Skip to main content
American Journal of Public Health logoLink to American Journal of Public Health
. 2011 Aug;101(8):1474–1480. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2010.300115

Negative Aspects of Close Relationships as a Predictor of Increased Body Mass Index and Waist Circumference: The Whitehall II Study

Anne Kouvonen 1,, Mai Stafford 1, Roberto De Vogli 1, Martin J Shipley 1, Michael G Marmot 1, Tom Cox 1, Jussi Vahtera 1, Ari Väänänen 1, Tarja Heponiemi 1, Archana Singh-Manoux 1, Mika Kivimäki 1
PMCID: PMC3134502  NIHMSID: NIHMS327373  PMID: 21680928

Abstract

Objectives. We investigated whether exposure to negative aspects of close relationships was associated with subsequent increase in body mass index (BMI) and waist circumference.

Methods. Data came from a prospective cohort study (Whitehall II) of 9425 civil servants aged 35 to 55 years at baseline (phase 1: 1985–1988). We assessed negative aspects of close relationships with the Close Persons Questionnaire (range 0–12) at phases 1 and 2 (1989–1990). We measured BMI and waist circumference at phases 3 (1991–1994) and 5 (1997–1999). Covariates at phase 1 included gender, age, marital status, ethnicity, BMI, employment grade, smoking, physical activity, fruit and vegetable consumption, and common mental disorder.

Results. After adjustment for sociodemographic characteristics and health behaviors, participants with higher exposure to negative aspects of close relationships had a higher likelihood of a 10% or greater increase in BMI and waist circumference (odds ratios per 1-unit increase 1.08 [95% confidence interval (CI) = 1.02, 1.14; P = .007] and 1.09 [CI = 1.04, 1.14; P ≤ .001], respectively) as well as a transition from the overweight (25 ≤ BMI < 30) to the obese (BMI ≥ 30) category.

Conclusions. Adverse social relationships may contribute to weight gain.


Obesity is a major public health concern because it is associated with numerous ill health conditions such as type 2 diabetes, coronary heart disease, hypertension, stroke, and certain forms of cancer.1 Obesity rates have rapidly increased to epidemic proportions. In England, for example, 24% of men and 25% of women are obese (body mass index [BMI, defined as weight in kg divided by height in m2] ≥ 30).2 The interplay among multiple factors—genetic factors, factors stemming from obesogenic environments, and individual, and cultural factors—is seen to be behind the obesity epidemic.3 However, increasing evidence suggests that social relationships may also play a role in determining weight gain.

Stress associated with poor-quality relationships may contribute to weight gain via various mechanisms. Negative aspects of close relationships may induce negative feelings,4 which can increase physiological arousal either through activation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis or through the fight-or-flight response and the secretion of adrenal medullary hormones.5 Eating high-fat and high-carbohydrate caloric content “comfort food” may reduce biological stress system activity and the concomitant negative emotions.6 Some evidence also suggests an association between chronic life stress and a greater preference for energy- and nutrient-dense foods, namely those that are high in fat and sugar.7 In addition, there may be further effects via other unhealthy coping mechanisms such as physical inactivity.

Childhood adversities related to close relationships, such as physical abuse, verbal abuse, humiliation, neglect, strict upbringing, physical punishment, conflict, or tension, have been associated with an increased risk of obesity in adulthood.8 However, limited and somewhat inconsistent evidence exists on the impact of negative aspects of close relationships in adulthood. One study revealed that heavier women had lower quality romantic relationships.9 Poor marital quality has also been associated with a higher risk of metabolic syndrome10 and obesity11 in women. Strain in relations with family but not with one's spouse or partner was associated with weight gain in women with high initial BMI.12 In addition, some studies have revealed an association between reports of insufficient social support and increased risk of obesity,13,14 but other studies suggest no such association.15

With a few exceptions,1012 the evidence is cross-sectional or derived from short follow-ups. Such data leave open the possibility of reverse causality (i.e., obesity negatively influencing close relationships). Because the development of obesity has a relatively long induction period, it is plausible that prolonged exposure to problems in social relationships affects weight more than do short-term problems. Moreover, it might be more informative to look at weight gain rather than obesity status at 1 time point. We are not aware of previous studies examining the association between negative aspects of close relationships and weight gain. In addition, most of the earlier studies did not assess waist circumference, a measure of central obesity. Waist circumference is probably a better indicator of health risk than is BMI alone, especially when used in combination with BMI.16

In this study using data from the Whitehall II cohort of British civil servants, we investigated the extent to which exposure to negative aspects of close relationships was associated with subsequent weight gain, as indicated by increase in BMI and waist circumference over a long follow-up period.

METHODS

The target population of the Whitehall II Study was all office staff based in London, United Kingdom, in 20 civil service departments in 1985. The baseline cohort included 6895 men and 3413 women (age range 35–55 years; response rate 73%). Full details on study design and measures are reported elsewhere.17 Briefly, negative aspects of close relationships were assessed at phase 1 (1985–1988) and phase 2 (1989–1990). We measured change in BMI and waist circumference between phase 3 (1991–1994) and phase 5 (1997–1999). Baseline covariates in our analysis are drawn from phase 1. Phases 1, 3 (n = 8815; 86% of phase 1 respondents), and 5 (n = 7870; 76% of phase 1 respondents) included a clinical examination and a questionnaire, whereas phase 2 (n = 8132; 79% of phase 1 respondents) included only a questionnaire. Phase 4 data did not include relevant variables, and therefore we did not use them in the study. The median length of the follow-up from phase 1 to phase 5 was 11.2 years.

Negative Aspects of Close Relationships

We assessed negative aspects of close relationships at phases 1 and 2 with a 4-item scale from the Close Persons Questionnaire.18 The questions refer to adverse exchanges and conflict within a relationship nominated by the respondents as their closest. The items are as follows: “How much in the last 12 months did this person give you worries, problems, and stress?”; “How much in the last 12 months would you have liked to have confided more in this person?”; “How much in the last 12 months did talking to this person make things worse?”; “How much in the last 12 months would you have liked more practical help with major things from this person?” We evaluated each of the 4 items on a Likert scale from 1 to 4, with higher scores indicating more negative aspects. We summed the Likert-scaled responses for the items. The Cronbach α was 0.63.18 At phase 1, 7384 participants completed the Close Persons Questionnaire. We asked only 74% of respondents to complete it because this measure was introduced after the start of the baseline survey. At phase 2, 7727 participants completed the questionnaire. Correlation coefficients of scores at phases 1 and 2 suggest a moderate degree of consistency (r  = 0.48; P < .001). Although the questionnaire assesses social relationships relative to a maximum of 4 close relationships, our analyses, similar to previous studies in the Whitehall II data, focused on the first close relationship only, for which the reliability was the highest.18

The reliability and validity of the Close Persons Questionnaire was examined in a previous study.18 A retest reliability study over a 4-week interval showed moderately high agreement for negative aspects of close relationships (r  = 0.72). To evaluate validity, the questionnaire was sent to the person closest to each of the last 60 interviewees who nominated a close relationship. Response from the person designated as the close relationship showed correlation with that reported by the participant (r = 0.65 for female spouse and r = 0.40 for male spouse).

Outcome Variables

Screenings at phases 3 and 5 included the measurement of height, weight, and waist circumference. BMI was calculated according to weight (kg) and height (m) assessed using standard protocols at the medical examination. Waist circumference was measured using a fiberglass tape measure at 600 g tension as the smallest circumference at or below the costal margin.19 Test–retest reliability of the waist circumference measurement during 1 month, in 490 participants, was 0.96 at the phase 3 clinical examination.

Covariates

We included several factors that have been associated with obesity or weight gain2022 in the analysis as covariates. We assessed all covariates at phase 1. Age, gender, ethnicity (White vs non-White), marital status, and BMI were measured. We derived employment grade from a questionnaire asking details about job title and job characteristics. As in earlier studies in the Whitehall II cohort, the hierarchy of employment grades consisted of 3 levels (administrative, professional or executive, and clerical) according to salary, work role, and occupational seniority.

Health behaviors included self-reported smoking status (never smoker, ex-smoker, current smoker), daily fruit and vegetable consumption (yes vs no), weekly moderate physical activity hours, and weekly vigorous physical activity hours. Physical activity was assessed with a standardized instrument. Participants were asked the average number of hours per week spent in “moderately energetic” (e.g., dancing, cycling, leisurely swimming) and “vigorous” (e.g., running, hard swimming, playing squash) physical activity.23

The General Health Questionnaire-30 is a self-administered, well-established screening instrument designed for community settings.24 It assesses common mental disorders such as depression and anxiety. As in previous studies, those with a total score of 5 or greater were defined as cases, and those scoring 0 to 4 as noncases.25 The threshold scores are set to correspond to a case definition equivalent to that of the average patient referred to a psychiatrist.26 In the Whitehall II Study, General Health Questionnaire “caseness” was validated against a clinical interview schedule; the sensitivity (73%) and specificity (78%) measures indicate that the definition of caseness is acceptable.27

Statistical Analysis

The complete case analyses of our study included 3703 (analyses on BMI increase) and 3224 (analyses on waist circumference increase) participants with no missing data on any of the study variables. The median length of the follow-up from phase 1 to phase 5 was 11.2 years; 273 individuals died during this period.

To explore potential selection bias resulting from missing data, we ran a subsidiary analysis in which we used multiple multivariate imputation28 using negative aspects of close relationships at phases 1 and 2; BMI at phases 1, 3, and 5; waist circumference at phases 3 and 5; and all covariates at phase 1 (age, gender, ethnicity, marital status, employment grade, BMI, smoking, fruit and vegetable consumption, moderate and vigorous physical activity, and common mental disorder) to impute values for missing values for measures on the 9425 participants with at least 1 measurement of negative aspects of close relationships. We used switching regression in Stata version 11.0 (StataCorp LP, College Station, TX) as described by Royston28 and carried out 10 cycles of regression switching and generated 10 imputation data sets. The multiple multivariate imputation approach creates numerous copies of the data (10 copies in this case) in which the missing values are imputed with an appropriate level of randomness by using chained equations. The estimates are obtained by averaging across the results from each of these 10 data sets using Rubin's rules.28 The procedure takes into account the uncertainty in the imputation as well as the uncertainty resulting from random variation, as undertaken in all multivariable analyses.

We used binary logistic regression to examine whether exposure to negative aspects of close relationships was associated with a 10% or greater increase in BMI and waist circumference in complete cases and in the imputed data set. The 10% change has been used in Whitehall II studies to assess meaningful change over time29; this categorization for BMI and waist circumference also provides sufficiently large groups for well-powered analyses. However, to ensure that our findings are robust and not attributable to a specific cutoff point, we ran sensitivity analyses. We repeated the analyses using 7.5% or greater and 15% or greater increases in BMI as outcomes in the complete case sample.

We ran separate analyses for negative aspects of close relationships at phases 1 and 2 as well as for the phase 1–phase 2 mean score. We used multinomial logistic regression to determine whether exposure to negative aspects of close relationships predicted transitions between BMI categories between phases 3 and 5. We used the models to assess the likelihood of

  1. recommended healthy weight (18.5 ≤ BMI < 25) at phases 3 and 5 (referent);

  2. from recommended healthy weight at phase 3 to overweight (25 ≤ BMI < 30) or obese (BMI ≥ 30) at phase 5;

  3. from overweight at phase 3 to recommended healthy weight at phase 5 or from obese at phase 3 to overweight or recommended healthy weight at phase 5;

  4. overweight at both phases or obese at both phases; and

  5. from overweight at phase 3 to obese at phase 5. Underweight participants, that is, those with a BMI of less than 18.5 at phase 3 or phase 5 were excluded from this analysis (n = 43 among complete cases).

In these analyses, we conducted adjustment for covariates in 2 steps to distinguish the different types of confounders and to assess potential pathways. First, we adjusted the association of adverse close relationships and weight gain for age, gender, ethnicity, and marital status. Second, we added employment grade, BMI at baseline, health behaviors (smoking, moderate and vigorous physical activity, and fruit and vegetable consumption), and common mental disorder to the model. We considered all P values (2-tailed) < .05 to be statistically significant.

There were no consistent differences in our results between men and women, so we pooled and gender-adjusted the data. We performed analyses using SAS version 9.2 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC) and Stata version 11.0 (StataCorp LP, College Station, TX).

RESULTS

Table 1 shows the characteristics of the total Whitehall II baseline cohort, the participants with complete data on all study variables, and the imputed sample. Any differences in baseline characteristics were small, although differences were greater between complete cases and the baseline cohort than between the imputed sample and the baseline cohort.

TABLE 1.

Characteristics of the Baseline Cohort, Study Participants With Complete Data, and the Imputed Sample: The Whitehall II Study, London, UK, 1985–1999

Baseline Cohort (Maximum n = 10 308)
Complete Cases (n = 3703)a
Imputed Sample (n = 9425)b
Baseline Characteristic No. % or Mean (SD) No. % or Mean (SD) Mean No. % or Mean (SD)
Gender
    Women 3413 33.1 1043 28.2 3039 32.2
    Men 6895 66.9 2660 71.2 6386 67.8
Age, y 10 308 44.4 (6.9) 3703 44.3 (5.9) 9425 44.4 (6.0)
Ethnicity
    White 9181 89.1 3403 91.9 8510 90.3
    Non-White 1127 10.9 300 8.1 915 9.7
Marital status
    Married or cohabiting 7608 74.1 2879 77.7 7086 75.2
    Other 2662 25.9 824 22.3 2339 24.8
Occupational position
    Highest grade 3028 29.4 1162 31.4 2821 29.9
    Intermediate grade 4943 50.0 1950 52.7 4582 48.6
    Lowest grade 2337 22.6 591 15.9 2022 21.5
Smoking status
    Never 5062 49.5 1929 52.1 4650 48.6
    Ex-smoker 3274 32.0 1247 33.7 3046 32.6
    Current 1883 18.4 527 14.2 1657 17.7
Moderate physical activity, hr/wk 9929 2.9 (3.5) 3703 3.0 (3.5) 9425 2.9 (3.5)
Vigorous physical activity, hr/wk 9919 1.0 (1.9) 3703 1.0 (1.8) 9425 1.0 (1.9)
Fruit and vegetable consumption
    Daily 5998 58.4 2107 56.9 5480 58.1
    Not daily 2744 41.6 1596 43.1 3945 41.9
Common mental disorder
    No 7445 73.1 2711 73.2 6827 73.0
    Yes 2744 26.9 992 26.8 2521 27.0
Negative aspects of close relationships 7384 2.8 (2.4) 3703 2.8 (2.3) 9425 2.8 (2.4)
Negative aspects of close relationships (repeated) 7727 2.8 (2.3) 3231 2.9 (2.3) 9425 2.9 (2.4)
a

Participants with no missing data on exposure, outcome, or covariate variables.

b

Multiple imputed sample of participants with at least 1 measurement of negative aspects of close relationships at phase 1 or 2.

Tables 2 and 3 display the odds ratios (ORs) of a 10% or greater increase in BMI and waist circumference by exposure to negative aspects of close relationships (score range 0–12). In complete cases, a higher exposure to negative aspects of close relationships at phase 1 was associated with an increase in BMI (per 1-unit increase in the negative aspects score OR = 1.06 [95% confidence interval (CI) = 1.02, 1.10; P = .007]), and a higher exposure to negative aspects of close relationships at phase 1 was also associated with an increase in waist circumference (OR = 1.06 [95% CI = 1.02, 1.10; P = .002]). However, we saw the strongest association with both of these outcomes for the phase 1–phase 2 mean score of negative aspects of close relationships (OR = 1.08 [95% CI = 1.02, 1.14; P = .007] and OR = 1.08 [95% CI = 1.03, 1.13; P = .001], respectively). Additional adjustment for baseline covariates affected these estimates very little. In the imputed sample, we found a similar pattern of results, except that the effect size was slightly smaller (e.g., the OR for mean score and waist circumference was 1.04 [95% CI = 1.00, 1.07; P = .03]).

TABLE 2.

Associations of Negative Aspects of Close Relationships at Phases 1 and 2 With ≥ 10% Increase in Body Mass Index (BMI) Between Phases 3 and 5 in Complete Cases and Imputed Sample: The Whitehall II Study, London, UK, 1985–1999

Model 1a
Model 2b
Exposure to Negative Aspects of Close Relationships (Range 0–12) No. (Cases) OR (95% CI) for ≥ 10% Increase in BMI P OR (95% CI) for ≥ 10% increase in BMI P
Complete cases
    Phase 1 score 3703 (436) 1.06 (1.02, 1.10) .007 1.06 (1.02, 1.11) .008
    Phase 2 score 3231 (372) 1.04 (0.99, 1.09) .1 1.04 (0.99, 1.09) .11
    Mean score 3231 (372) 1.08 (1.02, 1.14) .007 1.08 (1.02, 1.14) .007
Imputed sample
    Phase 1 score 9425 (1376) 1.03 (1.00, 1.07) .04 1.03 (1.00, 1.07) .06
    Phase 2 score 9425 (1376) 1.02 (0.99, 1.06) .22 1.02 (0.98, 1.06) .27
    Mean score 9425 (1376) 1.04 (1.00, 1.08) .07 1.04 (0.99, 1.08) .09

Note. CI = confidence interval; OR = odds ratio. BMI was calculated by dividing weight in kilograms by height in meters squared.

a

Adjusted for gender, age, marital status, and ethnicity.

b

Additionally adjusted for baseline BMI, employment grade, smoking status, moderate physical activity, vigorous physical activity, daily fruit and vegetable consumption, and common mental disorder.

TABLE 3.

Associations of Negative Aspects of Close Relationships at Phases 1 and 2 With ≥ 10% Increase in Waist Circumference Between Phases 3 and 5 in Complete Cases and Imputed Sample: The Whitehall II Study, London, UK, 1985–1999

Model 1a
Model 2b
Exposure to Negative Aspects of Close Relationships (Range 0–12) No. (Cases) OR (95% CI) for ≥ 10% Increase in Waist Circumference P OR (95% CI) for ≥ 10% Increase in Waist Circumference P
Complete cases
    Phase 1 score 3224 (807) 1.06 (1.02, 1.10) .002 1.06 (1.02, 1.10) .001
    Phase 2 score 2814 (698) 1.05 (1.01, 1.09) .01 1.05 (1.01, 1.10) .009
    Mean score 2814 (698) 1.08 (1.03, 1.13) .001 1.09 (1.04, 1.14) ≤ .001
Imputed sample
    Phase 1 score 9425 (2629) 1.02 (0.99, 1.05) .14 1.02 (0.99, 1.06) .15
    Phase 2 score 9425 (2629) 1.03 (1.00, 1.05) .01 1.03 (1.01, 1.05) .01
    Mean score 9425 (2629) 1.04 (1.00, 1.07) .02 1.04 (1.00, 1.07) .03

Note. CI = confidence interval; OR = odds ratio.

a

Adjusted for gender, age, marital status, and ethnicity.

b

Additionally adjusted for baseline BMI, employment grade, smoking status, moderate physical activity, vigorous physical activity, daily fruit and vegetable consumption, and common mental disorder.

We repeated the analyses using 7.5% or greater and 15% or greater increases in BMI as outcomes in the complete case sample. The results were very similar to those with a 10% or greater increase. For the phase 1 negative aspects of close relationships, OR = 1.04 (95% CI = 1.00, 1.07; P = .03) for a 7.5% or greater increase, and OR = 1.08 (95% CI = 1.00, 1.16; P = .04) for a 15% or greater increase (data not shown).

Table 4 summarizes the results from multinomial logistic regression analyses on the associations between negative aspects of close relationships and changes in BMI. Participants with high negative aspects of close relationships were more likely to experience a transition from overweight to obese BMI category than stay in the recommended healthy weight category throughout the study period compared with those who did not report negative aspects of close relationships (OR = 1.05; 95% CI = 1.00, 1.10 in the imputed sample). By contrast, a higher exposure to negative aspects of close relationships was not associated with transition from recommended weight to overweight or obesity; nor was the lack of negative close relationships associated with weight reduction among obese and overweight participants.

TABLE 4.

Multinomial Logistic Regression Models: The Whitehall II Study, London, UK, 1985–1999

Exposure to Negative Aspects of Close Relationships Recommended Healthy Weight at Phase 3 to Overweight or Obese at Phase 5 Overweight at Phase 3 to Recommended Healthy Weight at Phase 5 or Obese at Phase 3 to Overweight or Recommended Healthy Weight at Phase 5 Overweight at Both Phases or Obese at Both Phases Overweight at Phase 3 to Obese at Phase 5
Complete cases
    Mean score, OR (95% CI) 1.02 (0.97, 1.08) 0.96 (0.86, 1.07) 1.00 (0.96, 1.04) 1.06 (0.98, 1.14)
    P (No.) .4 (431) .46 (106) .95 (1218) .13 (205)
Imputed sample
    Mean score, OR (95% CI) 1.02 (0.99, 1.06) 1.00 (0.94, 1.07) 1.01 (0.99, 1.04) 1.05 (1.00, 1.10)
    P (No.) .22 (1349) .96 (389) .32 (3659) .04 (573)

Note. CI = confidence interval; OR = odds ratio. Body mass index (BMI) calculated by dividing weight in kilograms by height in meters squared. Recommended healthy weight (18.5 ≤ BMI < 25) at both phases (n = 1233 in complete cases and n = 3333 in imputed sample) was used as a reference category. Overweight was defined as 25 ≤ BMI < 30; obese was defined as a BMI≥30. Those considered underweight (BMI < 18.5 at phase 3 or phase 5) were excluded from the analysis (n = 43 in complete cases). Adjusted for gender, age, marital status, and ethnicity.

DISCUSSION

This prospective study suggests that negative interactions in close relationships may, albeit modestly, contribute to increases in BMI and waist circumference. Sociodemographic characteristics, health behaviors, and common mental disorders did not account for these effects. Analyses in repeat data indicated that a long-term exposure to negative aspects (indicated by mean score across 2 study phases) had a slightly stronger effect on weight gain than did a single measurement of the exposure.

Our results are in line with previous studies that have suggested a link between poor relationship quality or insufficient social support and obesity; however, these studies are limited because they did not specifically measure negative aspects of close relationships, did not assess cumulative exposure, and did not measure weight gain.9,10,13,14 By contrast, in a previous longitudinal study strain in relations with a spouse or partner was not associated with weight gain.12

Potential explanations for the associations between negative aspects of close relationships and weight gain involve neuroendocrine effects of chronic stress via psychological processes as well as behavioral effects, or both.18 More specifically, the presence of negative aspects of close relationships can induce psychological processes that are linked to negative appraisals and emotions or low mood. Dysfunctional social relationships may provoke negative feelings,4 which can increase physiological arousal.5 Marital strain has been shown to have deleterious effects on cardiovascular, endocrine, and immune functions.30 Dallman et al.6 proposed that people might eat high-fat and -carbohydrate caloric content “comfort food” in an attempt to reduce activity in the corticotrophin-releasing factor-driven central chronic stress response network with its attendant anxiety. Chronic life stress has been associated with a greater preference for energy-dense foods,7 possibly leading to weight gain in those experiencing chronic stress.31

Furthermore, there may be effects via health behaviors and adherence to medical regimens. For example, the individual may use unhealthy eating and a physically inactive lifestyle as adverse coping mechanisms. Psychological and behavioral pathways can also influence each other.32 However, in this study an adjustment for health behaviors had little effect on estimates, suggesting that the association between negative aspects of close relationships and weight change may be explained primarily by mechanisms other than those related to health behaviors.

The strengths of this study include the assessment of repeated exposure to negative aspects of close relationships and simultaneous inclusion of numerous covariates. Our study is derived from a large well-characterized cohort of British employees and a prospective study design with a median follow-up of 11.2 years. A further strength is that weight, height, and waist circumference were directly measured at both examinations (phases 3 and 5) and were not derived from questionnaires, thus minimizing the potential of recall bias and misclassification that occur when using self-reports.

However, several limitations need to be considered when interpreting the findings. First, our measure of negative aspects of close relationships was self-reported and may thus be influenced by personality traits or specific characteristics of respondents.18 For example, levels of social support are lower than normal in hostile individuals as the result of less-effective coping strategies in psychosocial stress situations, increasing the likelihood of breakdown of intimate relationships and unhealthy lifestyle.33,34 However, the subjective experience is exactly what gives meaning and significance to social environmental characteristics, and these subjective experiences finally get under one's skin or cause adverse behavioral changes. Therefore, self-rated measures, such as those used here, are relevant indicators of social relationships, expressly because of their subjectivity.

Second, our complete case sample included less than half of the original cohort. Loss to follow-up is inevitable in all long-term prospective studies and may lead to biased estimates. We examined potential bias by performing subsidiary analyses with imputed data sets. These analyses suggested that incompleteness of data might have contributed to an overestimation rather than an underestimation of the association between negative aspects of close relationships and weight gain. This finding is important because sample attrition in prospective studies is often speculated to attenuate the effect estimates.

Third, although we adjusted for numerous possible confounders the possibility of residual confounding cannot be excluded in observational studies. For example, information was not available on childhood factors35 or individual differences in genetic predisposition.36 Finally, the participants were mostly White, middle-aged civil servants based in the southeast of England, limiting the generalizability of our findings. Thus, more diverse samples are needed to extend the validity of our findings.

Despite these limitations, our results suggest that exposure to negative aspects of close relationships is associated with an increased risk of weight gain. The study adds to the evidence that the development of obesity may be related to the social environment in which people live. Future research is needed to study the specific biological, behavioral, and psychological mechanisms linking social environmental factors to weight gain and whether interventions designed to improve social relationships could decrease obesity risk.

Acknowledgments

The Whitehall II Study was supported by grants from the Medical Research Council (grant G0902037); British Heart Foundation (grant RG/07/008/23674); National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (grant R01HL036310); Health and Safety Executive, UK; National Institutes of Health (NIH); National Institute on Aging (grants R01AG013196 and R01AG034454); Agency for Health Care Policy Research (grant HS06516); and the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation Research Networks on Successful Midlife Development and Socioeconomic Status and Health. A. Kouvonen and T. Cox were supported by a grant from the Economic and Social Research Council (grant RES-000-22-3489). T. Cox was also supported by grants from the Health and Safety Executive, UK; Sheffield Hallam University, UK; Babcock Networks, UK; and the European Agency for Health and Safety at Work. R. De Vogli was partially supported by the Economic and Social Research Council (grant RES-070-27-0034). M. J. Shipley was supported by a grant from the British Heart Foundation. M. G. Marmot was supported by a MRC Research Professorship. J. Vahtera and M. Kivimäki were supported by the Academy of Finland (projects 124322, 124271, and 132944). M. Kivimäki was also supported by the EU New OSH ERA research programme; the BUPA Foundation, UK (grant 22094477); and the NIH (grants R01HL036310 and R01AG034454). A. Väänänen was supported by the Finnish Work Environment Fund (project 109395) and the Academy of Finland (project 128089). T. Heponiemi was supported by the Finnish Work Environment Fund (project 107154) and the Academy of Finland (project 128002). A. Singh-Manoux was supported by a “European Young Investigator Award” from the European Science Foundation and the National Institute on Aging, NIH (grants R01AG013196 and R01AG034454).

We thank all participating civil service departments and their welfare personnel and establishment officers; the Occupational Health and Safety Agency; the Council of Civil Service Unions; all participating civil servants in the Whitehall II Study; and all members of the Whitehall II Study team. The Whitehall II Study team comprises research scientists, statisticians, study coordinators, nurses, data managers, administrative assistants, and data entry staff who made the study possible.

Note. The researchers are all independent of the funders.

Human Participant Protection

We obtained ethical approval for the Whitehall II Study from the University College London, Medical School Committee on the ethics of human research. We gained written informed consent from all participants.

References

  • 1.James WP. WHO recognition of the global obesity epidemic. Int J Obes (Lond). 2008;32(suppl 7):S120–S126 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.The Health and Social Care Information Centre Health Survey for England–2008: physical activity and fitness. Summary of key findings. Available at: http://www.ic.nhs.uk/pubs/hse08physicalactivity. Accessed March 21, 2011
  • 3.Apovian CM. The causes, prevalence, and treatment of obesity revisited in 2009: what have we learned so far? Am J Clin Nutr. 2010;91(1):277S–279S [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Vinokur AD, van Ryn M. Social support and undermining in close relationships: their independent effects on the mental health of unemployed persons. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1993;65(2):350–359 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Seeman TE. Social ties and health: the benefits of social integration. Ann Epidemiol. 1996;6(5):442–451 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Dallman MF, Pecoraro N, Akana SF, et al. Chronic stress and obesity: a new view of “comfort food.” Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100(20):11696–11701 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Torres SJ, Nowson CA. Relationship between stress, eating behavior, and obesity. Nutrition. 2007;23(11–12):887–894 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Thomas C, Hyppönen E, Power C. Obesity and type 2 diabetes risk in midadult life: the role of childhood adversity. Pediatrics. 2008;121(5):e1240–e1249 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Boyes AD, Latner JD. Weight stigma in existing romantic relationships. J Sex Marital Ther. 2009;35(4):282–293 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Troxel WM, Matthews KA, Gallo LC, Kuller LH. Marital quality and occurrence of the metabolic syndrome in women. Arch Intern Med. 2005;165(9):1022–1027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Gallo LC, Troxel WM, Matthews KA, Kuller LH. Marital status and quality in middle-aged women: associations with levels and trajectories of cardiovascular risk factors. Health Psychol. 2003;22(5):453–463 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Block JP, He Y, Zaslavsky AM, Ding L, Ayanian JZ. Psychosocial stress and change in weight among US adults. Am J Epidemiol. 2009;170(2):181–192 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Cadzow RB, Servoss TJ. The association between perceived social support and health among patients at a free urban clinic. J Natl Med Assoc. 2009;101(3):243–250 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Strine TW, Chapman DP, Balluz L, Mokdad AH. Health-related quality of life and health behaviors by social and emotional support. Their relevance to psychiatry and medicine. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2008;43(2):151–159 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Poortinga W. Perceptions of the environment, physical activity, and obesity. Soc Sci Med. 2006;63(11):2835–2846 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Pischon T. Commentary: use of the body mass index to assess the risk of health outcomes: time to say goodbye? Int J Epidemiol. 2010;39(2):528–529 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Marmot MG, Smith GD, Stansfeld S, et al. Health inequalities among civil servants: the Whitehall II Study. Lancet. 1991;337(8754):1387–1393 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Stansfeld S, Marmot M. Deriving a survey measure of social support: the reliability and validity of the Close Persons Questionnaire. Soc Sci Med. 1992;35(8):1027–1035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Brunner EJ, Chandola T, Marmot MG. Prospective effect of job strain on general and central obesity in the Whitehall II Study. Am J Epidemiol. 2007;165(7):828–837 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Kimokoti RW, Newby PK, Gona P, et al. Diet quality, physical activity, smoking status, and weight fluctuation are associated with weight change in women and men. J Nutr. 2010;140(7):1287–1293 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.McLaren L. Socioeconomic status and obesity. Epidemiol Rev. 2007;29:29–48 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Baltrus PT, Lynch JW, Everson-Rose S, Raghunathan TE, Kaplan GA. Race/ethnicity, life-course socioeconomic position, and body weight trajectories over 34 years: the Alameda County Study. Am J Public Health. 2005;95(9):1595–1601 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Gimeno D, Elovainio M, Jokela M, De Vogli R, Marmot MG, Kivimäki M. Association between passive jobs and low levels of leisure-time physical activity: the Whitehall II cohort study. Occup Environ Med. 2009;66(11):772–776 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Goldberg D. The Detection of Psychiatric Illness by Questionnaire. London, England: Oxford University Press; 1972 [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Kivimäki M, Lawlor DA, Singh-Manoux A, et al. Common mental disorder and obesity: insight from four repeat measures over 19 years: prospective Whitehall II cohort study. BMJ. 2009;339:b3765. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Goldberg DP. Manual of the General Health Questionnaire. Windsor, England: NFER Publishing; 1978 [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Stansfeld SA, Marmot MG. Social class and minor psychiatric disorder in British civil servants: a validated screening survey using the General Health Questionnaire. Psychol Med. 1992;22(3):739–749 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Royston P. Multiple imputation of missing values. Stata J. 2004;4:227–241 [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Head J, Kivimäki M, Martikainen P, Vahtera J, Ferrie JE, Marmot MG. Influence of change in psychosocial work characteristics on sickness absence: the Whitehall II Study. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2006;60(1):55–61 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Robles TF, Kiecolt-Glaser JK. The physiology of marriage: pathways to health. Physiol Behav. 2003;79(3):409–416 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Wardle J, Gibson EL. Impact of stress on diet: processes and implications. : Stansfeld S, Marmot M, Stress and the Heart. Psychosocial Pathways to Coronary Heart Disease. London: BMJ Books; 2002:124–149 [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Uchino BN. Social support and health: a review of physiological processes potentially underlying links to disease outcomes. J Behav Med. 2006;29(4):377–387 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Miller TQ, Markides KS, Chiriboga DA, Ray LA. A test of the psychosocial vulnerability model of hostility: results from an 11-year follow-up study. Psychosom Med. 1995;57(6):572–581 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Miller TQ, Smith TW, Turner CW, Guijarro ML, Hallet AJ. A meta-analytic review of research on hostility and physical health. Psychol Bull. 1996;119(2):322–348 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Korkeila K, Korkeila J, Vahtera J, et al. Childhood adversities, adult risk factors and depressiveness: a population study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2005;40(9):700–706 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Way BM, Taylor SE. Social influences on health: is serotonin a critical mediator? Psychosom Med. 2010;72(2):107–112 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from American Journal of Public Health are provided here courtesy of American Public Health Association

RESOURCES