Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2011 Jul 13;6(7):e22144. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0022144

Polymorphism of the Tryptophan Hydroxylase 2 (TPH2) Gene Is Associated with Chimpanzee Neuroticism

Kyung-Won Hong 1,¤,#, Alexander Weiss 2,#, Naruki Morimura 3, Toshifumi Udono 4, Ikuo Hayasaka 4, Tatyana Humle 5, Yuichi Murayama 6, Shin'ichi Ito 7, Miho Inoue-Murayama 3,*
Editor: Sharon Gursky-Doyen8
PMCID: PMC3135609  PMID: 21765945

Abstract

In the brain, serotonin production is controlled by tryptophan hydroxylase 2 (TPH2), a genotype. Previous studies found that mutations on the TPH2 locus in humans were associated with depression and studies of mice and studies of rhesus macaques have shown that the TPH2 locus was involved with aggressive behavior. We previously reported a functional single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) in the form of an amino acid substitution, Q468R, in the chimpanzee TPH2 gene coding region. In the present study we tested whether this SNP was associated with neuroticism in captive and wild-born chimpanzees living in Japan and Guinea, respectively. Even after correcting for multiple tests (Bonferroni p = 0.05/6 = 0.008), Q468R was significantly related to higher neuroticism (β = 0.372, p = 0.005). This study is the first to identify a genotype linked to a personality trait in chimpanzees. In light of the prior studies on humans, mice, and rhesus macaques, these findings suggest that the relationship between neuroticism and TPH2 has deep phylogenetic roots.

Introduction

Serotonin (5-HT) production is mediated by the rate-limiting enzyme tryptophan hydroxylase (TPH) [1], and TPH2 is preferentially located in the dorsal raphe region of the brain [2] and in the peripheral myenteric neurons of the small intestine [3]. A loss of function mutation (R441H) in human TPH2 was identified in patients with major depression [4] and a loss-of-function mutation (P447R) in mice was associated with significantly reduced aggressive behavior [5]. Also, psychiatric disorders such as bipolar disorder [6], attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder [7], and suicidality [8], have been associated with TPH2 gene polymorphisms. Chen et al. (2010) also reported a functional polymorphism in the 3′ untranslated region of the TPH2 gene in rhesus macaques, which was related to differential hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis functioning, and, among peer-reared infants, was associated with the aggressive behavior [9].

The role of the serotonergic system in neuroticism, depression, anxiety-related traits, and disorders has been extensively studied [10], [11], [12], [13], [14], [15]. The serotonin transporter gene 5-HTT is the primary target of the most widely used class of psychiatric drugs, the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) [12]. A functional polymorphism referred to as the 5-hydroxytryptamine-linked polymorphic region (5-HTTLPR) is present in the regulatory region of 5-HTT gene [12]. The short allele (S) of this gene is transcribed less efficiently than the long allele (L), resulting in decreased 5-HTT expression [12]. Lesch et al. found that 5-HTTLPR was significantly associated with anxiety-related traits: individuals with the SS or SL genotype presented higher neuroticism scores than those with the LL genotype. Several studies [16], [17], [18], [19] have replicated these results, although studies of the general population failed to demonstrate an association [20], [21], [22], [23], [24], [25], [26].

Previous studies indicated that chimpanzee personality can be defined by five personality domains analogous to those of humans (neuroticism, extraversion, openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness) and a broad, chimpanzee-specific domain labeled dominance [27], [28], [29]. These personality dimensions are reliable across raters [27], [28], [29]. Moreover, subsequent studies have validated these dimensions by demonstrating their relationship with subjective well-being [30], observed behaviors [31], and neuroanatomy [32].

In a prior study, we found that the 5-HTTLPR gene in chimpanzees was monomorphic [33]. However, in another study we found a gain-of-function single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) in chimpanzees at the 1,404th position of the tryptophan hydroxylase 2 (TPH2) gene - the adenine (A) nucleotide was substituted with guanine (G) [34]. This substitution results in the replacement of the 468th glutamine (CAG, ch468Q) with arginine (CGG, ch468R). An enzyme activity assay of these genotypes indicated that the capacity of L-5-hydroxytrytophan (serotonin) biosynthesis was significantly higher for ch468R than ch468Q [34].

