Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2012 Aug 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Affect Disord. 2011 Apr 13;132(3):438–444. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.03.013

Hypersomnia in Inter-Episode Bipolar Disorder: Does it Have Prognostic Significance?

Katherine A Kaplan 1, June Gruber 2, Polina Eidelman 1, Lisa S Talbot 1, Allison G Harvey 1
PMCID: PMC3139494  NIHMSID: NIHMS284417  PMID: 21489637

Abstract

Background

Hypersomnia in inter-episode bipolar disorder has been minimally researched. The current study sought to document the prevalence of hypersomnia in a sample of inter-episode patients with bipolar disorder and to examine the relationship between hypersomnia and future bipolar depressive symptoms.

Methods

A total of 56 individuals with bipolar disorder (51 Type 1 + 5 Type II) who were currently inter-episode, along with 55 non-psychiatric controls, completed a baseline assessment, including semi-structured interviews for psychiatric diagnoses, sleep disorders, and a battery of indices that included assessment of hypersomnia. Approximately six months later, participants were recontacted by telephone and mood was re-evaluated.

Results

Three of six indices suggested that approximately 25% of participants with bipolar disorder endorsed symptoms of hypersomnia in the inter-episode period. Within the bipolar group, hypersomnia in the inter-episode period was associated with future depressive symptoms. This finding was independent of baseline depressive symptoms and medication use.

Limitations

Small sample size and concurrent psychopharmacology in the bipolar sample.

Discussion

Though no gold standard measure for hypersomnia currently exists, this research takes a step towards identifying a clinically and empirically useful hypersomnia assessment. This study demonstrates that hypersomnia in the inter-episode period of bipolar disorder relates to future depressive symptoms, and adds to the growing body of evidence on the importance of inter-episode symptoms predicting bipolar relapse.

Keywords: hypersomnia, sleep disturbance, bipolar disorder, long sleep

Introduction

Bipolar disorder is a serious and chronic psychiatric illness. One of the most important findings to emerge over the last decade is the significance of the inter-episode period. Individuals in the inter-episode period spend roughly 50% of their time unwell (Joffe et al. 2004; Judd et al. 2002), and these symptoms predict relapse into mania or depression (MacQueen et al. 2003). Hence, there is a critical need to identify aspects of the illness that contribute to inter-episode dysfunction and to relapse.

Hypersomnia may be one candidate contributing to inter-episode dysfunction and to relapse but is understudied in bipolar disorder. In unipolar depression, hypersomnia is present in approximately 30% of individuals (Kaplan & Harvey 2009) and associated with longer, more severe and more treatment-resistant depressions (Matza et al. 2003). Furthermore, individuals with hypersomnia are 2.4 to 2.9 times more likely to develop a subsequent unipolar depressive episode (Breslau et al. 1996; Ford & Cooper-Patrick 2001). Comparative studies show that hypersomnia is more prevalent in bipolar depression than in unipolar depression (Akiskal & Benazzi 2005; Benazzi 2006; Bowden 2005), and also that hypersomnia is highly recurrent across separate episodes of bipolar depression (Leibenluft et al. 1995). Despite its prevalence, four gaps remain in our understanding of hypersomnia in bipolar disorder.

First, although estimates of hypersomnia in bipolar depression range from 38% (Akiskal & Benazzi 2005) to 78% (Detre et al. 1972), the prevalence of hypersomnia during the inter-episode period is unknown. Hence, one aim of the current study was to estimate the prevalence of hypersomnia in an inter-episode sample of individuals with bipolar disorder.

Second, there is no gold standard measure for hypersomnia. Due to the heterogeneity across diagnostic systems (Kaplan & Harvey 2009), indices of hypersomnia probe for such disparate features as sleeping ‘more than 8 hours a day’ to ‘falling asleep while standing’ to the ambiguous ‘too much sleep’. Hence, a second aim was to evaluate indices of hypersomnia by including a multi-method assessment (i.e. self-report, clinical interview, and sleep diary) and prospectively examining the relationship between these indices and future depressive symptoms.

