Abstract
Facial masking in Parkinson’s disease is the reduction of automatic and controlled expressive movement of facial musculature, creating an appearance of apathy, social disengagement or compromised cognitive status. Research in western cultures demonstrates that practitioners form negatively biased impressions associated with patient masking. Socio-cultural norms about facial expressivity vary according to culture and gender, yet little research has studied the effect of these factors on practitioners’ responses toward patients who vary in facial expressivity. This study evaluated the effect of masking, culture and gender on practitioners’ impressions of patient psychological attributes. Practitioners (N=284) in the United States and Taiwan judged 12 Caucasian American and 12 Asian Taiwanese women and men patients in video clips from interviews. Half of each patient group had a moderate degree of facial masking and the other half had near-normal expressivity. Practitioners in both countries judged patients with higher masking to be more depressed and less sociable, less socially supportive, and less cognitively competent than patients with lower masking. Practitioners were more biased by masking when judging the sociability of the American patients, and American practitioners’ judgments of patient sociability were more negatively biased in response to masking than were those of Taiwanese practitioners. Practitioners were more biased by masking when judging the cognitive competence and social supportiveness of the Taiwanese patients, and Taiwanese practitioners’ judgments of patient cognitive competence were more negatively biased in response to masking than were those of American practitioners. The negative response to higher masking was stronger in practitioner judgments of women than men patients, particularly American patients. The findings suggest local cultural values as well as ethnic and gender stereotypes operate on practitioners’ use of facial expressivity in clinical impression formation.
Keywords: USA, Taiwan, culture, gender, stigma, Parkinson's disease, facial expression, nonverbal communication, health practitioners
Introduction
The global prevalence of Parkinson’s disease (PD), one of the most common age-related neurodegenerative disorders, is increasing with the aging of the population (Global Parkinson’s Disease Survey Steering Committee, 2002). This progressive disorder is diagnosed at the average age of 60 and is characterized by slowness, stiffness and difficulty in initiating, coordinating and maintaining movement throughout the body. The face can lose expressive speed, elasticity and coordinated expression in the brow, eyes, cheeks, and lips: a condition called hypomimia or facial masking. The frozen position of pressed or slack lips and an unbroken stare creates the impression, regardless of its accuracy, of an asocial, cold, incompetent or apathetic person who fails to reciprocate others’ feelings of warmth, concern, interest or excitement (Brozgold et al., 1998; Smith et al., 1996). The mask interferes with social observers’ formation of accurate impressions regardless of whether the observer is a lay person (Borod et al., 1990; Brozgold et al., 1998; Hemmesch et al., 2009) or a health care practitioner (Lyons et al., 2004; Pentland et al., 1988, 1987; Tickle-Degnen & Lyons, 2004). The experience for people with masking has been characterized as an imprisonment of the self by an unresponsive body (Anonymous, 1999). Socially and mentally competent people with masking are confronted with the challenge of clearly presenting a self that overrides observers’ automatic and confidently-formed impressions of incompetence (Lyons & Tickle-Degnen, 2003; Pentland, 1991).
Previous research findings have suggested that the mask is stigmatizing, yet these findings are limited by being conducted in western countries with predominantly Caucasian ethnicity. There has been little investigation of the socio-cultural factors underlying this stigmatization and the implications for health care. Our specific purpose was to test the role of universal facial expressiveness norms, local cultural values, and ethnicity and gender stereotypes in the health care context of both western and eastern culture. We studied practitioners’ first impressions of patients, a point in time that is critical to the subsequent development and effectiveness of the therapeutic relationship (Martin et al., 2000).
Facial masking and stigma
Kurzban and Leary (2001) theorize that one of the origins of stigmatization lies in the importance of dyadic cooperation for human survival and the predictability of human behavior for this cooperation to occur. When individuals can quickly decipher one another’s thoughts and actions, they are able to respond effectively as a unit to the task at hand and to predict social and task outcomes. Decipherability is contingent on visible and dynamic physical displays of emotion, thought and motivation. By reducing physical cues of psychological states and traits, facial masking makes it less possible for observers to form reliable impressions of people with PD. When patients are masked there is a smaller correlation between the patients’ tested social attributes and observers’ impressions of these attributes than when patients are more facially expressive (Hemmesch et al., 2009).
This indecipherability sets the stage for social response biases and stereotype activation as the social observer attempts to understand an ambiguous situation. In western populations, it has been found that people who are more nonverbally expressive are viewed more favorably than are less expressive individuals (Boone & Buck, 2003). Social observers are likely to perceive expressive individuals as having nothing to hide, of being open and worthy of trust, and less expressive individuals as being deceptive or untrustworthy. Poor ability to mobilize upper face muscles that produce the eye crinkling of genuine smiling can create a look of deceptive smiling in people with PD even though they are experiencing genuine happiness (Pitcairn et al., 1990). The lack of predictability and the reduced perceived trustworthiness of the facially masked patient in comparison to more expressive patients may contribute to negative response biases among observers (Hemmesch et al., 2009; Pentland et al., 1988, 1987; Pitcairn et al., 1990; Tickle-Degnen & Lyons, 2004).
According to an ecological theory of social perception (McArthur & Baron, 1983; Zebrowitz & Collins, 1997), health care practitioners may automatically over-generalize their everyday social and implicit use of the face into the clinical context (Tickle-Degnen & Lyons, 2004). The practitioner may believe that the patient “is” what is perceived, and although this impression may change over time, it is unlikely to do so as rapidly as when the first impression was formed. A patient with facial masking may enter the health care relationship with, in the words of Goffman (1963), a “spoiled identity” created by a special kind of association between attribute and stereotype (p. 4). The attribute is the muting of facial expressivity and the stereotype is observers’ preconceptions about the meaning of muted expressivity. Practitioners’ impressions of patients can be unreliable indicators of patients’ actual psychological attributes without being stigmatizing, leading to poor diagnostic discriminations (Burn, 2002). However if this unreliability is paired with a belief that inexpressive people have less favorable psychological attributes than expressive people, a systematic stigmatization can occur.