In the present study we examined the association between the Q468R polymorphism and neuroticism in chimpanzees. Like the short version of the human 5-HTTLPR gene which is related to human neuroticism [12], ch468R might be related to less efficient serotonin recovery at the synapse. Thus, we predicted that the ch468R allele would be associated with higher levels of neuroticism in chimpanzees.

Methods

Samples

The sample consisted of 57 chimpanzees. Of these chimpanzees, 21 were cared for at the Chimpanzee Conservation Center, Guinea, West Africa. These individuals were wild-born orphans, rescued from the illegal pet trade, and in the process of being rehabilitated. Of the 36 remaining chimpanzees, 26 lived in Chimpanzee Sanctuary Uto [35], 5 lived in 3 Japanese zoos, and 5 lived in the Kyoto University Primate Research Institute (see Table 1 for details about the age and sex ratio). For the Guinea sample, DNA was obtained non-invasively via fecal samples. For the Chimpanzee Sanctuary Uto sample, DNA was obtained via blood samples. To minimize suffering, the blood samples were not collected for the purpose of the present study, but as part of routine health examinations. During these examinations chimpanzees were sedated with oral midazolam (1 mg/kg) or droperidol (0.2 mg/kg), and their blood was collected while they were anesthetized with ketamine hydrochloride (7 mg/kg) or a combination of ketamine hydrochloride (3.5 mg/kg) and medetomidine hydrochloride (0.035 mg/kg).

Table 1. Summary table of the subjects' sex, age, genotype, and personality T-score.

Samples n Sex Age Genotype Chimpanzee Personality Trait (mean ± SD T-score)
M/F (mean ± SD years) AA/AG/GG Dom Ext Con Agr Neu Opn
Chimpanzee Sanctuary Uto 26 14/12 29.7±3.5 20/4/2 55.5±9.6 47.5±7.3 50.2±9.5 52.0±8.4 43.2±7.4 48.7±4.4
Other Japanese* 10 2/8 36.7±7.5 3/4/3 55.7±9.4 43.2±9.2 50.2±11.5 48.7±11.6 49.8±8.4 44.4±8.4
Guinea 21 11/10 5.4±1.6 12/5/4 50.3±10.4 60.1±5.1 55.8±9.4 58.2±8.0 47.1±8.7 55.1±5.9
Combined 57 27/30 35/13/9

Note.

Mean = 50 and SD = 10. Dom = Dominance, Ext = Extraversion, Con = Conscientiousness, Agr = Agreeableness, Neu = Neuroticism, Opn = Openness.

*Other Japanese chimpanzees include 5 subjects from the Kyoto University Primate Research Institute, 2 subjects from the Higashiyama Zoo, 1 chimpanzee from the Itouzu-no-mori Zoo, 1 chimpanzee from the Kouchi Zoo, and 1 chimpanzee from the Tama Zoo.

This study was carried out within the ethical guidelines and framework of Kyoto University and was approved by the Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University and Chimpanzee Sanctuary Uto (permission numbers P1988-08, P1990-15, P2000-04, P2005-01 and P2006-05). All procedures were conducted according to the second edition of the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Primates (Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University) and the Guideline for Care of Chimpanzees (Chimpanzee Sanctuary Uto). The details of animal welfare and steps taken to ameliorate suffering were in accordance with the recommendations of the Weatherall report, “The use of non-human primates in research”.