Third, though hypersomnia is traditionally viewed as a disorder of self-reported sleep time, all research utilizing objective measures of sleep suggests that sleep latencies and sleep durations of individuals with psychiatric hypersomnia do not differ from those of control participants (e.g., Billiard et al. 1994; Nofzinger et al. 1991). This raises an interesting empirical question: is hypersomnia better characterized as a disorder of increased time in bed (TIB), rather than increased total sleep time (TST) (Kaplan & Harvey 2009)? The third aim tested the prediction that TIB, prospectively calculated via sleep diaries, would correlate more highly with other indices of hypersomnia than TST.

The fourth aim of the current study was to evaluate the relationship between inter-episode hypersomnia and future depressive symptoms. Given the high rate of relapse in bipolar disorder (Perlis et al. 2006), identifying contributors to future depression is essential. As hypersomnia robustly predicts the onset of a unipolar depressive episode (Breslau et al. 1996; Ford & Cooper-Patrick 2001), and is especially prevalent (Detre et al. 1972) and frequently recurring (Leibenluft et al. 1995) in bipolar disorder, we predicted that hypersomnia in the inter-episode period would be associated with future depressive symptoms in our sample, even after controlling for baseline symptomotology.

Methods

Participants

A total of 56 individuals with bipolar disorder type I (N=51) or type II (N=5), along with 55 age- and sex-matched control individuals without a history of psychiatric or sleep disorder, were included in analyses. Sample demographics are presented in Table 1. A diagnosis of bipolar disorder type I or II was determined using the Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM-IV (SCID-NP; First et al. 1995), and inter-episode status was defined using established cutoff scores of ≤ 11 on the Inventory of Depressive Symptomotology, Clinician Version (IDS-C; Rush et al. 1996) and ≤ 7 the Young Mania Rating Scale (YMRS; Young et al. 1978). Bipolar participants were excluded if they met criteria for current substance abuse and/or dependence, given their myriad effects on sleep, and also excluded if they met criteria for sleep apnea or restless leg syndrome according to the Duke Structured Interview for Sleep Disorders (DSISD) based on research diagnostic criteria (Edinger et al. 2004). Bipolar participants were not excluded on the basis of comorbidities or pharmacological treatments, given that both are common features of the illness. Control participants were excluded if they met criteria for any current or lifetime Axis I disorder from the SCID-NP, or major sleep disorders on the DSISD. All participants were excluded for unstable major illnesses and severe neurological injuries.

Table 1.

Participant Characteristics at Baseline Assessment.

Demographic Variable Bipolar (N=56) Control (N=55) χ2 or t
Age 0.378
 Mean (SD) 35.6 (12.35) 36.5 (11.06)
Gender 0.92
 Male 15 19
 Female 55 34
Race/Ethnicity 3.16
 African American 6 4
 Asian American 3 7
 Caucasian 37 35
 Hispanic 4 3
 Other/Biracial 5 4
Employment Status 3.41
 Full Time/Part Time 36 43
 Unemployed/Retired 19 10
Marital Status 2.16
 Married/Partnered 17 13
 Separated/Divorced/Widowed 9 9
 Single 30 31
Annual Income 2.99
 Less than $50,000 27 31
 Greater than $50,000 18 9
IDS-C Total Score −7.27***
 Mean (SD) 7.92 (3.87) 3.02 (2.69)
YMRS Total Score −4.43***
 Mean (SD) 2.63 (2.33) 0.96 (1.37)
Psychotropic Medications
 None 6 55 91.28***
 Monotherapy 9 0 9.62**
 Polytherapy 41 0 63.85***
**

p<.01

***

p < .001

Diagnostic measures

Four semi-structured clinical interviews were administered: the SCID-NP, the DSISD, the IDS-C and the YMRS. To assess diagnostic inter-rater reliability, interviewers blind to diagnostic group evaluated a randomly-selected sample of SCID interviews (n=13); diagnoses matched those made by the original interviewer in all cases (k=1.00).

