Abstract
Increased age has been found to be associated with differences in affect and personality which have been interpreted in terms of better emotional regulation and increased maturity. However, these findings have largely been based on self-report data, and the primary goal of the current research was to investigate the hypothesis that age-related differences in affect and in certain desirable personality traits might, at least partially, reflect age differences in social desirability. As expected, increased age was associated with lower levels of negative affect and neuroticism and higher levels of positive affect, life satisfaction, agreeableness and conscientiousness, and scores on the social desirability scale were positively related to age and to desirable self-report characteristics, but negatively related to undesirable self-report characteristics. Importantly, controlling for the variance in the social desirability measure resulted in less positive age trends in both types of self-report measures.
Keywords: self-reports, affect, personality, social desirability, aging
Advancing age has been found to be associated with a decrease in self-reported negative affect (e.g., Charles, Reynolds, & Gatz, 2001; Gross, Carstensen, Pasupathi, Tsai, Götestam Skorpen, & Hsu, 1997; Kobau, Safran, Zack, Moriarty, & Chapman, 2004; Teachman, 2006), although possibly with an increase in later years of adulthood (e.g., Teachman, 2006). Furthermore, positive affect and other indicators of subjective well-being such as life satisfaction have been found either to remain stable with advancing age or to increase slightly (e.g., Carstensen, Pasupathi, Mayr, & Nesselroade, 2000; Charles et al., 2001; Mroczek & Kolarz, 1998; Siedlecki, Tucker-Drob, Oishi & Salthouse, 2008). Finally, a number of studies have reported age-related decreases in neuroticism but age-related increases in agreeableness and conscientiousness (e.g., Bleidorn, Kandler, & Spinath, 2009; McCrae, Martin & Costa, 2005; Roberts & Mroczek, 2008; Srivastava, John, Gosling & Potter, 2003; Terracciano, McCrae, Brant & Costa, 2005). Various combinations of these results have been interpreted as reflecting age-related increases in maturity (e.g., Robert & Mroczek, 2008) or adjustment (Staudinger & Kessler, 2009), and better emotion regulation (e.g., Carstensen, Fung, & Charles, 2003; Carstensen, et al., 2000; see also Labouvie-Vief, Diehl, Jain & Zhang, 2007).
The findings indicating age-related increases in aspects of affect and personality are intriguing because they are in contrast to results on cognitive functioning which present a less positive picture of aging (e.g., for reviews see Craik & Salthouse, 2008; Salthouse, 2010). However, many of the relationships between age and both affect and personality are based on self reports, and it is possible that they might at least partially reflect aspects of self-presentation bias or social desirability rather than true shifts in affect and personality traits. In fact, several empirical findings are consistent with this response bias interpretation. For example, Chen, Dai, Spector, and Jex (1997) found that items designed to assess positive affect were much more likely to be endorsed by people who score high on a measure of social desirability. Furthermore, personality traits judged more desirable are also much more likely to be endorsed by people who score high on measures of social desirability(e.g., Bäckström, Björklund, & Larsson, 2009; DeYoung, Peterson, & Higgins, 2002; Edwards, 1966; Konstabel, Aavik, & Allik, 2006; see also Kuncel & Tellegen, 2009). There are also several reports that simple instructions to fake good can distort the factor structure of Big Five personality measures (e.g., Ellingson, Sackett, & Hough, 1999; Hirsch & Peterson, 2008) and reduce the predictive validity of the personality questionnaire to insignificance (Hirsch & Peterson, 2008).
One possible method of investigating the influence of response bias on age relations in self-reported measures of personality and affect consists of administering a social desirability questionnaire designed to assess people’s tendency to present themselves in an overly positive manner, and then partialling social desirability variation from measures of personality and affect. If social desirability scales truly assess a favorable response style, removal of variance due to social desirability in personality and affect scales could be hypothesized to lead to more accurate assessments of personality and affect variables.
However, there has been controversy regarding whether social desirability scales assess a response style or bias that may distort self-reports, or whether they assess a substantively meaningful trait closely related to certain personality traits (e.g. McCrae & Costa, 1983; Nicholson & Hogan, 1990; Smith & Ellingson, 2002). To the extent that social desirability scales measure a stable disposition to behave in a particular manner, and not merelyto produce favorable self report responses, partialling variance associated with social desirability scales may be removing meaningful variance from the relevant trait, and may not increase the validity of personality measures or subjective well-being measures (e.g., Diener, 1994 for a review on relations between subjective well-being and social desirability scales; Diener, Sandvik, Pavot & Gallagher, 1991; Kozma & Stones, 1988; Kurtz, Tarquini, Iobst, 2008; Li & Bagger, 2006; McCrae & Costa, 1983; Ones, Vieswesvaran, & Reiss, 1996). For example, the Marlowe-Crowne scale used in the current project has been interpreted as measuring a trait of defensiveness (e.g., Paulhus, 1984; Thomsen, Mehlsen, Viidik, Sommerlund, & Zachariae, 2005; Weinberger, 1990; Weinberger, Schwartz & Davidson, 1979), in which case it would represent a stable coping style rather than merely a self-presentation bias.
Regardless of the interpretation of social desirability scales, it is of interest to examine the relation of age with personality and affect measures after removing the contribution of the Marlowe-Crowne social desirability measure. If the age relations remained unchanged, then it could be inferred that neither a self-presentation bias nor a stable disposition to think or behave in a socially desirable manner distorts the age trends in those characteristics. In contrast, if age differences in personality or affect are altered by control of scores on the social desirability scale, two alternative interpretations are possible. One interpretation is that self-presentation biases distort the relations between age and various self-report measures. However, an alternative interpretation is that there could be considerable overlap in the disposition to think and behave in a socially desirable manner and the self report measure under consideration. A discovery of significant mediation would be interesting in both cases, but in the former it would reflect the operation of a response bias and in the latter it would indicate the presence of covarying dispositions.
The primary goal of the current project was to investigate the hypothesis that some of the age differences on personality and affect are mediated through social desirability, as assessed with the Marlowe-Crowne scale. Several studies have investigated the overall relation of this particular social desirability scale to personality and affect variables (e.g., Kurtz et al., 2008; McCrae & Costa, 1983; Lewinsohn, Seeley, Roberts, & Allen, 1997; Tanaka-Matsumi & Kameoka, 1986). Furthermore, there is evidence that social desirability scores tend to be higher at older ages (e.g., Erskine, Kvavilashvili, Conway, & Myers, 2007; Kozma & Stones, 1988; Ray, 1988; see also Ray & Lovejoy, 2003; Stöber, 2001; Thomsen et al., 2005). However, there have apparently not been any prior investigations of possible age differences in the role of social desirability in personality and affect assessments.
Specifically, we examined the role of social desirability as a potential mediator of the relations between age and the Big Five personality traits and self-ratings of positive and negative affect (mediation model). Because correlation does not necessarily reflect causality, an overlap between social desirability and self-reports of personality and affect would be consistent either with the possibility that age-related differences in social desirability contribute to age differences in personality and affect measures, or with the idea that age-related differences in personality and affect contribute to age differences in levels of social desirability. Although it is not possible to determine causal direction between the two sets of variables in the current study, the degree of overlap of the variables can be examined after controlling influences of the other variable. The primary goal of the current project was therefore to decompose the age relations in self-report measures of personality and affect by examining age trends before and after statistical control of social desirability.
Based on prior studies, we expected significant relations of social desirability to Neuroticism, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and to positive and negative affect (e.g., Diener et al., 1991; Ones et al., 1996; Thomsen et al., 2005), in the direction of lower levels of Neuroticism and negative affect, and higher levels of Agreeableness, Conscientiousness and positive affect for people who score higher on social desirability scales. Only weak relations of social desirability have been reported with Extraversion and Openness (e.g., Ones et al., 1996), but these traits were also examined in the current project for comparison purposes.
We also expected that increased age would be associated with more positive, or less negative, self-reports, in the form of a decrease in measures of negative affect and neuroticism, and an increase in measures of positive affect, life satisfaction, agreeableness and conscientiousness. (Although this relation could be obscured if a suppressor variable was operating, significant age relations were evident on most of the variables in the current study). Second, advancing age was expected to be associated with higher levels of social desirability. Third, higher social desirability was predicted to be associated with lower levels of undesirable characteristics such as negative affect and neuroticism, and with higher levels of desirable characteristics such as positive affect, life satisfaction, agreeableness and conscientiousness when controlling for age. Fourth, and most importantly, if social desirability is functioning as a mediator, the relation of age to self reports of affect and personality was predicted to be reduced after controlling the variation in social desirability measure.
In order to determine whether the mediational results were specific to people’s tendency to present themselves in a particular manner, and therefore to self-report variables, similar analyses were carried out with objective composite measures of cognitive ability as potential mediators of the age differences in the self-report variables, and with social desirability as a potential mediator of the age differences in the cognitive measures.
The predictions were investigated in a moderately large sample of adults across a wide age range, who each completed several questionnaires assessing aspects of affect and personality in addition to the Marlowe-Crowne social desirability scale (Crowne & Marlowe, 1960). The sample size of over 1,100 provided sufficient power to examine moderate and small effect sizes.
Method
Participants
The sample consisted of 1,175 adults, 63% female, ranging from 18 to 93 years of age who all had scores on the MMSE (Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975) of 27 or higher. Participants were recruited through newspaper advertisements, flyers and referrals from other participants. Further descriptions of the participants, and the methods of recruitment, are provided elsewhere (e.g. Salthouse, Pink, & Tucker-Drob, 2008). Most of the participants had at least some college education, and the correlation between age and amount of education was .24 (p<.01). Self-reported health status and health-related limitations were somewhat lower with increased age, with correlations of .08 (p<.01) and .22 (p<.01), respectively, on scales where 1 was “excellent” or ”none” and 5 was ”poor” or “a great deal”.
Materials and Procedure
Social desirability measure
The Need for Approval Scale (Crowne & Marlowe, 1960) was administered to assess social desirability. This scale consists of 33 true-false statements to which the participants indicated whether they agreed or not. Items in the scale relate to behaviors which are “culturally sanctioned or approved but which are improbable of occurrence” (Crowne & Marlowe, 1960, p.350). Examples of items are “I have never deliberately said something that hurt someone’s feelings” or “I have never been irked when people expressed ideas very different from my own”. The coefficient alpha reliability for the total scale in this sample was .83.
Paulhus (1984) proposed distinguishing two components of Social Desirability Self-deceptive Enhancement (SDE) related to an honest but overly positive view of oneself, and Impression Management (IM), referring to the deliberate distortion of responses in order to create a more positive social image. Earlier studies (e.g., Leite & Beretvas, 2005) with the Marlowe-Crowne questionnaire have considered these two subscales separately, and therefore they were examined in the current project along with the total score. In the current sample, the SDE subscale had a coefficient alpha reliability coefficient of .74, and the IM scale had a reliability coefficient of .70. However, the two subscales were correlated .69, and because they had nearly identical correlations with the affect and personality variables and with age, only the total score was considered in subsequent analyses.
Affect measures
Measures of affect or mood included the Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression scale (CES-D; Radloff, 1977) which had a coefficient alpha reliability of .90 in this study, the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; Spielberger, Gorsush, Lushene, Vagg, & Jacobs, 1983) with a coefficient alpha reliability of .94, the Positive and Negative Affect Scale (PANAS; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988), with coefficient alpha of .90 for the positive affect subscale, and .88 for the negative affect subscale, and the Satisfaction with Life Scale (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985; Pavot & Diener, 1993) which had a coefficient alpha reliability of .91.
Personality measures
The participants also completed the International Personality Item Pool questionnaire (Goldberg, 1999; 50-item version). All five factors had good reliability: neuroticism (α=.89), extraversion (α=.87), openness (α=.79), agreeableness (α=.78), and conscientiousness (α=.79).
Cognitive measures
Composite cognitive scores of fluid intelligence, perceptual speed and episodic memory were used in some analyses. Fluid Intelligence was assessed with tests of reasoning and spatial visualization. Reasoning was assessed with the Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices (Raven, 1962), Shipley Abstraction (Zachary, 1986), and Letter Sets (Ekstrom, French, Harman, & Dermen, 1976) tests. Spatial visualization was assessed with the Spatial Relations test from the Differential Aptitude Test Battery (Bennett, Seashore & Wesman, 1997), the Paper Folding test from the Educational Testing Service Kit of Factor-Referenced Cognitive Tests (Ekstrom et al., 1976), and the Form Boards test (Ekstrom et al., 1976). Speed was measured with Digit Symbol (Wechsler, 1997a), Letter Comparison (Salthouse & Babcock, 1991), and Pattern Comparison (Salthouse & Babcock, 1991) tests. Episodic memory was assessed with Logical Memory from the Wechsler Memory Scale-Third Edition (Wechsler, 1997b), the Word List Test from the Wechsler Memory Scale-Third Edition (Wechsler, 1997b), and a Paired Associates test developed locally (Salthouse, Fristoe & Rhee, 1996). The cognitive variables were converted into z-scores, and composite cognitive scores were created by averaging z-scores of cognitive variables assumed to measure the same ability.
Results
We first examined if our data replicated previous findings regarding the relations of age, social desirability, and self-report variables of personality and affect. Advancing age (the independent variable) was expected to be associated with higher levels of social desirability (the mediator). Higher levels of social desirability (the mediator) were expected to be associated with lower levels of undesirable characteristics such as negative affect and neuroticism, and with higher levels of desirable characteristics such as positive affect, life satisfaction, agreeableness and conscientiousness (dependent variables).
To determine the magnitudes of the mediated, or indirect, effects, we examined the relations of age to affect and personality after controlling the variation in the social desirability measure. Indirect effects were estimated with the bootstrap procedure described by Preacher et al., (2007; see also Hayes, 2009). Information from the bootstrap sampling distribution was used to generate confidence intervals for the indirect effects (Preacher, Rucker, & Hayes, 2007).
Relations of age to personality and affect measures
The correlations among age, affect and personality variables are displayed in Table 1. It can be seen that there were significant correlations of age with all of the affect and personality variables, except Extraversion and Openness. Increased age was negatively related to variables assessing negative affect (i.e. depression, anxiety, negative affect and neuroticism), and positively related to positive affect, life satisfaction, agreeableness and conscientiousness. These results replicate earlier findings in that increased age was related to higher scores in all of the desirable characteristics, i.e. positive affect, life satisfaction, agreeableness, conscientiousness and to lower scores in all of the undesirable characteristics, i.e. negative affect and neuroticism.
Table 1.
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 | 12 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Age | 1 | |||||||||||
2. Social Desirability | .23* | 1 | ||||||||||
3. Neuroticism | −.21* | −.42* | 1 | |||||||||
4. Extraversion | .01 | .11* | −.17* | 1 | ||||||||
5. Openness | −.06 | −.03 | −.04 | .24* | 1 | |||||||
6. Agreeableness | .11* | .29* | −.21* | .31* | .19* | 1 | ||||||
7. Conscientiousness | .21* | .30* | −.30* | .04 | −.04 | .26* | 1 | |||||
8. CES-D | −.21* | −.32* | .64* | −.21* | −.02 | −.23* | −.37* | 1 | ||||
9. PANAS Negative | −.17* | −.22* | .49* | −.11* | −.06 | −.18* | −.25* | .59* | 1 | |||
10. Trait Anxiety | −.28* | −.41* | .76* | −.26* | −.03 | −.25* | −.39* | .82* | .56* | 1 | ||
11. PANAS Positive | .16* | .30* | −.32* | .31* | .20* | .27* | .22* | −.34* | −.12* | −.40* | 1 | |
12. Life Satisfaction | .17* | .26* | −.47* | .24* | .01 | .22* | .25* | −.61* | −.38* | −.65* | .31* | 1 |
Mean | 51.8 | 16.6 | 25.6 | 31.8 | 36.7 | 40.8 | 36.9 | 11.4 | 12.9 | 35.0 | 30.5 | 23.1 |
SD | 17.7 | 6.1 | 8.2 | 7.7 | 6.2 | 5.5 | 6.2 | 8.7 | 4.6 | 10.6 | 7.8 | 7.3 |
p<.01
The slight negative relation of age to Openness was also consistent with previous reports (e.g. Roberts & Mroczek, 2008; Srivastava et al., 2003), although the trend was not significant in the current study. There was no relation between age and Extraversion, and prior research has been inconsistent as there are reports of both positive (e.g., Roberts & Mroczek, 2008) and negative (e.g., Srivastava, et al., 2003) age relations.
Nonlinear age relations on the affect and personality measures were also examined. The quadratic relation of age to Conscientiousness was significant (β = −.09, t[1131] = −2.87, p<.01), and accounted for .07% of variance (R2 = .007, F[1, 1131] = 8.211, p<.01). The cubic relation of age to CES-D was significant (β =.18, t[1149] = 2.68, p<.01), and accounted for 0.6% of variance (R2 = .006, F[1, 1149] = 7.16, p<.01). Both quadratic (β = .14, t[1166] = −4.72, p<.01) and cubic (β = −.21, t[1166] = −3.06, p<.01) age relations were significant with the life satisfaction variable, accounting for 1.8% (R2 = .018, F[1, 1166] = 22.23, p<.01) and 0.8% (R2 = .008, F[1, 1165] = 9.34, p<.01) of variance, respectively. Because the R2 values associated with the nonlinear terms were very small, and because inclusion of these terms had very little effect on the conclusions, only the linear relations were considered in subsequent analyses.
Relation of age to social desirability
The correlation between age and the social desirability total score was .23, indicating higher levels of social desirability with increased age. Nonlinear age trends were also examined in regression analyses, but neither the quadratic nor the cubic age terms were significant, suggesting that the relation of age to social desirability was primarily linear.
Relations of social desirability to self-reported affect and personality
The second column in Table 1 contains correlations between social desirability and the self-reported affect and personality variables. It can be seen that all of the correlations were significant, in a negative direction for the variables related to negative affect (depression, anxiety, negative affect and neuroticism), and in a positive direction for self-reported positive affect, life satisfaction, agreeableness and conscientiousness. Extraversion was also slightly related to social desirability, in a positive direction. Nonlinear trends between social desirability and affect and personality were also examined. There was a significant quadratic relation of social desirability on neuroticism (β = −.07, t[1127] = −2.75, p<.01) but the quadratic term was associated with only 0.6% of the variance (R2 = .006, F[1, 1127] = 7.58, p<.01), and thus it was ignored in subsequent analyses.
Relation of age to self-reported affect and personality before and after controlling for social desirability
Analyses were conducted to formally test whether at least some of the age-related decrease in negative affect and neuroticism and some of the age-related increase in positive affect, life satisfaction, agreeableness and conscientiousness might be attributable to an age-related increase in social desirability.
The indirect effect was quantified as the product of its constituent paths, which is equivalent to the difference between the total effect of the independent variable on the dependent variable and the effect of the independent variable on the dependent variable after the statistical control of the mediator (e.g., Preacher et al., 2007; Hayes, 2009). Results of mediation analyses, in the form of age correlations before and after controlling for social desirability (from regression analyses), are reported in the second and third columns of Table 2. It can be seen that all of the negative age correlations were less negative, and all of the positive correlations were less positive, after controlling the variance in the social desirability score. Results were unchanged after also controlling for sex, self-reported health, and education. Moreover, similar patterns were obtained when the Self-Deception and Impression Management subscales were used as separate indicators of social desirability.
Table 2.
Age | Age after SD | Stand. IE | Boot. Unstand. IE | Boot. 95% CI | Age variance explained by SD(%) | N | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Social Desirability | .23* | ||||||
Self-report variables | |||||||
Neuroticism | −.21* | −.12* | −.10* | −0.04 | [−0.05, −0.03] | 67.4 | 1130 |
Extraversion | .01 | −.02 | .03* | 0.01 | [0.00, 0.02] | NA | 1136 |
Openness | −.06 | −.06 | −.01 | 0.00 | [−0.01, 0.00] | NA | 1139 |
Agreeableness | .11* | .04 | .07* | 0.02 | [0 .01, 0.03] | 86.8 | 1139 |
Conscientiousness | .21* | .15* | .07* | 0.02 | [0.02, 0.03] | 49.0 | 1134 |
PANAS Positive | .16* | .09* | .07* | 0.03 | [0.02, 0.04] | 68.4 | 1167 |
PANAS Negative | −.17* | −.13* | −.05* | − 0.01 | [−0.02, −0.01] | 41.5 | 1170 |
CES-D | −.21* | −.14* | −.07* | −0.03 | [−0.04, −0.02] | 55.6 | 1153 |
Anxiety Trait | −.28* | −.19* | −.09* | −0.05 | [−0.07, −0.04] | 54.0 | 1158 |
Life Satisfaction | .17* | .12* | .06* | 0.02 | [0.03, 0.02] | 50.2 | 1169 |
Cognitive variables | |||||||
Fluid ability | −.45* | −.42* | − .05 | 0.00 | [0.00, 0.00] | NA | 694 |
Memory | −.43* | −.41* | −.04 | 0.00 | [0.00, 0.00] | NA | 709 |
Speed | −.59* | −.57* | −.04 | 0.00 | [0.00, 0.00] | NA | 711 |
Note – The relations of age to each personality, affect and cognitive variable before (Column 1 of the Table) and after (Column 2 of the Table) controlling for social desirability variation were examined in regression analyses. Percentages of age variance associated with social desirability are reported in Column 6. Age after SD is age relations after controlling for the variance in social desirability (SD) score. Stand. IE are standardized indirect effects; Boot.Unstand. IE are bootstrapped unstandardized indirect effects; Boot. 95% CI are bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals for the indirect effect.
p<.01
The bootstrapping procedure described by Preacher and al. (2007) was used to estimate indirect effects and generate 95% confidence intervals for those effects. The indirect effects are reported in fourth and fifth columns of Table 2, with the confidence intervals reported in the sixth column. The mediation effect of social desirability on the age relations was significant (p<.01) for all self-report variables except for Openness. Percentages of age-related variance in the self-report variables that were explained by social desirability are reported in the seventh column of Table 2. They varied from 41.5% for the PANAS Negative scale to 86.8% for Agreeableness.
Because it is possible that the relations between social desirability and self-reports of mood and personality vary as a function of age, interactive effects of age and social desirability on self-reports were examined. None of the interactions was significant, and thus there was no evidence that the relations between social desirability and the self report measures were moderated by age.
We also tested moderated mediation models (Preacher et al., 2007; Hayes, 2009) in which age was both an independent variable and a moderator variable. In none of the models was there evidence that that the mediation relations were contingent on age, and thus there was no evidence that the relations between social desirability and the self report measures were moderated by age.
Because many studies on age effects involve a contrast of only young and old adults, the self-report measures were compared before and after controlling for social desirability in young adults (under age 30) and older adults (over age 65) to provide an indication of the amount distortion in comparisons of self-reported affect and personality between these two age groups. The d values for the differences between the young and older adults before and after controlling for social desirability were, respectively, .70 and .38 for Neuroticism, .68 and .39 for CES-D, .99 and .62 for Anxiety, .64 and .44 for Negative Affect, −.50 and −.26 for Positive Affect, −.49 and −.27 for Life Satisfaction, −.28 and −.05 for Agreeableness, −.65 and −.42 for Conscientiousness, −.05 and .05 for Extraversion, and .22 and .20 for Openness. The percentage reductions in the d value after controlling for the social desirability score were therefore 45% for Neuroticism, 42% for CES-D, 38% for Anxiety, 31% for Negative Affect, 48% for Positive Affect, 45% for Life Satisfaction, 80% for Agreeableness, 36% for Conscientiousness, 12.5% for Extraversion and 10.1% for Openness.
Two sets of control analyses with objective cognitive ability measures were also carried out to examine the specificity of the social desirability mediational results. In one set of analyses, composite scores of fluid intelligence, episodic memory, and perceptual speed were used as potential mediators of the age differences in the self report variables. Our rationale was that mediation of the relation of age to self-report variables should be limited to social desirability. Objective composite measures of cognitive ability were not expected to mediate age differences in the self-report variables. Results of the analyses showed few significant changes in age relations to personality and affect variables after controlling the variance in cognitive variables. And, unlike the social desirability variable, controlling the variance in these cognitive variables resulted in a slight increase in the magnitude of the age relations in self-report variables (with the exception of the relation between Age and Extraversion and Age and PANAS Positive scale which resulted in a slight decrease after control of fluid ability). Bootstrapped indirect effects and 95% confidence intervals are reported in Table 3. It is noteworthy that there were significant indirect effects of the cognitive measures in the relations of age to Openness, and controlling for the level of the cognitive abilities resulted in slightly positive age-Openness relations . These results suggest that the lower levels of the cognitive abilities with age tend to mask small positive relations between age and Openness.
Table 3.
Age | Age after cognition | Stand. IE for Gf/Memory/Speed | Boot. Unstand IE for Gf/Memory/Speed | Boot. 95% CI | N | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Neuroticism | −.23* | |||||
After Gf | −.26* | −.02 | 0.02 | [0.00, 0.03] | 668 | |
Memory | −.25* | −.03 | 0.01 | [−0.01, 0.02] | 681 | |
Speed | −.26* | −.06 | 0.02 | [−0.01, 0.04] | 711 | |
Extraversion | .00 | |||||
After Gf | −.06 | .05* | 0.03* | [0.01, 0.04] | 671 | |
Memory | .01 | −.01 | 0.00 | [−0.02, 0.01] | 686 | |
Speed | .04 | −.02 | −0.01 | [−0.03, 0.01] | 688 | |
Openness | .01 | |||||
After Gf | .11 | −.08* | −0.04* | [−0.05, −0.02] | 673 | |
Memory | .09 | −.07* | −0.03* | [−0.05, −0.02] | 686 | |
Speed | .12 | −.07* | −0.04* | [−0.06, −0.02] | 688 | |
Agreeableness | .11* | |||||
After Gf | .08 | .05 | 0.01 | [0.00, 0.02] | 669 | |
Memory | .13* | .00 | −0.01 | [−0.02, 0.00] | 684 | |
Speed | .19* | −.02* | −0.02* | [−0.04,−0.01] | 685 | |
Conscientiousness | .22* | |||||
After Gf | .23* | .04 | 0.01 | [0.00, 0.02] | 669 | |
Memory | .21* | .04 | 0.00 | [−0.01, 0.01] | 685 | |
Speed | .30* | .03* | −0.03* | [−0.04, −0.01] | 687 | |
PANAS Positive | .17* | |||||
After Gf | .11* | .09* | 0.03* | [0.01, 0.05] | 688 | |
Memory | .13* | .07 | 0.02 | [0.00, 0.04] | 703 | |
Speed | .18* | .06 | 0.00 | [−0.02, 0.02] | 705 | |
PANAS Negative | −.22* | |||||
After Gf | −.24* | −.02 | 0.01 | [−0.01, 0.02] | 691 | |
Memory | −.24* | −.02 | 0.01 | [0.00, 0.01] | 706 | |
Speed | −.26* | −.04 | 0.01 | [0.00, 0.03] | 708 | |
CES-D | −.24* | |||||
After Gf | −.27* | −.02 | 0.02 | [−0.01, 0.04] | 681 | |
Memory | −.26* | −.02 | 0.01 | [−0.01;0.03] | 696 | |
Speed | −.26* | −.07 | 0.01 | [−0.01, 0.04] | 698 | |
Anxiety Trait | −.30* | |||||
After Gf | −.33* | −.04 | 0.02 | [−0.01, 0.04] | 684 | |
Memory | −.32* | −.04 | 0.01 | [−0.01, 0.03] | 699 | |
Speed | −.34* | −.08 | 0.03 | [−0.01, 0.06] | 701 | |
Life Satisfaction | .18* | |||||
After Gf | .22* | .00 | 0.02 | [−0.03, 0.00] | 691 | |
Memory | .22* | .00 | 0.02 | [−0.03, 0.00] | 706 | |
Speed | .25* | −.01 | 0.03 | [−0.04, 0.00] | 708 |
Note: Boot.Unstand. IE are bootstrap unstandardized indirect effects; Boot. 95% CI are bootstrap 95% confidence intervals for the indirect effect.
p<.01
In the second set of analyses social desirability was used as a potential mediator of the age relations on the composite cognitive scores. The purpose of these analyses was to determine whether the mediational results were specific to people’s tendency to present themselves in a particular manner, and therefore to self-report variables. Bootstrap indirect effects and 95% confidence intervals are reported in the bottom of Table 2. None of the age trends in the cognitive scores were significantly changed after controlling for the variance in social desirability scale. Taken in combination, these results suggest that the reduction of the age trends is specific to the social desirability measure and its relations with self-reported personality and affect variables.
Discussion
The major goal of this study was to investigate the role of social desirability in the relations between age and several measures of affect and personality. Previous research has led to a positive view of aging regarding emotional functioning and personality, with increased age associated with lower levels of negative affect, stability and a slight increase in positive affect, and increases in agreeableness and conscientiousness. However, these findings have primarily been based on self-reports, and there have been a number of studies suggesting that social desirability may alter the patterns in self-reported data. We were therefore interested in investigating the possibility that age-related decreases in negative affect and age-related increases in positive affect and some desirable personality traits such as agreeableness and conscientiousness might, at least partially, reflect age differences in social desirability.
As expected from past research, increased age was associated with lower levels of negative affect and neuroticism and higher levels of positive affect, life satisfaction, agreeableness and conscientiousness. Increased age was also related to higher scores on the social desirability scale. Importantly, social desirability was significantly associated with the self-report variables, in a positive direction for desirable characteristics, and in a negative direction for undesirable characteristics.
Our key hypothesis, that some of the age-related differences in self-reported affect and personality might be attributable to the age-related increase in social desirability, was supported. We found that social desirability is an important and significant factor accounting for some of the age variation in self-reported personality and affect measures. That is, statistically controlling the variance in the social desirability measure reduced the magnitude of the age-related variance in the self report variables by between 41% and 87%. These results indicate that the age trends in a variety of self-report measures are somewhat less positive after controlling for influences associated with social desirability.
Control analyses with objective cognitive ability measures were carried out to examine whether the reduction in age trends was specific to the social desirability measure and its relations with self-reported personality and affect variables. In one set of analyses, composite scores of cognitive abilities were used as potential mediators of the age differences in the self report variables. In the second set of analyses social desirability was used as a potential mediator of the age relations on the composite cognitive scores. The results revealed few significant changes in the relations of age to personality and affect variables after controlling the variance in cognitive variables, with the exception of Openness. In addition, none of the age trends in the cognitive scores were significantly changed after controlling for the variance in social desirability scale, suggesting that the reduction of the age trends is specific to the social desirability measure and its relations with self-reported personality and affect variables.
Although the findings supported in the current project are clear and consistent, and were specific to the relations between the social desirability scale and self reports, there are at least two alternative interpretations of the results. One interpretation is based on the idea that the Marlowe-Crowne scale assesses a response bias that distorts “true” trends in self report measures. According to this interpretation, advancing age is associated with a stronger tendency for to present oneself in a positive light, and adjusting the self-report scores for social desirability results in more valid evaluations of the relations between age and self-reported personality and affect.
In contrast, a second interpretation is that social desirability is a substantively meaningful disposition, and not simply a bias to respond in a particular way. From the perspective of this interpretation, the statistical control results indicate that there is overlap between this disposition and certain self report measures. To the extent that the social desirability scale reflects another stable disposition or personality trait, possibly related to defensiveness (e.g., Paulhus, 1984; Thomsen et al., 2005; Weinberger, 1990; Weinberger, Schwartz & Davidson, 1979) and that age trends in self-reports variables reflect true developmental changes and not birth cohort effects (see Charles et al., [2001], Roberts & Mroczek [2008], and Twenge & Im [2007] for analyses of affect, personality and need for approval longitudinal data, respectively), our results suggest that people who score higher on social desirability scales may experience more positive age-related changes in other personality traits and affect than people with lower scores on these scales. Higher levels of this social desirability disposition might therefore be associated with age-related increases in maturity and emotional functioning.
Regardless of how the social desirability scale is interpreted, the major finding of this project is that when everyone is effectively compared at the same level of social desirability, at least as measured by this particular instrument, the age differences in a variety of self-report measures are not as large as they are without this control. In fact, social desirability was found to account for 50% or more of the age-related variance in many of the self report variables. Researchers interested in relations between age and self-report measures with an evaluative dimension may therefore need to consider the role of social desirability when interpreting their results since at least some of the age differences in the self-report measures may be associated with age differences in this characteristic.
To the best of our knowledge, there have not been any prior studies in which the role of social desirability in the age differences in self-report variables has been investigated. Furthermore, this study has a number of strengths, including a moderately large sample, with powerful analyses and fairly precise estimates of the relevant relations. In addition, all of the variables had acceptable levels of internal consistency reliability.
However, there are also some limitations. For example, several personality and affect variables were examined but only one social desirability scale. The Marlowe-Crowne scale had acceptable reliability and in analyses not reported here was found to have similar structure at different ages, but inclusion of alternative measures (e.g., Balanced Inventory of Desirable Responding scale, Paulhus, 1998) may have resulted in a broader assessment of this construct. Second, several of the effects in the project were rather small, with most of the variables having less than 5% of the variance associated with age. Nevertheless, the results were consistent across eight different self-report variables, and control analyses indicated that the meditational patterns were specific to the relations of Marlowe-Crowne scale and self-reported variables.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by a grant from the National Institute on Aging to TAS (R37 024270).
References
- Bäckström M, Björklund F, Larsson MR. Five-factor inventories have a major general factor related to social desirability which can be reduced by framing items neutrally. Journal of Research in Personality. 2009;43:335–344. [Google Scholar]
- Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1986;51:1173–1182. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.51.6.1173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bennett GK, Seashore HG, Wesman AG. Differential Aptitude Test. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Bleidorn W, Kandler C, Spinath FM. Patterns and sources of adult personality development: Growth curve analyses of the NEO PI-R scales in a longitudinal twin study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2009;97(1):142–155. doi: 10.1037/a0015434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carstensen LL, Fung HH, Charles ST. Socioemotional selectivity theory and the regulation of emotion in the second half of life. Motivation & Emotion. 2003;27(2):103–123. [Google Scholar]
- Carstensen LL, Pasupathi M, Mayr U, Nesselroade JR. Emotional experience in everyday life across the adult life span. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2000;79:644–655. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Charles ST, Reynolds CA, Gatz M. Age-related differences and change in positive and negative affect over 23 years. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2001;80(1):136–151. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen PY, Dai T, Spector PE, Jex SM. Relation between negative affectivity and positive affectivity: effects of judged desirability of scale items and respondents’ social desirability. Journal of Personality Assessment. 1997;69(1):183–198. [Google Scholar]
- Craik FIM, Salthouse TA. The Handbook of Aging and Cognition. 3. New York: Psychology Press; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Crowne DP, Marlowe D. A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology. Journal of Consulting Psychology. 1960;24(4):349–354. doi: 10.1037/h0047358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeYoung CG, Peterson JB, Higgins DM. Higher-order factors of the Big Five predict conformity: Are these neurons of health? Personality and Individual Differences. 2002;33:533–552. [Google Scholar]
- Diener E. Assessing subjective well-being: Progress and opportunities. Social Indicators Research. 1994;31:103–157. [Google Scholar]
- Diener E, Sandvik E, Pavot W, Gallagher D. Response artifacts in the measurement of subjective well-being. Social Indicators Research. 1991;24:35–56. [Google Scholar]
- Diener E, Emmons RA, Larsen RJ, Griffin S. The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment. 1985;49(1):71–75. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edwards AL. Relationship between probability of endorsement and social desirability scale value for a set of 2,824 personality statements. Journal of Applied Psychology. 1966;50:238–239. doi: 10.1037/h0023366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ekstrom RB, French JW, Harman HH, Dermen D. Manual for Kit of Factor-Referenced Cognitive Tests. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service; 1976. [Google Scholar]
- Ellingson JE, Sackett PR, Hough LM. Social desirability corrections in personality measurement: Issues of applicant comparison and construct validity. Journal of Applied Psychology. 1999;84:155–166. [Google Scholar]
- Erskine JAK, Kvavilashvili L, Conway MA, Myers L. The effects of age on psychopathology, well-being and repressive coping. Aging and Mental Health. 2007;11:394–404. doi: 10.1080/13607860600963737. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-Mental State”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 1975;12(3):189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldberg LR. A broad-bandwidth, public domain, personality inventory measuring the lower-level facets of several five-factor models. In: Mervielde I, Deary I, De Fruyt F, Ostendorf F, editors. Personality psychology in Europe. Vol. 7. Tilburg, the Netherlands: Tilburg University Press; 1999. pp. 7–28. [Google Scholar]
- Gross JJ, Carstensen LL, Pasupathi M, Tsai J, Götestam Skorpen C, Hsu AYC. Emotion and aging: Experience, expression, and control. Psychology and Aging. 1997;12(4):590–599. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.12.4.590. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hirsch JB, Peterson JB. Predicting creativity and academic success with a “Fake–Proof” measure of the Big Five. Journal of Research in Personality. 2008;42:1323–1333. [Google Scholar]
- Judd CM, Kenny DA. Process analysis: Estimating mediation in treatment evaluations. Evaluation Review. 1981;5:602–619. [Google Scholar]
- Kobau R, Safran MA, Zack MM, Moriarty DG, Chapman D. Sad, blue, or depressed days, health behaviors and health-related quality of life, behavioral risk factor surveillance system, 1995–2000. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes. 2004;2:40–48. doi: 10.1186/1477-7525-2-40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Konstabel K, Aavik T, Allik J. Social desirability and consensual validity of personality traits. European Journal of Personality. 2006;20:549–566. [Google Scholar]
- Kozma A, Stones MJ. Social desirability in measures of subjective well- being: Age comparisons. Social Indicators Research. 1988;20:1–14. [Google Scholar]
- Kuncel NR, Tellegen A. A conceptual and empirical reexamination of the measurement of the social desirability of items: Implications for detecting desirable response style and scale development. Personnel Psychology. 2009;62:201–228. [Google Scholar]
- Kurtz JE, Tarquini SJ, Iobst EA. Socially desirable responding in personality assessment: Still more substance than style. Personality and Individual Differences. 2008;45:22–27. [Google Scholar]
- Labouvie-Vief G, Diehl M, Jain E, Zhang F. Six-year change in optimization and affect complexity across the adult life span: A further examination. Psychology and Aging. 2007;22(4):738–751. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.22.4.738. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leite WL, Beretvas N. Validation of scores on the Marlowe-Crowne social desirability scale and the balanced inventory of desirable responding. Educational and Psychological Measurement. 2005;65(1):140–154. [Google Scholar]
- Lewinsohn PM, Seeley JR, Roberts RE, Allen NB. Center for epidemiologic studies depression scale (CES-D) as a screening instrument for depression among community-residing older adults. Psychology and Aging. 1997;12(2):277–287. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.12.2.277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li A, Bagger J. Using the BIDR to distinguish the effects of the impression management and self-deception on the criterion validity of personality measures: A meta-analysis. International Journal of Selection and Assessment. 2006;14:131–141. [Google Scholar]
- MacKinnon DP, Fairchild AJ, Fritz MS. Mediation analysis. Annual Review of Psychology. 2007;58:593–614. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085542. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCrae RR, Costa PT., Jr Social desirability scales: More substance than style. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1983;51(6):882–888. [Google Scholar]
- McCrae RR, Martin TA, Costa PT., Jr Age trends and age norms for the NEO Personality Inventory-3 in adolescents and adults. Assessment. 2005;12:363–373. doi: 10.1177/1073191105279724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mroczek DK, Kolarz CM. The effect of positive and negative affect: A developmental perspective on happiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998;75(5):1333–1349. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.75.5.1333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nicholson RA, Hogan R. The construct validity of social desirability. The American Psychologist. 1990;45:290–292. [Google Scholar]
- Ones DS, Viswesvaran C, Reiss AD. Role of social desirability in personality testing for personnel selection: The red herring. Journal of Applied Psychology. 1996;81:660–679. [Google Scholar]
- Paulhus DL. Manual for the Paulhus Deception Scales: BIDR Version. Vol. 7. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Paulhus DL. Two-component models of socially desirable responding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1984;46:598–609. [Google Scholar]
- Pavot W, Diener E. Review of the satisfaction with life scale. Psychological Assessment. 1993;5(2):164–172. [Google Scholar]
- Preacher KJ, Rucker DD, Hayes AF. Addressing moderated mediation hypotheses: Theory, methods, and prescriptions. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 2007;42(1):185–227. doi: 10.1080/00273170701341316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement. 1977;1:385–401. [Google Scholar]
- Raven J. Advanced Progressive Matrices, Set II. London: H. K. Lewis; 1962. [Google Scholar]
- Ray JJ. Personality and Individual Differences. 2. Vol. 9. 1988. Lie scales and the elderly; pp. 417–148. [Google Scholar]
- Ray JJ, Lovejoy FH. Age-related social desirability responding among Australian women. The Journal of Social Psychology. 2003;143(5):669–671. doi: 10.1080/00224540309598470. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts BW, Mroczek D. Personality trait change in adulthood. Current directions in Psychological Science. 2008;17(1):31–35. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00543.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salthouse TA. Major issues on cognitive aging. Oxford University Press; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Salthouse TA, Babcock RL. Decomposing adult age differences in working memory. Developmental Psychology. 1991;27:763–776. [Google Scholar]
- Salthouse TA, Fristoe N, Rhee SH. How localized are age-related effects on neuropsychological measures? Neuropsychology. 1996;10:272–285. [Google Scholar]
- Salthouse TA, Pink JE, Tucker-Drob EM. Contextual analysis of fluid intelligence. Intelligence. 2008;36:464–486. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2007.10.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siedlecki KL, Tucker-Drob EM, Oishi S, Salthouse TA. Life satisfaction across adulthood: different determinants at different ages? The Journal of Positive Psychology. 2008;3:153–164. doi: 10.1080/17439760701834602. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith DB, Ellingson JE. Substance versus style: A new look at social desirability in motivating contexts. Journal of Applied Psychology. 2002;87(2):211–219. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.87.2.211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spielberger CD, Gorsush RL, Lushene R, Vagg PR, Jacobs GA. Manual of the state-trait anxiety inventory (Form Y) Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Srivastava S, John OP, Gosling SD, Potter J. Development of personality in early and middle adulthood: Set like plaster or persistent change? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2003;84(5):1041–1053. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.5.1041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Staudinger UM, Kessler E-M. Adjustment and growth Two trajectories of positive personality development across adulthood. In: Smith MC, De Frates-Densch N, editors. Handbook of Research on Learning and Development. New York and London: Routledge; 2009. pp. 241–268. [Google Scholar]
- Stöber J. The Social Desirability Scale 17 (SDS-17). Convergent validity, discriminant validity, and relationship with age. European Journal of Psychological Assessment. 2001;17:222–232. [Google Scholar]
- Tanaka-Matsumi J, Kameoka VA. Reliabilities and concurrent validities of popular self-report measures of depression, anxiety, and social desirability. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1986;54(3):328–333. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.54.3.328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teachman BA. Aging and negative affect: The rise and fall and rise of anxiety and depression symptoms. Psychology and Aging. 2006;21(1):201–207. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Terracciano A, McCrae RR, Brant LJ, Costa PT., Jr Hierarchical linear modeling analyses of the NEO-PI-R scales in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Psychology and Aging. 2005;20:493–506. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.3.493. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomsen DK, Mehlsen MY, Viidik A, Sommerlund B, Zachariae R. Age and gender differences in negative affect-is there a role for emotion regulation? Personality and Individual Differences. 2005;38:1935–1945. [Google Scholar]
- Twenge JM, Im C. Changes in the need for social approval, 1958–2001. Journal of Research in Personality. 2007;41:171–189. [Google Scholar]
- Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1988;54(6):1063. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.54.6.1063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. 3. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1997a. [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. Wechsler Memory Scale. 3. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1997b. [Google Scholar]
- Weinberger DA. The construct validity of the repressive coping style. In: Singer JL, editor. Repression and dissociation: Implications for personality theory, psychopathology, and health. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1990. pp. 337–386. [Google Scholar]
- Weinberger DA, Schwartz GE, Davidson RJ. Low-anxious, high-anxious, and repressive coping styles: Psychometric patterns and behavioral and physiological responses to stress. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1979;88:369–380. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.88.4.369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zachary RA. Shipley Institute of Living Scale—Revised. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services; 1986. [Google Scholar]