Genotyping

Genotyping of Q468R by PCR-RFLP is described in detail by Hong and his colleagues [34]. Briefly, a pair of PCR primers (TPH2F: 5-TTCTGTTTATTCTGCA-GGGACT-3′ and TPH2R: 5′-TTAGCCAAGCCATGACACAG-3′) were used to amplify the 1404th SNP containing fragments of TPH2, which was then digested using a restriction enzyme, HpyCH4V (New England BioLabs, Beverly, MA). The digested fragments were subsequently separated by electrophoresis on a 2.0% agarose gel (see Table 1 for details about genotype frequencies).

Personality ratings

Chimpanzees were scored by raters using a Japanese translation of the Hominoid Personality Questionnaire [36]. Raters were comprised of researchers or keepers from each chimpanzee facility, i.e. zoos, research institutes, Chimpanzee Sanctuary Uto, or the Chimpanzee Conservation Center. Each rater had a minimum of 2 years of experience with the chimpanzees they rated. As in our prior study [27], raters had no previous practice in rating chimpanzees. The translated version of the questionnaire yielded the same dimensions as the original English-language version and had similar psychometric properties, including high inter-rater reliabilities [36]. For the present study, we defined the six chimpanzee personality domains via unit-weighting and using the definitions described in our previous study [36]. The raw scores were standardized and then converted into T-scores (mean = 50; SD = 10).

Statistical analysis

To cross-validate results, we first separately tested for the association between the TPH2 genotype and personality in the chimpanzees living at the Chimpanzee Conservation Center in Guinea and those living in Chimpanzee Sanctuary Uto in Japan. We then tested for this association in all 57 subjects (the combined study sample), which included the 10 chimpanzees living in the Kyoto University Primate Research Institute and Japanese zoos. In our first model we examined the possibility of additive genetic effects. We therefore coded AA, AG, and GG as 0, 1, and 2, respectively. In our second model we examined the possibility of genetic dominance by coding We then tested the dominance genetic mode by comparing individuals with the AA genotype (coded 0) and individuals with either the AG or GG genotype (coded as 1). Chimpanzee personality traits and TPH2 genotypes were analyzed using linear regressions controlling for sex and age. SPSS (15.0) was used to conduct all analyses.

Results

Table 2 shows the means of each personality domain score for each genotype for the Chimpanzee Sanctuary Uto, Guinea, and combined samples. Among the chimpanzees living in Chimpanzee Sanctuary Uto, the Q468R allele was significantly related to higher dominance scores in the additive and dominance model; there was also a statistically non-significant trend indicating that this allele was associated with higher neuroticism (see Table 3). These associations were not statistically significant in the Guinea sample; however, the association between the ch468R genotype and neuroticism was similar with respect to direction and effect size (see Table 3). In addition, neuroticism, but not dominance, was significantly associated with ch468R in the total sample (see Table 3) and this association was significant even after a Bonferroni correction for multiple tests (p = 0.05/6 = 0.008).

Table 2. Mean personality domain scores by genotype for chimpanzees living in Chimpanzee Sanctuary Uto, the sanctuary in Guinea, and the combined sample.

Genotype
AA AG GG
Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
Chimpanzee Sanctuary Uto
Dominance 53.05 7.81 61.14 12.66 68.84 9.15
Extraversion 46.59 6.89 45.48 2.36 60.60 6.66
Conscientiousness 50.31 9.70 45.99 5.78 57.14 14.41
Agreeableness 52.34 6.49 42.97 4.14 66.40 13.37
Neuroticism 41.77 7.16 48.54 2.33 46.63 12.13
Openness 48.37 4.32 48.27 4.01 53.08 5.44
Guinea
Dominance 51.58 11.55 50.33 10.29 46.45 8.23
Extraversion 60.39 5.69 58.96 5.68 60.48 3.04
Conscientiousness 55.89 7.27 56.72 12.30 54.45 13.91
Agreeableness 57.50 7.42 56.62 11.01 62.29 5.91
Neuroticism 45.20 8.26 49.68 11.46 49.49 6.74
Openness 56.53 6.98 52.12 3.47 54.28 4.11
Combined
Dominance 52.29 8.84 55.61 12.14 55.93 11.64
Extraversion 51.61 9.38 48.22 10.53 54.97 9.37
Conscientiousness 52.35 9.93 49.52 9.83 55.81 10.72
Agreeableness 54.10 7.51 47.02 11.36 61.74 6.99
Neuroticism 43.34 8.06 51.62 8.64 46.84 7.97
Openness 51.16 6.76 48.20 6.92 49.95 7.83

The combined samples includes 26 subjects from Chimpanzee Sanctuary Uto, 21 subjects from Guinea, and a total of 10 subjects from the Kyoto University Primate Research Institute (n = 5), Higashiyama Zoo (n = 2), Itouzu-no-mori Zoo (n = 1), Kouchi Zoo (n = 1), and Tama Zoo (n = 1).

Table 3. Effect of Tryptophan hydroxylase 2 polymorphism on chimpanzee personality trait: Linear regression analysis with sex and age as the covariates.

Additive mode (AA, AG, GG) Dominant mode (AA, AG+GG)
Unstandardized Standardized Unstandardized Standardized
Sample n MAF HPQ factors β SE β p β SE β p
Chimpanzee Sanctuary Uto 26 0.154 Dominance 7.377 2.923 0.472 0.019 4.959 2.023 0.442 0.023
Extraversion 4.271 2.476 0.360 0.098 1.639 1.780 0.192 0.367
Conscientiousness −0.161 3.103 −0.010 0.959 −0.996 2.124 −0.090 0.644
Agreeableness 1.675 3.041 0.123 0.587 −1.193 2.091 −0.122 0.574
Neuroticism 4.442 2.501 0.378 0.090 3.304 1.699 0.391 0.065
Openness 1.013 1.476 0.144 0.500 0.425 1.022 0.084 0.681
Guinea 21 0.310 Dominance −1.628 2.102 −0.126 0.449 −0.652 1.696 −0.063 0.705
Extraversion −0.301 1.488 −0.047 0.842 −0.537 1.179 −0.107 0.655
Conscientiousness −0.633 2.275 −0.054 0.784 −0.374 1.813 −0.040 0.839
Agreeableness 1.975 2.141 0.199 0.369 0.809 1.736 0.102 0.647
Neuroticism 2.500 2.126 0.231 0.256 2.332 1.668 0.272 0.180
Openness −1.391 1.596 −0.188 0.396 −1.514 1.246 −0.258 0.241
Combined* 57 0.272 Dominance 2.171 1.706 0.163 0.209 1.783 1.309 0.174 0.179
Extraversion 1.027 1.266 0.080 0.421 −0.198 0.979 −0.020 0.841
Conscientiousness 0.472 1.579 0.036 0.766 −0.391 1.215 −0.038 0.749
Agreeableness 1.765 1.591 0.141 0.272 −0.589 1.235 −0.061 0.636
Neuroticism 3.214 1.502 0.279 0.037 3.309 1.115 0.372 0.005
Openness −1.008 1.093 −0.110 0.360 −1.124 0.833 −0.159 0.183

Note.

MAF: minor allele frequency.

*The combined samples includes 26 subjects from Chimpanzee Sanctuary Uto, 21 subjects from Guinea, and a total of 10 subjects from the Kyoto University Primate Research Institute (n = 5), Higashiyama Zoo (n = 2), Itouzu-no-mori Zoo (n = 1), Kouchi Zoo (n = 1), and Tama Zoo (n = 1). Boldfaced values indicate statistically significant effects (p<.05). Underlined values indicate trends (p<0.1).

Discussion

The ch468R allele was associated with higher dominance in a sample of chimpanzees living in Chimpanzee Sanctuary Uto. The association between this allele and dominance was not present among the wild-born sanctuary chimpanzees in Guinea or in the total sample. There was also a statistically non-significant trend suggesting that neuroticism in chimpanzees was associated with the ch468R allele in Chimpanzee Sanctuary Uto. Moreover, while this relationship was not statistically significant, the effect size and direction of the effect were comparable in the chimpanzee sample from Guinea. Finally, in the total sample, there was a significant association between the presence of the ch468R allele and higher levels of neuroticism.

One possible reason for the failure to cross-validate the dominance findings is that the wild-born sanctuary chimpanzees were younger than those at Chimpanzee Sanctuary Uto. As such, the dominance dimension may not yet have as clearly been expressed as in the more mature individuals. A second possible explanation for our failure to cross-validate the dominance findings in the wild sample is because they were separated from their mothers early in life and may have been subjected to other trauma.

There does appear to be evidence of an association between neuroticism and ch468R. This is consistent with earlier studies of humans [4] and mice [5] which found that Q468R mutations are associated with major depression and aggressive behavior, respectively. This is also consistent with a recent study which found that the 3′-untranslated-region polymorphism of TPH2 in rhesus macaques was associated with aggressive behavior [9]. The chimpanzee TPH2 polymorphism (Q468R) is a gain-of-function mutation, which increases serotonin biosynthesis [34]. In other words, like the S allele of 5-HTTLPR which has been related to human neuroticism, the ch468R allele of the TPH2 gene works to increase serotonin storage in the synapse by increasing production of and decreasing the re-absorption of serotonin.

One shortcoming of the present study was the small sample size and thus these results require replications in larger independent samples. A second shortcoming is our poor knowledge of the background of the chimpanzees which prevented us from testing for any gene by environment interaction effects. A third shortcoming is that, while we included a model for dominance effects, the mean neuroticism across genotypes were only suggestive with respect to whether the G allele was dominant, though this may reflect the small sample size of each group.

Chimpanzees have highly-developed brains and exhibit a variety of psychological and behavioral traits in their elaborate social interactions. The present study is the first to identify a genotype related to a personality trait in chimpanzees. Understanding differences in the genes responsible for behavioral variation could lead to a better understanding of the evolutionary history of humans and chimpanzee [37], including hominization [33]. The finding of similar associations between the TPH2 gene and phenotypes related to neuroticism in humans, mice and rhesus macaques suggests that the relationship between neuroticism and TPH2 has deep phylogenetic origins.

This is the first report of a relationship between a personality trait and genotype in great apes. Genetic markers for behavior may be useful for primate conservation, welfare and management in zoos. Therefore, the association between Q468R polymorphism and neuroticism identified in this study should be a focus of future studies which seek to understand individual differences in chimpanzee personality.

Acknowledgments

We thank E. Inoue, Faculty of Applied Biological Sciences, Gifu University, for his invaluable discussion and comments. We are grateful to Y. Ueda, Faculty of Applied Biological Sciences, Gifu University, for her technical assistance in the analysis. We also thank the raters at the different sites for completing the questionnaires.

Footnotes

Competing Interests: The authors have the following competing interests. TU and IH are employed by a commercial company, Sanwa Kagaku Kenkyusho Co., Ltd. There are no patents, products in development or marketed products to declare. This does not alter the authors' adherence to all the PLoS ONE policies on sharing data and materials, as detailed online in the guide for authors.

Funding: This study was financially supported by the Ministry of Education, Science, Sports and Culture, Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (B) (#18310152 and #21310150 to MIM) and the Cooperation Research Program of PRI, Kyoto University. Alexander Weiss's work on this project was funded by a University of Edinburgh Development Trust Grant (#2828) and a Daiwa Foundation Small Grant (6515/6818). These funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. TU and IH are employed by a commercial company, Sanwa Kagaku Kenkyusho Co., Ltd. and helped perform the experiments.

References

  • 1.Kaufman S. New tetrahydrobiopterin-dependent systems. Annu Rev Nutr. 1993;13:261–286. doi: 10.1146/annurev.nu.13.070193.001401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Walther DJ, Peter JU, Bashammakh S, Hortnagl H, Voits M, et al. Synthesis of serotonin by a second tryptophan hydroxylase isoform. Science. 2003;299:76. doi: 10.1126/science.1078197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Cote F, Thevenot E, Fligny C, Fromes Y, Darmon M, et al. Disruption of the nonneuronal tph1 gene demonstrates the importance of peripheral serotonin in cardiac function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100:13525–13530. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2233056100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Zhang X, Gainetdinov RR, Beaulieu JM, Sotnikova TD, Burch LH, et al. Loss-of-function mutation in tryptophan hydroxylase-2 identified in unipolar major depression. Neuron. 2005;45:11–16. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.12.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Kulikov AV, Osipova DV, Naumenko VS, Popova NK. Association between Tph2 gene polymorphism, brain tryptophan hydroxylase activity and aggressiveness in mouse strains. Genes Brain Behav. 2005;4:482–485. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2005.00145.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Lopez VA, Detera-Wadleigh S, Cardona I, Kassem L, McMahon FJ. Nested association between genetic variation in tryptophan hydroxylase II, bipolar affective disorder, and suicide attempts. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;61:181–186. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Walitza S, Renner TJ, Dempfle A, Konrad K, Wewetzer C, et al. Transmission disequilibrium of polymorphic variants in the tryptophan hydroxylase-2 gene in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Mol Psychiatry. 2005;10:1126–1132. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Zill P, Baghai TC, Zwanzger P, Schule C, Eser D, et al. SNP and haplotype analysis of a novel tryptophan hydroxylase isoform (TPH2) gene provide evidence for association with major depression. Mol Psychiatry. 2004;9:1030–1036. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001525. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Chen GL, Novak MA, Meyer J, Kelly BJ, Vallender EJ, et al. The effect of rearing experience and TPH2 genotype on HPA axis function and aggression in rhesus monkeys: A retrospective analysis. Horm Behav. 2010;57:184–191. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.10.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Alaerts M, Ceulemans S, Forero D, Moens LN, De Zutter S, et al. Support for NRG1 as a susceptibility factor for schizophrenia in a northern Swedish isolated population. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66:828–837. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Frodl T, Zill P, Baghai T, Schule C, Rupprecht R, et al. Reduced hippocampal volumes associated with the long variant of the tri- and diallelic serotonin transporter polymorphism in major depression. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2008;147B:1003–1007. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30680. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Lesch KP, Bengel D, Heils A, Sabol SZ, Greenberg BD, et al. Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science. 1996;274:1527–1531. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5292.1527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Tadic A, Victor A, Baskaya O, von Cube R, Hoch J, et al. Interaction between gene variants of the serotonin transporter promoter region (5-HTTLPR) and catechol O-methyltransferase (COMT) in borderline personality disorder. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2009;150B:487–495. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30843. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Xu X, Duman EA, Anney R, Brookes K, Franke B, et al. No association between two polymorphisms of the serotonin transporter gene and combined type attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2008;147B:1306–1309. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30737. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Zaboli G, Jonsson EG, Gizatullin R, De Franciscis A, Asberg M, et al. Haplotype analysis confirms association of the serotonin transporter (5-HTT) gene with schizophrenia but not with major depression. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2008;147:301–307. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Greenberg BD, Li Q, Lucas FR, Hu S, Sirota LA, et al. Association between the serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism and personality traits in a primarily female population sample. Am J Med Genet. 2000;96:202–216. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-8628(20000403)96:2<202::aid-ajmg16>3.0.co;2-j. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Murakami F, Shimomura T, Kotani K, Ikawa S, Nanba E, et al. Anxiety traits associated with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region in the Japanese. J Hum Genet. 1999;44:15–17. doi: 10.1007/s100380050098. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Ricketts MH, Hamer RM, Sage JI, Manowitz P, Feng F, et al. Association of a serotonin transporter gene promoter polymorphism with harm avoidance behaviour in an elderly population. Psychiatr Genet. 1998;8:41–44. doi: 10.1097/00041444-199800820-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Sen S, Villafuerte S, Nesse R, Stoltenberg SF, Hopcian J, et al. Serotonin transporter and GABAA alpha 6 receptor variants are associated with neuroticism. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55:244–249. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Ball D, Hill L, Freeman B, Eley TC, Strelau J, et al. The serotonin transporter gene and peer-rated neuroticism. Neuroreport. 1997;8:1301–1304. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199703240-00048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Ebstein RP, Gritsenko I, Nemanov L, Frisch A, Osher Y, et al. No association between the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region polymorphism and the Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire (TPQ) temperament of harm avoidance. Mol Psychiatry. 1997;2:224–226. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Herbst JH, Zonderman AB, McCrae RR, Costa PT., Jr Do the dimensions of the temperament and character inventory map a simple genetic architecture? Evidence from molecular genetics and factor analysis. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157:1285–1290. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.8.1285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Jorm AF, Henderson AS, Jacomb PA, Christensen H, Korten AE, et al. An association study of a functional polymorphism of the serotonin transporter gene with personality and psychiatric symptoms. Mol Psychiatry. 1998;3:449–451. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Middeldorp CM, de Geus EJ, Beem AL, Lakenberg N, Hottenga JJ, et al. Family based association analyses between the serotonin transporter gene polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and neuroticism, anxiety and depression. Behav Genet. 2007;37:294–301. doi: 10.1007/s10519-006-9139-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Munafo MR, Freimer NB, Ng W, Ophoff R, Veijola J, et al. 5-HTTLPR genotype and anxiety-related personality traits: a meta-analysis and new data. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2009;150B:271–281. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30808. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Willis-Owen SA, Turri MG, Munafo MR, Surtees PG, Wainwright NW, et al. The serotonin transporter length polymorphism, neuroticism, and depression: a comprehensive assessment of association. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;58:451–456. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.04.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.King JE, Figueredo AJ. The Five-Factor Model plus dominance in chimpanzee personality. J Res Person. 1997;31:257–271. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.King JE, Weiss A, Farmer KH. A chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) analogue of cross-national generalization of personality structure: zoological parks and an African sanctuary. J Pers. 2005;73:389–410. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00313.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Weiss A, King JE, Hopkins WD. A cross-setting study of chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) personality structure and development: zoological parks and Yerkes National Primate Research Center. Am J Primatol. 2007;69:1264–1277. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20428. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Weiss A, King JE, Enns RM. Subjective well-being is heritable and genetically correlated with dominance in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Pers Soc Psychol. 2002;83:1141–1149. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Pederson AK, King JE, Landau VI. Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) personality predicts behavior. J Res Person. 2005;39:534–549. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Weissglas-Volkov D, Pajukanta P. Genetic causes of high and low serum HDL-cholesterol. J Lipid Res. 2010;51:2032–2057. doi: 10.1194/jlr.R004739. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Inoue-Murayama M, Niimi Y, Takenaka O, Murayama Y. Evolution of personality-related genes in primates. In: Miyoshi K, Shapiro CM, Gaviria M, Morita Y, editors. Contemporary Neuropsychiatry. Tokyo: Springer-Verlag; 2001. pp. 425–428. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Hong KW, Sugawara Y, Hasegawa H, Hayasaka I, Hashimoto R, et al. A new gain-of-function allele in chimpanzee tryptophan hydroxylase 2 and the comparison of its enzyme activity with that in humans and rats. Neurosci Lett. 2007;412:195–200. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.11.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Morimura N, Idani G, Matsuzawa T. The first chimpanzee sanctuary in Japan: an attempt to care for the “surplus” of biomedical research. Am J Primatol. 2011;73:226–232. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20887. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Weiss A, Inoue-Murayama M, Hong KW, Inoue E, Udono T, et al. Assessing chimpanzee personality and subjective well-being in Japan. Am J Primatol. 2009;71:283–292. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Di Fiore A. Molecular genetic approaches to the study of primate behavior, social organization, and reproduction. American Journal of Physical Anthropology Supplement. 2003;37:62–99. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.10382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES