Abstract
Background
Mutations that cause learning and memory defects in Drosophila melanogaster have been found to also compromise visual responsiveness and attention. A better understanding of attention-like defects in such Drosophila mutants therefore requires a more detailed characterization of visual responsiveness across a range of visual parameters.
Methodology/Principal Findings
We designed an automated behavioral paradigm for efficiently dissecting visual responsiveness in Drosophila. Populations of flies walk through multiplexed serial choice mazes while being exposed to moving visuals displayed on computer monitors, and infra-red fly counters at the end of each maze automatically score the responsiveness of a strain. To test our new design, we performed a detailed comparison between wild-type flies and a learning and memory mutant, dunce 1. We first confirmed that the learning mutant dunce 1 displays increased responsiveness to a black/green moving grating compared to wild type in this new design. We then extended this result to explore responses to a wide range of psychophysical parameters for moving gratings (e.g., luminosity, contrast, spatial frequency, velocity) as well as to a different stimulus, moving dots. Finally, we combined these visuals (gratings versus dots) in competition to investigate how dunce 1 and wild-type flies respond to more complex and conflicting motion effects.
Conclusions/Significance
We found that dunce 1 responds more strongly than wild type to high contrast and highly structured motion. This effect was found for simple gratings, dots, and combinations of both stimuli presented in competition.
Introduction
Animals respond reflexively to motion that they see in their environment. This reflex has been termed an optomotor or optokinetic response, depending on whether movement of the whole animal or just the eye is measured, respectively [1], [2], [3]. Such responses have been extensively studied in insects [4], [5], [6], and recent work in the fly Drosophila melanogaster has identified key peripheral circuits in the fly visual system believed to be involved in mediating these responses [7], [8], [9], [10]. However, it is likely that visual responses can be modulated or even suppressed by processing occurring in the central brain since, like many animals, flies must be able to ignore certain motion cues while moving through the environment. Investigations of Drosophila learning and memory mutants have uncovered a wide range of effects on visual responses in flies [11], [12], [13], [14], [15], [16]. In particular, mutants affecting cyclic AMP signaling, such as the phosphodiesterase mutant dunce 1, or the adenylyl cyclase mutant rutabaga2080, were found to display increased visual responsiveness compared to wild type in a choice maze paradigm [11], and this behavior was associated with an attention-like defects in the mutants [13]. A subsequent screen of long-term memory mutants uncovered other strains with increased visual responsiveness in the same paradigm, and these were also associated with attention-like defects at the level of both behavior and electrophysiology [15].
A systematic analysis of visual psychophysics in a learning and memory mutant such as dunce 1 has never been done. In part, this is because there have been few paradigms available to efficiently test a variety of visual scenarios in fly populations, and also because mutants such as dunce 1, which do not fly readily, are difficult to investigate thoroughly in the best visual paradigm to date, the tethered flight arena [5]. We have therefore applied our automated visual maze design to better characterize vision in dunce 1 flies compared to wild type. We questioned whether increased visual responsiveness of the dunce mutant in our paradigm was due to improved visual processing in general or increased responsiveness to a narrow range of physical parameters. We addressed this problem by testing dunce 1 and wild-type flies to a wide range of moving visual stimuli, including different gratings, moving dots, or more complex visual stimuli. Our comparative psychophysical study of dunce 1 against wild type shows that dunce 1 flies respond strongly to highly structured motion stimuli, whether these are gratings or dots. The tight association between a learning mutant and a stronger visual response across different physical parameters suggests that our automated paradigm will be useful for efficiently screening other genes involved in plasticity mechanisms.
Results
Visual Responses
We used an automated paradigm to measure visual responsiveness in walking Drosophila (Figure 1A,B, and see Materials and Methods). In this set-up, dunce 1 flies display increased visual responsiveness to moving gratings, compared to wild type [13], [15]; this result was replicated with a good level of reliability in our high-throughput design (Figure 1 C,D; N = 50 mazes for dunce 1 and 488 mazes for wild type, with about 25 flies per maze). Our design allowed us to then efficiently test other visual stimuli, probing for example whether dunce 1 has improved visual acuity compared to wild type. We tested this possibility by manipulating the visual stimulus across a range of physical parameters and motion complexity.
We first investigated whether increased visual responsiveness in dunce 1 generalizes across different stimulus parameters, or whether it is specific to the one grating stimulus tested above (see Materials and Methods). We found that, with the exception of changes in grating contrast, dunce 1 mutants were generally not significantly different from wild type when we changed grating luminosity, spatial frequency, and temporal frequency (Figure 2A–D). Indeed, dunce 1 visual responsiveness decreased to zero at the same physical settings that abolished responsiveness in wild type, such as low luminance, low contrast, and high spatial and temporal frequencies (see Table 1 for correlation statistics between the strains). This suggests that the increased responsiveness in the mutant for our standard stimulus (e.g., Figure 1C, and see Methods) is not due to improved visual acuity, but is instead an increased response under “optimal” visual condition, such as high contrast and luminosity. Furthermore, we observed in both strains a predictable loss of visual responsiveness for gratings with equiluminant green and blue alternating bars (Figure 2E), resulting in a high correlation between the two strains (0.92; p<0.05; Table 1). Loss of optomotor responsiveness at color equiluminance has been reported previously as evidence for segregation of color and motion processing in flies in a tethered flight paradigm [17]. Finally, the increased responsiveness of dunce 1 was maintained for higher monitor refresh rates (200 vs 60 Hz, Figure 2F). Together, these data suggest that dunce 1 visual responses are unlikely to stem from peripheral visual processing stages likely to affect detecting luminance, low contrast, spatial frequency, flicker, or velocity computations. In addition, our results validate the usefulness of the maze paradigm to investigate fly vision. Although the maze paradigm is a population assay comprising various behaviors other than classical optomotor responses, it produces visual responses in accord with expectations for the optomotor conditions explored by researchers in the past walking or flight paradigms. For example, flies in the maze display syndirectional responses to moving gratings (Figure 1C) [5], [18], responses are maximal under high luminosity and high contrast (Figures 2A & 2B) [5], [19], flies lose responsiveness at high spatial frequencies (Figure 2C) and flies display a velocity response curve (Figure 2D) [4], [5], and flies lose responsiveness under color equiluminance (Figure 2E) [17]. These results encouraged us to proceed to explore responses to other, more complex visual stimuli in the maze paradigm.
Table 1. Correlation statistics between dunce 1 and visual parameter (Stimulus, in referenced Figure panel number), wild type and visual parameter, and dunce 1 versus wild type.
dunce 1: correlation to visual stimuli | Wild type: correlation to visual stimuli | Correlation of wild-type and dunce 1 visual responses | ||||
Stimulus | r value | p-value | r value | p-value | r value | p-value |
Figure 2 A | 0.466 | 0.000 | 0.065 | 0.221 | 0.591 | 0.094 |
Figure 2 B | 0.138 | 0.046 | −0.038 | 0.356 | 0.441 | 0.203 |
Figure 2 C | −0.512 | 0.000 | −0.296 | 0.000 | 0.851 | 0.001 |
Figure 2 D | −0.122 | 0.234 | 0.016 | 0.795 | 0.790 | 0.020 |
Figure 2 E | −0.328 | 0.001 | −0.078 | 0.133 | 0.920 | 0.009 |
Figure 4 A | 0.586 | 0.001 | 0.122 | 0.124 | 0.508 | 0.661 |
Figure 4 B | −0.127 | 0.315 | −0.133 | 0.086 | 0.694 | 0.038 |
Figure 4 C | 0.460 | 0.000 | 0.184 | 0.034 | 0.274 | 0.553 |
Figure 4 D | 0.157 | 0.130 | 0.369 | 0.000 | 0.310 | 0.303 |
Figure 4 E | −0.372 | 0.022 | −0.300 | 0.012 | 0.686 | 0.201 |
Figure 4 F | 0.202 | 0.067 | 0.305 | 0.001 | 0.498 | 0.143 |
Figure 6 A | −0.042 | 0.636 | 0.118 | 0.119 | −0.210 | 0.560 |
Figure 6 B | −0.632 | 0.000 | −0.280 | 0.000 | 0.793 | 0.001 |
Significant correlations (P<0.05) are indicated in bold type.
Visual responses explored thus far have been to moving gratings, a stimulus with straight edges and a high level of regularity (as exemplified by the single peaks in a spectral analysis of the image, Figure 3A) that evokes strong visual reflexes in flies and other insects [4], [5], [20]. One possible explanation for dunce 1 behavior in the visual maze is that the mutant is more responsive to moving straight edges. We therefore tested responses to a natural scene (Figure 3B) and to random dots (Figure 3C), two stimuli that include a variety of moving edges with a range of spatial frequencies (see Materials and Methods). Random dot kinematograms (RDK) have been used extensively in visual studies in humans and monkeys for investigating how local motion is integrated into global motion [21], [22], [23], and for studies of visual attention in humans [24]. RDKs also reveal responses to irregular wide-field motion more typical of natural scenes, and thereby provide a more flexible stimulus in terms of the number and types of parameters that can be manipulated compared to gratings.
The separation between dunce 1 and wild type was lost when we tested responsiveness to a natural scene (Australian bushes [25]), but was maintained with a green random dot stimulus (Figure 3D, and see Materials and Methods for image parameters). Given the high energy of the natural scene at low spatial frequencies (see spectrogram inset in Figure 3B), this result may be surprising. However, we showed earlier that dunce 1 responsiveness was reduced to wild-type levels for most luminance levels (Figure 2A) and spatial frequencies (Figure 2C) beyond our standard grating (see Methods), suggesting that dunce 1 increased responsiveness to the grating operates within a narrow range of physical parameters not captured by the natural scene we tested. It was therefore surprising that the random dot stimulus, which more resembles the natural scene spectrally (Figure 3C), resurrected the dunce 1 phenotype. We therefore focused on RDKs to better explore the physical parameters of this alternate stimulus that might be evoking a stronger response in dunce 1.
As for gratings, dunce 1 responsiveness to moving dots was stronger than wild type only for a narrow range of physical parameters, and responses in both strains were mostly well correlated (Figure 4, and Table 1). Both strains for example required a similar level of coherent motion to evoke a response (about 80% motion coherence), both were similarly affected by changes in dot velocity (losing responsiveness at the same high and low velocities), both lost responsiveness when dots were equiluminant to the background, and dunce 1 was also not more sensitive to smaller dots (Figure 4A–D, and see Table 1). Where we did find a significant difference and lack of correlation between the strains was in response to dot densities: dunce 1 responded strongly to intermediate densities, where wild type showed no significant response (Figure 4E). This effect was true for blue dots as well (Figure 4F). Interestingly, wild-type flies displayed a tendency toward a negative response for sparse blue dot densities, which is consistent with another study examining fly responses to sparse moving dots [8]. Positive responses to fewer moving dots in dunce 1 suggests a decreased arousal threshold – or increased sensitivity – to wide-field motion in the mutant.
Visual competition
To better understand how flies might be integrating motion cues, we combined our dot and grating stimuli (Figure 5A), thereby asking: how do wild-type and mutant flies respond behaviorally to the stimuli presented in competition? A human observer can easily attend to one or the other stimulus separately (see http://web.qbi.uq.edu.au/vanswinderen/Movie3.mpeg), but what about flies? One possibility is that flies might respond - like humans paying attention - alternately to two competing percepts (e.g., wide-field gratings versus dots); another possibility is that the combined visuals present a degraded motion percept to the flies. Both possibilities could produce a zero response score on our paradigm, and to separate these two possibilities behaviorally is difficult. However, electrophysiological recordings in insects have identified neurons that respond specifically to small moving targets, such as dots [26], [27], and other neurons that respond specifically to wide-field motion [28], [29], so in principle it is conceivable that either system might be modulated separately to affect behavioral choices.
We first tested whether superimposed moving dots could alter the response to the moving grating. A set number of blue dots (∼500) were moved coherently over the standard grating in eight different relative directions (Figure 5B). We found that superimposed dots evoked different responses in wild type and dunce 1 depending on their motion direction relative to the grating (Figure 5C). Notably, dunce 1 responsiveness was increased by one superimposed orientation (45°, VR = 1.52±0.172) and correspondingly decreased by the opposite orientation (225°, VR = 0.52±0.134, Figure 5D). This suggests that the combined stimuli may be acting additively for dunce 1, while effects on wild type are not additive or not as salient.
To further probe additive effects of the combined stimuli, we changed dot shape or luminosity over the grating stimulus. When square dots were presented instead of round (at 180°, or against the grating direction), both strains lost responsiveness to the grating, and this effect was repeated for different dot densities (Figure 5E). It is impossible for this result to be explained by the dots simply subtracting grating surface area since the number of pixels per square or circle was the same; rather, square dots provide a more salient competing stimulus than round dots. We also changed the luminosity of superimposed green dots on the green grating, and found that differences between wild type and dunce 1 were lost when the competing stimuli were equiluminant (Figure 5F). Together, these data show that dunce 1 is especially sensitive physical aspects of the superimposed dot stimulus, even though the grating surface covered by the dots may remain unchanged. This does not appear to be the case for wild type, which is less responsive to the superimposed stimulus, as it is less responsive to the grating alone.
Having changed the orientation, shape, and luminance of the superimposed dots, we next assessed the effect of their velocity and number on the grating response. Increasing the velocity of the (∼500) competing blue dots had no significant effect on wild-type responsiveness to the grating; responsiveness even increased slightly (Figure 6A, gray circles). In contrast, dunce mutants exposed to the same scenario showed a generally degraded visual response with increasing dot velocity (Figure 6A, black circles). Notably, at high dot velocities (>400 pixels/s), responsiveness levels between dunce 1 and wild type were not significantly different from each other. Interestingly, a resurrection of dunce 1 responsiveness to the grating was noted at highest dot velocities (1000 pixels/s, as seen in Figure 6A). This would be expected because the competing dots probably lose motion coherence at high velocities (because they would we “skipping” incoherently when displayed at a 60 Hz refresh). The behavior of dunce 1 in this last experiment supports the possibility that dots are acting as a competing percept, rather than merely subtracting from the grating response.
As we have seen, dunce 1 displayed increased responsiveness to fewer moving dots than wild type (Figure 4E&F). To better understand how either strain might be responding to the combined wide-field stimuli, we presented increasing numbers of blue dots moving coherently against the direction of motion of the grating. We found that above the threshold when dunce 1 responds to dots alone (∼200 dots), dunce 1 responsiveness decreased linearly with increasing dot number over the grating (r = −0.62), whereas, in wild-type flies, the same experiment revealed a lower correlation to dot number (r = −0.28, Figure 6B). Although the response profile for either strain is different, responsiveness to the grating is lost at similar dot numbers for wild type and the mutant. dunce 1 responsiveness shifted significantly toward negative values (in favor of dots) at higher densities of the competing stimulus, as shown in a plot of the distribution of flies in the maze end-tubes (Figure 6C). The linear response of dunce 1 to increasing numbers of competing dots is consistent with effects due to dot orientation we uncovered in Figure 5C, suggesting additive (or subtractive) effects on the motion pathway.
In our experiments, we have so far assumed what the flies perceive based on a number of defined visual parameters (e.g. grating resolution, dot number). Another way of addressing differences in visual perception between mutants and wild type would be to quantify a common metric across all of the different visuals. One interpretation based on our results is that dunce mutants could for example be responding more to wide-field image regularity, regardless of whether these are dots or gratings or combined stimuli. In this view, dunce 1 visual responsiveness to the combined stimuli would be less tied to competing motion effects, and more tied to overall image regularity. One way of quantifying image regularity is by measuring the power of the dominant spatial frequency in the visuals (See Methods). Exploiting the data-mining capacities of our automated system (all data are appended to a Matlab structure), we analyzed all of the movies used in this study and plotted the maximal spatial frequency power against all visual responses for every experiment where dots and grating were combined (1067 maze runs). The result of our meta-analysis suggests that dunce 1 are indeed responding strongly to increased image regularity: the stronger the dominant frequency in the visual, the stronger is the mutant's response (Figure 6D). This approach to analyzing our data explains some observations quite well, for instance the stronger effect of moving square dots compared to round dots over a grating (from Figure 5E, shown here for dunce 1 in yellow and green, respectively). However, this approach does not adequately explain why in other instances, even when the dominant frequency is similar, visual responses in dunce 1 can be significantly different (e.g., dots moving in opposite directions over a grating, from Figures 5C; corresponding data points are highlighted in Figure 6D for 45° in red and 225° in blue).
Discussion
In this study, we have applied a level of visual investigation routinely used in human visual perception studies to Drosophila populations. We achieved this using an automated visual testing paradigm for flies, which combines computer-generated visual displays with simple serial choice mazes and commercially available Drosophila counting devices. As a first test, we applied our device to better understanding visual behavior in a classical olfactory learning mutant, dunce 1. Our results confirm that dunce 1 affects visual responsiveness, and that visual processing is likely disturbed by the developmental genetic manipulation. The dunce 1 mutant responds more strongly than wild type to highly structured motion, especially under conditions of high contrast and luminosity. Furthermore, dunce 1 also responds more strongly to another wide-field motion stimulus, moving dots, and fewer moving dots were required to evoke a positive visual response in the mutant than wild type. Comparisons with wild type suggested two distinct possibilities: either dunce 1 is responding more strongly due to improved visual processing, or arousal thresholds to wide-field motion have been altered in the mutant. The former would involve peripheral systems, whereas the latter might involve central processing in the brain. Our results suggest that visual responses are not generally improved in the mutant, arguing for a more central arousal threshold defect in dunce 1. Our experiments show that dunce 1 is highly responsive to wide field motion even across a variety of more complex scenes comprised of overlapping dots and gratings. In general, responsiveness levels in the mutant can be predicted by image regularity (e.g. the amplitude of the dominant frequency (Figure 6D), but our paradigm also opens up the possibility to screen for responses to competing motion stimuli (as in Figure 5).
Insect visual responses have been originally described as hard-wired reflexes, and most studies have therefore logically focused on visual processing in the eye to explain these behaviors. Yet, why (olfactory) learning mutants such as dunce 1 exert such strong effects on elementary visual behaviors requires some explanation. We propose that processing in the central brain, such as the dunce 1 effects explored here, may set a responsiveness threshold for motion signals from the periphery, to guide the visual choices made by flies. This view of visual behavior in flies, where central neural processing also drives the behavior, is not necessarily in conflict with the classical “bottom-up” view, which has been aimed at dissecting visual behavior from the periphery. Recent work has shown that central neurons modulate optomotor responses for flight behavior [30]. A necessary next step in Drosophila visual studies will be to map the interface between central processes, such as those overlapping with memory systems [15], and the front-end of fly vision.
One explanation for increased visual responsiveness in dunce 1 may be that the mutant is less able to suppress responses to salient wide-field motion stimuli. In this perhaps counter-intuitive view, increased visual responsiveness in dunce 1 would represent a failure to suppress a salient visual stimulus. Failed suppression mechanisms as an explanation for improved performance would be consistent with the attention-like defects uncovered for dunce 1 in electrophysiology paradigms [13], [15]. Humans are able to suppress optokinetic reflexes by directing their attention to a visual target surrounded by wide-field motion, but this ability can degrade with age or cognitive dysfunction [31], [32]. Whether similar suppression mechanisms occur in flies is debatable, although some form of stimulus suppression is evident in all fly attention experiments conducted to date [11], [13], [15], [16], [33], [34]. A view centered on attention-like behavior therefore suggests that visual responses in wild-type flies are shaped to some extent by suppression mechanisms.
The counterargument for this suppression hypothesis suggests that dunce mutants simply have increased responsiveness to moving objects, without actually being defective in suppressing motion cues. Although this may be possible, we do not believe this to be true for the following reasons. First, dunce 1 does not respond more strongly than wild type across all stimulus conditions; rather, dunce 1 responsiveness peaks under rather narrow optimal conditions that for the most part coincide with wild-type peak responsiveness levels. Second, our RDK experiments showed that dunce 1 responsiveness depends on the visual context: although dunce 1 responds strongly to fewer moving dots than wild type, the mutant does not respond to a lower percentage of coherently moving dots (Figure 4A). This shows that the increased responsiveness of dunce 1 to fewer moving dots is abolished when incoherent moving distracters are present (such as a few dots moving in random directions relative to the overall direction of motion), suggesting detection of wide-field motion is not improved in the mutant. Further, in a previous study we found that producing a transient defect in the fly brain by silencing the output of central neurons also increased responsiveness in transgenic flies to levels as high as observed for dunce mutants [15]. A failure in brain function, as induced by such transient synaptic silencing experiments, should not be associated with improved performance, unless that function was involved with attenuating performance. Together, this genetic and behavioral evidence makes an interesting prediction: other manipulations that may compromise brain function, such as aging, neurodegeneration, or stress, should also increase visual responsiveness in wild-type flies in our paradigm. Such increase in performance as a signature of failure would provide a powerful screen in studies aimed at uncovering attention-related mechanisms in Drosophila.
Materials and Methods
Animals and Preparation
Our wild-type strain was from the Canton-S genetic background (sourced from the Neurosciences Institute (NSI), San Diego), and this specific background was introgressed 5–6 generations into the dunce 1 mutant that was used for all of our experiments. Flies were raised on standard Drosophila yeast-based media and kept on 12∶12 hr light-dark cycle. Adult females (2–7 days old) were collected under CO2 anesthesia and put in batches of ∼25 in “jumbo” plastic transfer pipettes (Fisher Scientific) and supplied with a drop of water but no food. Flies were kept overnight in cycle-matched light-dark incubators set at room temperature (22°C) and were tested the next morning, about 16–20 hrs after collection.
Mazes
A version of the maze paradigm has been reported previously [11]. Earlier designs to fractionate fly populations according to optomotor responses relied on physically rotating drums and manual scoring [2], [18], whereas our design makes use of computer-generated images and automated analysis. The plexiglas 8-point choice mazes are placed over upturned computer monitors on which moving images can be displayed. Turns made by the flies as they walk through the mazes (see [11], [13] for a description of individual fly behavior in the maze, and http://web.qbi.uq.edu.au/vanswinderen/Movie1.avi for a sample experiment) determine their visual response, which is calculated from their distribution among nine collection points at the end of each maze (See Figure 1). Although it is likely that the assay combines a variety of behaviors in addition to visual motion responses (e.g., following or reversing) we did not detect a significant effect of population size on the visual response in female flies [11]. Experiments performed on runs of single flies yielded similar levels of responsiveness as observed in large groups of female flies (data not shown).
The mazes are flanked to the left and right by angled (70°) liquid crystal display (LCD) screens on which images can also be displayed, although for this study the flanking screens always displayed a uniform red background to illuminate the maze for visual inspection (Figure 1A,B). We modified our maze design in order to allow for a high-throughput, automated assessment of visual behavior in a strain. Multiple identical mazes were constructed based on a standard modular design (J&M Specialty Parts, San Diego, California), and these mazes were run in parallel. Upon completion of an experiment (∼2 minutes), flies were trapped in any of nine holding chambers and then cleared by vacuum suction through attached Tygon tubing (Fisher Scientific). The number of flies in each chamber was counted using infrared sensors (modified “Hi-Speed” Drosophila Activity Monitors, Trikinetics, Waltham, Massachusetts) placed along the vacuum route; all flies were disposed into a collection trap after being automatically counted (Figure 1B). Any flies remaining in the maze were forced through by air pressure afterwards and excluded from the final tally. All maze experiments were balanced for image direction, with an equivalent number of flies within a strain tested for responses to images moving in the opposite direction on the computer monitor. For this study, each experiment comprised four mazes (with ∼25 flies each) in either direction, thus ∼200 female flies per data point, unless stated otherwise. Upon completion of an experiment (<5 min for both directions), the visual response was automatically calculated, distributions plotted, significance tested, and data saved using custom Matlab (Mathworks) software, and the next experiment could immediately follow. To generate the data for this paper, one individual (OE) loaded over 80,000 flies into about 2500 maze runs.
Visual Stimuli
All visual stimuli were made in Vision Egg [25] using Python programming language. Physical parameters of the stimuli were measured using a spectrophotometer (Ocean Optics). Refresh rate of the CRT monitors (NEC Diamond Pro) were set at 60 Hz. We ran subset of experiments at a 200 Hz refresh on a specialized monitor (Iiyama HM204DT Japan) using a Nividia GeForce 8800 GT graphics card to check for effects of the refresh rate on fly behavioral responses in our assay. Results with a 200 Hz vertical refresh rate (presumably above the flicker fusion frequency for fly vision [5]) were not significantly different to 60 Hz refresh experiments (See Figure 2F). A standard green/black grating stimulus was used throughout, unless specified (green level 255, spatial frequency 0.016 cycles/degree, temporal frequency 3 Hz, velocity 130 degrees/s, Michelson contrast 1.0, based on a maximum of 255 and minimum of 0). Our standard dot stimulus, unless otherwise specified, was green level 255, 1500 dots on the screen, 100% motion coherence left or right, 10 s lifespan, 130 degrees/s, 13.8 degrees width. The visual parameters tested in each of the other experiments are described in the figure legends. See http://web.qbi.uq.edu.au/vanswinderen/Movie2.mpeg for the dot stimulus and http://web.qbi.uq.edu.au/vanswinderen/Movie3.mpeg for the combined dot/grating stimulus. To measure image regularity, movies were first decomposed into their individual frames and converted to black and white. The luminance levels along a horizontal line across the images were then Fourier transformed into frequency space using Matlab, and the power of the spatial frequencies was calculated for each image, yielding an average spectrum. The maximum power of each movie was compared with the Visual Response values for wild type and dunce 1, in a meta-analysis of the strains' performance in all of our combined movies (Figure 6D).
Data Analysis and Statistics
Following an experiment (typically 8 mazes of ∼25 flies each), a Visual Response (VR) was automatically calculated (in Matlab, from Trikinetics counts) as a weighted average of flies in each terminal position of the maze (VR = (# flies in tube N)*N/(total # flies), where N = −4 to+4, or the location of the tube endings) [11]. A positive VR indicates that flies on average were turning in the direction of image motion displayed on the screen. The VR data was tested for normality using the Lilliefors test and a significant VR response was when the values were significantly different from zero, which was determined by t-test (or U-test for non-parametric data) against zero and set at P<0.01 for the psychophysics experiments and P<0.05 for other experiments. All data points are plotted as means ± standard error of the mean (s.e.m.). When images were presented in competition with one another (e.g., dots versus gratings), the grating direction was set as the baseline positive direction. Comparisons between strains for specific stimuli were done by t-test (or U-test for non-parametric data) and set at P<0.01 for significance. Comparisons between responses to different visual parameters were also made using one-way ANOVA, coorected for multiple comparisons, with P<0.01 set for significance. Pairwise linear correlations were also performed between the visual parameters and the VR values and for VR values between strains, and significant correlations set at P<0.05.
Footnotes
Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Funding: This research was funded by the Australian Research Council Future Fellowship FT100100725 to BVS. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Kalmus H. Animals as Mathematicians. Nature. 1964;202:1156–1160. doi: 10.1038/2021156a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Gotz KG. Fractionation of Drosophila populations according to optomotor traits. J Exp Biol. 1970;52:419–436. doi: 10.1242/jeb.52.2.419. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Masseck OA, Hoffmann KP. Comparative neurobiology of the optokinetic reflex. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009;1164:430–439. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.03854.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Gotz KG, Wenking H. Visual Control of Locomotion in the Walking Fruitfly Drosophila. J Comp Physiol. 1973;85:235–266. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Heisenberg M, Wolf R. Berlin, New York: Springer-Verlag; 1984. Vision in Drosophila: genetics of microbehavior. p. ix, 250. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Borst A, Haag J, Reiff DF. Fly Motion Vision. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2010 doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-153155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Borst A. Drosophila's view on insect vision. Curr Biol. 2009;19:R36–47. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Katsov AY, Clandinin TR. Motion processing streams in Drosophila are behaviorally specialized. Neuron. 2008;59:322–335. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.05.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Rister J, Pauls D, Schnell B, Ting CY, Lee CH, et al. Dissection of the peripheral motion channel in the visual system of Drosophila melanogaster. Neuron. 2007;56:155–170. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.09.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Zhu Y, Nern A, Zipursky SL, Frye MA. Peripheral visual circuits functionally segregate motion and phototaxis behaviors in the fly. Curr Biol. 2009;19:613–619. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.02.053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.van Swinderen B, Flores KA. Attention-like processes underlying optomotor performance in a Drosophila choice maze. J Neurobiol. 2007;67:129–145. doi: 10.1002/dneu.20334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Liu G, Seiler H, Wen A, Zars T, Ito K, et al. Distinct memory traces for two visual features in the Drosophila brain. Nature. 2006;439:551–556. doi: 10.1038/nature04381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.van Swinderen B. Attention-like processes in Drosophila require short-term memory genes. Science. 2007;315:1590–1593. doi: 10.1126/science.1137931. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Pan Y, Zhou Y, Guo C, Gong H, Gong Z, et al. Differential roles of the fan-shaped body and the ellipsoid body in Drosophila visual pattern memory. Learn Mem. 2009;16:289–295. doi: 10.1101/lm.1331809. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.van Swinderen B, McCartney A, Kauffman S, Flores K, Agrawal K, et al. Shared visual attention and memory systems in the Drosophila brain. PLoS One. 2009;4:e5989. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.van Swinderen B, Brembs B. Attention-like deficit and hyperactivity in a Drosophila memory mutant. J Neurosci. 2010;30:1003–1014. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4516-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Yamaguchi S, Wolf R, Desplan C, Heisenberg M. Motion vision is independent of color in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105:4910–4915. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0711484105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Pflugfelder GO, Heisenberg M. Optomotor-blind of Drosophila melanogaster: a neurogenetic approach to optic lobe development and optomotor behaviour. Comp Biochem Physiol A Physiol. 1995;110:185–202. doi: 10.1016/0300-9629(94)00159-q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Strauss R, Schuster S, Gotz KG. Processing of artificial visual feedback in the walking fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster. J Exp Biol. 1997;200:1281–1296. doi: 10.1242/jeb.200.9.1281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Srinivasan MV, Poteser M, Kral K. Motion detection in insect orientation and navigation. Vision Res. 1999;39:2749–2766. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(99)00002-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Williams DW, Sekuler R. Coherent global motion percepts from stochastic local motions. Vision Res. 1984;24:55–62. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(84)90144-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Baker CL, Jr, Hess RF, Zihl J. Residual motion perception in a “motion-blind” patient, assessed with limited-lifetime random dot stimuli. J Neurosci. 1991;11:454–461. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.11-02-00454.1991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Newsome WT, Pare EB. A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT). J Neurosci. 1988;8:2201–2211. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.08-06-02201.1988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Andersen SK, Muller MM. Behavioral performance follows the time course of neural facilitation and suppression during cued shifts of feature-selective attention. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107:13878–13882. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1002436107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Straw AD. VisionEgg: An open-source libraru for realtime visual stimulus generation. 2008. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics 10.3389/neuro.11.0044.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 26.Nordstrom K, O'Carroll DC. Feature detection and the hypercomplex property in insects. Trends Neurosci. 2009;32:383–391. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2009.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Bolzon DM, Nordstrom K, O'Carroll DC. Local and large-range inhibition in feature detection. J Neurosci. 2009;29:14143–14150. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2857-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Joesch M, Plett J, Borst A, Reiff DF. Response properties of motion-sensitive visual interneurons in the lobula plate of Drosophila melanogaster. Curr Biol. 2008;18:368–374. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.02.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Schnell B, Joesch M, Forstner F, Raghu SV, Otsuna H, et al. Processing of horizontal optic flow in three visual interneurons of the Drosophila brain. J Neurophysiol. 2010;103:1646–1657. doi: 10.1152/jn.00950.2009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Haag J, Wertz A, Borst A. Central gating of fly optomotor response. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107:20104–20109. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1009381107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Williams IM, Mulhall L, Mattingley J, Lueck C, Abel L. Optokinetic nystagmus as an assessment of visual attention to divided stimuli. J Clin Neurosci. 2006;13:828–833. doi: 10.1016/j.jocn.2005.10.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Rubinstein NJ, Abel LA. Optokinetic nystagmus suppression as an index of the allocation of visual attention. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2011;52:462–467. doi: 10.1167/iovs.10-6016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Tang S, Juusola M. Intrinsic activity in the fly brain gates visual information during behavioral choices. PLoS One. 2011;5:e14455. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0014455. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Sareen P, Wolf R, Heisenberg M. Attracting the attention of a fly. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108:7230–7235. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1102522108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]