Hypersomnia Indices

The precise wording of five potential indices of hypersomnia is presented in Table 2. Indices included diagnosis based on the DSISD, items from the IDS-C, the Inventory of Depressive Symptomotology, Self Report (IDS-SR; Rush et al. 1996), the Beck Depression Inventory, Second Edition (BDI-II; Beck et al. 1996), and standard sleep diaries kept for seven days. Participants completed the log prior to sleep and upon waking, and all participants were required to call a voicemail twice daily with their answers to ensure compliance. TST was calculated by subtracting all time spent awake from all time spent in bed nightly. TIB was scored by summing all intended sleep periods, excluding periods of reading or television watching in bed. Inter-rater reliability between interviewers and a trained rater blind to diagnostic status on the hypersomnia section of the DSISD was excellent (n=15; k=1.00). Inter-rater reliability was acceptable on the hypersomnia item of the IDS-C (n=14; ICC absolute agreement=0.65, 93% agreement).

Table 2.

Definitions and Prevalence of Hypersomnia Symptoms at Baseline by Potential Index.

Index Precise Wording of Question(s) Hypersomnia Prevalence
Bipolar (N=56) Control (N=55) χ2 Test Value
n % n %
DSISDa Do you often fall asleep or do you have to struggle to stay awake when you are in any of the following situations? (Talking with others, driving, talking on phone, standing, performing your work, other activities?). Do you sleep for long periods of time? [IF YES] Has your sleepiness caused you any problems in the daytime such as poor concentration, poor memory, reduced work performance, or irritability? OR Has your sleepiness ever interfered in any way with your family or social activities? 6 11.5 0 0 6.74**
IDS-SRb 1 I sleep no longer than 10 hours in a 24-hour period including naps. 17 28.6 11 20
2 I sleep no longer than 12 hours in a 24-hour period including naps. 11 21.4 2 3.6
3 I sleep longer than 12 hours in a 24-hour period including naps. 0 0 0 0
Total 28 50 13 23.6 7.11**
IDS-Cc How many hours on average have you been sleeping in a 24-hour period in the past week, including naps? What is the longest you’ve slept in a 24-hour period last week?
1 I sleep no longer than 10 hours in a 24-hour period including naps. 11 20.8 5 9.3
2 I sleep no longer than 12 hours in a 24-hour period including naps. 2 3.8 1 1.9
3 I sleep longer than 12 hours in a 24-hour period including naps. 0 0 0 0
Total 13 24.6 6 11.2 2.96
BDI-IId 1 I sleep somewhat more than usual. 11 19.6 2 3.6
2 I sleep a lot more than usual. 4 7.1 1 1.8
3 I sleep most of the day. 0 0 0 0
Total 15 26.7 3 5.4 9.29**
Sleep Diary Total Sleep Timee Last night, I think I slept a total of ____hours and ____min.
9 – 9.99 hours 2 4.1 0 0
10-10.99 hours 0 0 0 0
11+ hours 0 0 0 0
Sleep Diary Time in Bede Total 2 4.1 0 0 2.08
Last night, I got in to bed at ______. This morning, I got out of bed at______. 3 3
9 – 9.99 hours 14 28.6 4 7.7
10-10.99 hours 2 4.1 2 3.8
11+ hours 1 2 0 0
Total 17 34.7 6 11.5 7.69**
a

DSISD=Duke Structured Interview for Sleep Disorders;

b

IDS-SR=Inventory of Depressive Symptomotology, Self Report Version,

Question 4;

c

IDS-C=Inventory of Depressive Symptomotology, Clinician Version, Question 4;

d

BDI-II=Beck Depression Inventory, Second Edition, Question 16;

e

Average taken from seven nights of sleep diary;

*

p <.05

**

p <.01

Procedure

Adult participants between the ages of 18 and 65 were recruited from advertisements, online bulletins and referrals. Following a telephone screen, participants were sent a copy of the sleep diary with detailed instructions and asked to keep the diary for seven days (6.76±0.65 days completed). Once diaries were complete, participants were invited to UC Berkeley for the baseline assessment. Trained doctoral candidates and a postdoctoral fellow conducted all semi-structured interviews. Approximately six months after the baseline visit (216±47 days), bipolar participants were re-contacted via telephone and a trained interviewer reassessed depressive and manic symptomotology.

Statistical Analysis

Between groups comparisons were evaluated using two-tailed χ2 or independent sample t tests. Fisher’s exact test was used to compare categorical data with expected cell counts of less than five. Bivariate correlations evaluated coherence across hypersomnia indices, and signal detection statistics assessed the predictive values of hypersomnia indices. Partial correlations evaluated associations between hypersomnia at baseline and depressive symptoms at 6-month follow-up, controlling for baseline depressive symptoms.

Participants listed the name, dose, and length of use for all medications taken at baseline assessment. Each medication was subsequently re-categorized into medication classes, and post-hoc χ2 tests compared the number of individuals endorsing and not endorsing hypersomnia within each medication class. Because class-level analyses are limited by reduced sample size and an inability to consider dosing, we also calculated the medication load for each participant (Almeida et al. 2009; Phillips et al. 2008). Each medication’s dose was classified as ‘low’ or ‘high’, and assigned a corresponding score of 1 or 2, based on published parameters and chlorpromazine-equivalent mean effective daily doses (ED50; Sackeim 2001; Davis & Chen 2004). A composite measure of medication load was created for each participant by summing across medication codes and classes, reflecting both dose and diversity of medications taken by each participant (Almeida et al., 2009). The medication load scores of individuals endorsing and not endorsing hypersomnia were compared using Student’s t tests.

Results

Participant Characteristics

Participant characteristics are presented in Table 1. There were no significant differences between the groups on age, gender, race/ethnicity, employment status, marital status, or annual income level. Although established to be interepisode, the bipolar group reported a greater number of both manic (YMRS) and depressed (IDS-C) symptoms than control participants at baseline (p<.001 for both).

A total of 42 individuals in the bipolar group (75%) completed the follow-up assessment. Reasons for non-participation in the follow-up included: declined to participate (n=2), unable to contact (n=8), and unable to schedule before conclusion of the study (n=4). Completers and non-completers did not differ with respect to baseline IDS-C and YMRS scores, age, gender, ethnicity, employment status, or marital status (p>0.10 for all). Mean depression (IDS-C) score at follow-up was 11.3±9.4, and mean mania (YMRS) score at follow-up was 2.6±3.9.

Inter-episode Hypersomnia Prevalence

Table 2 lists the definitions and symptom prevalence rates of inter-episode hypersomnia by hypersomnia index. Inter-episode prevalence estimates of hypersomnia varied from 4% to 50% in the bipolar group depending on the index. Hypersomnia was more common in the bipolar group than in the control group on the DSISD, IDS-SR, BDI-II and Sleep Diary TIB (p<0.05 for all). No differences were found between groups on the IDS-C (χ2=2.96, p=0.09) or on Sleep Diary TST (χ2=2.08, p>0.10).

Comparing Hypersomnia Indices

Intercorrelations were first calculated to understand the degree to which the six indices relate to one another. Zero-order correlations between the IDS-SR, IDS-C, BDI-II, Sleep Diary TIB and Sleep Diary TST were all statistically significant (r≥.41, p≤0.05); only the DSISD did not correlate with any other index. Second, standard signal detection statistics were used to establish the predictive power of various hypersomnia indices to detect depressive symptoms at follow-up. Following standard cutoffs, we defined symptomatic depression as a score of ≥12 on the IDS-C at 6-month follow-up, excluding the hypersomnia item from follow-up score tabulation. Of individuals meeting criteria for depressive symptomotology at follow-up, mean score after excluding the hypersomnia item was 20.5±8.9 (range 14–41); for those not meeting criteria, mean score was 5.6±3.0 (range 0–11).

Table 3 lists signal detection statistics for each potential hypersomnia index. Sensitivity refers to how well a hypersomnia index identifies persons with depressive symptoms. The sensitivity of the DSISD, Sleep Diary TST and Sleep Diary TIB for future depressive symptoms was low (21%, 9% and 36%, respectively). Specificity refers to how well a hypersomnia index identifies persons without depressive symptoms. Given their low false-positive rates, the DSISD and the Sleep Diary TST had the highest specificity (93% and 98%); the IDS-SR exhibited the poorest specificity (44%). Positive predictive value (PPV) refers to the probability that a person with hypersomnia will have depressive symptoms at follow-up, and negative predictive value (NPV) refers to the probability that a person without hypersomnia will not have depressive symptoms at follow-up. The IDS-C and the BDI-II suggest that roughly seven of ten individuals with inter-episode hypersomnia will have depressive symptoms at follow-up (PPV=67% and 73%, respectively). Likewise, these indices suggest that seven or eight of ten individuals without inter-episode hypersomnia will not have depressive symptoms at follow-up (NPV=83% and 68%, respectively). The discriminability statistic d’ is a measure of test responsiveness, and the area under d’ (AUC) is an overall measure of test accuracy with chance equal to 0.5. The IDS-SR and the Sleep Diary TIB have low PPV and AUC statistics, showing limited predictive ability. The IDS-C and the BDI-II have the most favorable balance of test statistics.

Table 3.

Predictive power of hypersomnia indices at baseline for IDS-C depressive symptoms at follow-up in the bipolar group (N=42).

Hypersomnia Index Sensitivity Specificity Positive Predictive Value Negative Predictive Value d' Area Under Curve
DSISDa 0.21 0.93 0.60 0.69 0.65 0.68
IDS-SRb 0.47 0.44 0.32 0.60 -0.22 0.43
IDS-Cc 0.46 0.86 0.67 0.83 0.92 0.74
BDI-IId 0.43 0.81 0.73 0.68 0.71 0.69
Sleep Diary Total Sleep Time 0.09 0.98 0.67 0.71 .073 0.70
Sleep Diary Time in Bed 0.36 0.64 0.31 0.70 0.01 0.50
a

DSISD = Duke University Structured Interview for Sleep Disorders

b

IDS-SR = Inventory of Depressive Symptomotology, Self Report Version

c

IDS-C = Inventory of Depressive Symptomotology, Clinician Version

d

BDI-II = Beck Depression Inventory, Second Edition

Association Between Hypersomnia and Future Depression

To examine the relationship between inter-episode hypersomnia and future depressive symptoms, bivariate partial correlations between hypersomnia indices at baseline and follow-up depressive symptoms were calculated, controlling for initial level of depression severity and excluding the hypersomnia item from follow-up symptom tabulation. Two of six hypersomnia indices were associated with future depressive symptoms, the IDS-C (r=0.32, p<.05) and the BDI-II (r=0.38, p<.05).

Confounding Effects of Medications

We carefully evaluated medications as a potential confound. Fifty participants in the bipolar group reported taking at least one medication, and 41 reported taking more than one medication. Chi square tests compared the number of bipolar individuals who did and did not report hypersomnia within each medication class. Hypersomnia presence was evaluated using the IDS-C and BDI-II, as these two indices demonstrated the most favorable balance of test statistics. There were no differences between individuals reporting and not reporting hypersomnia by medication class (p>0.05 for all). Chi square tests of hypersomnia rates between individuals who were not medicated, taking one medication, and taking more than one medication were not significant. Comparisons of medication load between individuals endorsing and not endorsing hypersomnia revealed no differences between groups (p>0.05).

Discussion

The first aim of the current study was to document the prevalence of hypersomnia in the inter-episode period. Three of six indices converged on a rate of around 25%. This rate is less than the prevalence of hypersomnia in bipolar depression (Akiskal & Benazzi 2005; Detre et al. 1972). Even so, the fact that one in four participants reported experiencing hypersomnia is notable, given that the functional consequences of hypersomnia include interpersonal impairment, substance use and decreased productivity (Kaplan & Harvey 2009). This finding adds to previous research documenting considerable symptomotology in the inter-episode period, including depressive symptoms (Judd et al. 2002), comorbid anxiety (MacQueen et al. 2003), and insomnia (Harvey et al. 2005).

Our second aim was to make progress towards identifying indices of hypersomnia. We did this by evaluating concordance between five hypersomnia indices. Despite considerable differences in wording, administration format (self-report, semi-structured clinical interview, prospective daily diary), and number of persons identified as having hypersomnia, correlations between the indices were medium-to-large (0.42 to 0.57). One interpretation of this finding is that each index shares an underlying construct (Campbell & Fiske 1959). The only exception to this concordance was the semi-structured DSISD. This may be explained by a difference in emphasis; unlike the other four indices which emphasize prolonged nighttime sleep, the DSISD focuses on excessive daytime sleepiness.

We further evaluated the ability of each index to predict future depressive symptoms. Two indices, the BDI-II and the IDS-C, displayed the best balance of detection statistics. We offer two possible accounts for this finding. First, whereas all other indices asked only about the quantity of time slept or sleepiness, the BDI-II asked about change from the usual time slept – thereby differentiating individuals experiencing hypersomnia from mere long sleepers. Second, the IDS-C yielded a much lower rate of hypersomnia than the IDS-SR (24.6% vs. 50%) and also displayed considerably higher predictive power relative to the self-report version (PPV=0.67 vs. 0.32). Perhaps the prompts included in this clinician-administered measure more carefully discerned patients with hypersomnia. Taken together, these results raise the possibility that an improved assessment for hypersomnia might take the form of a clinician-administered interview and include an emphasis on change from habitual sleep time.

Previous research has raised the possibility that hypersomnia may be better characterized by TIB rather than by TST (Billiard et al. 1994; Nofzinger et al. 1991). Hence, our third aim was to investigate this distinction. Interestingly, only two individuals with bipolar disorder slept on average greater than nine hours across seven days of sleep measurement. In contrast, 17 individuals with bipolar disorder spent greater than nine hours in bed. TIB was more highly correlated with all other indices of hypersomnia than TST. These results, consistent with our hypothesis (Kaplan & Harvey, 2009) and the findings from previous researchers (Billiard et al. 1994; Nofzinger et al. 1991), point to the importance of TIB in understanding hypersomnia.

Our fourth and final aim was to evaluate the relationship between hypersomnia in the inter-episode period and future depressive symptoms. Partially supporting our hypothesis, we found that hypersomnia, determined via indices with the best balance of predictive statistics (the BDI-II and the IDS-C), was correlated with future depressive symptoms. This relationship held even after controlling for baseline depression symptoms and omitting the hypersomnia item from the total depression symptom score at follow-up. Ths finding is consistent with prospective research on sleep disturbance and unipolar depression (Breslau et al. 1996; Ford & Cooper-Patrick 2001), and extends it by raising the possibility that hypersomnia also plays a role in bipolar depression. Even so, we emphasize the association may not be interpreted causally.

Several limitations should be noted. First, several of the hypersomnia indices were items taken from depression symptom questionnaires. Though any one individual item from a measure is less reliable than the measure as a whole, these items were included as a first step in indexing hypersomnia. Second, prospective studies on hypersomnia and unipolar depression have utilized a one-year follow-up (Breslau et al. 1996; Ford & Cooper-Patrick 2001), although studies suggest bipolar depression can last anywhere from two to four months (Frankle et al. 2002; Furukawa et al. 2000). Hence, future research should consider multiple assessment points and/or the use of retrospective charting techniques (e.g. Denicoff et al. 2000) to fully elucidate temporal relationships. Third, we emphasize the need for replication in larger samples. Fourth, we acknowledge that this study was based entirely on subjective estimates of sleep. Though sleep diaries are the current gold standard in insomnia research (Buysse et al. 2006) and patient estimates of sleep are typically the only information clinicians can draw upon, future research should use a multi-method approach utilizing both subjective (self-report) and objective (actigraphy and polysomnography) estimates of sleep. Finally and regrettably, we did not reassess medication use at follow-up. Medication adjustment or discontinuation may have influenced depression symptoms. This limitation should be corrected in future studies.

