Skip to main content
Neurology logoLink to Neurology
. 2011 Jul 19;77(3):264–268. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182217ee8

Probing consciousness with event-related potentials in the vegetative state

F Faugeras 1, B Rohaut 1, N Weiss 1, TA Bekinschtein 1, D Galanaud 1, L Puybasset 1, F Bolgert 1, C Sergent 1, L Cohen 1, S Dehaene 1, L Naccache 1,
PMCID: PMC3136052  PMID: 21593438

Abstract

Objective:

Probing consciousness in noncommunicating patients is a major medical and neuroscientific challenge. While standardized and expert behavioral assessment of patients constitutes a mandatory step, this clinical evaluation stage is often difficult and doubtful, and calls for complementary measures which may overcome its inherent limitations. Several functional brain imaging methods are currently being developed within this perspective, including fMRI and cognitive event-related potentials (ERPs). We recently designed an original rule extraction ERP test that is positive only in subjects who are conscious of the long-term regularity of auditory stimuli.

Methods:

In the present work, we report the results of this test in a population of 22 patients who met clinical criteria for vegetative state.

Results:

We identified 2 patients showing this neural signature of consciousness. Interestingly, these 2 patients showed unequivocal clinical signs of consciousness within the 3 to 4 days following ERP recording.

Conclusions:

Taken together, these results strengthen the relevance of bedside neurophysiological tools to improve diagnosis of consciousness in noncommunicating patients.


Evaluating abnormal states of consciousness may be extremely challenging when relying only on the clinical examination alone. EEG-based paradigms have many advantages over fMRI for monitoring patients with altered consciousness because of 1) the millisecond-range resolution, 2) the low cost and noninvasiveness, 3) the ability to monitor at the bedside, and 4) the possibility of designing dedicated systems for clinical use.

We recently designed a new test of consciousness using high-density scalp EEG in an auditory odd-ball paradigm.1 This test capitalizes on 2 properties which are specific to conscious processing24: one has to be conscious of a mental representation to actively maintain it in working memory, and to use it strategically. Our test evaluates cerebral responses to violations of temporal regularities. Short-interval violations due to the unexpected occurrence of a single deviant sound among a repeated train of standard sounds led to an early and automatic response in auditory cortex, the mismatch negativity ERP component. Moreover, long-term violations, defined as the presentation of a rare and unexpected series of 5 sounds, led to a late and spatially distributed response that was present only when subjects were attentive and aware of the auditory rule and of its violations (P3b component). Our observations showed that this rule violation effect is a specific signature of conscious processing, although it can be absent in conscious subjects unaware of long-term auditory regularities.

In this work, we explored the relevance of this rule violation effect test in 31 patients who were in vegetative states of various chronicity. Our main objective was to assess the added value of our test in patients in whom detailed clinical examination and Coma Recovery Scale–Revised (CRS-R) scoring failed to detect any reliable evidence of consciousness. The second objective of this study was to explore the prognostic value of the test by following each of these patients, and to correlate the ERP test with early and late outcomes.

METHODS

Standard protocol approvals, registrations, and patient consents.

This study has been approved by the ethical committee of the Salpêtrière Hospital (Paris, France).

Controls.

Ten controls were recorded (age 20.3 ± 0.7 years; sex ratio [M/F] 2.3). Data from 2 subjects were discarded due to excessive movement artifacts.

Patients.

We report here all recordings of patients in vegetative state (VS) from November 2008 to February 2010. Patients with clinical criteria of VS, irrespective of delay from disease onset (both early and longstanding states), were included. Patients were recorded without sedation since at least 24 hours. Among the 31 recordings 9 were discarded after evaluation of EEG quality (appendix e-1 on the Neurology® Web site at www.neurology.org). This high rate of rejection (29%) reveals one of the limits of this technique. The 22 valid datasets included 13 men and 9 women, aged from 22 to 76 years (mean 51.7 years), with both early and late recordings (mean 190 days; median 29 days; SD 546 days; earliest 14 days; latest 2,555 days; table).

Table.

Patients' characteristics and outcomes

graphic file with name znl02711-8919-t01.jpg

Abbreviations: ACoA = anterior communicating artery; CRS-R = Coma Recovery Scale–Revised; CS = conscious state; EH = extradural hematoma; ERP = event-related potential; ICA = internal carotid artery; IVH = intraventricular hemorrhage; MCA = middle cerebral artery; MCS = minimally conscious state; SAH = subarachnoid hemorrhage; SDH = subdural hematoma; UA = unresponsive awake state (criteria of vegetative state irrespectively of delay); VS = vegetative state.

a

The patient died from a fatal hemorrhage recurrence on day 34.

Behavior.

The clinical definition of VS was based on the French version of the CRS-R scale.5 It was carried out after careful neurologic examination by trained neurologists (F.F., L.N.), immediately before ERP recording.

Stimulation and ERPs.

We used our previously published auditory protocol while recording high-density scalp EEG (EGI, Eugene, OR). See reference1 and appendix e-1 for details.

RESULTS

A rule violation ERP effect was present in each of the 8 controls (100%) within the 300–700 msec temporal window after the onset of the fifth sound, replicating our previous findings (see control group 1 in reference1). Among the 22 patients, 2 (9%) showed a significant effect (figure). None of the remaining 20 patients was deaf, and early cortical responses to the tones could be identified on all ERP recordings, thus discarding a trivial interpretation of the absence of rule violation effect.

Figure. Test design and illustration of bedside recording in intensive care unit (ICU).

Figure

(A) Bedside recording in ICU. Photography of the recording setting in a patient in the ICU (with the patient's permission). Installation of the net and EEG calibration requires about 15 minutes. Earphones are then applied, task instruction delivered, and EEG recording starts. (B) Auditory paradigm. On each trial 5 sounds were presented. Each block started with 20–30 frequent series of sounds to establish the long-term regularity before delivering the first infrequent rule deviant stimulus. (C) Three representative results. Global field power of rule standard (green) and rule deviant (red) trials are plotted for one conscious control subject (C.a), for a patient with a rule violation effect (C.b), and for a patient without rule violation effect (C.c). Early peaks to each of the 5 sounds (S1 to S5) are indicated for the control subject. Statistical significance of event-related potential (ERP) differences within the time window of the rule violation effect is indicated by a color code on the X axis. Voltage topography maps averaged across time windows of significant ERP effects are displayed on the right. Panel C is reprinted from Bekinschtein et al.1

One of the 2 patients was a 62-year-old woman with a severe form of acute disseminated encephalomyelitis following a spontaneously resolving flu-like episode. MRI showed extensive bilateral hemispheric hyperintensities on fluid-attenuated inversion recovery images, with gadolinium enhancement on T1-weighted sequence. She was recorded 25 days after disease onset. Neurologic examination immediately before ERP recording showed preserved brainstem reflexes, with slight anisocoria (right < left). Babinski and Hoffmann signs were observed on the left side. All tendon reflexes were present. Eye-opening was systematically obtained under auditory or nociceptive stimulation. However, even when arousal was stimulated, no behavioral sign of consciousness could be obtained (CRS-R = 1/23).

The second patient with a positive ERP test was a 51-year-old man who had a severe traumatic brain injury with a massive acute right-hemispheric subdural hematoma which required surgical treatment. MRI then revealed additional hemorrhagic cortical contusions located in both occipital and frontal areas, and in the left mesial temporal lobe. He was recorded 15 days after trauma. Neurologic examination immediately before ERP recording showed preserved brainstem reflexes, with a slow stereotyped flexion response to nociceptive stimulation. A left Babinski sign was present, and all tendon reflexes were present. Eye-opening was systematically obtained under auditory or nociceptive stimulation, and CRS-R reached 5/23.

Both patients reached criteria of minimally conscious state (MCS) 3 and 4 days after ERP recording, respectively. By contrast, in the 20 remaining patients with a negative result, early recovery of consciousness was observed in only 2 cases within the first week (χ2 = 9.90, p = 0.002; Fisher exact test: p = 0.026), indicating that the global effect was significantly predictive of overt consciousness recovery. When studying outcome within a longer time frame (>6 months), 7/20 initially VS patients without ERP effect reached either an MCS or conscious state (χ2 test = 3.18, unilateral p = 0.037).

DISCUSSION

A rule violation effect was observed in 2 patients who met clinical criteria of VS, suggesting that they consciously identified rule deviants. The relative weakness of their effect may correspond to fluctuations of consciousness or to partial execution of the task (e.g., conscious identification of targets without counting). In any case, as shown previously,1 the mere identification of rule deviant trials requires conscious processing of the stimuli, while nonconscious P300/N400-like ERP responses have been reported with simpler paradigms in controls and patients.69 Therefore, the positivity of this ERP test is a strong argument to correct the clinical diagnosis in these 2 patients, and to classify them as conscious in spite of the negative behavioral assessment. In both patients, the negativity of clinical examination and of CRS scoring could not be explained by motor impairments. These 2 cases are reminiscent of recent reports of the few patients clinically assessed as VS who showed evidence of consciousness in active fMRI paradigms.10,11

Our test, however, presents several limitations: the high rate of data rejection is inherent to EEG recording in awake and nonsedated patients. Moreover, our test lacks sensitivity in as much as it requires the patient not only to be conscious, but also to understand task instructions, to keep them in working memory, to continuously keep attention focused on the stimuli, and to mentally count global deviants.

The second objective of our study was to explore value of the ERP global effect for the prognosis of patients in VS. Interestingly, in terms of consciousness, the early outcome was much better in patients with a rule violation effect than in those lacking it. This differential outcome was less pronounced on a longer time scale. This is compatible with our proposal that the rule violation effect is a neural signature of consciousness per se rather than a predictor of consciousness recovery. Long-term (≥2 years) follow-up will be addressed in a dedicated study.

The auditory rule violation ERP test can be used to probe consciousness, and its positivity in patients who meet clinical criteria of VS therefore questions the clinical diagnosis.

Supplementary Material

Data Supplement

ACKNOWLEDGMENT

The authors thank Prof. Chastre, Prof. Similowski, Prof. Samson, Prof. Rouby, and Dr. Patte-Karsenti for referring some of the patients. This study is dedicated to the patients and to their close relatives.

Supplemental data at www.neurology.org

Inline graphic Scan this code with your smartphone to access this feature

CRS-R
Coma Recovery Scale–Revised
ERP
event-related potential
MCS
minimally conscious state
VS
vegetative state.

DISCLOSURE

Dr. Faugeras, Dr. Rohaut, and Dr. Weiss report no disclosures. Dr. Bekinschtein has received fellowship support from the European Union. Dr. Galanaud reports no disclosures. Prof. Puybasset serves as a consultant for Actelion Pharmaceuticals Ltd. Dr. Bolgert reports no disclosures. Dr. Sergent receives research support from the European Union. Prof. Cohen reports no disclosures. Prof. Dehaene receives research support from ERC, INSERM, and CEA. Prof. Naccache reports no disclosures.

REFERENCES

  • 1. Bekinschtein TA, Dehaene S, Rohaut B, Tadel F, Cohen L, Naccache L. Neural signature of the conscious processing of auditory regularities. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2009;106:1672–1677 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Dehaene S, Kerszberg M, Changeux JP. A neuronal model of a global workspace in effortful cognitive tasks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1998;95:14529–14534 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Dehaene S, Naccache L. Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition 2001;79:1–37 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Dehaene S, Changeux JP, Naccache L, Sackur J, Sergent C. Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: a testable taxonomy. Trends Cogn Sci 2006;10:204–211 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Schnakers C, Majerus S, Giacino J, et al. A French validation study of the Coma Recovery Scale–Revised (CRS-R). Brain Inj 2008;22:786–792 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Perrin F, Schnakers C, Schabus M, et al. Brain response to one's own name in vegetative state, minimally conscious state, and locked-in syndrome. Arch Neurol 2006;63:562–569 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Luck SJ, Vogel EK, Shapiro KL. Word meanings can be accessed but not reported during the attentional blink. Nature 1996;383:616–618 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Kotchoubey B. Apallic syndrome is not apallic: is vegetative state vegetative? Neuropsychol Rehabil 2005;15:333–356 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Schoenle PW, Witzke W. How vegetative is the vegetative state? Preserved semantic processing in VS patients: evidence from N 400 event-related potentials. Neurorehabilitation 2004;19:329–334 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Owen AM, Coleman MR, Boly M, Davis MH, Laureys S, Pickard JD. Detecting awareness in the vegetative state. Science 2006;313:1402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Monti MM, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Coleman MR, et al. Willful modulation of brain activity in disorders of consciousness. N Engl J Med 2010;362:579–589 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data Supplement

Articles from Neurology are provided here courtesy of American Academy of Neurology

RESOURCES