INTRODUCTION

The effect of the physical activity on the brain and cognition has grown in interest in recent years, with an increasing number of reports indicating that chronic participation and single, acute bouts of exercise, benefit a host of cognitive processes.[1] Coe and colleagues administered a three-day physical activity recall questionnaire to sixth grade children and observed their academic performance in four core classes (i.e., mathematics, science, English, world studies) and Terra Nova standardized test scores. They found an increased performance in the core academic classes for those children who reported vigorous physical activity outside of the school relative, to those who reported no physical activity.[2] Several other studies have appeared in literature as well, indicating positive relations between physical activity and the aspects of academic performance.[35] Although, several studies have also observed no relation between physical activity and academic performance.[6]

Cognitive control is a term used to describe a subset of goal-directed, self-regulatory operations involved in the selection, scheduling, and coordination of computational processes underlying perception, memory, and action. Core cognitive processes collectively termed ′cognitive control′ or ′executive control′ include inhibition, working memory, and cognitive flexibility.[7] Of these core processes, inhibition has been the most studied in the acute exercise literature and relates to the ability to act on the basis of choice rather than impulse.[810]

Event-related potentials (ERPs) reflect invariant changes in the ongoing electroencephalographic (EEG) activity evoked by the stimulus. The ERP components are usually identified by their peak amplitudes and peak latencies. The most prominent ERP components observed in the studies of selective attention, using the auditory oddball paradigm are, N100, P200, N200, and P300, with peak latencies at about 100, 150, 200, and 300 ms after the stimulus onset, respectively. The long-latency potentials of the auditory evoked potentials (AEP) comprise of the N1 (N100) and P2 (P200) peaks, the so-called vertex potentials. The vertex potentials do not represent the first volley of sensory information into the primary auditory cortex, which occurs on the order of tens of milliseconds rather than hundreds. In addition, these vertex potentials represent several components with different brain generators, some of which are not purely sensory, but are modulated by subjective mental operations. For example, attention to the tones can lead to an increase in the amplitudes of N1 and P2, and hence, there is some endogenous aspect to the vertex potentials. The N1-P2 deflection has also been considered as an indicator for the cortical arousal response.[11] Thus, the N1 and P2 are probably the best considered event-related potentials (ERPs) encompassing both exogenous and endogenous potentials.[12] The N200 (N2) component is usually assumed to reflect the classification or categorization of deviant stimuli.[11] This reflects processes of transient arousal triggered by unattended discrimination processes, which in turn trigger a target reaction.[13] Typically evoked between 180 to 325 ms following the presentation of a specific visual or auditory stimulus, the N2 is a negativity resulting from a deviation in the form or context of a prevailing stimulus. N2 is typically evoked before the motor response, suggesting its link to the cognitive processes of stimulus identification and distinction.[14] Sumi et al. showed that by using the evaluation of the P300 interpeak latency (IPL), we can assess the disturbed steps in the cognitive process in schizophrenic patients.[15] Very few studies have been found in literature to access the role of acute exercise on cognition using event-related potentials (N1, P2, and N2) and inter-peak latencies of ERP, that is the reason for our having designed this study, to see if there is any effect of an acute bout of exercise on the ERP of people having sedentary lifestyles.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Sixty right-handed participants (34 males, 26 females) of age group 15–30 years, having a sedentary lifestyle, were recruited from undergraduate and postgraduate students and staff members of a medical college. All the participants reported no adverse health conditions, such as, previous history of stroke, diabetes, depression, hypertension, osteoarthritis, chronic obstructive lung disease, visual or auditory impairment, or smoking habits. Basic anthropometric measurements such as age, sex, height, and weight were recorded and the body mass index (BMI) was calculated [Table 1].

Table 1.

The demographic pattern of the study group

graphic file with name IJPsyM-32-131-g001.jpg

Before taking part in the experiment, all the participants signed written consent forms and were fully informed about the protocol. The participants visited the laboratory at the same time of the day, between 9 to 11 a.m. The basal recording of the event-related potential (ERP) was recorded using Recorders and Medicare Systems-Electromyography (RMS-EMG), EP Mark-II (Chandigarh) in a sound proof room, and then the subjects were asked to do moderate exercise for five minutes on an Electronic bicycle ergometer (MAGCYCLE). The severity of the exercise was moderate (60–80% of maximum load of effort during exercise, where 100%=200-Age). Immediately after doing the exercise, the ERP was recorded again.

ERP recordings

An Ag-AgCl electrode was placed at Cz actively, according to the international 10–20 system,[16] referenced to earlobe electrodes, and with a forehead electrode as a ground electrode. Impedances were maintained below 5 kΩ and were measured from each lead at the beginning and end of each session. The P3 potentials were recorded with a band-pass of 0.1–100 Hz. Recordings were made for 300 seconds, excluding the rejection errors. P300 potentials were obtained from an auditory oddball paradigm. The target tones were presented with a probability of 20%, whereas, the non-target tones were presented with a probability of 80% binaurally over headphones, at a sound level intensity of 80 dB. The tone bursts were presented with an inter-stimulus interval of one second, randomly. At the end of each session, each participant's count was compared with the actual number of target tones given, to assess the accuracy of the task performance. All participants performed the tasks with an error rate <5% in all trials.

The principal peaks and their identification were made according to the standard recommendations for long latency auditory event-related potentials of The International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology using the principal component analysis technique.[16] Waveforms from each electrode and stimulus condition were analyzed with the P300 component defined as the largest positive going peak occurring for all electrode sites after the N100-P200-N200 complex, within the latency window, between 250 and 500 ms. Interpeak latencies were measured between the peaks of the consecutive waves.

Data thus collected was subjected to statistical analysis using the paired student's ′t′ test, comparing the before and after exercise values.

RESULTS

Table 2 shows that on doing an acute bout of moderate exercise there was a significant (P<0.01) decrease in the latency of wave N1 of all females, for both frequent and rare stimuli, while there was no significant change in males. Also there was a significant decrease in the latency of wave P2 in females, for both frequent (P<0.001) and rare (P<0.05) stimuli. However, there was no significant change seen in males. A significant decrease in latency was seen in wave N2, with rare stimulus in both females (P<0.01) and males (P<0.001), while for frequent stimulus, a significant (P<0.05) decrease was seen only in females.

Table 2.

Mean±S.D. of latencies (ms) of different ERP waves before and after doing exercise

graphic file with name IJPsyM-32-131-g002.jpg

Table 3 shows that in total there was a significant decrease in N2-P3 interpeak latency for both target (P<0.001) and non-target (P<0.05) stimuli. N2-P3 interpeak latency significantly decreased in males, while there was no significant decrease in females. Also there was a significant (P<0.05) decrease in P2-N2 interpeak latency for the target stimuli in total, although there was no significant decrease when males and females were compared separately.

Table 3.

Mean±S.D. of interpeak latencies (ms) between different ERP waves before and after doing exercise

graphic file with name IJPsyM-32-131-g003.jpg

DISCUSSION

There is evidence that aerobic exercise produces its effect on event-related potentials by increasing arousal. Exercise helps contribute to decreased peak latency.[17] Yagi et al. examined the auditory and visual P3 and reaction times in young volunteers before and after exercise on a cycle ergometer.[18] Bulut et al. reported that acute and regular exercise shortens the latency of sensory evoked potentials.[19] Two basic mechanisms have been suggested to explain the effect of aerobic fitness on cognitive processes: the cerebral circulation hypothesis and the neurotrophic-stimulation hypothesis, which predicts a beneficial effect of neuromuscular activity on higher brain centers; both mechanisms may contribute simultaneously.[20]

Event-related potentials offer the requisite temporal precision to gain insight into covert cognitive operations that occur between stimulus engagement and response selection, which may be more sensitive to these processes than overt behavioral measures of task performance. ERPs refer to patterns of neuroelectric activation that occur in response to, or in preparation for, a stimulus or response. Previous studies have examined the P3 component of the stimulus-locked ERP, to understand the alterations in stimulus engagement.[21]

In our study, an acute bout of moderate exercise led to a significant decrease in the latency of wave N1 and P2 of females for both frequent and rare stimuli. Significant decrease in latency was seen in wave N2 with rare stimulus in both females and males, while for frequent stimulus, a significant decrease was seen only in females. In a study by Ozkaya et al., the latencies of the P2 and N2 components at the Fz and Cz sites, decreased significantly in the strength training (ST) group compared to the endurance training (ET) group, and after training, the latencies of the N1, N2, and P2 components shortened significantly in the ST group compared to the control group. Also the latencies of the N2 and P2 components shortened significantly in the ET group compared to the control group.[22] In contrast Pontifex et al. observed longer N2 and P3 latencies during exercise (cycling at 60% of maximal HR) relative to rest.[23] Also Stroth et al., in their study, suggested that physical fitness, but not an acute bout of aerobic exercise, enhanced cognitive processing by increasing attention allocation to stimulus encoding, during task preparation.[24]

In our study, we found that the N2-P3 interpeak latency significantly decreased in males, while there was no significant decrease in females, suggesting that males had a specific effect on information processing between the generation of the P2 and P3 waves. No study was found in the literature regarding exercise and interpeak latencies of event-related potentials.

CONCLUSION

We conclude that there is a definite role of exercise in enhancing the cognitive functions as evidenced by its effect on event-related potentials. Sedentary females are benefited more than males from a short duration of moderate exercise. Males feel its effect on the later parts of information processing, as suggested by the interpeak latencies. Extensive research is needed in this field to assess the role of exercise at a molecular level.

Footnotes

Source of Support: Nil

Conflict of Interest: None.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Hillman CH, Erickson KI, Kramer AF. Be smart, exercise your heart: Exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:58–65. doi: 10.1038/nrn2298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Coe DP, Pivarnik JM, Womack CJ, Reeves MJ, Malina RM. Effect of physical education and activity levels on academic achievement in children. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2006;38:1515–9. doi: 10.1249/01.mss.0000227537.13175.1b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Fields T, Diego M, Sanders CE. Exercise is positively related to adolescents′ relationships and academics. Adolescence. 2001;36:105–10. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Kim EY, Iwaki N, Imashioya H, Uno H, Fujita T. Error-related negativity in a visual go/no-go task: Children vs.adults. Dev Neuropsychol. 2007;31:181–91. doi: 10.1080/87565640701190775. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Lindner KJ. The physical activity participation-academic performance relationship revisited: Perceived and actual performance and the effect of banding (academic tracking) Pediatr Exerc Sci. 2002;14:155–69. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Tomporowski PD, Davis CL, Miller PH, Naglieri JA. Exercise and children's intelligence, cognition, and academic achievement. Educ Psychol Rev. 2008;20:111–31. doi: 10.1007/s10648-007-9057-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Diamond A. The early development of executive functions. In: Bialystok E, Craik FI, editors. Lifespan cognition: Mechanisms of change. New York: Oxford University Press; 2006. pp. 70–95. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Hillman CH, Snook EM, Jerome GJ. Acute cardiovascular exercise and executive control function. Int J Psychophysiol. 2003;48:307–14. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(03)00080-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Kamijo K, Nishihira Y, Hatta A, Kaneda T, Wasaka T, Kida T, et al. Differential influences of exercise intensity on information processing in the central nervous system. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2004;92:305–11. doi: 10.1007/s00421-004-1097-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Davidson MC, Amso D, Anderson LC, Diamond A. Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44:2037–78. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.02.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Mueller V, Brehmer Y, Oertzen TV, Li SC, Lindenberger U. Electrophysiological correlates of selective attention: A lifespan comparison. BMC Neurosci. 2008;9:18. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-9-18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Salisbury DF, Collins KC, McCarley RW. Reductions in the N1 and P2 auditory event-related potentials in first-hospitalized and chronic schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2010;36:991–1000. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Becker F, Reinvang I. Successful syllable detection in aphasia despite processing impairments as revealed by event-related potentials. Behav Brain Funct. 2007;3:6. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-3-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Patel SH, Azzam PN. Characterization of N200 and P300: Selected studies of the event-related potential. Int J Med Sci. 2005;2:147–54. doi: 10.7150/ijms.2.147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Sumi N, Nan-no H, Fujimoto O. Inter-peak latency of auditory event related potentials (P300) in senile depression and dementia of the Alzheimer type. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2000;54:679–84. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1819.2000.00769.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Heinze HJ, Münte TF, Kutas M, Butler SR, Naatanen R, Nuwer MR, et al. Cognitive event related potentials.The International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol Suppl. 1999;52:91–7. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Polich J, Lardon MT. P300 and long-term physical exercise. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1997;103:493–8. doi: 10.1016/s0013-4694(97)96033-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Yagi Y, Coburn KL, Estes KM, Arruda JE. Effects of aerobic exercise and gender on visual and auditory P300, reaction time, and accuracy. Eur J Appl Physiol Occup Physiol. 1999;80:402–8. doi: 10.1007/s004210050611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Bulut S, Ozmerdivenli R, Bayer H. Effects of exercise on somatosensory-evoked potentials. Int J Neurosci. 2003;113:315–22. doi: 10.1080/00207450390162119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Dustman RE, Shearer DE, Emmerson RY. EEG and event-related potentials in normal aging. Prog Neurobiol. 1993;41:369–401. doi: 10.1016/0301-0082(93)90005-d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Hillman CH, Pontifex MB, Raine LB, Castelli DM, Hall EE, Kramer AF. The effect of acute treadmill walking on cognitive control and academic achievement in preadolescent children. Neuroscience. 2009;159:1044–54. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.01.057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Ozkaya GY, Aydýn H, Toraman FN, Kýzýlay F, Ozdemir O, Cetinkaya V. Effect of strength and endurance training on cognition in older people. J Sports Sci Med. 2005;4:300–3. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Pontifex MB, Hillman CH. Neuroelectric and behavioral indices of interference control during acute cycling. Clin Neurophysiol. 2007;118:570–80. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2006.09.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Stroth S, Kubesch S, Dieterle K, Ruchsow M, Heim R, Kiefer M. Physical fitness, but not acute exercise modulates event-related potential indices for executive control in healthy adolescents. Brain Res. 2009;1269:114–24. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.02.073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Indian Journal of Psychological Medicine are provided here courtesy of Indian Psychiatric Society South Zonal Branch

RESOURCES