Facial masking and culture
There are commonalities in the social meaning of behavior across cultures. Regardless of culture people believe facial behavior indicates emotion, character, and health (Yu, 2001), and favor pleasant affect over negative affect (DePaulo, 1992). There is also systematic variation in the meaning of social behavior as groups develop specific methods for living within their social environments (Markus & Kitayama, 2010; Zebrowitz & Rhodes, 2002). Yang and Kleinman (2008) and Yang and colleagues (Yang et al., 2007) have observed that individuals are stigmatized when others perceive them as failing to uphold important moral obligations and transactions of everyday social life. The stigmatized lose moral character in the eyes of the public and suffer diminished human value in domains that “matter most” to the local community.
We chose to study Taiwanese and American culture because of differing views of the social self in the world as related to expressive behavioral displays (Markus & Kitayama, 2010). East Asians construe the self as interdependent with others in one’s group and value submission of self in favor of the greater good. Personal development and intellectual achievement is a moral obligation to family and social life (Yang & Kleinman, 2008). A humble muting of personal expressiveness is seen as supportive of harmonious relationships with others and is highly valued within the Confucian tradition (Chang & Holt, 1994). Americans on the other hand construe the self as independent of others and value autonomous action and individuation of self identity. Personal skill at influencing others and drawing positive attention to the self and one’s opinions are valued (Markus & Kitayama, 2010).
A common stereotype is that Asians are less openly expressive than Americans. Consistent with the stereotype, Asians have a stronger normative system of display rules that control facial expressivity in response to emotionally evocative stimuli (Ekman, 1972; Fernandez et al., 2000; Okazaki et al., 2002). Asians, compared to Americans, view themselves and are viewed by others as less extraverted and expressive, especially related to assertiveness, personal agency, and activity (Cheung et al., 2001; Kashima et al., 1995; McCrae et al., 1998; Williams et al., 1998; Zhang et al., 1999). Vibrant and expressive sociable behavior is favored over quiet retiring behavior in American culture (Williams et al., 1998; Zillig et al., 2002). The implications, tested for the first time in this study, are that facial masking would be more deviant for Americans on attributes related to sociability, whereas more deviant for Taiwanese on attributes related to social harmony or intelligence.
Facial masking and gender
Universally held stereotypes and norms expect women to be more expressive emotionally than men. In fact women as compared to men are more emotionally expressive generally; their behavior is more responsive to the emotions and behavior of others, and their social interactions in close relationships are characterized by more emotionally supportive behavior (Eagly, 2009; Fischer & Manstead, 2000; Hall, 2006). The implications are that facial masking would be more deviant for women than men and put them more at risk of stigmatization. Consistent with this implication, Hemmesch et al. (2009) found that older adult peers judged the social supportiveness and interpersonal appeal of Americans with PD more favorably with lower than higher masking, and this effect was larger for women than men with PD. The present study extends these findings by investigating cross-cultural judgments of men and women with PD by health care practitioners.
Although women have been found to be more expressive than men across cultures, gender differences are larger in western cultures (Fischer & Manstead, 2000; LaFrance et al., 2003). Women in these cultures may be socialized to be more responsible for mediating interpersonal cohesion through their expressive behavior than women in communally-oriented cultures where family and community infrastructure and values support this cohesion. The variation of gender differences in expressivity across cultures suggests that gender effects of masking on judgments would be moderated by patient culture, with the most negative effects for American women relative to American men.
Specific Aims
This study was designed to respond to a gap in research on socio-cultural and multi-dimensional origins of global health care stigma (Keusch et al., 2006; Stuber et al., 2008). It aimed to document the degree to which there was a universal main effect of patient masking on practitioner judgments, and the degree to which practitioner culture (American v Taiwanese), patient culture (Caucasian American v Asian Taiwanese) and patient gender moderated the effect of patient masking on judgments. It was hypothesized that there would be a main effect of masking on practitioner judgments, such that practitioners would rate facially masked patients during videotaped interviews less favorably-- more depressed, less sociable, less socially supportive, and less cognitively competent-- than those showing normal to near-normal expressivity. We evaluated the degree to which practitioners’ judgments were more negatively biased in response to masking when evaluating patient attributes that were highly valued in the practitioners’ own culture (sociability for American practitioners, and social supportiveness or cognitive competence for Taiwanese practitioners), or those stereotypically related to patient ethnicity and culture (sociability in Caucasian American patients, and social supportiveness or cognitive competence in Asian Taiwanese patients). Finally we predicted that masking would yield less favorable judgments of the socio-emotional attributes (depression, sociability and social supportiveness) of women patients than men patients, and that this gender difference would be larger for practitioner judgments of American patients compared to Taiwanese patients.
Method
Study design
A balanced cross-cultural impression formation experimental design involved practitioners and patients in the United States and Taiwan (Elfenbein et al., 2002). American (n = 156) and Taiwanese practitioners (n = 128) viewed 80-sec excerpts from standardized, videotaped interviews of 24 patients, and rated each patient’s psychological attributes. Half of the patients had normal to near-normal facial expressivity (lower masking) and half a moderate degree of masking (higher masking), with equal numbers of Caucasian American and Asian Taiwanese women and men in each group. Participants provided informed consent through procedures approved by the institutional review boards of Boston University, participating Boston-area hospitals, and National Cheng Kung University Hospital. The study protocol was developed in English, translated into Mandarin Chinese and back-translated into English for cross-cultural consistency. The study was conducted from 2003 through 2008.
Practitioner participants
A total of 284 practitioners (n= 159 working clinicians, n=125 students in training) participated at their clinical or educational setting in a one-hour session. All clinical settings treated PD and all educational programs taught about PD. Working clinicians compared to students, on the average, were 10 years older in age (clinicians, M = 31.9, SD = 8.26; students, M = 22.4, SD = 3.01; p <.0001), had 76 months more clinical experience (clinicians, M = 90.1, SD = 91.14; students, M = 13.7, SD = 20.63; p <.0001), and rated themselves as more experienced in working specifically with PD [2.3 points higher on a five point rating scale from no experience (0) to high level of experience (5), p <.0001]. Practitioners came from the disciplines of physical therapy (n=96), occupational therapy (n=66), nursing (n=33), speech and language pathology (n=27), medicine (n=26) and others (such as psychotherapy, n=36). All but five practitioners had resided in their country for five or more years, indicating enculturation into local norms. Of the 156 Americans, 138 (88%) were women, 132 (85%) were Caucasian, 16 (10%) were of Asian ethnicity, and the remaining eight represented other ethnicities. Of the 128 Taiwanese, 93 (73%) were women, and all practitioners except one of unidentified ethnicity, were of Asian ethnicity.
Target patients
There were 24 target patients, a number that practitioners could judge feasibly in one hour. Equal numbers of Taiwanese and American men and women were selected from 45 community-living patients recruited purposefully for the study design. Americans were recruited through the Center for Neurorehabilitation at Boston University’s Sargent College of Health & Rehabilitation Sciences, USA, and Taiwanese were recruited through the Department of Neurology of National Cheng Kung University Hospital in Tainan, Taiwan. Inclusion criteria were diagnosis of PD, 40 or more yrs old, walking without supervision, independent communication, no disfigurement or abnormal choreiform movement in upper body, and 25 or more on the Mini-Mental Status Exam (Folstein et al., 1975). American patients scored ≤ 5 on the Geriatric Depression Scale-Short Form (GDS-SF) and Taiwanese patients scored ≤ 7 on the Taiwanese validated GDS-SF, indicating low probability of depression in their respective cultures (Chiu et al., 1998; Sheikh & Yesavage, 1986).
Target patients were in the earlier stages of PD (Hoehn &Yahr (1967) disease severity scale, M = 2.1, SD = 0.56, range 1–3), averaged 67.5 yrs old (SD = 8.12, range 54–81), averaged 5.7 yrs in duration of the disease (SD = 3.22, range <1–11), and reported mild to moderate impact of the disease on their quality of life (Parkinson’s Disease Questionnaire-39 summary index (Peto et al., 1998), M = 18.2, SD = 12.23, range 3.7 – 47.2). At screening patients had been assigned a preliminary score using the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale – Facial Expression score (UPDRS-F, Fahn & Elton, 1987), with 0 = normal to 4 = masked or fixed face, lips parted ¼ of inch or more with complete loss of expression. Final masking assessment was completed by five trained researchers using methods validated against the UPDRS-F (Lyons & Tickle-Degnen, 2005). They independently rated each video clip on a 5-point scale (1= low to 5= high duration, intensity or frequency) on five items-- eye and forehead expressivity, blinking, lower face expressivity, open lips when not speaking, and changing emotion in the face-- with adequate effective interrater reliability (ICC range= .78 – .88). The ratings were combined into a masking composite (Cronbach’s α = .86) and validated against ratings of active facial expressivity made by an independent set of four trained research assistants (r = −.92). Patients were categorized into lower versus higher masking along a median split within each gender by culture group. The average masking composite of lower masked patients (M = 2.5, SD = 0.40), who had normal to near-normal expressivity, was significantly lower than higher masked patients (M = 3.5, SD = 0.43, t(22) = 6.08, p < .0001) who had definite to moderate masking impairment on the UPDRS-F. There were no differences in masking due to gender (p =.87), culture (p = .71), or the interaction of gender and culture (p = .71).
Videotapes of patients
The patients were videotaped while being interviewed in their native language by a graduate research assistant blind to hypotheses. The camera was focused on the face and torso of the sitting patient. Video clips of 80-sec duration were extracted from standardized points when the patient was talking about a satisfying event or activity of the past week, a topic that stimulates expressive behavior in PD (Takahashi et al., 2010). Clips as short as 10 to 30 sec are valid for eliciting first impressions of behavior that predict important social outcomes (Ambady & Rosenthal, 1992; Carney et al., 2007). Patient speech content was electronically filtered from the audio channel of the clips by using a low-pass band at 400 Hz, which preserved vocal prosody (Van Bezooijen & Boves, 1986).
Measures administered to patients
In addition to tests administered at screening, patients were administered tests and self-report scales shown in Table 1 to compare to practitioners’ judgments. These measures were derived from research on psychological traits and values in America and Asia (Cheung et al., 1992; Gosling et al., 2003; Williams et al., 1998), formation of stereotypes and trait impressions (Chiu et al., 1997; Fiske et al., 2002; Mignon & Mollaret, 2002), and clinically-relevant competencies in PD (DeGroat et al., 2006; Gladsjo et al., 1999; Martin et al., 2000; Pentland et al., 1987). People with PD have been found to accurately self-report symptoms and traits as determined by correspondence with reports and observations made by family, friends, or trained researchers (Brown et al., 1989; Lyons & Tickle-Degnen, 2005; Mendelsohn et al., 1995; Mikos et al., 2009). There was no main effect of patient masking or interaction effect of gender or culture with masking (all p’s >.35) on any patient score. Furthermore, when patient scores were used as control measures against which to assess practitioner judgment bias due to masking (see Supplemental Document, [INSERT LINK TO ONLINE FILE]), findings generally replicated those reported in the Results section, which did not control for patient scores.
Table 1.
Dependent Variables and Measures (Cronbach’s α)
Variables | Patient Measures | Practitioner Judgments |
---|---|---|
Depression | Geriatric Depression Scale (Short Form) |
I feel the patient is… • Depressed, withdrawn |
Sociability |
I see myself as… (α = .52) • Outgoing, enthusiastica • Reserved, quiet (rev)a I prefer… (α = .53) • Having a lot of friends and family around • Talking with a lot of people |
I feel the patient is… (α = .47) • Outgoing, enthusiastica • Reserved, quiet (rev)a The patient would like …(α = .89) • Having a lot of friends and family around • Talking with a lot of people |
Social supportiveness |
I see myself as… (α = .19) • Sympathetic, warma • Critical, quarrelsome (rev)a I prefer …(α = .76) • Doing thoughtful things for friends and family • Activities that can promote interpersonal harmony |
I feel the patient is…(α = .78) • Sympathetic, warma • Critical, quarrelsome (rev)a • Likable, easy to develop rapport with The patient would like …(α = .89) • Doing thoughtful things for friends and family • Activities that can promote interpersonal harmony |
Cognitive competence |
I see myself as… (α = .53) • Open to new experiences, having broad interestsa • Conventional, uncreative (rev)a • Dependable, self-disciplineda • Disorganized, careless (rev)a I prefer … (α = .48) • Trying to learn new things • Keeping up with current events rather than ignoring them • Doing mentally-challenging rather than easy activities • Making my own decisions about daily activities Category Fluency Test (Animals) |
I feel the patient is… (α = .78) • Lower in intelligence, cognitively impaired (rev) • Dependable, self-disciplineda • Disorganized, careless (rev)a • Capable of participating in treatment and adhering to healthcare recommendations The patient would like…(α = .90) • Trying to learn new things • Keeping up with current events rather than ignoring them • Doing mentally-challenging rather than easy activities • Making his or her own decisions about daily activities |
Notes. All measures except Geriatric Depression Scale (Short Form) and Category Fluency Test (Animals) were self-report 7-point scales from 1 (disagree strongly) to 7 (agree strongly); (rev) = reverse-scored. Item groupings were derived from principal component analyses (PCA). Two activity preference items were dropped (not shown) from the study, self-managing medications and doing things alone, because PCA loadings were on social dimensions for patients and on cognitive competence dimensions for practitioners.
Items modified from Ten-Item Personality Inventory (Gosling et al., 2003).
Practitioner judgment procedure
Groups of practitioners were randomly assigned to view one of two random video clip orders, and, within each group, practitioners were assigned randomly to one of two 10-item judgment forms that corresponded to patient measures shown in Table 1. One form measured perceived traits related to depression, sociability, social supportiveness, and cognitive competence: “I feel the patient is [trait]”. The other form measured perceived preferences for engaging in sociable, socially supportive and cognitively challenging activities: “I think the patient would like [activity].” Principal component analyses and preliminary tests of the hypotheses showed that the trait and activity preference forms provided parallel assessments of perceived sociability, social supportiveness, and cognitive competence.
Statistical analyses
The practitioner was the unit of analysis. Each practitioner (N = 284) received a judgment score for sociability, social supportiveness and cognitive competence from the trait or activity preference form. Only those practitioners completing the trait form (N = 141) had a score for judged depression. For each dependent variable, each practitioner received eight scores derived by averaging the judgment scores of the three patients within each masking (lower v higher) by culture (American v Taiwanese) by gender (man v woman) group. Hypotheses were tested using four analyses of variance, one ANOVA for each dependent variable, with three between factors and three repeated measures. The between factors were practitioner culture (American v Taiwanese), practitioner status (clinician v student) and the controlled factor of judgment form (trait v activity preference). The repeated measures were patient masking, culture and gender. Hypothesis tests involved the independent variable of masking, either as a main effect or as an interaction effect. Effect size r’s were calculated to demonstrate the magnitude of support for the hypotheses and interpreted with Cohen’s (1988) guidelines for small (.10), medium (.30) and large (.50) effects. For interpretation of the results, the term masking bias refers to the difference score created by subtracting the ratings of patients with high masking from the ratings of patients with low masking, and reversing the sign for depression ratings only. A higher bias score indicates less favorable judgments of higher masked patients. Practitioner status (clinician v student) and its interaction with patient masking had little systematic or interpretable effects on practitioner judgments, and therefore, are described no further (see Schwab (2008) for a review of similar findings).
Results
Main effect of masking on practitioner judgments
The findings supported a universal biasing effect of patient masking on practitioner judgments with effects of large magnitude. Means and standard errors of practitioner judgments, averaged across practitioner culture, are shown in Table 2. Practitioners judged patients with higher relative to lower masking to be more depressed, F(1,137) = 389.11, p < .0001, r = .96, less sociable, F(1,276) = 1,105.37, p < .0001, r = .89, less socially supportive, F(1,276) = 203.48, p < .0001, r = .65, and less cognitively competent, F(1,276) = 405.11, p <.0001, r = .77.
Table 2.
Means and Standard Errors (in Parentheses) for Practitioner Ratings of Patients
Practitioner judgment |
Mask | American patients | Taiwanese patients | All patients | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Men | Women | All | Men | Women | All | Men | Women | All | ||
Depression (N = 141) | Lower | 3.44 (0.09) | 3.19 (0.08) | 3.31 (0.07) | 2.92 (0.08) | 2.94 (0.08) | 2.93 (0.07) | 3.18 (0.07) | 3.07 (0.07) | 3.12 (0.06) |
Higher | 4.41 (0.09) | 4.33 (0.08) | 4.37 (0.07) | 3.85 (0.08) | 4.06 (0.07) | 3.96 (0.06) | 4.13 (0.07) | 4.19 (0.06) | 4.16 (0.06) | |
All | 3.92 (0.07) | 3.76 (0.07) | 3.84 (0.06) | 3.39 (0.07) | 3.50 (0.06) | 3.45 (0.06) | 3.66 (0.06) | 3.63 (0.06) | 3.64 (0.05) | |
Sociability (N = 284) | Lower | 4.42 (0.05) | 4.95 (0.05) | 4.69 (0.04) | 4.81 (0.05) | 4.94 (0.05) | 4.88 (0.04) | 4.62 (0.05) | 4.95 (0.04) | 4.78 (0.04) |
Higher | 3.54 (0.05) | 3.53 (0.05) | 3.53 (0.05) | 3.92 (0.05) | 4.01 (0.05) | 3.97 (0.04) | 3.73 (0.04) | 3.77 (0.04) | 3.75 (0.04) | |
All | 3.98 (0.04) | 4.24 (0.04) | 4.11 (0.04) | 4.37 (0.05) | 4.48 (0.04) | 4.42 (0.04) | 4.17 (0.04) | 4.36 (0.04) | 4.27 (0.03) | |
Social supportiveness (N = 284) | Lower | 4.08 (0.05) | 5.03 (0.05) | 4.55 (0.04) | 4.56 (0.05) | 5.13 (0.04) | 4.84 (0.04) | 4.32 (0.04) | 5.08 (0.04) | 4.70 (0.04) |
Higher | 4.26 (0.05) | 4.72 (0.05) | 4.49 (0.04) | 4.01 (0.05) | 4.38 (0.04) | 4.20 (0.04) | 4.14 (0.04) | 4.55 (0.04) | 4.34 (0.04) | |
All | 4.17 (0.04) | 4.87 (0.04) | 4.52 (0.04) | 4.29 (0.04) | 4.75 (0.04) | 4.52 (0.04) | 4.23 (0.04) | 4.81 (0.03) | 4.52 (0.03) | |
Cognitive competence (N = 284) | Lower | 4.78 (0.05) | 5.45 (0.04) | 5.12 (0.04) | 4.82 (0.05) | 5.12 (0.04) | 4.97 (0.04) | 4.80 (0.04) | 5.29 (0.04) | 5.05 (0.04) |
Higher | 4.63 (0.05) | 4.76 (0.05) | 4.70 (0.05) | 4.44 (0.04) | 4.38 (0.05) | 4.41 (0.04) | 4.54 (0.04) | 4.57 (0.04) | 4.55 (0.04) | |
All | 4.70 (0.04) | 5.11 (0.04) | 4.91 (0.04) | 4.63 (0.04) | 4.75 (0.04) | 4.69 (0.04) | 4.67 (0.04) | 4.93 (0.04) | 4.80 (0.04) |
Interaction of masking and practitioner culture
Practitioner culture moderated the effect of patient masking on practitioner judgments of sociability and cognitive competence, with interaction effects generally of small magnitude. Fig. 1 shows that the bias of practitioners perceiving higher masked patients less favorably than lower masked patients was stronger for American practitioners than Taiwanese practitioners when judging sociability, F(1,276) = 4.91, p = .03, r = .13, and stronger for Taiwanese practitioners than American practitioners when judging cognitive competence, F(1,276) = 23.87, p < .0001, r = .28. American and Taiwanese practitioners were similarly sensitive to masking bias when judging depression, F(1,137) = 0.77, p = .38, r = .14, and social supportiveness, F(1,276) = 1.30, p = .26, r = .07. There were no significant three or four way interactions of masking and practitioner culture with patient culture or patient gender.
Figure 1.
American (A) and Taiwanese (T) practitioner ratings of low mask and high mask patients. Error bars are standard error.
Interaction of masking and patient culture
Patient culture moderated the effect of patient masking on practitioner judgments of sociability, social supportiveness and cognitive competence, with interaction effects varying from small to large magnitude. Table 2 shows that the negative impact of high patient masking was stronger in practitioner judgments of sociability of American compared to Taiwanese patients, F(1,276) = 23.35, p < .0001, r = .28. However masking bias was stronger when judging the social supportiveness, F(1,276) = 177.82, P < .0001, r = .63, and cognitive competence, F(1,276) = 13.16, P = .0003, r = .21 of Taiwanese patients relative to American patients. Practitioners demonstrated no significant difference in masking bias when judging depression in American versus Taiwanese patients, F(1,137) = 0.13, p = .72, r = .06.
Interaction of masking and patient gender
Patient gender moderated the effect of patient masking on practitioner judgments of the four psychological attributes, with interaction effects of medium to large magnitude. Table 2 shows that practitioners’ negative bias in response to higher masking was stronger when judging women than men for depression, F(1,137) = 5.14, p = .03, r = .35, sociability, F(1,276) = 32.05, p < .0001, r = .32, social supportiveness, F(1,276) = 73.36, p < .0001, r = .46, and cognitive competence, F(1,276) = 141.27, p < .0001, r = .58.
Interaction of masking, patient gender and patient culture
Patient culture moderated the gender-based masking bias in practitioner judgments of sociability, social supportiveness and cognitive competence, with interaction effects of small to moderate magnitude. Table 2 shows that practitioner gender-based masking bias was stronger when judging American than Taiwanese patients for sociability, F(1,276) = 27.11, p < .0001, r = .30, social supportiveness, F(1,276) = 12.10, p = .0006, r = .20, and cognitive competence, F(1,276) = 6.61, p = .01, r = .15. Practitioners demonstrated no significant difference in gender-based masking bias when judging depression in American versus Taiwanese patients, F(1,137) = 0.02, p = .90, r = .02.
Discussion
Practitioner judgments were less favorable for patients with more facial masking, extending previous findings in the United States and Europe to Asian culture. The masking symptom of PD interacted with practitioner culture, patient culture and patient gender to create a multi-dimensional potential for stigmatization. The same health symptom yielded less favorable health care judgments toward one group of patients than toward a different group. These discriminations might lead to health care disparities: the unjustifiable inequality of health care access, intervention, and outcomes across different groups of individuals (Commission on Social Determinants of Health, 2008).
Practitioners’ negative responses to masking applied their local cultural values when judging patients from either culture while simultaneously holding patients accountable to the patients’ own local cultural values and ethnic stereotypes (Albright et al., 1997; Yang & Kleinman, 2008; Yang et al., 2007). Specifically, practitioners from both cultures were more biased by masking when judging the sociability of the American patients, and American practitioners’ judgments of patient sociability were more negatively biased in response to masking than were those of Taiwanese practitioners. These results are consistent with the effect of masking to violate expectations for Americans to be outgoing, reflecting traditional American values of personal initiative and charisma. In contrast, practitioners from both cultures were more biased by masking when judging the cognitive competence and social supportiveness of the Taiwanese patients, and Taiwanese practitioners’ judgments of patient cognitive competence were more negatively biased in response to masking than were those of American practitioners. These results are consistent with expectations for Asians to be intelligent and interdependent, reflecting traditional Confucian values of self-development in the service of one’s social obligations (Kitayama & Markus, 2010; Shinn, 1986).
As predicted, practitioners’ judgments were more sensitive to facial masking in women than men patients. These gender results replicate findings with American lay observers’ social judgments of American patients (Hemmesch, 2010; Hemmesch et al., 2009). The present findings indicate that the gendered masking bias is resistant to traditional health care training and experience, extends to Taiwanese patients, and generalizes beyond impressions of socio-emotional attributes to cognitive competence. However, as predicted, the stronger negative response to masking in women than men was weaker for Taiwanese than American patients. This larger gender difference in the United States is compatible with the theory that women are held more accountable than men for behaving in a manner that promotes social cohesion in individualistic than in collectivistic cultures (Fischer & Manstead, 2000).
Limitations
The current study is limited to practitioner judgments of video clips. The practitioner role was one of observer and evaluator of distal patient behavior. This control over role and the selection of patients and video clips afforded a strong test of masking on first-impression bias and the influence of cultural values. Yet the context was artificial compared to actual clinical care, in which impressions are formed while dynamically engaged with patients, and repeated encounters create relational history. Extensive research findings (Rubin & Schrag, 1999; Schwab, 2008; Street & Haidet, 2011; Tversky & Kahneman, 1974) show first-impression bias to be resistant to revision in subsequent encounters, yet studies often address relationships of shorter duration than those required for long-term care of PD. New studies are needed to address effects of dynamic interaction on the initial strength of masking bias as well as the relative change in the strength of bias across repeated encounters of health care for PD and other chronic diseases.
Implications for clinical practice
Neurological practitioners have knowledge that relates masking to the movement disorder of PD, yet overuse the patient’s degree of facial expressivity to infer psychological traits. It is difficult for anyone to ignore the face and remain unbiased given the social mores surrounding the face, along with the physical confounding of parkinsonian masking with the expressive features of depression and dementia (Burn, 2002; Pentland, 1991). This difficulty may be compounded for practitioners, whose role in first encounters is to watch for behavior that is consistent or inconsistent with various psychological and physical diagnoses.
Biased impressions from facial expressivity can give rise to interpersonal self-fulfilling prophecies (Snyder et al., 1977). By believing highly masked patients to be less psychologically and socially capable than low-masked patients, practitioners may aggravate facial masking symptoms through their bias-confirmatory behavior, thus perpetuating the apparent validity of their initial impressions. They may interview the patient differently based on facial expressiveness and consequently elicit different behavior, such as social withdrawal in the case of higher masking and social engagement in the case of lower masking. For example, Takahashi et al. (2010) found that interviewers elicited more positive expressive facial behavior and less behavior typical of masking when they asked adaptive-coping questions of people with PD (e.g., “what did you find satisfying last week?”) compared to problem-oriented questions (e.g., what did you find difficult last week?). The patients appeared to be more hopeless and apathetic during the problem-oriented than adaptive-coping questions. Biased observations and interviewing may result in prescribed health regimens that are ill-suited to patient needs and poorly developed therapeutic relationships necessary for managing chronic disease (Martin et al., 2000).
People with PD can demonstrate their personalities through stories about their daily enjoyments and frustrations, and through self- and family advocacy (DeGroat et al., 2006; Lyons & Tickle-Degnen, 2003). In general, practitioners have been found to more accurately understand patients’ health preferences and beliefs when patients are active rather than passive participants in the encounter (Street & Haidet, 2011). Therefore, when working with people with PD, practitioners can draw upon patients’ story-telling and self-advocacy capacities by letting go of their reliance on the unresponsive face and asking about and attending to what matters most for people living with the disease.
To counter biases effectively, practitioners need training that addresses processes underlying accurate and faulty impression formation. When a situation is uncertain, perceivers resolve uncertainty by drawing upon previously formed implicit cultural and normative attitudes and beliefs to form a rapid judgment, after which they become overconfident in the correctness of the judgment. This overconfidence is augmented by automatic selective attention to evidence that confirms the impression and neglect of disconfirming evidence (Rubin & Schrag, 1999; Tversky & Kahneman, 1974). Consequently, training would involve developing and analyzing alternative explanations to the immediate impression, repeated feedback about accuracy, increasing awareness of the content and origin of one’s implicit attitudes, learning how to use statistical models of prediction (which are not yet available in the case of facial masking), and strategies for engaging in scientific reasoning, data monitoring, and triangulation of different sources of information to test one’s diagnostic success over time (Schwab, 2008).
The current study demonstrates that practitioners must learn how their clinical impressions are influenced by their cultural values associated with moral character and self-presentation, the intensity and implicitness of their ethnic stereotypes, and their expectations for what constitutes gender-appropriate demeanor and behavior. Practitioners can create alternative explanations for patient masking by varying the verbal content of interviews to elicit a broad range of potentially valid nonverbal and verbal cues of psychological attributes (Takahashi et al., 2010). They should translate immediate impressions based on physical signs of PD into hypotheses rather than conclusions, and test these hypotheses by triangulating information from the patient, family members, and the research literature.
Research highlights.
This study addressed gaps in research on socio-cultural and multi-dimensional origins of global health care stigma.
Tested for first time cross-cultural bias due to parkinsonian facial masking in practitioner judgments of patients.
Compared American and Taiwanese practitioners’ judgments of American and Taiwanese men and women patients.
Found that practitioners are biased by local cultural values and ethnic and gender stereotypes about facial expressivity.
Documents bias due to health conditions that mimic behavior locally perceived to be indicative of moral character.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
This study was supported in part by Grant Number NS048059 (PI, Tickle-Degnen; Co-I, Ma; Consultant, Zebrowitz) from the National Institute of Neurological Disorders & Stroke of the U.S. National Institutes of Health under the Fogarty International Center’s Stigma and Global Health Research Program. The authors thank, in Taiwan, Dr. Wen-Juh Hwang (Department of Neurology, National Cheng Kung University) and research assistant Pei-Ching Wu, and, in the U.S., Professor Melanie Matthias (Department of Communication Disorders, Sargent College of Health & Rehabilitation Sciences, Boston University) for consultation on sound editing, and many Boston University and Tufts University research associates and assistants, most notably Pai-chuan Huang. We appreciate the help of the research coordinators at the hospitals and clinics where the study sessions were conducted.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Contributor Information
Linda Tickle-Degnen, Department of Occupational Therapy, Health Quality of Life Lab, Tufts University, Medford, MA, USA.
Leslie A. Zebrowitz, Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, Waltham, MA, USA.
Hui-ing Ma, Department of Occupational Therapy and Institute of Allied Health Sciences, College of Medicine, National Cheng Kung University, Tainan, Taiwan.
References
- Albright L, Malloy TE, Dong Q, Fang X, Kenny DA, Fang X, Winquist L, Da Y. Cross-cultural consensus in personality judgments. Journal of Personal and Social Psychology. 1997;72:558–569. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.72.3.558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ambady N, Rosenthal R. Thin slices of expressive behavior as predictors of interpersonal consequences: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 1992;111:256–274. [Google Scholar]
- Anonymous. Parkinsonism. In: Kapur N, editor. Injured brains of medical minds: Views from within. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1999. pp. 169–171. [Google Scholar]
- Boone T, Buck R. Emotional expressivity and trustworthiness: The role of nonverbal behavior in the evolution of cooperation. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior. 2003;27:163–182. [Google Scholar]
- Borod JC, Welkowitz J, Alpert M, Brozgold AZ, Martin C, Peselow E, Diller L. Parameters of emotional processing in neuropsychiatric disorders: Conceptual issues and a battery of tests. Journal of Communication Disorders. 1990;23:247–271. doi: 10.1016/0021-9924(90)90003-h. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brozgold AZ, Borod JC, Martin CC, Pick LH, Alpert M, Welkowitz J. Social functioning and facial emotional expression in neurological and psychiatric disorders. Applied Neuropsychology. 1998;5:15–23. doi: 10.1207/s15324826an0501_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown RG, MacCarthy B, Jahanshahi M, Marsden D. Accuracy of self-reported disability in patients with parkinsonism. Archives of Neurology. 1989;46:955–959. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1989.00520450025014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burn DJ. Beyond the iron mask: Towards better recognition and treatment of depression associated with Parkinson's disease. Movement Disorders. 2002;17:445–454. doi: 10.1002/mds.10114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carney DR, Colvin CR, Hall JA. A thin slice perspective on the accuracy of first impressions. Journal of Research in Personality. 2007;41:1054–1072. [Google Scholar]
- Chang HC, Holt GR. A Chinese perspective on face as inter-relational concern. In: Ting-Toomey S, editor. The challenge of facework: cross-cultural and interpersonal issues. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press; 1994. pp. 95–132. [Google Scholar]
- Cheung FM, Leung K, Zhang J-X, Sun H-F, Gan Y-Q, Song W-Z, Xie D. Indigenous Chinese personality constructs: Is the Five-Factor model complete? Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2001;32:407–433. [Google Scholar]
- Cheung PC, Conger JJ, Hau K-T, Lew WJF, Lau S. Development of the multi-trait personality inventory (MTPI): Comparison among four Chinese populations. Journal of Personality Assessment. 1992;59:528–551. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa5903_8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chiu C-Y, Hong Y-Y, Dweck CS. Lay dispositionism and implicit theories of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1997;73:19–30. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.73.1.19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chiu HF, Lam LC, Chi I, Leung T, Li SW, Law WT, Chung DW, Fung HH, Kan PS, Lum CM, Ng J, Lau J. Prevalence of dementia in Chinese elderly in Hong Kong. Neurology. 1998;50:1002–1009. doi: 10.1212/wnl.50.4.1002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 3rd ed. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1988. pp. 79–81. [Google Scholar]
- Commission on Social Determinants of Health. Final Report of the Commission on Social Determinants of Health. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2008. Closing the gap in a generation: health equity through action on the social determinants of health. [Google Scholar]
- DeGroat E, Lyons KD, Tickle-Degnen L. Verbal content during favorite activity interview as a window into the identity of people with Parkinson’s disease. Occupational Therapy Journal of Research: Occupation, Participation, & Health. 2006;26:56–68. [Google Scholar]
- DePaulo BM. Nonverbal behavior and self-presentation. Psychological Bulletin. 1992;11:203–243. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.111.2.203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eagly AH. The his and hers of prosocial behavior: An examination of the social psychology of gender. American Psychologist. 2009;64:644–658. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.64.8.644. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ekman P. Universals and cultural differences in facial expressions of emotion. In: Cole J, editor. Nebraska symposium on motivation. Vol. 1971. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press; 1972. pp. 207–283. [Google Scholar]
- Elfenbein HA, Mandal MK, Ambady N, Harizuka S, Kumar S. Cross-cultural patterns in emotion recognition: Highlighting design and analytical techniques. Emotion. 2002;1:75–84. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.2.1.75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fahn S, Elton R. Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale. In: Fahn S, et al., editors. Recent developments in Parkinson’s Disease II. New York, NY: MacMillan; 1987. pp. 153–163. [Google Scholar]
- Fernandez I, Carrera P, Sanchez F, Paez D, Candia L. Differences between cultures in emotional verbal and nonverbal reactions. Psicothema. 2000;12 Suppl:83–92. [Google Scholar]
- Fischer AH, Manstead ASR. The relation between gender and emotion in different cultures. In: Fischer AH, editor. Gender and emotion: social psychological perspectives. Paris: Cambridge University Press; 2000. pp. 71–94. [Google Scholar]
- Fiske ST, Cuddy AJC, Glick P, Xu J. A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2002;82:878–902. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 1975;12:189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gladsjo JA, Miller SW, Heaton RK. Norms for letter and category fluency: Demographic corrections for age, education, and ethnicity. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Global Parkinson’s Disease Survey Steering Committee. Factors impacting on quality of life in Parkinson’s disease: Results from an international survey. Movement Disorders. 2002;17:60–67. doi: 10.1002/mds.10010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goffman E. Stigma: Notes on the management of spoiled identity. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall; 1963. [Google Scholar]
- Gosling SD, Rentfrow PJ, Swann WB., Jr A very brief measure of the big five personality domains. Journal of Research in Personality. 2003;37:504–528. [Google Scholar]
- Hall JA. Women’s and men’s nonverbal communication: Similarities, differences, stereotypes, and origins. In: Manusov V, Patterson ML, editors. The Sage handbook of nonverbal communication. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 2006. pp. 201–218. [Google Scholar]
- Hemmesch AR. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Waltham, MA: Brandeis University; 2010. The stigmatizing effects of facial masking and abnormal bodily movement on older adults’ first impressions of individuals with Parkinson’s disease. [Google Scholar]
- Hemmesch AR, Tickle-Degnen L, Zebrowitz LA. The influence of facial masking and sex on older adults’ impressions of individuals with Parkinson's disease. Psychology and Aging. 2009;24:542–549. doi: 10.1037/a0016105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoehn MM, Yahr MD. Parkinsonism: Onset, progression and mortality. Neurology. 1967;17:427–442. doi: 10.1212/wnl.17.5.427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kashima Y, Yamaguchi S, Kim U, Gelfand MJ, Choi S, Yuki M. Culture, gender, and self: A perspective from individualism-collectivism research. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1995;69:925–937. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.69.5.925. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keusch GT, Wilentz J, Kleinman A. Stigma and global health: Developing a research agenda. Lancet. 2006;367:525–527. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(06)68183-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kurzban R, Leary MR. Evolutionary origins of stigmatization: The functions of social exclusion. Psychological Bulletin. 2001;127:187–208. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.2.187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LaFrance M, Hecht MA, Paluck EL. The contingent smile: A meta-analysis of sex differences in smiling. Psychological Bulletin. 2003;129:305–334. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.2.305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lyons KD, Tickle-Degnen L. The dramaturgical challenges of Parkinson’s disease. Occupational Therapy Journal of Research. 2003;23:27–34. [Google Scholar]
- Lyons KD, Tickle-Degnen L, Henry A, Cohn E. Impressions of personality in Parkinson’s disease: Can rehabilitation practitioners see beyond the symptoms? Rehabilitation Psychology. 2004;49:328–333. [Google Scholar]
- Lyons K, Tickle-Degnen L. Reliability and validity of a videotape method to describe expressive behavior in Parkinson’s disease. American Journal of Occupational Therapy. 2005;59:41–49. doi: 10.5014/ajot.59.1.41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markus HR, Kitayama S. Cultures and selves: A cycle of mutual constitution. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2010;5:420–430. doi: 10.1177/1745691610375557. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin DJ, Garske JP, Davis MK. Relation of the therapeutic alliance with outcome and other variables: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology. 2000;68:438–450. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McArthur LZ, Baron RM. Toward an ecological theory of social perception. Psychological Review. 1983;90:215–238. [Google Scholar]
- McCrae RR, Yik MSM, Trapnell PD, Bond MH, Paulhus DL. Interpreting personality profiles across cultures: Bilingual, acculturation, and peer rating studies of Chinese undergraduates. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998;74:1041–1055. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.74.4.1041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mendelsohn GA, Dakof GA, Skaff M. Personality change in Parkinson’s disease patients: Chronic disease and aging. Journal of Personality. 1995;63:233–257. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1995.tb00809.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mignon A, Mollaret P. Applying the affordance conception of traits: A person perception study. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2002;28:1327–1334. [Google Scholar]
- Mikos AE, Springer US, Nisenzon AN, Kellison IL, Fernandez HH, Okun MS, Bowers D. Awareness of expressivity deficits in non-demented Parkinson disease. Clinical Neuropsychology. 2009;23:805–817. doi: 10.1080/13854040802572434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Okazaki S, Liu JF, Longworth SL, Minn JY. Asian American-White American differences in expressions of social anxiety: A replication and extension. Cultural Diversity & Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2002;8:234–247. doi: 10.1037/1099-9809.8.3.234. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pentland B. Body language in Parkinson’s disease. Behavioural Neurology. 1991;4:181–187. doi: 10.3233/BEN-1991-4306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pentland B, Gray JM, Riddle WJR, Pitcairn TK. The effects of reduced non-verbal communication in Parkinson’s disease. British Journal of Disorders of Communication. 1988;23:31–34. doi: 10.3109/13682828809019874. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pentland B, Pitcairn TK, Gray JM, Riddle WJR. The effects of reduced expression in Parkinson’s disease on impression formation by health professionals. Clinical Rehabilitation. 1987;1:307–313. [Google Scholar]
- Peto V, Jenkinson C, Fitzpatrick R. PDQ-39: a review of the development, validation and application of a Parkinson’s disease quality of life questionnaire and its associated measures. Journal of Neurology. 1998;245 suppl 1:S10–S14. doi: 10.1007/pl00007730. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pitcairn TK, Clemie S, Gray JM, Pentland B. Nonverbal cues in the self-presentation of Parkinsonian patients. British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1990;29:177–184. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1990.tb00867.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rubin M, Schrag JL. First impressions matter: A model of confirmatory bias. Quarterly Journal of Economics. 1999;114:37–82. [Google Scholar]
- Schwab AP. Putting cognitive psychology to work: Improving decision-making in the medical encounter. Social Science & Medicine. 2008;67:1861–186. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheikh JI, Yesavage JA. Geriatric Depression Scale GDS: recent evidence and development of a shorter version. In: Brink TL, editor. Clinical gerontology: A guide to assessment and intervention. New York, NY: Haworth Press; 1986. pp. 165–173. [Google Scholar]
- Shinn DC. Education and the quality of life in Korea and the United States: A cross-cultural perspective. Public Opinion Quarterly. 1986;50:360–370. [Google Scholar]
- Smith MC, Smith MK, Ellgring H. Spontaneous and posed facial expression in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 1996;2:383–391. doi: 10.1017/s1355617700001454. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snyder M, Tanke ED, Bersheid E. Social perception and interpersonal behavior: On the self-fulfilling nature of social stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1977;35:656–666. [Google Scholar]
- Street RL, Haidet P. How well do doctors know their patients? Factors affecting physician understanding of patients’ health beliefs. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2011;26:21–27. doi: 10.1007/s11606-010-1453-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stuber J, Meyer I, Link B. Stigma, prejudice, discrimination and health. Social Science & Medicine. 2008;67:351–357. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.03.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takahashi K, Tickle-Degnen L, Coster WJ, Latham N. Expressive behavior in Parkinson’s disease as a function of interview context. American Journal of Occupational Therapy. 2010;64:483–494. doi: 10.5014/ajot.2010.09078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tickle-Degnen L, Lyons KD. Practitioners’ impressions of patients with Parkinson’s disease: The social ecology of the expressive mask. Social Science & Medicine. 2004;58:603–614. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(03)00213-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tversky A, Kahneman D. Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science. 1974;185:1124–1131. doi: 10.1126/science.185.4157.1124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Bezooijen R, Boves L. The effects of low-pass filtering and random splicing on the perception of speech. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research. 1986;15:403–417. doi: 10.1007/BF01067722. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams JE, Satterwhite RC, Saiz JL. The importance of psychological traits: A cross-cultural study. New York: Plenum; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Yang LH, Kleinman A. ‘Face’ and the embodiment of stigma in China: The cases of schizophrenia and AIDS. Social Science & Medicine. 2008;67:398–408. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.03.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang LH, Kleinman A, Link BG, Phelan JC, Lee S, Good B. Culture and stigma: Adding moral experience to stigma theory. Social Science & Medicine. 2007;64:1524–1535. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.11.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yu N. What does our face mean to us? Pragmatics & Cognition. 2001;9:1–36. [Google Scholar]
- Zebrowitz LA, Collins MA. Accurate social perception at zero acquaintance: The affordances of a Gibsonian approach. Personality and Social Psychology Review. 1997;1:204–223. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0103_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zebrowitz LA, Rhodes G. Facial attractiveness: Evolutionary, cognitive, and social perspectives. Westport, CT: Ablex; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Zhang K, Lee Y, Liu Y, McCauley C. Chinese-American differences: A Chinese View. In: Lee Y, McCauley CR, Draguns JG, editors. Personality and person perception across cultures. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1999. pp. 127–138. [Google Scholar]
- Zillig LMP, Hemenover SH, Dienstbier RA. What do we assess when we assess a Big 5 trait? A content analysis of the affective, behavioral, and cognitive processes represented in Big 5 personality inventories. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2002;28:847–858. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.