Interestingly, in the present study there were no differences in medication class, number of medications taken, or overall medication load scores between individuals who did and did not report hypersomnia at baseline. However, we note that medications present a dilemma in research on serious mental illness. As we have reviewed elsewhere (Harvey et al. 2009), sleep-related medication side effects are present in 4% to 37% of patients with bipolar disorder (Glaxo Smith Kline 2005; PDR Staff 2007), and atypical antipsychotics in particular are known for their sedative properties (e.g. Kane & Sharif 2008). However, several medications commonly prescribed for bipolar disorder can be associated with either sedating or alerting side effects (e.g., ariprazole, venlafaxine, sertraline; Glaxo Smith Kline 2005; PDR Staff 2007), and it is not uncommon for individuals to experience side effects only early in treatment (Ketter & Wang 2002). As research in medication-free bipolar samples is unrepresentative and lacks generalizeability, we did not pursue this approach here.

Acknowledgments

The authors also wish to thank Ilana Hairston, Ph.D. for her contributions to the study protocol and Julia Fitton, Amanda Purcell, & Quinn Yowell for their assistance.

The authors thank Sheri Johnson, Ph.D. for her help and suggestions. This project was supported by a National Institute of Mental Health Grant No. R34 MH080958 awarded to AGH, a National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship Grant awarded to KAK, and Ruth L. Kirschtein National Research Service Awards (NRSA) Institutional Research Training Grant T32 in Affective Science awarded to LST and JG.

Role of Funding Source:

This project was supported by a National Institute of Mental Health Grant No. R34 MH080958 awarded to AGH, a National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship Grant awarded to KAK, and Ruth L. Kirschtein National Research Service Awards (NRSA) Institutional Research Training Grant T32 in Affective Science awarded to LST and JG.These institutions had no further role in study design; in the collection, analysis and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; and in the decision to submit the paper for publication.

Footnotes

Disclosure:

All authors report no competing interests.

Contributors:

The study was designed and data were collected by A.G. Harvey, J. Gruber, L. Talbot & P. Eidelman. The bulk of the manuscript was written by K. Kaplan and A.G. Harvey. J. Gruber assisted with writing and editing the manuscript, and L.S. Talbot and P. Eidelman contributed to the research and writing on medication use. All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Akiskal HS, Benazzi F. Atypical depression: a variant of bipolar II or a bridge between unipolar and bipolar II? Journal of affective disorders. 2005;84(2):209–217. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2004.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Almeida JR, et al. Reduced gray matter volume in ventral prefrontal cortex but not amygdala in bipolar disorder: Significant effects of gender and trait anxiety. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging. 2009;171(1):54–68. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.02.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Beck AT, Steer R, Brown GK. Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II) San Antonio (TX): Psychological Corporation; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  4. Benazzi F. Symptoms of depression as possible markers of bipolar II disorder. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry. 2006;30(3):471–477. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2005.11.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Billiard M, et al. Hypersomnia associated with mood disorders: a new perspective. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 1994;38(Suppl 1):41–7. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(94)90134-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bowden CL. A different depression: clinical distinctions between bipolar and unipolar depression. Journal of affective disorders. 2005;84(2):117–125. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(03)00194-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Breslau N, et al. Sleep disturbance and psychiatric disorders: A longitudinal epidemiological study of young Adults. Biological psychiatry. 1996;39(6):411–418. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(95)00188-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Buysse DJ, et al. Recommendations for a standard research assessment of insomnia. Sleep. 2006;29(9):1155–1173. doi: 10.1093/sleep/29.9.1155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Campbell DT, Fiske DW. Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychological bulletin. 1959;56(2):81–105. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Davis JM, Chen N. Dose response and dose equivalence of antipsychotics. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2004;24(2):192–200. doi: 10.1097/01.jcp.0000117422.05703.ae. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Denicoff KD, et al. Validation of the Prospective NIMH-Life-Chart Method (NIMH-LCMTM-P) for Longitudinal Assessment of Bipolar Illness. Psychological Medicine. 2000;30(06):1391–1397. doi: 10.1017/s0033291799002810. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Detre T, et al. Hypersomnia and manic-depressive disease. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 1972;128(10):1303–5. doi: 10.1176/ajp.128.10.1303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Edinger JD, et al. American Academy of Sleep Medicine Work Group. Derivation of research diagnostic criteria for insomnia: report of an American Academy of Sleep Medicine Work Group. Sleep. 2004;27(8):1567–1596. doi: 10.1093/sleep/27.8.1567. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. First MB, et al. Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV non-patient edition (SCID-NP) New York State Psychiatric Institute; New York: 1995. [Google Scholar]
  15. Ford DE, Cooper-Patrick L. Sleep disturbances and mood disorders: An epidemiologic perspective. Depression and Anxiety. 2001;14(1):3–6. doi: 10.1002/da.1041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Frankle WG, et al. Bipolar depression: relationship between episode length and antidepressant treatment. Psychological Medicine. 2002;32(08):1417–1423. doi: 10.1017/s0033291702006165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Furukawa TA, et al. Course and outcome of depressive episodes: comparison between bipolar, unipolar and subthreshold depression. Psychiatry Research. 2000;96(3):211–220. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(00)00212-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Glaxo Smith Kline. Psychotropic prescribing guide (Physician's desk reference) 8. NY: Thomson PDR; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  19. Harvey AG, et al. Sleep-related functioning in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder, patients with insomnia, and subjects without sleep problems. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2005;162(1):50–57. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.1.50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Harvey AG, Talbot LS, Gershon A. Sleep disturbance in bipolar disorder across the lifespan. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. 2009;16:256–277. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2850.2009.01164.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Joffe R, et al. A prospective, longitudinal study of percentage of time spent ill in patients with bipolar I or bipolar II disorders. Bipolar Disorders. 2004;6:62–66. doi: 10.1046/j.1399-5618.2003.00091.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Judd LL, et al. The Long-term Natural History of the Weekly Symptomatic Status of Bipolar I Disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002;59(6):530–537. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.6.530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Kane JM, Sharif ZA. Atypical Antipsychotics: Sedation Versus Efficacy. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2008;69(1):18–31. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kaplan KA, Harvey AG. Hypersomnia across mood disorders: A review and synthesis. Sleep Medicine Reviews. 2009;13:275–285. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2008.09.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Ketter TA, Wang PW. Psychotropic medications in bipolar disorder: Pharmacodynamics, pharmacokinetics, drug interactions, adverse effects and their management. In: Yatham LN, Kutcher SP, Kusumakar V, editors. Bipolar disorder: A clinician's guide to biological treatments. London: Routledge; 2002. p. 320. [Google Scholar]
  26. Leibenluft E, Clark CH, Myers FS. The reproducibility of depressive and hypomanic symptoms across repeated episodes in patients with rapid-cycling bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders. 1995;33(2):83–88. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(94)00077-m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. MacQueen GM, et al. Subsyndromal symptoms assessed in longitudinal, prospective follow-up of a cohort of patients with bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders. 2003;5(5):349–355. doi: 10.1034/j.1399-5618.2003.00048.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Matza LS, et al. Depression With Atypical Features in the National Comorbidity Survey: Classification, Description, and Consequences. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60(8):817–826. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.8.817. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Nofzinger E, et al. Hypersomnia in bipolar depression: a comparison with narcolepsy using the multiple sleep latency test. Am J Psychiatry. 1991;148(9):1177–1181. doi: 10.1176/ajp.148.9.1177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. PDR Staff. Physicians Desk Reference 2008: Hospital/Library Version (Physicians' Desk Reference) 62. Physician's Desk Reference; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  31. Perlis RH, et al. Predictors of Recurrence in Bipolar Disorder: Primary Outcomes From the Systematic Treatment Enhancement Program for Bipolar Disorder (STEP-BD) Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163(2):217–224. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.2.217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Phillips ML, et al. Medication Effects in Neuroimaging Studies of Bipolar Disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165(3):313–320. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07071066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Rush AJ, et al. The Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology (IDS): psychometric properties. Psychological Medicine. 1996;26(3):477. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700035558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Sackeim HA. The definition and meaning of treatment-resistant depression. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2001;62(Suppl 16):10–17. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Young R, et al. A rating scale for mania: reliability, validity and sensitivity. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 1978;133(5):429–435. doi: 10.1192/bjp.133.5.429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES