Abstract
Preschoolers with spina bifida (SB) were compared to typically developing (TD) children on tasks tapping mathematical knowledge at 36 months (n = 102) and 60 months of age (n = 98). The group with SB had difficulty compared to TD peers on all mathematical tasks except for transformation on quantities in the subitizable range. At 36 months, vocabulary knowledge, visual–spatial, and fine motor abilities predicted achievement on a measure of informal math knowledge in both groups. At 60 months of age, phonological awareness, visual–spatial ability, and fine motor skill were uniquely and differentially related to counting knowledge, oral counting, object-based arithmetic skills, and quantitative concepts. Importantly, the patterns of association between these predictors and mathematical performance were similar across the groups. A novel finding is that fine motor skill uniquely predicted object-based arithmetic abilities in both groups, suggesting developmental continuity in the neurocognitive correlates of early object-based and later symbolic arithmetic problem solving. Models combining 36-month mathematical ability and these language-based, visual–spatial, and fine motor abilities at 60 months accounted for considerable variance on 60-month informal mathematical outcomes. Results are discussed with reference to models of mathematical development and early identification of risk in preschoolers with neurodevelopmental disorder.
Keywords: Child, Preschool, Counting, Arithmetic, Mathematical problem solving, Visual-spatial ability, Fine motor skill, Phonological awareness
INTRODUCTION
Spina bifida (SB) is a common congenital neurodevelopmental disorder that affects the development of the spine and brain and is associated with a modal cognitive and academic phenotype (Dennis, Landry, Barnes, & Fletcher, 2006); in academics this includes better-developed word reading than mathematical skills (Ayr, Yeates, & Enrile, 2005; Barnes & Dennis, 1992; Barnes et al., 2002, 2006; Fletcher et al., 2004, 2005). Because SB is associated with a high rate of math disability and the condition is diagnosed before or at birth, SB affords an opportunity to investigate early math abilities and their cognitive predictors in this disorder.
Approximately 50% of children and adolescents with SB who are not intellectually disabled have math difficulties, and over half of these children have specific math difficulties (or MD); that is, difficulties in math that are not accompanied by problems in reading (or RD) (Fletcher et al., 2005). Cognitive studies of single- and multi-digit arithmetic in school-age children with SB show that the difficulties of those with MD or MD+RD (Ayr et al., 2005; Barnes et al., 2002, 2006) are remarkably similar to those of children with no neurodevelopmental disorder who have MD and MD+RD (e.g., Hanich, Jordan, Kaplan, & Dick, 2001; Raghubar et al., 2009). In adults with SB, math skills are stronger predictors of level of independence than IQ or literacy levels (Dennis & Barnes, 2002; Hetherington, Dennis, Barnes, Drake, & Gentili, 2006). Although the nature of mathematical difficulties in school age children and adults with SB is becoming better understood, little is known about early developing math abilities in SB and the neurocognitive underpinnings of their early mathematical performance.
Correlates of Early Mathematical Development
Visual–spatial processes and early math
Visual–spatial abilities (encoding and mental manipulation of spatial information) and visual–spatial memory (ability to remember spatial locations or spatial sequences) may be particularly important in mathematical performance in young children and in new mathematical learning in older children (review in Raghubar, Barnes, & Hecht, 2010). Based on longitudinal studies, both preschool visual–spatial abilities (Assel, Landry, Swank, Smith, & Steelman, 2003) and visual–spatial working memory (Bull, Espy, & Wiebe, 2008) predict math achievement in the early grades. It has been suggested that preschoolers use mental models, which are nonverbal representations of the mathematical situation, to accomplish some mathematical tasks, particularly those involving transformations on quantity (Mix, Huttenlocher, & Levine, 2002). An association between visual–spatial memory and quantity transformation tasks, such as object-based arithmetic and inversion, has been found for preschoolers (Rasmussen, Ho, & Bisanz, 2003), consistent with the idea that visual mental models are used to solve these problems (Bisanz, Sherman, Rasmussen, & Ho, 2005).
SB is associated with deficits in some aspects of visual–spatial processing (e.g., Dennis, Fletcher, Rogers, Hetherington, & Francis, 2002; Dennis et al., 2006). However, visual–spatial abilities are only weakly related to single- and multi-digit arithmetic in school-age children with and without SB (Ayr et al., 2005; Barnes et al., 2002, 2006), although they are related to other domains of mathematics, including geometry and estimation (Barnes et al., 2002). Given that visual spatial abilities may be used in the development of mental models, which in turn predict performance on informal mathematical tasks, the relation of visual spatial abilities to early math in SB is of considerable interest and has not been studied.
Language-based abilities and early math skills
Language-based abilities such as word knowledge, verbal working memory, and phonological skills predict performance on some mathematical tasks in both preschoolers and school-age children (Durand, Hulme, Larkin, & Snowling, 2005; LeFevre et al., 2010; review in Raghubar et al., 2010). Language is thought to be important for mapping number words and symbols, representing exact number (Dehaene, Piazza, Pinel, & Cohen, 2005), and using verbal strategies to solve some types of mathematical problems (Jordan, Kaplan, Olah, & Locuniak, 2006). Phonological processes, specifically phonological awareness and phonological working memory, are related to learning the number–word sequence in children from kindergarten to third grade (Krajewski & Schneider, 2009), predict growth in early math achievement (Hecht, Torgesen, Wagner, & Rashotte, 2001), are related to math disabilities (Simmons & Singleton, 2008; Swanson & Jerman, 2006), and mediate individual differences in direct retrieval of answers to small addition problems (De Smedt, Taylor, Archibald, & Ansari, 2009). In school age children with SB, phonological ability is only weakly related to arithmetic skills (Barnes et al., 2006). However, little is known about the relation between language-based abilities such as vocabulary knowledge and phonological processes and early mathematical skills in either typically developing preschool children or in preschoolers with SB.
Fine motor abilities and early math
Finger gnosis has been linked to achievement in math in the early school grades (Noel, 2005). Such findings have been used to argue for common neural representations of fingers and numbers because of their functional developmental connections through the use of fingers to count and calculate (e.g., Butterworth, 1999). Fine motor skills involved in finger counting and pointing may also help young children compensate for limited working memory capacity by avoiding having to internally store a mental representation of each counted object (Alibali & DiRusso, 1999). Finger agility has also been linked to an ability to use fingers to perform counting and calculation procedures (Penner-Wilger et al., 2007). Older children with SB use their fingers more often than typically developing children when solving single digit arithmetic problems (Barnes et al., 2006). The influence of fine motor skills on math in preschoolers with SB may be of particular importance because the brain anomalies associated with SB affect fine motor skills such as finger function (Dennis et al., 2006; Friedrich, Lovejoy, Shaffer, Shurtleff, & Beilke, 1991; Lomax-Bream, Barnes, Copeland, Taylor, & Landry, 2007; Wills, 1993).
Mathematical performance may draw on language-based, visual–spatial, and finger representations to greater or lesser extents depending on the cognitive characteristics of the child, the age of the child, and whether particular mathematical problems can be solved using different strategies (Ansari & Dhital, 2006; Bull, 2007; LeFevre et al., 2010). Whether preschoolers with SB and typically developing preschoolers differ in the neurocognitive abilities they bring to bear in performing informal mathematical tasks is unknown.
The Present Study
This study has two aims: (1) to compare the performance of children with SB and their typically developing peers at 36 months and 60 months of age on measures of informal mathematical abilities that have been found to predict later math achievement in longitudinal studies (Jordan, Kaplan, Locuniak, & Ramineni, 2007; Jordan et al., 2006); and (2) to determine what neurocognitive abilities are related to different mathematical skills at each age in children with SB and in age peers.
Based on theories of mathematical development in which informal mathematical knowledge acquired by preschoolers forms the foundation for the acquisition of formal mathematical abilities at school-age (Ginsburg, Klein, & Starkey, 1998), we hypothesize that difficulties in mathematics might be present as early as 36 and 60 months of age, such that preschoolers with SB will score lower than their typically developing (TD) peers on both standardized and experimental tests of informal mathematical knowledge. We also hypothesize that by 60 months of age, in comparison to TD peers, children with SB will demonstrate deficits on those informal math skills thought to draw more heavily on visual–spatial abilities (e.g., object-based arithmetic) and show less difficulty on math skills thought to be more strongly related to verbal ability (e.g., oral counting). Given differences in the neurocognitive profiles of children with SB and TD children, we ask whether the patterns of relations between math performance and abilities in domains other than mathematics (e.g., visual–spatial, language, fine motor) might differ between groups.
METHOD
Participants
This study is part of a longitudinal project on cognitive, motor, and social development in children with SB in Toronto, Ontario, and Houston, Texas. Starting in 1997, children with SB were identified by treating neurosurgeons and pediatricians and recruited into the study and examined several times between 6 and 60 months of age. Detailed inclusion and exclusion criteria for the larger study are in Lomax-Bream et al. (2007). In the current study, children from this larger group were excluded if: (1) they did not have complete correlates data (i.e., language, visual–spatial, fine motor variables) within an assessment point; (2) their performance on more than one mathematical task at each assessment point was unreliable; and (3) they had global intellectual impairment (i.e., their scores on both Vocabulary and Pattern Analysis subtests of the Stanford-Binet-IV [SB-IV: Thorndike, Hagen, & Sattler, 1989] were more than 2 standard deviations (SD) below the population mean at 36 months, and their scores on both Picture Vocabulary and Spatial Relations subtests from the Woodcock Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities-Revised [WJ-R: Woodcock & Johnson, 1989] were more than 2 SD below the population mean at 60 months) (Table 1). This resulted in the loss of two children with SB at 36 months of age and at 60 months of age. Differences in numbers of participants on tasks within an assessment most often reflect lack of reliability for a task as coded by assessors (e.g., child fatigue, behavior). Differences in numbers between assessments were most often due to the inability of the family to attend the second assessment. At 60 months, SB-IV Quantitative was added after testing had begun, accounting for the lower sample size for this task.
Table 1.
36 Months of age |
60 Months of age |
|||
---|---|---|---|---|
SB | Control | SB | Control | |
Age (months) | 37.56 | 37.92 | 61.83 | 61.29 |
Sex (female) | 65% | 42% | 68% | 38% |
Hollingshead SES | 32.82* | 45.27 | 33.06* | 42.82 |
Ethnicity | ||||
African American | 9% | 2% | 13% | 2% |
Caucasian | 60% | 70% | 52% | 65% |
Hispanic | 28% | 15% | 35% | 24% |
Other | 2% | 13% | 0 | 9% |
SB-IV Vocabulary | 94.94* | 107.66 | — | — |
SB-IV Pattern Analysis | 83.77* | 102.49 | — | — |
WJ-R Picture Vocabulary | — | — | 93.89* | 112.65 |
WJ-R Spatial Relations | — | — | 86.15* | 104.65 |
CTOPP Sound Matching | — | — | 8.83* | 10.08 |
CTOPP Elision | — | — | 8.05* | 10.04 |
Phonological Awareness Composite | — | — | 6.02* | 10.43 |
Purdue Pegs | 1.77 | 0.62 | 3.43* | 6.46 |
Note.
Denotes a significant group difference. Values are standard scores except for Purdue Pegs which was number of errors at 36 months and pegs placed at 60 months. SB-IV = Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales, Fourth Edition; WJ-R = Woodcock-Johnson Test of Cognitive Abilities—Revised; CTOPP = Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing; Phonological Awareness Composite = CTOPP Sound Matching and CTOPP Elision raw scores.
The distribution of participants was relatively equal between the two sites. Most of the children had hydrocephalus treated with a diversionary shunt; nine children had arrested hydrocephalus and no shunt. The majority had lower spinal lesions below L1 (87% and 89% depending on the sample at each time point).
Table 1 shows the distributions of gender, ethnicity, and socioeconomic status (SES) as assessed with the Hollings-head (1975) four-factor scale. The sociodemographics of the Texas and Ontario sites differed because the Texas site included more children of Hispanic origin. The TD group had a higher SES than the group with SB [(t(95) = 12.46, p<.001 at 36 months; t(90) = 3.25, p<.01 and at 60 months], mainly reflecting the greater number of economically disadvantaged Hispanic children with SB in Texas. Consequently, SES was used as a covariate in analyses involving group comparisons. Groups differed in gender with more female participants in the group with SB (χ2(1) = 5.84; p<.05 at 36 months; χ2(1) = 9.08; p<.01 at 60 months). Gender was not used as a covariate as it was not associated with math outcomes.
Measures and Procedures
Participants were assessed in a single session lasting between 1.5 and 3 hours depending on the assessment. Most were assessed at facilities associated with the project and some were assessed in their homes. Consent was obtained from parents in accordance with the institutional review boards at the University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston and the Toronto Hospital for Sick Children.
Preschool math measures at 36 months
The Test of Early Mathematics Ability-2 or TEMA-2 (Ginsburg & Baroody, 1990) measures informal mathematics skills. The first several items measure counting small and larger sets of objects, showing number using fingers, quantity comparison including understanding of “more,” and understanding of cardinality. The test yields an age-standardized math quotient (internal consistency coefficient = .95 at 3 years of age). Because 36 months is the entry level for the test and to provide a larger range of scores at this age, we also used an experimental score derived by giving children credit for each correct answer they provided on the test (e.g., some items contain more than one question, but require that all be answered correctly to score 1 point),
Preschool math measures at 60 months
Counting principles
In this measure of counting knowledge (Briars & Siegler, 1984; Gelman & Galistell, 1978), a hand puppet pointed to and counted colored dots on a page (12 dots for half of the trials, and 16 for the other half). Children were instructed to tell the puppet whether or not he/she was counting correctly. This measure assessed knowledge of one-to-one correspondence (one counting tag is applied to each object), stable order (number tags must be applied in an invariant order), and cardinality (the last number counted refers to the total quantity) principles.
The puppet counted correctly on 8 trials and incorrectly on 16 trials. One-to-one correspondence was violated on 4 trials (e.g., puppet skipped counting a dot); stable order on 6 trials (e.g., reversing two numbers in a count); and cardinality on 6 trials (e.g., puppet gives an incorrect answer when he is asked “how many things he counted”). Because we did not have different hypotheses corresponding to each principle, correct detection of counting errors was collapsed across principles, consistent with procedures used in other studies (e.g., LeFevre et al., 2006). Unconventional counts (e.g., puppet counted all blue then all red dots) were administered, but not analyzed, given the findings of LeFevre et al. (2006) and Kamawar et al. (2010) suggesting a curvilinear relation between age and the acceptance of these as correct counts that is moderated by individual differences in numeration abilities.
Oral counting
Procedural counting was measured by asking children to count as high as they could (Miller, Smith, Zhu, & Zhang, 1995). The score was the highest number counted without any errors, with correct counts above 100 capped at 100.
Object-based arithmetic
In this task (based on Jordan, Huttenlocher, & Levine, 1992), the child watched the examiner place poker chips (from the examiner’s box of 10 chips) on the examiner’s mat. Then a screen was placed in front of the array to occlude the array from the child, and the child watched while the examiner added chips all at one time to the array (addition) or removed chips all at one time from behind the screen (subtraction). Children could not see the current quantity behind the screen but clearly saw the quantity that was added or removed. Children were then asked to use their chips (from their box of 10 chips) to match the quantity that was hidden after the transformation. Explicit quantity information was used in the instructions (e.g., “How many do I have under here now? Show me on your mat. Make yours like mine”). There were 12 trials (half addition and half subtraction). Five problems involved small size sets in the subitizable range (subitizing refers to the ability to discern exact quantity without counting up to 3 or 4; in this study, problems with quantities in the subitizable range involved a sum of 3 or less for addition and a subtrahend of 3 or less for subtraction; 1+1; 2−1; 2+1; 3−1; 3−2) and seven problems involved quantities of 4 or greater (sum or subtrahend 4 or greater; 1+3; 2+2, 4−1, 4−3, 4−2, 1+4, 2+3). Two demonstration items involving matching of one and two chips without covering the display were given followed by a matching phase (see Jordan, Huttenlocher, & Levine, 1994) in which the examiner placed between one and five disks on her mat, covered the display and asked the participant to make the same number on his/her mat. The task was discontinued for children who obtained less than two correct on matching, resulting in the loss of one participant from each group.
Stanford-Binet IV Quantitative (Thorndike et al., 1989)
For 5-year-olds, items measure quantitative concepts including matching on the basis of number using blocks, counting using blocks, and addition using blocks and pictures.
Language-Based, Visual–Spatial, and Fine Motor Predictors
At 36 months, measures of visual–spatial ability (Pattern Analysis from the SB-IV), vocabulary knowledge (Vocabulary from the SB-IV), and fine motor skill (Purdue Pegboard, both hands) were used as predictors of mathematical performance. At 60 months, visual–spatial ability was measured using the Spatial Relations subtest of the WJ-R, vocabulary was assessed with WJ-R Picture Vocabulary, phonological awareness was assessed using a composite score combining the Elision and Sound Matching subtests from the Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing (Wagner, Torgesen, & Rashotte, 1999), and fine motor skill was measured using Purdue Pegboard.
RESULTS
36-Month Math Group Comparisons
At 36 months, analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) controlling for SES showed that the group with SB was lower than the TD group on the TEMA-2 Math Quotient, F(1,98) = 10.57; p < .01; η2 = .10, and on the TEMA-2 experimental score, F(1,98) = 4.10; p < .05; η2 = .04 (Table 2).
Table 2.
Task | SB |
Control |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
n | M | SD | n | M | SD | |
36 Months of age | ||||||
TEMA-2 Math Quotient* | 51 | 81.47 | 10.61 | 51 | 91.96 | 13.43 |
TEMA-2 Experimental Score* | 51 | 10.90 | 10.20 | 51 | 18.12 | 12.48 |
60 Months of age | ||||||
Counting Principles* | 41 | 10.17 | 4.12 | 49 | 13.55 | 3.87 |
Oral Counting* | 45 | 25.84 | 18.08 | 53 | 45.85 | 32.92 |
SB-IV Quantitative* | 29 | 7.07 | 3.20 | 53 | 9.45 | 4.15 |
Small set object-based arithmetic | 43 | 3.65 | 1.41 | 53 | 3.79 | 1.43 |
Large set object-based arithmetic* | 42 | 2.55 | 2.10 | 51 | 3.69 | 2.21 |
Note.
Denotes a significant group difference after covarying for the effect of SES; SB-IV = Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales, Fourth Edition.
60-Month Math Group Comparisons
In ANCOVAs covarying for SES, the group with SB scored lower than the TD group on Oral Counting, F(1,95) = 8.08; p < .01; η2 = .08; Large Set Object-Based Arithmetic F(1,90) = 6.44; p < .05; η2 = .07); Counting Principles, F(1,87) = 9.81; p < .01; η2 = .10; and SB-IV Quantitative, F(1,79) = 4.36; p < .05; η2 = .05. No differences were found on Small Set Object-Based Arithmetic, F(1,93) < 1(Table 2).
Correlations among the non-math predictors at 36 months and 60 months are in Tables 3a and 3b, respectively.
Table 3a.
Vocabulary |
Visual–spatial ability |
Fine motor skills |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
C | SB | C | SB | C | SB | |
Vocabulary | — | — | .49** | .31* | .34* | .01 |
Visual–spatial ability | — | — | — | — | .52** | .34* |
Note.
p < .05
p < .01
Fine motor = Purdue Pegs; Visual–spatial ability = SB-IV Pattern Analysis; Vocabulary = SB-IV Vocabulary.
Table 3b.
Phonological awareness |
Visual–spatial ability |
Fine motor skills |
Vocabulary |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
C | SB | C | SB | C | SB | C | SB | |
Phonological awareness | — | .41** | .41** | .30* | −.03 | .42** | .42** | |
Visual–spatial ability | — | — | .34* | .19 | .42** | .20 | ||
Fine motor skills | — | — | — | .25 | −.15 |
Note.
p < .05
p < .01
Fine motor = Purdue Pegs; Visual–spatial ability = WJ-R Spatial Relations; Phonological Awareness = Composite of CTOPP Sound Matching and CTOPP Elision; Vocabulary = WJ-R Picture Vocabulary.
Non-Math Correlates of 36-Month Math Outcomes
Correlations among the non-math predictor variables (language, visual–spatial, and fine motor) and the TEMA-2 at 36 months are in Table 4a. Tests comparing the size of the correlations between groups revealed no significant findings. Hierarchical regressions were conducted to examine the predictors of math outcomes and to determine whether these varied as a function of group. The regressions included group in the first block, all of the non-math predictors in the second block, and the interaction between group and each of these predictors in the third block. The overall model accounted for 46% of the variance in TEMA-2 scores. The non-math correlates accounted for additional unique variance above that accounted for by group membership, F(3,85) = 15.97; p < .001; R2 change = .31, with vocabulary, visual–spatial ability, and fine motor skills, emerging as significant predictors. The interaction terms of group with each of the correlates did not account for additional variance (Table 5).
Table 4a.
Fine motor skills |
Visual–spatial ability |
Vocabulary |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
SB | C | SB | C | SB | C | |
TEMA-2 Math Quotient | .14 | .39** | .29* | .54** | .55** | .52** |
Note.
p < .05
p < .01
Fine motor = Purdue Pegs; Visual–spatial ability = SB-IV Pattern Analysis; Vocabulary = SB-IV Vocabulary.
Table 5.
Predictors | B | SE | β | t | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
TEMA-2 Math Quotient | |||||
Step 1 | R2 = .14 ** | ||||
Group | 10.04 | 2.63 | .38 | 3.82 | <.001 |
Step 2 | ΔR2 = .31 ** | ||||
Group | −1.09 | 2.78 | −.04 | −.39 | .696 |
Fine motor | 1.41 | .66 | .20 | 2.15 | .034 |
Visual–spatial ability | 1.26 | .52 | .29 | 2.43 | .017 |
Vocabulary | 1.17 | .30 | .39 | 3.90 | <.001 |
Step 3 | ΔR2 = .01 |
Note.
p < .05
p < .01.
Non-Math Correlates of 60-Month Math Outcomes
The correlations among math measures and the non-math predictors at 60 months are in Table 4b. The size of the correlations between groups did not differ. visual–spatial, phonological, vocabulary, and fine motor abilities were included in all models in the second block, with group and the interaction of group and each predictor in the first and third blocks, respectively (Table 6).
Table 4b.
Fine motor skills |
Visual–spatial ability |
Phonological awareness |
Vocabulary |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
SB | C | SB | C | SB | C | SB | C | |
Counting principles | .06 | .17 | .44** | .15 | .55** | .41** | .30 | .34* |
Oral counting | .25 | .22 | .34* | .52** | .29* | .59** | .16 | .35** |
Quantitative | .29 | .27* | .71** | .36** | .43* | .17 | .42* | .32* |
Small set object-based arithmetic | .36* | .37** | .18 | .12 | .14 | .30* | −.05 | .17 |
Large set object-based arithmetic | .45** | .50** | .46** | .48** | .27 | .36** | −.10 | .36* |
Note.
p < .05
p < .01
Fine motor = Purdue Pegs; Visual–spatial ability = WJ-R Spatial Relations; Phonological Awareness = Composite of CTOPP Sound Matching and CTOPP Elision; Vocabulary = WJ-R Picture Vocabulary.
Table 6.
Predictors | B | SE | β | t | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Oral counting | |||||
Step 1 | R2 = .13 ** | ||||
Group | 20.88 | 5.83 | .36 | 3.58 | <.001 |
Step 2 | ΔR2 = .28 ** | ||||
Group | 4.97 | 6.54 | .09 | .76 | .450 |
Fine motor | .70 | 1.25 | .06 | .56 | .576 |
Visual–spatial ability | 1.75 | .72 | .26 | 2.45 | .016 |
PA | 1.79 | .47 | .41 | 3.85 | <.001 |
Vocabulary | −.24 | .73 | −.03 | −.32 | .749 |
Step 3 | ΔR2 = .06 | ||||
Counting principles | |||||
Step 1 | R2 = .17 ** | ||||
Group | 3.26 | .82 | .41 | 3.95 | <.001 |
Step 2 | ΔR2 = .20 ** | ||||
Group | 1.40 | .98 | .18 | 1.43 | .157 |
Fine motor | .02 | .19 | .01 | .12 | .908 |
Visual–spatial ability | .08 | .11 | .08 | .71 | .483 |
PA | .20 | .07 | .33 | 2.89 | .005 |
Vocabulary | .19 | .11 | .19 | 1.66 | .101 |
Step 3 | ΔR2 = .05 | ||||
Quantitative | |||||
Step 1 | R2 = .06 * | ||||
Group | 2.04 | .99 | .24 | 2.05 | .044 |
Step 2 | ΔR2 = .25 ** | ||||
Group | −.60 | 1.12 | −.07 | −.53 | .595 |
Fine motor | .33 | .21 | .21 | 1.60 | .115 |
Visual–spatial ability | .36 | .13 | .38 | 2.73 | .008 |
PA | −.03 | .08 | −.05 | −.41 | .685 |
Vocabulary | .18 | .13 | .18 | 1.35 | .183 |
Step 3 | ΔR2 = .01 | ||||
Small set object-based arithmetic | |||||
Step 1 | R2 = .00 | ||||
Group | .04 | .30 | .02 | .14 | .889 |
Step 2 | ΔR2 = .16 ** | ||||
Group | −.75 | .37 | −.28 | −2.04 | .045 |
Fine motor | .21 | .07 | .42 | 3.00 | .004 |
Visual–spatial | −.02 | .04 | −.05 | −.37 | .714 |
PA | .04 | .03 | .21 | 1.59 | .116 |
Vocabulary | .00 | .04 | −.00 | −.01 | .993 |
Step 3 | ΔR2 = .01 | ||||
Large set object-based arithmetic | |||||
Step 1 | R2 = .06 * | ||||
Group | 1.04 | .48 | .24 | 2.17 | .033 |
Step 2 | ΔR2 = .34 ** | ||||
Group | −.71 | .51 | −.16 | −1.39 | .169 |
Fine motor | .35 | .10 | .42 | 3.51 | .001 |
Visual–spatial ability | .19 | .06 | .37 | 3.10 | .003 |
PA | .04 | .04 | .11 | .99 | .325 |
Vocabulary | −.04 | .06 | −.08 | −.67 | .504 |
Step 3 | ΔR2 = .02 |
Note.
p < .05
p < .01
PA =Phonological Awareness.
Oral counting
The model accounted for 47% of the variance in oral counting. Non-math abilities accounted for unique variance, F(4,82) = 9.81; p < .001; R2 change = .28, with significant contributions from both phonological awareness and visual–spatial ability. The interaction terms did not account for unique variance.
Counting principles
This model accounted for 42% of variance in error detection. Non-math abilities accounted for unique variance, F(4,74) = 5.96; p < .001; R2 change = .20, with phonological awareness emerging as the only significant predictor. The interaction terms did not contribute to the model.
Quantitative
The model accounted for 32% of the variance in Quantitative scores. Non-math abilities accounted for additional unique variance, F(4,66) = 6.06; p < .001; R2 change = .25, with a significant contribution of visual–spatial ability. The interaction terms did not account for additional variance.
Small set arithmetic
In this model, only the non-math predictors were significant, F(4,77) = 3.79; p < .01; R2 = .16, specifically, fine motor skill.
Large set arithmetic
This model accounted for 42% of the variance in large set arithmetic with non-math abilities accounting for significant additional variance, F(4,75) = 10.36; p < .001; R2 change = .34. Visual–spatial ability and fine motor skills emerged as significant predictors. The interaction terms did not account for additional unique variance.
Predicting 60-month math outcomes from 36 month math and 60 month non-math abilities
We were interested in whether language-based, visual–spatial and fine motor abilities from 60 months predicted unique variance in 60-month math outcomes after controlling for the autoregressive effects of mathematical skills at 36 months (TEMA-2) and whether such effects varied as a function of group. In longitudinal academic research when cognitive variables are significant after the inclusion of the autoregressor (measure of academic skill from an earlier time point), this is taken as evidence of their importance for the performance of that skill (de Jong & van der Leij, 2002). Here, the skill of interest is mathematical knowledge and so TEMA-2 performance at 36 months was used as the autoregressor. The question is whether visual–spatial, language-based, and fine motor skills are important for mathematical performance at 5 years of age after accounting for the child’s level of mathematical knowledge from an earlier developmental time-point.
For all analyses group membership was in the first block, raw scores from the TEMA-2 experimental measure from 36 months were entered in the second block, the 60-month non-math abilities were entered in the third block, and the interaction term of group with each of the relevant abilities was entered in the fourth block (Table 7).
Table 7.
Predictors | B | SE | β | t | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Oral counting | |||||
Step 1 | R2 = .13 ** | ||||
Group | 20.89 | 6.41 | .37 | 3.26 | .002 |
Step 2 | ΔR2 = .17 ** | ||||
Group | 14.31 | 6.04 | .25 | 2.37 | .021 |
TEMA | 1.04 | .26 | .42 | 4.00 | <.001 |
Step 3 | ΔR2 = .11 * | ||||
Group | 7.57 | 7.36 | .13 | 1.03 | .308 |
TEMA | .59 | .33 | .24 | 1.81 | .076 |
Fine motor | .50 | 1.40 | .05 | .36 | .723 |
Visual–spatial ability | 1.62 | .81 | .25 | 1.99 | .050 |
PA | 1.13 | .55 | .25 | 2.04 | .046 |
Vocabulary | −.76 | .88 | −.11 | −.87 | .386 |
Step 4 | ΔR2 = .04 | ||||
Counting principles | |||||
Step 1 | R2 = .15 ** | ||||
Group | 2.96 | .90 | .39 | 3.30 | .002 |
Step 2 | ΔR2 = .19 ** | ||||
Group | 2.04 | .83 | .27 | 2.45 | .017 |
TEMA | .15 | .04 | .45 | 4.12 | <.001 |
Step 3 | ΔR2 = .06 | ||||
Group | 1.42 | 1.07 | .19 | 1.33 | .189 |
TEMA | .10 | .05 | .30 | 2.05 | .045 |
Fine motor | .06 | .20 | .05 | .32 | .748 |
Visual–spatial ability | .06 | .12 | .07 | .51 | .610 |
PA | .16 | .08 | .26 | 2.01 | .049 |
Vocabulary | −.04 | .13 | −.04 | −.28 | .778 |
Step 4 | ΔR2 = .06 | ||||
Quantitative | |||||
Step 1 | R2 = .08 * | ||||
Group | 2.38 | 1.13 | .27 | 2.10 | .041 |
Step 2 | ΔR2 = .08 * | ||||
Group | 1.76 | 1.13 | .20 | 1.55 | .127 |
TEMA | .10 | .04 | .29 | 2.20 | .032 |
Step 3 | ΔR2 = .16 * | ||||
Group | −.30 | 1.32 | −.04 | −.23 | .822 |
TEMA | −.00 | .06 | −.01 | −.05 | .957 |
Fine Motor | .32 | .24 | .22 | 1.35 | .183 |
Visual–spatial ability | .35 | .16 | .38 | 2.24 | .029 |
PA | −.02 | .10 | −.03 | −.16 | .872 |
Vocabulary | .13 | .17 | .14 | .81 | .424 |
Step 4 | ΔR2 = .02 | ||||
Small set object-based arithmetic | |||||
Step 1 | R2 = .01 | ||||
Group | .22 | .34 | .08 | .64 | .524 |
Step 2 | ΔR2 = .10 * | ||||
Group | .02 | .34 | .01 | .05 | .963 |
TEMA | .04 | .02 | .32 | 2.60 | .012 |
Step 3 | ΔR2 = .12 | ||||
Group | −.43 | .41 | −.15 | −1.03 | .307 |
TEMA | .03 | .02 | .22 | 1.44 | .155 |
Fine Motor | .16 | .08 | .32 | 2.09 | .041 |
Visual–spatial ability | −.04 | .05 | −.13 | −.87 | .388 |
PA | .06 | .03 | .26 | 1.81 | .075 |
Vocabulary | −.03 | .05 | −.09 | −.61 | .546 |
Step 4 | ΔR2 = .00 | ||||
Large set object-based arithmetic | |||||
Step 1 | R2 = .08 * | ||||
Group | 1.26 | .53 | .29 | 2.39 | .020 |
Step 2 | ΔR2 = .10 ** | ||||
Group | .95 | .52 | .22 | 1.84 | .070 |
TEMA | .060 | .02 | .32 | 2.73 | .008 |
Step 3 | ΔR2 = .25 ** | ||||
Group | −.30 | .56 | −.07 | −.53 | .596 |
TEMA | .03 | .03 | .16 | 1.22 | .226 |
Fine motor | .32 | .11 | .40 | 2.96 | .004 |
Visual–spatial ability | .16 | .07 | .31 | 2.35 | .022 |
PA | .04 | .04 | .12 | .90 | .370 |
Vocabulary | −.11 | .07 | −.22 | −1.64 | .106 |
Step 4 | ΔR2 = .02 |
Note.
p < .05
p < .01
PA = Phonological Awareness.
Oral counting
Non-math abilities accounted for significant variance, F(4,64) = 2.86; p < .05; R2 change = .11, after accounting for 36-month math performance, with significant contributions from both phonological awareness and visual–spatial abilities. The inclusion of the interaction terms did not add significantly to the model.
Counting principles
The TEMA-2 accounted for additional variance in error detection when added after group, but neither the predictor abilities nor the interaction terms added significantly to the model.
Quantitative
Non-math abilities accounted for additional variance after accounting for 36-month math performance, F(4,49) = 2.93; p < .05; R2 change = .16, with visual–spatial abilities emerging as a significant predictor. The interaction terms did not account for additional unique variance.
Small set arithmetic
The TEMA-2 significantly predicted small set arithmetic performance when added after group. However, neither the non-math abilities nor the interaction terms significantly contributed to the model.
Large set arithmetic
Non-math abilities accounted for unique variance, F(4,59) = 6.30; p < .001; R2 change = .25, after accounting for 36-month math performance with significant contributions from visual–spatial and fine motor skills. The inclusion of the interaction terms did not contribute to the model.
DISCUSSION
SB is a disorder that is associated with a high risk of math disability by school-age (Fletcher et al., 2005). In this study of preschoolers, we hypothesized that difficulties in mathematics would be present in 3- and 5-year-old children with SB. Support for this hypothesis raw provided by the findings that at 36 months, the group with SB had less mathematical knowledge than TD peers on a standardized test of informal mathematics. At 60 months, they were less skilled on measures of counting knowledge, oral counting, large set object-based arithmetic, and a standardized test of quantitative concepts. Thus mathematical difficulties can be discerned early in development in SB, and these difficulties extend to most areas of informal mathematics as might be expected based on theories of mathematical development in which informal mathematical abilities lay the foundation for the development of formal mathematical skills at school-age (Ginsburg et al., 1998).
Small set object-based arithmetic, involving the ability to copy transformations on quantities in the subitizable range, was the only math task on which preschoolers with SB did not differ from their TD peers, and although performance was high, neither group was at ceiling. It has been hypothesized that quantities in the subitizable range are handled by object files (Trick & Plyshyn, 1994), which are episodic visual representations involved in the storage and updating of information about small sets of objects as they move in time and space (Noles, Scholl, & Mitroff, 2005). Object-based representations, which involve categorical visual perception, are better developed in SB than other aspects of visual–spatial performance (Dennis et al., 2002; review in Dennis & Barnes, 2010). Whether object-file tracking is intact in SB and related to small set arithmetic performance is not known.
Our hypotheses that the group with SB would show relative strengths in math skills presumed to rely on language-based representations and greater difficulty on tasks thought to rely on visual–spatial representations were not supported. Although children with SB had difficulty on large set object-based arithmetic, which is related to visual–spatial memory in preschoolers (Rasmussen & Bisanz, 2005), their performance on tasks measuring counting knowledge and procedures was equally disrupted. Counting is often characterized as drawing on verbal/phonological abilities, but the findings discussed below suggest that, in preschoolers, both language-based and visual–spatial processes may be important. Furthermore, even though verbal and phonological abilities of the group with SB were average, they were lower than those of the TD group, suggesting that the phonological representations supporting counting are of lower quality in preschoolers with SB than in their TD peers.
Because SB is associated with both assets and deficits in the cognitive correlates of math (Dennis et al., 2006; Dennis & Barnes, 2010), and because a particular mathematical task may be performed using different strategies or representational systems (Ansari & Dhital, 2006; Bull, 2007), we hypothesized that the correlates of mathematical performance might differ in preschoolers with SB and their TD peers. The findings revealed similarities rather than differences between groups.
At 36 months, performance on the TEMA-2 was uniquely related to visual–spatial ability, vocabulary knowledge, and fine motor skill in both groups. The TEMA-2 assesses a range of informal mathematical skills in the preschool period (quantity comparison, counting, comprehension of mathematical language) so it is not surprising that several neurocognitive abilities predict performance on this measure as mathematical thinking does not reflect a unitary skill in either younger or older children (Ginsburg et al., 1998; LeFevre, 2000; LeFevre et al., 2010).
Findings for 60-month mathematical outcomes are discussed for each math task below.
Phonological awareness uniquely predicted the ability to detect violations of counting principles in both groups. This is consistent with studies of older TD children in which phonological memory seems to be important for monitoring counting errors (Geary, Hoard, & Hamson, 1999). For these younger children in the current study, phonological awareness likely indexes the quality of their phonological representations, which may be important for monitoring what the puppet says.
Both phonological awareness and visual–spatial ability were uniquely predictive of oral counting even in the longitudinal analyses accounting for performance on the TEMA-2, which involves several counting items at age 3. These findings are consistent with a recent study showing that visual–spatial attention is a unique predictor of performance on both verbal and nonverbal measures of early numeracy (LeFevre et al., 2010). Although phonological codes are directly related to learning the first several words in the verbal counting string (Krajewski & Schneider, 2009), counting into higher numbers may additionally involve the mapping of verbal number symbols onto space, perhaps reflecting the emergence of the mental number line in children this age (Dehaene, Izard, Spelke, & Pica, 2008). Our findings in preschoolers are compatible with adult neuropsychological studies (Piazza, Mechelli, Price, & Butterworth, 2006) in which counting activates not only language association areas, but also those regions of brain implicated in other mathematical tasks that do not involve counting (Piazza, Mechelli, Butterworth, & Price, 2002). Importantly, the data suggest that the correlates of what appears to be an overtly verbal task may include visual–spatial competence during skill development (Ansari et al., 2003; LeFevre et al., 2010).
Of interest, phonological awareness emerged as the unique language-based predictor at 5 years of age for both oral counting and counting knowledge. As suggested by De Smedt et al. (2009), phonological awareness, more so than measures of word meaning, tap the quality of lexical representations, which might be an important mediator of some mathematical skills such as math fact retrieval. Because counting is a developmental precursor to learning arithmetic facts, phonological representations might also be important for acquiring those counting skills that lay the foundation for arithmetic problem solving.
Visual–spatial and fine motor abilities uniquely predicted large set object-based arithmetic for both groups. Given that tests of visual–spatial ability and visual–spatial working memory largely measure similar constructs (Miyake, Friedman, Rettinger, Shah, & Hegarty, 2001), our findings are consistent with studies relating visual–spatial memory to object-based arithmetic in typically developing preschoolers (LeFevre et al., 2010; Rasmussen & Bisanz, 2005), and with the idea that mental models are used to solve these types of problems (Bisanz et al., 2005).
The findings on the relations of fine motor skill and large set object-based arithmetic are interesting for three reasons: (1) Fine motor skill is typically not assessed in studies of the correlates of these informal math abilities so the finding that it was a unique predictor of object-based arithmetic is novel. (2) Because fine motor skill is also a unique predictor of multi-digit calculation in older TD children (Noel, 2005) and children with SB (Barnes, Smith-Chant, Landry, 2005), the findings provide evidence for cognitive continuity in object-based or nonverbal and symbolic arithmetic problem solving. (3) Because fine motor skill was specifically related to transformations on number and not to other mathematical abilities, the findings provide evidence for the view that the development of early math skills is multi-componential (LeFevre et al., 2010).
What is the nature of the relation between fingers and arithmetic? One hypothesis is that because young children use fingers to solve simple arithmetic problems, fingers and calculation come to have functional and neural connections (Butterworth, 1999). It is assumed that fingers are used to represent objects during problem solving. However, in object-based arithmetic the objects are actually present so children might use their fingers to help them keep track of the hidden transformation on number. It is also possible that finger skills are only related to performance on object-based arithmetic tasks when the task requires a motor response (e.g., vs. Hodent, Bryant, & Houde, 2005). Another possibility for the relation of fine motor skill and object-based arithmetic is that both may rely on the integrity of parietal lobes (Penner-Wilger & Anderson, 2008), which are thinned in SB (Fletcher et al., 2005). Given that finger counting habits moderate the association between space and number (Fischer, 2008), understanding the connections between finger skills, visual–spatial ability, and calculation in preschoolers is important.
Because the abilities that were related to small set object-based arithmetic differed from those for large set object-based arithmetic, operations on small sets may be processed differently from those involving transformation on larger quantities. However, most studies do not separately analyze small and large object-based arithmetic problems. As was argued for oral counting, these findings underscore the importance of considering that different strategies and/or representational systems may be involved in mathematical operations that, on the surface, appear more similar than different.
Recent studies show that mathematical performance in children with and without math disabilities is predicted by both domain-specific number and domain-general cognitive skills (Fuchs et al., 2010; Geary et al., 2009; LeFevre et al., 2010). Consequently, we thought that math performance at 5 years would be predicted by concurrent neurocognitive abilities after accounting for earlier-developing number abilities at 3 years. The combination of earlier mathematical skill and later language-based, visual–spatial, and fine motor abilities accounted for substantial proportions of variance in informal mathematical outcomes. One view is that it is the combination of early deficits in domain-general and domain-specific math abilities that best predicts later severe mathematical disability (Geary et al., 2009). Whether deficits in domain-specific and domain-general abilities at preschool will predict mathematical disability in school-age children with SB is a question of considerable interest in our ongoing longitudinal study.
LIMITATIONS AND IMPLICATIONS
Consistent with most studies of early mathematical performance, the current study lacks measures of strategy-use, which would be helpful in deciding whether the findings for cognitive correlates reflect differences in the underlying representational systems and strategies brought to bear in mathematical problem solving. Another limitation is that we were unable to use all the same tasks at both ages. We also did not include some cognitive correlates such as working memory that are related to mathematical achievement in younger and older children with and without SB (Blair & Razza, 2007; Bull et al., 2008; English, Barnes, Taylor, & Landry, 2009). Finally, although the TD group was average on most standardized cognitive and academic measures, their vocabulary skills at 60 months were relatively high, suggesting a cautious approach to the interpretation of group differences on mathematical tasks.
This is the first study to test the emergence of mathematical difficulties and their correlates in very young children with SB. The findings have implications for both early identification of risk and for intervention strategies. Several of the 60-month math tasks measure early “number sense” (Berch, 2005) and have been useful in predicting later math achievement in longitudinal studies (e.g., Jordan et al., 2006, 2007). Whether these measures of early number sense in preschoolers with SB also predict individual differences and growth in their later mathematical achievement could have implications for the early identification of risk in these children. Finally, similarities in the neurocognitive correlates of mathematical performance in TD children and in those with SB suggest that effective math interventions for TD children might also hold promise for individuals with SB (see Coughlin & Montague, 2010).
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This research was supported by grants from the National Institute of Child Health and Development, P01 HD35946, Spina Bifida: Cognitive and Neurobiological Variability and R01HD046609, Longitudinal Effects of Spina Bifida on Learning, and by funding from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research and from the CIHR Institute of Human Development, Child and Youth Health. We thank Stephanie Lane, Michelle Ladd, Monica Gomez, Francis Leal, and Laura Lomax-Bream for their assistance.
Footnotes
The authors report no financial, professional, or personal conflicts of interest in the conduct of this study.
REFERENCES
- Alibali MW, DiRusso AA. The function of gesture in learning to count: More than keeping track. Cognitive Development. 1999;14:37–56. [Google Scholar]
- Ansari D, Dhital B. Age-related changes in the activation of the intraparietal sulcus during nonsymbolic magnitude processing: An event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2006;18:1820–1828. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.11.1820. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ansari D, Donlan C, Thomas MSC, Ewing SA, Peen T, Karmiloff-Smith A. What makes counting count? Verbal and visuo-spatial contributions to typical and atypical number development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2003;85:50–62. doi: 10.1016/s0022-0965(03)00026-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Assel MA, Landry SH, Swank P, Smith KE, Steelman LM. Precursors to mathematical skills: Examining the roles of visual-spatial skills, executive processes, and parenting factors. Applied Developmental Science. 2003;7:27–38. [Google Scholar]
- Ayr LK, Yeates KO, Enrile BG. Arithmetic skills and their cognitive correlates in children with acquired and congenital brain disorder. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2005;11:249–262. doi: 10.1017/S1355617705050307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnes MA, Dennis M. Reading in children and adolescents after early onset hydrocephalus and in their normally developing age-peers: Phonological analysis, word recognition, word comprehension, and passage comprehension skill. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 1992;17:445–456. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/17.4.445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnes MA, Pengelly S, Dennis M, Wilkinson M, Rogers T, Faulkner H. Mathematics skills in good readers with hydrocephalus. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2002;8:72–82. doi: 10.1017/s1355617702811079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnes MA, Smith-Chant B, Landry S. Number processing in neurodevelopmental disorders: Spina bifida myelomeningocele. In: Campbell J, editor. Handbook of mathematical cognition. Psychology Press; New York: 2005. pp. 299–314. [Google Scholar]
- Barnes MA, Wilkinson M, Khemani E, Boudesquie A, Dennis M, Fletcher JM. Arithmetic processing in children with spina bifida: Calculation accuracy, strategy use, and fact retrieval fluency. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2006;39:174–187. doi: 10.1177/00222194060390020601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berch DB. Making sense of number sense: Implications for children with mathematical disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2005;38:333–339. doi: 10.1177/00222194050380040901. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bisanz J, Sherman JL, Rasmussen C, Ho E. Development of arithmetic skills and knowledge in preschool children. In: Campbell JID, editor. Handbook of mathematical cognition. Psychology Press; New York: 2005. pp. 143–162. [Google Scholar]
- Blair C, Razza RP. Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development. 2007;78:647–663. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01019.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Briars D, Siegler RS. A featural analysis of preschoolers’ counting knowledge. Developmental Psychology. 1984;20:607–618. [Google Scholar]
- Bull R. Commentary Part II, Section III: Neuropsychological factors. In: Berch DB, Mazzocco MMM, editors. Why is math so hard for some children? The nature and origins of mathematical learning difficulties and disabilities. Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.; Baltimore: 2007. pp. 265–278. [Google Scholar]
- Bull R, Espy KA, Wiebe SA. Short-term memory, working memory, and executive functioning in preschoolers: Longitudinal predictors of mathematical achievement at 7 years. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2008;33:205–228. doi: 10.1080/87565640801982312. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butterworth B. The mathematical brain. Macmillan; London: 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Coughlin J, Montague M. The effects of cognitive strategy instruction on the mathematical problem solving of adolescents with spina bifida. Journal of Special Education. 2010 doi:10.1177/0022466910363913. [Google Scholar]
- de Jong PF, van der Leij A. Effects of phonological abilities and linguistic comprehension on the development of reading. Scientific Studies of Reading. 2002;6:51–77. [Google Scholar]
- De Smedt B, Taylor J, Archibald L, Ansari D. How is phonological processing related to individual differences in children’s arithmetic skills? Developmental Science. 2009;13:508–520. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00897.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dehaene S, Izard V, Spelke E, Pica P. Log or linear? Distinct intuitions of the number scale in western and Amazonian indigene cultures. Science. 2008;30:1217–1220. doi: 10.1126/science.1156540. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dehaene S, Piazza M, Pinel P, Cohen L. Three parietal circuits for number processing. In: Campbell JID, editor. Handbook of mathematical cognition. Psychology Press; New York: 2005. pp. 433–453. [Google Scholar]
- Dennis M, Barnes M. Math and numeracy in young adults with spina bifida and hydrocephalus. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2002;21:141–155. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN2102_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dennis M, Barnes MA. The cognitive phenotype of spina bifida meningomyelocele. Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. 2010;16:31–39. doi: 10.1002/ddrr.89. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dennis M, Fletcher JM, Rogers S, Hetherington R, Francis D. Object-based and action-based visual perception in children with spina bifida and hydrocephalus. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2002;8:95–106. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dennis M, Landry SH, Barnes M, Fletcher JM. A model of neurocognitive function in spina bifida over the life span. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2006;12:285–296. doi: 10.1017/S1355617706060371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Durand M, Hulme C, Larkin R, Snowling M. The cognitive foundations of reading and arithmetic skills in 7- to 10-year olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2005;91:113–136. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2005.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- English LH, Barnes MA, Taylor HB, Landry SH. Mathematical development in spina bifida. Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. 2009;15:28–34. doi: 10.1002/ddrr.48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischer MH. Finger counting habits modulate spatialnumerical associations. Cortex. 2008;44:386–392. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2007.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fletcher JM, Copeland K, Frederick J, Blaser SE, Kramer LA, Northrup H, Dennis M. Spinal lesion level in spina bifida meningomyelocele: A source of neural and cognitive heterogeneity. Journal of Neurosurgery. 2005;102(Suppl. 3):268–279. doi: 10.3171/ped.2005.102.3.0268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fletcher JM, Dennis M, Northrup H, Barnes MA, Hannay HJ, Landry S, Francis DJ. Spina bifida: Genes, brain, and development. In: Glidden L, editor. International review of research in mental retardation. Elsevier; San Diego, CA: 2004. pp. 63–117. [Google Scholar]
- Friedrich WN, Lovejoy MC, Shaffer J, Shurtleff DB, Beilke RL. Cognitive abilities and achievement status of children with myelomeningocele: A contemporary sample. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 1991;16:423–428. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/16.4.423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuchs LS, Geary DC, Compton DL, Fuchs D, Hamlett CL, Bryant JD. The contributions of numerosity and domain-general abilities to school readiness. Child Development. 2010;81(5):1520–1533. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01489.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geary DC, Bailey DH, Littlefield A, Wood P, Hoard MK, Nugent L. First-grade predictors of mathematical learning disability: A latent class trajectory analysis. Cognitive Development. 2009;24:411–429. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2009.10.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geary DC, Hoard MK, Hamson CO. Numerical and arithmetical cognition: Patterns of functions and deficits in children at risk for a mathematical disability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1999;74:213–239. doi: 10.1006/jecp.1999.2515. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gelman R, Galistell CR. Harvard University Press; Cambridge, MA: 1978. The child’s understanding of number. [Google Scholar]
- Ginsburg HP, Baroody AJ. Test of early mathematics ability. 2nd ed. Pro-Ed.; Austin, TX: 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Ginsburg HP, Klein A, Starkey P. The development of children’s mathematical thinking: Connecting research with practice. In: Damon W, Sigel IE, Renninger AK, editors. Handbook of child psychology: Child psychology in practice. 5th ed. Vol. 4. John Wiley & Sons Inc.; NJ: 1998. pp. 401–476. [Google Scholar]
- Hanich LB, Jordan NC, Kaplan D, Dick J. Performance across different areas of mathematical cognition in children learning difficulties. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2001;93:615–626. [Google Scholar]
- Hecht SA, Torgesen JK, Wagner RK, Rashotte CA. The relations between phonological processing abilities and emerging individual differences in mathematical computation skills: A longitudinal study from second to fifth grades. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2001;79:192–227. doi: 10.1006/jecp.2000.2586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hetherington R, Dennis M, Barnes M, Drake J, Gentili F. Functional outcome in young adults with spina bifida and hydrocephalus. Child Nervous System. 2006;22:117–124. doi: 10.1007/s00381-005-1231-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hodent C, Bryant P, Houde O. Language-specific effects on number computation in toddlers. Developmental Science. 2005;8:420–423. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00430.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hollingshead J. A four-factor index of social position. Author; New Haven, CT: 1975. [Google Scholar]
- Jordan NC, Huttenlocher J, Levine SC. Differential calculation abilities in young children from middle- and lower-income families. Developmental Psychology. 1992;28:644–653. [Google Scholar]
- Jordan NC, Huttenlocher J, Levine SC. Assessing early arithmetic abilities: Effects of verbal and nonverbal response types on the calculation performance of middle- and low-income children. Learning and Individual Differences. 1994;6:413–432. [Google Scholar]
- Jordan NC, Kaplan D, Locuniak MN, Ramineni C. Predicting first-grade math achievement from developmental number sense trajectories. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice. 2007;22:36–46. [Google Scholar]
- Jordan NC, Kaplan D, Olah LN, Locuniak MN. Number sense growth in kindergarten: A longitudinal investigation of children at risk for mathematics difficulties. Child Development. 2006;77:153–175. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00862.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kamawar D, LeFevre J-A, Bisanz J, Fast L, Skwarchuk S-L, Smith-Chant B, Penner-Wilger M. Knowledge of counting principles: How relevant is order irrelevance? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2010;105:138–145. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krajewski K, Schneider W. Exploring the impact of phonological awareness, visual-spatial working memory, and preschool quantity-number competencies on mathematics achievement in elementary school: Findings from a 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2009;103:516–531. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.03.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LeFevre J-A. Research on the development of academic skills: Introduction to the special issue on early literacy and early numeracy. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2000;54:57–60. doi: 10.1037/h0088185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LeFevre J-A, Fast L, Skwarchuk S-L, Smith-Chant BL, Bisanz J, Kamawar D, Penner-Wilger M. Pathways to mathematics: Longitudinal predictors of performance. Child Development. 2010;81(6):1753–1767. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01508.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LeFevre J-A, Smith-Chant BL, Fast L, Skwarchuk S-L, Sargla E, Arnup JS, Kamawar D. What counts as knowing? The development of conceptual and procedural knowledge of counting from kindergarten through grade 2. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2006;93:285–303. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2005.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lomax-Bream LE, Barnes M, Copeland K, Taylor HB, Landry SH. The impact of spina bifida on development across the first three years. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2007;31:1–20. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn3101_1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller KF, Smith CM, Zhu J, Zhang H. Preschool origins of cross-national differences in mathematical competence: The role of number-naming systems. Psychological Science. 1995;6:56–60. [Google Scholar]
- Mix KS, Huttenlocher J, Levine SC. Quantitative development in infancy and early childhood. Oxford University Press; New York: 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Miyake A, Friedman NP, Rettinger DA, Shah P, Hegarty M. How are visuospatial working memory, executive functioning, and spatial abilities related? A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2001;130:621–640. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.130.4.621. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noel M. Finger gnosia: A predictor of numerical abilities in children? Child Neuropsychology. 2005;11:413–430. doi: 10.1080/09297040590951550. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noles NS, Scholl BJ, Mitroff SR. The persistence of object file representations. Perception & Psychophysics. 2005;67:324–334. doi: 10.3758/bf03206495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Penner-Wilger M, Anderson ML. An alternative view of the relation between finger gnosis and math ability: Redeployment of finger representations for the representation of number. In: Love BC, McRae K, Sloutsky VM, editors. Proceedings of the 30th Annual Cognitive Science Society; Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society. 2008.pp. 1647–1652. [Google Scholar]
- Penner-Wilger M, Fast L, LeFevre J, Smith-Chant BL, Skwarchuk S, Kamawar D, Bisanz J. The foundations of numeracy: Subitizing, finger gnosia, and fine-motor ability. In: McNamara DS, Trafton JG, editors. Proceedings of the 29th Annual Cognitive Science Society; Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society. 2007.pp. 1385–1390. [Google Scholar]
- Piazza M, Mechelli A, Butterworth B, Price CJ. Are subitizing and counting implemented as separate or functionally overlapping processes? Neuroimage. 2002;15:435–446. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piazza M, Mechelli A, Price CJ, Butterworth B. Exact and approximate judgments of visual and auditory numerosity: An fMRI study. Brain Research. 2006;1106:177–188. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.05.104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raghubar KP, Barnes MA, Hecht SA. Working memory and mathematics: A review of developmental, individual difference, and cognitive approaches. Learning and Individual Differences. 2010;20:110–122. [Google Scholar]
- Raghubar K, Cirino P, Barnes M, Ewing-Cobbs L, Fletcher J, Fuchs L. Errors in multi-digit arithmetic and behavioral inattention in children with math difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2009;42:356–371. doi: 10.1177/0022219409335211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rasmussen C, Bisanz J. Representation and working memory in early arithmetic. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2005;91:137–157. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2005.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rasmussen C, Ho E, Bisanz J. Use of the mathematical principle of inversion in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2003;85:89–102. doi: 10.1016/s0022-0965(03)00031-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simmons FR, Singleton C. Do weak phonological representations impact on arithmetic development? A review of research into arithmetic and dyslexia. Dyslexia. 2008;14:77–94. doi: 10.1002/dys.341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swanson HL, Jerman O. Math disabilities: A selective meta-analysis of the literature. Review of Educational Research. 2006;76:249–274. [Google Scholar]
- Thorndike RL, Hagen EP, Sattler JM. Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale. 4th ed. Riverside; Chicago: 1989. [Google Scholar]
- Trick LM, Plyshyn ZW. Why are small and large numbers enumerated differently? A limited-capacity preattentive stage in vision. Psychological Review. 1994;10:80–102. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.101.1.80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wagner R, Torgesen JK, Rashotte CA. Comprehensive test of phonological processing. Pro-Ed.; Austin, TX: 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Wills KE. Neuropsychological functioning in children with spina bifida and/or hydrocephalus. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 1993;22:247–265. [Google Scholar]
- Woodcock RM, Johnson MB. Woodcock-Johnson Psychoeducational Battery – Revised. DLM Teaching Resources; Allen, TX: 1989. [Google Scholar]